A Colour Harmony Model For Two-Colour Combinations: Li-Chen Ou, M. Ronnier Luo
A Colour Harmony Model For Two-Colour Combinations: Li-Chen Ou, M. Ronnier Luo
Two-Colour Combinations
Abstract: This study investigates harmony in two-colour an accurate prediction.13–17 These studies can be divided
combinations in order to develop a quantitative model. A into two categories: those based on the orderly arrangement
total of 1431 colour pairs were used as stimuli in a psycho- of colours and those based on the interrelationship between
physical experiment for the visual assessment of harmony. colours.
These colour pairs were generated using 54 colours se- Studies based on orderly arrangement of colours included
lected systematically from CIELAB colour space. During those by Ostwald,1 Munsell,2 Itten,3 and Nemcsics.4 The
the experiment, observers were presented with colour pairs general idea behind these studies was that colours could
displayed individually against a medium gray background harmonize only when they were selected systematically
on a cathode ray tube monitor in a darkened room. Colour from a hue circle or from a specific “path” within an ordered
harmony was assessed for each colour pair using a 10- colour space. This is only an assumption, however, and
category scale ranging from “extremely harmonious” to seems to be true only if the selected colours are presented to
“extremely disharmonious.” The experimental results the viewer in an ascending or descending sequence of hue,
showed a general pattern of two-colour harmony, from lightness, or chroma. As Granville14 pointed out, “it is the
which a quantitative model was developed and principles smooth visual progression through colour space that gives
for creating harmony were derived. This model was tested the harmonious results, not that the colours bear some
using an independent psychophysical data set and the re- unique harmonious relationship to each other.”
sults showed satisfactory performance for model prediction. The studies based on the interrelationship of colours
The study also discusses critical issues including the defi-
included those by Goethe,5 Chevreul,6 Moon and Spen-
nition of colour harmony, the relationship between harmony
cer,7–9 Albers,10 and Chuang and Ou.11 The general idea
and pleasantness, and the relationship between harmony
was that colours could harmonize only when they were
and order in colour. © 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 31,
complementary or analogous (similar) in ether hue, light-
191–204, 2006; Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.
ness, or chroma. While the definitions of “complementary”
wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.20208
and “analogous” varied from study to study, they were
Key words: colour harmony; colour psychology; colour determined by considering the effect of colour contrast
order; colour pleasantness; colour combinations (in terms of colour difference) between each constituent
colour in a combination. The theories in this category
INTRODUCTION have been tested experimentally, but none of them was
acceptable in providing an accurate prediction of colour
Colour harmony has been of enormous significance in art harmony.15–17
and design. While the basic mechanisms of colour percep- As a result, some artists claimed that there were no laws
tion have been extensively studied, the semantic and aes- of colour harmony15 and that colour harmony was important
thetic aspects of colour—the very phenomena that give only for scientists.18 However, the poor performance of
colour its psychological power—are still poorly understood. existing theories was in fact due to the difficulties that the
There is a long tradition of research into colour harmo- earlier studies encountered. These difficulties included a
ny,1–11 but none of the studies was acceptable either in wide disagreement over the definition of colour harmony,
explaining the concept of colour harmony12 or in providing a large number of factors that may influence colour
harmony, and a lack of modern colour-science techniques
*Correspondence to: L. Ou (e-mail: [email protected]) for the earlier studies. Discussion of these issues is given
© 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. below.
Derby, UK. Among them were 4 from the School of Art & indicating the degree of harmony for each colour pair; the
Design and 13 from the Colour & Imaging Institute. Each higher the score, the more harmonious the colour pair appeared
observer has passed Ishihara’s Tests for Colour Deficiency.29 to the observers. These scores were analyzed using CIELAB.
During the experiment, each observer was presented with To examine the consistency of observer responses, experi-
colour stimuli on a CRT monitor at a viewing distance of 50 mental data were evaluated in terms of inter-observer and
cm and a visual field size of 10° for each colour stimulus. intra-observer agreement. The inter-observer agreement is
As shown in Fig. 2, each colour pair was displayed in the concerned with how well the observers agreed with each
center of the screen with 10 numbered buttons arranged in other. This agreement was determined by averaging the root
a line underneath. These buttons were divided into two mean squared (RMS) between each observer’s data and the
sides, i.e., “harmonious” and “disharmonious.” Each side overall mean:
冑 冘共 x ⫺ x 兲
consisted of five categories labeled using numbers from 1 to 2
5, representing “just perceptibly,” “quite,” “moderately,” i i
i
“very,” and “extremely,” respectively (see the Appendix). RMS ⫽ , (1)
By pressing one of these buttons, observers indicated their N
judgements as to how harmonious the colour pair appeared. where xi represents the category number given by an ob-
All of the colour pairs were presented individually in a server for stimulus i, x i represents the mean category num-
random sequence. In each pair, the left–right position of ber of all observers for stimulus i, and N is the number of
each constituent colour was also randomized. stimuli.
The experiment was divided into six sessions, each pre- As a result, the agreement was slightly better among females
senting 239 colour pairs (except for Session 6, which pre- (with an RMS value of 2.05) than among males (2.35).
sented only 237 pairs). In each session, observers assessed Intra-observer agreement, on the other hand, is concerned
harmony, each taking approximately 15 min. To examine with the repeatability of each single observer’s colour har-
repeatability of observer responses, Session 1 was dupli- mony judgements. This agreement was determined by av-
cated once after all six sessions had been finished. This eraging the RMS for each observer’s experimental results
replicated session is called Session 7. For each observer, the between Sessions 1 and 7. This was done by replacing x i of
whole experiment took about 3 weeks to complete. Eq. (1) with yi where xi and yi are category numbers for
stimulus i obtained in Sessions 1 and 7, respectively. The
results show that the intra-observer agreement was slightly
RESULTS
better among females (with an RMS value of 2.18) than
Torgerson’s Law of Categorical Judgement30 was used in this among males (2.57).
study to transform the experimental data onto an interval scale. These results were then compared with the authors’ pre-
The resulting scale values are called colour harmony scores, vious studies into colour emotion and colour prefer-
ence.23–25 The comparison results showed that all of these Testing Existing Colour Harmony Principles
studies, including the present research, had the same level of
Some of the existing colour harmony principles were tested
consistency of observer responses for both inter-observer
using the present experimental data. These principles in-
and intra-observer agreements. Note that in these studies,
cluded the following:
the number of observers participating in each experiment
ranged from 17 to 31. This suggests that the consistency of
visual responses in such studies does not vary significantly ● Complementary hue1–7,10,11— colours can harmonize if
once there are sufficient observers (in this case approxi- they are complementary in hue. In the present study, the
mately 17). following hue pairs were regarded as being complementary:
TABLE II. Test results of existing colour harmony principles in terms of CD values.
Complementary hue Equal hue Equal lightness Equal chroma Equal tone
represent the locations of the four green colours. White the observers. Note that in the other two lightness sections,
bubbles with broken outlines represent the corresponding a few gray bubbles are presented near the corresponding
locations of each green colour shown at different lightness locations of the vivid green. This suggests that this vivid
sections. green can harmonize with a colour if they have different
In Fig. 3a, the vivid green is located in the low-lightness lightness but similar hue and chroma. This pattern was also
section, where only white bubbles having fine outlines are identified for all the other green colours, as shown in Figs.
shown at the same lightness level. This means that any 3b to d. It was even found for all the other colour pairs used
colour pair generated by this vivid green and a low-lightness in this study. This pattern had two components, one con-
colour tended to have a negative value for the colour har- cerned with the colour difference in lightness and the other
mony score, i.e., these colour pairs were disharmonious to the difference in chroma and hue. The former will be
discussed later in the next section. The latter is called in this number of gray bubbles with bigger sizes seen in the high-
study the effect of chromatic difference. This effect indi- lightness section. This suggests two tendencies for two-
cates that the colour harmony score tends to decrease as the colour harmony:
chromatic difference (a mixture of chroma difference and
hue difference) becomes larger between the constituent co- ● small lightness difference between the constituent colours
lours. This can be illustrated in Figs. 4a and b, where the in a colour pair tended to reduce the harmony in that pair
curve is determined by the following equation: and
● high lightness values of the constituent colours tend to
H C ⫽ 0.04 ⫹ 0.53tanh共0.8 ⫺ 0.045⌬C兲, (3)
enhance the harmony.
in which
The former is labeled the effect of lightness difference
1
⌬C ⫽ 关共⌬H *ab兲2 ⫹ 共⌬C*ab/1.46兲2 兴2 , (⌬L), as previously mentioned. The latter is labeled the
effect of lightness sum (Lsum). This lightness sum effect was
where HC represents colour harmony score; ⌬H*ab and ⌬C*ab also identified in the results shown in Fig. 3c. In this
are CIELAB colour difference values in hue and chroma, diagram, the gray bubbles in the high-lightness section are
respectively. greater in both their area and their number than in the
The effect of chromatic difference agreed with a conven- low-lightness section.
tional colour harmony theory called the equal hue principle. A somewhat clearer relationship between harmony and
This principle asserts that colours can harmonize if they lightness sum is shown in Fig. 6a, where the colour har-
share the same hue 1,2,4,6,7. mony score is plotted against lightness sum. The diagram
shows that the harmony score tends to increase with light-
ness sum. This trend can be represented by the curve shown
Lightness: Difference and Sum
in Fig. 6b, which was defined using the equation
In addition to the effect of chromatic difference (⌬C), the
results shown in Figs. 3a to d also suggest another colour H Lsum ⫽ 0.28 ⫹ 0.54tanh共 ⫺ 3.88 ⫹ 0.029Lsum) (4)
harmony factor, the effect of lightness difference (⌬L), as in which
previously mentioned. This factor indicates that the har-
mony of a colour pair tends to decline if the constituent L sum ⫽ L*1 ⫹ L*2 ,
colours share the same lightness. However, according to a where HLsum represents colour harmony score; L*1 and L*2
further analysis, this effect did not show a significant impact
on harmony as did the effect of chromatic difference. As
illustrated in Fig. 5, colour harmony score is plotted against
lightness difference. This diagram depicts wide scattering,
indicating no clear connection between lightness difference
and harmony. This implies there are still many factors other
than lightness difference that can affect harmony.
Another lightness-related factor was also identified in this
study and was labeled lightness sum (Lsum). This factor was
again revealed by means of the bubble charts method. As
shown in Fig. 3a, no gray bubble is presented in the low-
lightness section and only a few gray bubbles are shown in FIG. 5. Colour harmony score plotted against lightness
the medium-lightness section. However, there are a larger difference.
are lightness values of the constituent colours in a colour diction can be solved. The resulting Eq. (6) had significantly
pair. better predictive performance, as shown in Figs. 9a and b.
To ensure validity of the effect of lightness sum, mean
colour harmony scores of the following nine colour combi- H L ⫽ H Lsum ⫹ H⌬L (6)
nation types were examined: LL, LM, LH, ML, MM, MH, In the literature, the effect of lightness sum (Lsum) was
HL, HM, and HH. For each combination type, L represents never mentioned. The effect of lightness difference (⌬L), on
a low-lightness colour; M a medium-lightness colour; and H the other hand, agreed to some extent with findings by
a high-lightness colour. According to Eq. (4), these combi- Moon and Spencer7 and Chuang and Ou.11 The two studies
nation types should exhibit the following trend: HH ⬎ both claimed that small lightness difference led to colour
HM ⬎ HL, MH ⬎ MM ⬎ ML, and LH ⬎ LM ⬎ LL, as disharmony.
shown in Fig. 7a, where the greater-than sign stands for
“being more harmonious than”. However, the test results
showed a slightly different trend. As illustrated in Fig. 7b, Hue Effect
HH has a lower mean score than HM, and MM has a lower In the literature, hue has been the main focus of colour
mean score than ML. harmony studies.1–11 According to the experimental results,
The results shown in Fig. 7b indicate that Eq. (4) over- as shown earlier, hue had a significant impact on two-colour
predicts when the two constituent colours are of the same harmony in terms of the hue difference, i.e., the less the hue
lightness. This problem echoes the effect of lightness dif- difference (and likewise for chroma difference) between the
ference, i.e., a small lightness difference between constitu- constituent colours in a colour pair, the more likely it is that
ent colours in a pair reduces the harmony of that pair. This the colour pair appears harmonious. The present section
effect can be illustrated by Fig. 8, which is defined as describes a further investigation into the effect of hue on
H ⌬L ⫽ 0.14 ⫹ 0.15tanh共 ⫺ 2 ⫹ 0.2⌬L兲, (5) harmony, where the hue angle of each constituent colour
was treated as the main variable.
where As shown in Fig. 10a, the mean colour harmony scores
for each of the seven hues used in this study are plotted: red,
⌬L ⫽ 兩L* 1 ⫺ L* 2兩.
orange, yellow, green, cyan, blue, and purple (see Table I).
By combining Eqs. (4) and (5), the problem of overpre- In this diagram, “blue colour pairs” (i.e., the colour pairs
FIG. 7. (a) Predicted results from Eq. (4). (b) Visual results for mean colour harmony scores of colour pairs generated from
three lightness levels: high (H), medium (M), and low (L).
FIG. 10. (a) Mean colour harmony scores for colour pairs
FIG. 9. Performance of model prediction by (a) Eq. (4) and that contain a colour of a specific hue. (b) The results shown
(b) Eq. (6). in (a) separated by seven tones.
1. Ostwald W. The color primer. From the German edition “Die Farben-
fibel” of 1916. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1969.
2. Munsell AH. A grammar of color. From the original version of 1921.
New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1969.
3. Itten J. The art of color. Translated by Ernst van Haagen from German
edition “Kunst der Farbe”. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1961.
4. Nemcsics A. Colour dynamics: environmental colour design. New
York: Ellis Horwood; 1993.
5. Goethe JW. Theory of colours. Translated by C. L. Eastlake (1840)
from German edition “Farbenlehre” of 1810. Introduction by D. B.
Judd. MIT Press; 1970.
FIG. 16. The model applies to harmony in the area of a 6. Chevreul ME. The principles of harmony and contrast of colors.
boundary between two uniform colours. Translated by C. Martel (1854) from the first French edition of 1839.