Sport, Physical Activity and Educational Achievement - Towards An Explanatory Model

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/305691772

Sport, physical activity and educational achievement – towards an


explanatory model

Article  in  Sport in Society · July 2016


DOI: 10.1080/17430437.2016.1207756

CITATIONS READS

25 2,189

1 author:

Richard Bailey

96 PUBLICATIONS   3,052 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Player Development in Sport View project

HEPAS (Healthy and Physically Active Schools in Europe) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Bailey on 11 December 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Sport in Society
Cultures, Commerce, Media, Politics

ISSN: 1743-0437 (Print) 1743-0445 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/fcss20

Sport, physical activity and educational


achievement – towards an explanatory model

Richard Bailey

To cite this article: Richard Bailey (2016): Sport, physical activity and educational achievement
– towards an explanatory model, Sport in Society, DOI: 10.1080/17430437.2016.1207756

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17430437.2016.1207756

Published online: 27 Jul 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=fcss20

Download by: [University of Central Lancashire] Date: 28 July 2016, At: 15:30
Sport in Society, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17430437.2016.1207756

Sport, physical activity and educational achievement –


towards an explanatory model
Richard Baileya,b
a
International Council of Sport Science and Physical Education, Berlin, Germany; bRBES Ltd, Birmingham, UK
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

ABSTRACT
Sport and other forms of physical activities have traditionally held
an ambiguous place within schooling, often being pushed to the
margins. At the same time, there is a consensus that such activities
are necessary for the healthy development of young people. This
was proven during the second half of the last century, representing
a revolution in the understanding of health. Recent developments
in neurology, psychology and related sciences hint at a second
revolution in which a strict distinction between mind and body has
become redundant. This article examines the case that sport and
physical activity can make distinctive contributions to educational
achievement, and a host of wider benefits. Focusing on cognitive
functioning, psychosocial development, school engagement and
general educational attainment, the article reviews the available
evidence and concludes that there is sufficient reason to believe
that sports and physical activity can make useful contributions to
educational achievement.

Introduction – Mens sana in corpore sano?


The Roman poet Juvenal (undated), writing around the first century AD, popularized the
view that there was a close relationship between physical health and mental power, with
the maxim, Mens sana in corpore sano (‘a sound mind in a healthy body’). In fact, Juvenile
was paraphrasing an earlier saying attributed to the ancient Greek philosopher Thales,
who offered a simple prescription for the good life that still seems plausible today: ‘Who is
happy? The person who has a healthy body, a resourceful mind and a docile nature’ (Laertius
1925). Juvenile’s words were an early signal of a tradition of linking the development and
functioning of the mind and of body that continues to the present day. For most of this
time, this tradition competed unsuccessfully with another, present in the writing of Plato,
and strands of the Christian religion, but most associated with the French philosopher René
Descartes. This ‘Cartesian’ view portrayed the body not as an inseparable partner of the
mind, but as its rather brutish vessel. It is not difficult to see the influences of this tradition
on most models of schooling and their disembodied conceptions of human beings, in which
the mind is both the subject and object of education, and the body is a service and transport
role that is separate and inferior (Overton 2008).

CONTACT  Richard Bailey  [email protected]


© 2016 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2    R. Bailey

It is only quite recently that science has caught up with the insights of the ancient Greeks
to the extent that it can now speak with any confidence about the relationships between
the body and the mind, and between physical activity and mental functioning. The evi-
dence is beginning to suggest that Juvenile and Thales may have been on to something.
Recent developments in educational research, psychology and brain science have led to a
new level of understanding of the relationships between physical activity and educational
achievement. New evidence has brought about a revolution in the understanding of how
young people learn and develop.
Schools are complex places and exert their influence in many ways. The first part of
this article reviews the historical context in which physical activities find themselves in
discussions of their place in education. As the introduction makes clear, there is a strongly
held cultural bias that presumes that matters of the body are adjunct to education, rather
than part of it, and this continues to delineate and limit the use of physical activity in the
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

curriculum. The article goes on to examine a number of factors that seem to be particularly
important to the issue of the nexus of physical activity and educational achievement:
• cognitive functioning;
• psychosocial development;
• school engagement;
• educational attainment.
So, this article discusses the potential contributions sport and other forms of physical activ-
ity can make to educational achievement. The relevance of some of these mechanisms to
achievement in school might seem self-evident. Cognition has a powerful effect on learning,
as do psychosocial factors, like emotional security and the development of friendships. The
importance of school engagement, which is primarily about relationships between young
people and adults, has been less discussed by researchers. However, there is evidence to
suggest that young people’s perceptions of school as a place that is friendly and caring are
key influences on academic achievement (Lawson and Lawson 2013). Most studies that have
explored possible associations between activity and schooling have focused on examina-
tion results and formal assessment scores, and the appeal of these relative simple measures
for researchers is obvious. Improving performance in standardized tests is important, of
course. Examination results and school grades increase access to further educational and
vocational opportunities. But it does not follow from this acknowledgement that they are
the only, or even the primary, goals of schooling. There is certainly a practical imperative
for supporting child to succeed in terms of test scores and examinations, but this should not
be at the expense of wider and less easily measured outcomes. As this article makes clear,
sport and other physical activities can contribute to both the narrow aim of examination
success and the less-easily defined psychological and social outcomes that help children
live lives of flourishing.

Physical activity and the brain


Serious scientific investigations of the relationship of the brain and physical activity date
back to the early 1980s, when scientists discovered that activity increased secretion of
endorphins (hormones secreted within the brain and nervous system). This effect has since
been linked to a variety of psychological changes, such as: positive mood state changes and
Sport in Society   3

euphoria; physical changes to the brain, such as increased cerebral blood flow; growth of
nerve cells in the brain’s centre of learning and memory, increased density of networks of
nerve cells and increased brain tissue volume (Harber and Sutton 1984). These changes make
a compelling list since they affect learning directly. One group of changes represent what
could be called the psychology or ‘mindset’ of learning; another reflects the neurophysiol-
ogy or ‘mechanism’ of learning. It is necessary to examine the effects of physical activity on
both the mindset and the mechanism of learning to understand the ways in which it might
impact on educational achievement. Figure 1 offers a simplified heuristic of this relationship.
Numerous tests and measures have been developed since those early days. Some of these
infer cognitive processes from the observation of behaviour (e.g. the ability to complete cog-
nitive tests), but rapid advances in understanding truly began when reliable and accessible
methods were developed that allowed direct measures of brain activity. The development
of these technologies has offered a new perspective on the subject of physical activity and
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

the brain (Boecker et al. 2012).

Physical activity and cognitive function


Most studies in the area of physical activity and cognitive functioning have been with
older adults, but there is growing evidence from research with young people to support
these findings, too (Hillman, Erickson, and Kramer 2008). Physical activity play during
early childhood is known to have profound effects on youth people’s overall development,
supporting health muscular–skeletal growth, perceptual and motor skill development and
the ability to calibrate emotional reactions to an unpredictable world (Doherty and Bailey
2003). Movement affects all aspects of the young child’s life, including the developing brain,
and activity influences the electrical activity occurring in the cerebral cortex, which is the
area of the brain most responsible for cognition (Lardon and Polich 1996). Studies report
increasing levels of physical activity and aerobic fitness lead to faster reaction time, improved
attention and faster cognitive processing (Hillman et al. 2009). Learning and practicing
movement skills, such as those used in sports, add an extra dimension to this contribution.
While scientists are still trying to understand the precise mechanisms underlying these
changes, the pattern of findings suggests that changes are most marked during childhood
(Hillman, Castelli, and Buck 2005). This supports the contention that the first 10 or so
years represent a crucial period in development, and consequently that the preschool and
primary phases of schooling are not just the first, but are also the most important in terms
of predicting later educational and career success (Bailey et al. 2010).
Research has shown that regular physical activity leads to improvements in a range of
cognitive functions, including information processing, attention and executive function
(Chaddock et al. 2011). Positive relations have been reported between physical activity

Figure 1. The relationships between mindset and mechanisms of learning and educational achievement.
4    R. Bailey

and cognitive performance in school-age children in a range of measurement categories,


including perceptual skills, IQ, verbal and mathematic tests. Studies have found a pos-
itive relationship between increased physical activity and concentration and sleep, and
both of these factors can affect the ability to engage with school (Short et al. 2013). A US
study tested the hypothesis that regular physical activity has its greatest effect in terms of
executive function (processes used it to perform activities such as planning, organizing,
paying attention to and remembering details and managing time and space). Inactive and
overweight primary school-aged children undertook a controlled experiment involving
13-week after-school programme. Cognitive tests of different aspects of executive function
carried out before and after the experiment showed that physical activity was associated
with improved performance in each task, with the most positive results being found among
those who exercised the longest each day. Significant improvements in mathematics test
scores were also observed (Davis et al. 2011).
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

Even relatively brief bursts of physical activity seem to be able to contribute to improved
cognitive functioning. In fact, there seems to be residual effects of short bursts of activity:
benefits continue to contribute to executive functioning after the activity has finished. A two-
year study of primary school students found that 10–15 min of classroom-based physical
activity that were matched to core academic subjects such as spelling and mathematics saw
they were more likely to have higher scores than those who did not experience the interven-
tion (Hollar et al. 2010). An earlier study found that aerobic exercise for 5–10 min improved
arithmetic performance (Molloy 1989). The beneficial effects of a burst of moderate intensity
exercise for as little as five minutes have been found to increase brain processes, improve
focus and enhances cognitive control for up to one hour later (Pontifex et al. 2013), leading
one set of researchers to the conclusion that physical activity should be scheduled ‘before
important subjects like mathematics and not at the end of the school day’ (Kubesch et al.
2009). Cognitive function may also benefit from increased energy generation, as well as
breaks from sedentary (such as classroom-based) work. Whilst most studies have tested the
effects of short-term interventions, effects may be more sustainable when physical activity
is maintained over a period of time. An important set of studies in this regard relates to
the Executive Function Hypothesis, with findings suggesting that aerobic exercise results
in improved general intellectual performance, and that the most profound effects are seen
when tests were focused on executive function (Tomporowski et al. 2008). Findings like
these support the case for so-called ‘Active Classrooms’, which are standard classrooms
incorporating physical activity breaks or movement activities throughout the day.
The relationships between physical activity and the brain are complex, and there is a great
deal more to learn. Although it is known that exercise modifies the biological structures
and systems of the brain. It is less well understood how exactly this happens and how these
changes affect human behaviour. In addition, the scale of the effects of physical activity
on the brain are probably not as great as is sometimes represented in the popular media,
and caution needs to be shown when basing decisions and ambitions on interpretations of
scientific research. Nevertheless, cautious optimism seems to be justified.

Fitness and cognitive functioning


Physical fitness, especially cardiorespiratory fitness can affect the brain in numerous ways.
As with measures of physical activity, some of the most persuasive evidence related to
Sport in Society   5

fitness comes from older people, and research with young people is much less advanced.
The evidence base of the relationship between exercise and cognitive functioning in younger
adults is mixed, with some studies reporting associations between cardiovascular fitness and
cognitive performance in young adults (Hillman, Castelli, and Buck 2005), and others sug-
gest no relationship (Etnier et al. 2006). Generally speaking, it seems likely that fitness does
help cognitive functioning. For example, a seminal study reported a relationship between
participation in an intensive physical education programme (that included activity breaks
during classroom lessons) and educational achievement in a primary school (Ismail 1967).
Standardized tests showed that children taking part in the 8-month programme improved
their performance in a range of academic tests compared to those in traditional physical
education classes. Studies of recess/break times have corroborated these results (Donnelly
and Lambourne 2011).
An important study from Sweden examined physical fitness, intelligence scores, school
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

achievement and sibling data from every man born between 1950 and 1976 who enlisted for
military service at age 18 (Åberg et al. 2009). Analysis revealed a positive association between
cardiovascular fitness and cognitive performance at 18 years of age. In addition, changes
in physical fitness measures between 15 and 18 years predicted cognitive performance at
18 years, and cardiovascular fitness during early adulthood predicted socio-economic status
and educational attainment later in life. The authors of this study interpreted these findings
in terms of support for the development of ‘brain plasticity’ (the ability to adapt to external
conditions and challenges), and the ‘cardiovascular fitness hypothesis’ (the hypothesis that
cardiovascular (i.e. aerobic) fitness is the physiological mediator that explains the rela-
tionship between exercise and improved cognitive performance) as a result of exercise in
early adulthood. These findings are relevant, as they not only suggest that physical activity
improves academic performance, but also that activity enhances the general efficiency of
the brain (Hillman, Erickson, and Kramer 2008).
In support of these claims, intervention studies using vigorous aerobic-based exercise
reported enhanced performance in a range of cognitive tasks (Davis et al. 2007). The effect
seems to be restricted to cardiovascular fitness and does not include muscular strength,
which suggests that fitter people have greater cardiorespiratory efficiency and benefit from
facilitating the brain functions that positively influence brain plasticity and cognitive func-
tion (Cotman, Berchtold, and Christie 2007).

Physical activity and psychosocial factors


Psychosocial factors are the psychological and social aspects of human development. They
are often treated as a pair because they are inseparable. These factors influence the edu-
cational experience of young people; they can support or hinder success because being
successful at school requires students to perform a range of social as well as personal and
academic tasks. And, of course, in addition to mastering subject matter and performing
well in tests, school is also an important setting for the development of friendships, social
identities and a sense of community belonging. The potential number of psychosocial factors
is vast. This section focuses on a small number of themes, chosen because of their close
relationship with educational achievement.
6    R. Bailey

Self-esteem
Self-esteem indicates the degree to which an individual feels positively about him or herself,
and individuals with high self-esteem as individuals that respect themselves, acknowledge
their own limitations and expect improvement and growth (Rosenberg et al. 1995). It gen-
erally arises when an individual succeeds, is praised, or experiences love from another, and
is lowered by failure, harsh criticism and rejection. Self-esteem is associated with emotional
stability and adjustment, and low self-esteem features in many forms of mental illness and
poor health behaviours (Fox 1999). Self-esteem is often presented as both a marker for
general well-being and as an indicator for recovery from personal difficulties. Some psy-
chologists go so far as to say that self-esteem is the core of mental health as it represents a
self-rating of overall worth (Landers and Arent 2001).
A widely held belief among both researchers and practitioners is that self-esteem signif-
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

icantly influences achievement in education and other areas of life. Research conducted on
the relationship between academic achievement and self-esteem has generally supported
a correlational relationship, and a causal relationship between self-esteem and students’
future academic and career aspirations has been suggested (Chiu 1990). The persuasiveness
of this hypothesis lies in the fact that ability and effort are complements, so that, under the
premise that individuals are uncertain about their own ability, higher self-esteem causes
better performance in educational assessments. Additionally, many studies have reported
positive relationships between high self-esteem and variables that may be assumed to be
related to educational achievement, such as greater persistence on difficult tasks, independ-
ence and achievement-oriented traits (Baumeister et al. 2003). These qualities are obviously
desirable in educational settings, and students who possess the abilities to persist in the face
of challenge, work independently and in a goal-orientated way are more likely to perform
well at school than those lacking such capabilities.
Physical activity can contribute to self-esteem. Physical competence and appearance
(especially among women) are prestigious (albeit in different forms) in all cultures.
Competence can lead to a sense of personal effectiveness and feelings of autonomy, and
these are associated with the promotion of self-esteem. In fact, among young people, the
physical self may be particularly important in the development of self-esteem. Recently,
studies have suggested that the situation may be more complex than appearance suggest.
Part of the difficulty is that many of the most positive studies have been of relatively low
quality. The situation is also confused by the fact that self-esteem can be measured and
expressed in a variety of ways (Ekeland, Heian, and Birger Hagen 2005). There is evidence
that physical activity can strengthen individual’s physical self-worth, and this is an impor-
tant factor underlying positive perception of oneself, where global (general) self-esteem is
assessed, however, findings are mixed (Sonstroem and Potts 1996).
For both boys and girls, the relationship between sport participation and global self-es-
teem seems to be dependent on peer acceptance, and girls can be particularly vulnerable to
negative judgements when participating in sport (Daniels and Leaper 2006). A Canadian
study of 16–17-year olds found that, whilst participation had a slight positive association
with self-esteem (both physical and global) for boys and girls, more feminine individuals
who participated in competitive sports reported lower levels of perceived athletic com-
petence and global self-worth, but reported higher self-esteem when they participated in
non-competitive sports (Bowker, Gadbois, and Cornock 2003).
Sport in Society   7

Self-efficacy
Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s belief that he or she can execute the courses of action
required to obtain a desired outcome. Unlike self-esteem, which reflects how young people
feel about their worth or value, self-efficacy reflects how confident individuals are about
performing specific tasks. Self-efficacy is specific to the task being attempted, and indicates
the extent to which individuals believe they have the skills to succeed. A growing body of
research reveals that there is a positive relationship between self-efficacy beliefs and aca-
demic performance. Those with a strong academic self-efficacy have a greater willingness
to choose challenging tasks, more effective use of learning strategies, less anxiety, enhanced
effort and approved academic achievement, compared to those with a weak a sense of
self-efficacy (Pintrich, Roeser, and de Groot 1994).
Research has found that regular physical activity can support the development of self-effi-
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

cacy. A study from Chile evaluated the effects of a school-based physical activity programme
on physical fitness and mental health among adolescents. The experimental study found
that the programme was associated with increased self-efficacy among the young people
(Bonhauser et al. 2005). A similar study from the US measured the physical activity of
young people (10–16 years of age) over the course of a week and found that self-efficacy
and social influences correlated with more intense physical activity (Strauss et al. 2001).
Finally, a high-quality, systematic review found the majority reported physical activity was
positively associated with self-efficacy, while a minority saw no effect (Cataldo et al. 2013).

Friendships and peer relationships


Friendships and peer relationships are important for young people’s healthy development.
Adolescents often spend more time with their peers than with their family members. The
development and maintenance of peer acceptance contributes to inclusion in the classroom
and play activities, and provide emotional support. They can also affect current and future
well-being, and academic achievement (Carroll et al. 2009). Educational achievement, of
course, is the focus of this article, and research has demonstrated a significant link between
peer relationships and success at school (Ryan and Ladd 2012). For example, children enter-
ing kindergarten with existing friends and those able to develop new friendships appear
to make better adjustment to school than others (Wentzel and Caldwell 1997). Similarly,
Primary-aged and lower secondary-aged students who are not well accepted by their class-
mates tend to do less well than more popular children, and have a greater risk of becoming
disaffected or dropping out of school completely during the secondary years (Woodward
and Fergusson 2000). During adolescence, friends influence levels of engagement with
school, as well as decisions to spend time studying or socializing.
Physical activity can be a catalyst of developing friendships during childhood and adoles-
cents. Early friendships begin within the context of playing, and physical activity play is the
first occurring and most frequent form of such play behaviour. Consequently, frequent and
appropriate physical activity play experiences may help young children develop friendships,
and it has been found to be especially helpful for those with limited or poor social skills.
There is a widely held belief that sport and other socially orientated activities are natural
settings for the development of friendships. Sport offers a range of social development
opportunities, and friendships with other players have been identified as a primary reason
8    R. Bailey

many young people participate (Denault and Poulin 2009). In addition, participants spend
a considerable amount of time together outside of the activity, such as travelling to and from
sessions or waiting between competitions. This informal time provides opportunities to
share experiences and discuss values and future aspirations. In a seminal study, researchers
from the US and Belgium undertook in-depth interviews with a group of sports participants
(8–16 years), asking how they perceived friendship in sport. They spoke of the importance
of companionship, pleasant play, self-esteem enhancement, help and guidance, prosocial
behaviour, intimacy, loyalty, things in common, attractive personal qualities, emotional
support, the absence of conflicts and conflict resolution. Participants also acknowledged
negative aspects of friendship in sports: conflict, unattractive personal qualities, betrayal
and inaccessibility. This study’s primary purpose was to map out the dimensions of young
people’s views of friendship in sport (Weiss and Petlichkoff 1989), but it also highlights the
complexity of the issue. However, it is difficult to doubt the importance of physical activity
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

contexts for the development of friendships throughout childhood and youth.

Preventing harmful behaviours


Sexual activity, substance use and psychological adjustment change over the course of child-
hood and adolescence, and risky behaviours and emotional problems are most likely to
appear during early adolescents and continuing to increase over the teenage years (Institute
of Medicine and National Research Council 2011). Early drug and alcohol use is associated
with lower levels of educational attainment, and young people who begin drinking or using
drugs, or who become dependent on alcohol, nicotine or marijuana as teens are less likely to
finish college than those who do not use these substances until later in life or never become
dependent (Grant et al. 2012). Similarly, adolescent pregnancy and parenthood are major
causes for dropping out of school and low educational attainment, even among students
with high ambitions (McCarthy and Grodsky 2011).
Sport and other physical activities have often been promoted as constructive alternatives
to harmful behaviours. The evidence, however, is mixed. Most studies have found generally
positive relationships between out-of-school participation in athletic activities and healthy
adolescent development. In terms of risky behaviours like under-age drinking and substance
use, teenage sex and similar harmful behaviour, sports participation can be protective for
both boys and girls. Girls, in particular, seem to be less likely to engage in these activities
(Miller et al. 2005), although some studies have found sports participation related to higher
levels of substance use and higher rates of sexual activity for males (Feldman and Matjasko
2005). Perhaps these negative outcomes can be explained by the effects of peer pressure in
sub-cultures or by a propensity for risky behaviours. These negative findings do not refer
to significant numbers of people, but highlight an inherent danger within some sporting
environments. An important influence on whether sports participation is associated with
positive or negative behaviours is the length of engagement with the activity, with the
greatest benefits coming with greater time (Zaff et al. 2003).

The special issue of competitive team sports


There is evidence of a variety of psychological and social benefits associated with partici-
pation in sport during childhood and adolescents. However, recent research has tended to
Sport in Society   9

prioritize the case for physical activity, in general, over sport. This is, in part, in response
to the clear shortcomings in some forms of sports provision, such as bullying and other
abusive behaviour by adults, overemphasis on competition, a narrow range of activities
offered, and a popular perception that competitive sport appeals primarily to boys and
young men (Dworkin and Larson 2006).
Whilst not denying any of these concerns, it is worthwhile preserving a place for team
sports among the menu of physical activities presented to young people, especially with
regard to the psychosocial outcomes they can offer. Research indicates that children who
regularly play sport are more likely to be active in adulthood than those who do not take part
in childhood sport (Zimmermann-Sloutskis et al. 2010). Child development and health are
multifaceted, and different types of activity are associated with different outcomes. So, it is
not surprising that there is evidence suggesting young people benefit most from sampling
a variety of activities (Côté, Strachan, and Fraser-Thomas 2008), although there may be a
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

danger of over-commitment, where young people pass through a threshold after which the
benefits start to reduce. Participation in competitive sport has been associated with positive
social and psychological outcomes that would be expected to contribute to success at school,
such as: improved self-esteem; self-regulation; goal attainment; and leadership skills (Bailey
et al. 2013). These outcomes seem to be closely tied to their social contexts.
Relationships with others are vitally important for healthy child and adolescent devel-
opment, and sport offers an engaging context for social interactions. Positive involvement
in team sports has been associated with warm relationships with coaches, skill develop-
ment and peer support (Boone and Leadbeater 2006). Additionally, sports participation
has been shown to build skills in preparation, persistence and increased sense of belonging
(Rosewater 2009). Each of these findings is compatible with the idea that team sport, appro-
priately presented, offers a positive forum for social connectedness. Some of the benefits
from participation in team sports during childhood and adolescents last a long time after
individuals stop playing (Bailey et al. 2013). It is important, though, to acknowledge that
there are also reasons to question the simplistic presumption that sport necessarily leads
to positive outcomes. Although participation in sports contexts can potentially promote
healthy development in young people, ‘it is best not to take the relationship [between
participation and healthy development] as a “given”; it can be difficult to achieve; and can
only be realized in association with a series of conducive “change mechanisms’’’(Whitelaw
et al. 2010, 65). A study from the US provides an interesting example (Dawkins, Williams,
and Guilbault 2006). While it is true that there was an association between sports team
membership and reduced risky behaviours for some students (specifically black females),
for others the pattern was reversed (black males, white females and males). Likewise, a
survey of 10,000 middle and high school students found that those who participated in
daily sports had higher odds of having intercourse than their peers who did not play a
sport daily (Habel et al. 2010). And an analysis of longitudinal data-sets examined the
relationships between involvement in high school sports and antisocial behaviours, and
found the relationship between athletic involvement and deviance varied, depending upon
the behaviours examined.
A significant, perhaps determining, factor in these studies seems to be the social climate
in which sports take place. Most of studies treat team sports as a unitary experience, and
take no account of the values of the programmes, behaviour of coaches and social envi-
ronment within teams. This presents researchers and practitioners with something of a
10    R. Bailey

challenge, as positive evidence demonstrates the potential for team sports to contribute to
social development, whilst the negative findings highlight the danger of inappropriate or
ill-thought-out provision. At the least, this predicament would seem to demand a somewhat
more cautious stance when making claims on behalf of team sports than is sometimes the
case, but it also highlights the point that values that are embedded in those experiences
influence the outcomes of participation. At least in terms of psychosocial development,
positive development through physical activities requires teachers, coaches and parents
who enforce positive values.

Physical activity and school engagement


School engagement refers to students’ sense of belonging, attachment or emotional engage-
ment with the life of the school. Engagement is a motivational foundation of competence,
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

where competence reflects a balance between adherence to social expectations and the
achievement of positive outcomes for the individual. In other words, engagement is the
pursuit of goals that are both socially and personally important. With respect to schools, this
perspective suggests that students are engaged if they pursue goals that are valued by them-
selves, as well as by teachers, parents and peers. This is an important vehicle for increasing
positive outcomes for students, including successful school completion because engaged
students achieve more and are happier at school than their disaffected peers (Ackerman
2013). There are, of course, degrees of engagement, but the evidence is strong that those who
are motivated to achieve will obtain higher levels of educational attainment, and conversely,
low levels of attainment discourage students (Linnenbrink and Pintrich 2002). The most
engaged with schooling outperform those who are less engaged or absent because engaged
students are likely to exhibit behaviours related to learning, such as concentrating, exerting
effort, taking the initiative and being persistent in the face of failure (Hughes et al. 2008).
The problem of truancy and exclusion from school has become serious problem in many
countries. The impact of this on educational attainment has been a concern for policy-­
makers, but there is also a wider concern about the issue. Emerging research evidence sug-
gests that truancy/exclusion has been identified as a significant antecedent to delinquency,
dropout and other undesirable outcomes (Maynard et al. 2012). Despite these significant
efforts and millions of Euros spent, there is little evidence that any positive impact has been
made on school attendance (Attwood and Croll 2006).
Many studies have tested and found empirical support for the claim that students feel
more motivated to engage with school and to value what it can do for them when they feel
supported by adults, while disconnection from school can lead to disaffection, truancy
and dropout (Archambault et al. 2009), as well as increased risk of substance abuse and
mental health problems (Bond et al. 2007). To date, few studies have demonstrated a direct
association between connectedness and academic achievement, and the most plausible
explanation for this is that connectedness alone is not enough; and, students also require
teachers who hold high yet achievable expectations.
The interaction between motivation and learning is close, and creates either a virtuous or
vicious cycle: good outcomes inspire student motivation, which drives positive outcomes;
poor results negatively influence motivation, which in turn influences negative outcomes.
The virtuous cycle tends to encourage continued engagement with schooling, while the
vicious cycle often results in discouragement, disaffection and dropout (Tinto 1987). Early
Sport in Society   11

intervention is needed that focuses on individual motivation if the vicious cycle is to be


broken, and this has been one of the most frequently given justifications for the use of
sports-based settings into the educational domain.
School engagement is used here as an umbrella concept for several different components
of students’ attitudes, behaviours and feelings – psychological; cognitive; and academic.
Psychological engagement refers to a feeling of belonging with the school, connection to
teachers. Time spent in organized sports activities predicts a positive attitude to school and
belonging, and higher educational attainment some years later (Fredricks and Eccles 2006).
Participants in organized sports experience a greater sense of school belonging and satisfac-
tion, and a negative association has been found between school attachment and unstructured
pursuits, such as watching television, or spending time with friends (Blomfield and Barber
2010). Adolescents who participate in sports, performing arts, school-involvement activities
or academic clubs have all been found to experience a greater enjoyment of school during
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

the latter part of secondary schooling (Eccles et al. 2003). Extracurricular involvement in
a variety of activities has been associated with lower dropout rates and reduced incidence
of delinquency and substance use (Mahoney and Cairns 1997). More generally, students
participating in leisure activities after-school tend to have positive feelings towards their
teachers and other adults.
Cognitive engagement refers to issues of self-regulation and valuing of learning. There is
some evidence that young people who are physically active during the school day tend to be
more disposed to learn and develop more positive attitudes towards learning and discipline
(Keays and Allison 1994). One study used a nationally representative sample of about 1200
public high schools to examine the relationship between school sports participation rates
and in-school delinquent behaviours. Results indicated schools with higher proportions
of sports participants reported fewer serious crimes and suspensions occurring on school
grounds (Veliz and Shakib 2012). Most data in this area were gathered with relatively small
samples, so some caution is called for, and, in light of the obvious influence of contextual
factors (e.g. school and team ethos, teachers’ values and behaviours, money) it is not sur-
prising that a subset of sports participants (especially young men) have been identified as
placing lower value on school and being more likely to take part in risky behaviour that
may affect their schooling (Fredricks and Eccles 2006).
Finally, academic engagement relates to factors like attendance, participation, grades and
credits earned. Studies have found that students who participate in sports activities were
twice as likely to attend school as opposed to those who did not. A 5-month programme
involving attendance monitoring, sports participation and a ‘moral character’ class found
significant differences between intervention and control groups in terms of reduced absen-
teeism, increased educational expectations, improved attitude toward education and general
school engagement. One review examined sports and physical activity programmes designed
to address antisocial behaviours such as truancy and more serious crimes. Although the
authors acknowledged there was limited evidence, their conclusion was that there was some
evidence to support the claim that these activities provided an effective vehicle through
which personal and social development in young people can be positively affected (Makkai
et al. 2003). It has been claimed that school-sponsored sports programmes can also build
the illusive ‘school spirit’, which might be better translated as school engagement. One
study found that sports participation in high school was positively related to a number of
12    R. Bailey

outcomes at and following the end of compulsory schooling, including self-esteem and
academic achievement (Marsh 1993).

Physical activity and educational attainment


The discussion has reached the heart of the issue. Educational attainment, such as success
in examinations and grades, is highly valued by parents, teachers and policy-makers. There
is no doubt that educational achievement relates to later career success, especially among
marginalized groups within society (Jacobson and Mokher 2009). The standard position
among many decision-makers has been that classroom-based subject areas like mathematics
and reading are of the highest value, and it is essential that they are prioritized. A second
assumption is that any interference with time for these areas must be resisted. So, schools
have found themselves under pressure to squeeze out non-core subjects like the arts and
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

sport, not because they are perceived as unimportant, but because they are of a lower priority
in a crowded curriculum space. These practices have been reinforced by parents’ fears of the
consequences of time away from the academically prestigious subject areas. Consequently,
physical education and sport are often relegated to the positions of enjoyable, but frivolous,
recreations that distract from the main business of schooling.
Many studies have examined the relationships between physical activity, physical fitness
and academic achievement. A particularly relevant and recent study is based on the impor-
tant Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, based at the University of Bristol
in the UK. This birth cohort study has intensively tracked 14,000 pregnant women, their
children and partners over two decades (Boyd et al. 2012). The Avon researchers measured
almost 5000 children’s physical activity for a week at age 11 with accelerometers. They found
that the 11-year-old girls in their sample who were more active were performed better on
standardized school tests of English, mathematics and science. Also, those who were more
physically active at age 11 did better at school as teenagers, and the most active girls did
especially well in science (Booth et al. 2013). Since the researchers adjusted the results to
account for factors that could affect school performance, including birth weight, current
weight, a mother's smoking while pregnant and the family's socio-economic situation, it
seems that physical activity had either a direct or indirect effect of the educational achieve-
ment recorded.
The majority of these studies in this area have focused on fitness, and there have con-
sequently been difficulties in separating variables (for example, it might be argued that
children from relatively wealthy family have better access to both high-quality physical
activity opportunities and high quality schooling). However, more recently, there have been
a number of studies examining the effects of both short bouts and sustained physical activity
on academic performance (Hillman, Erickson, and Kramer 2008). Overall, studies support
the claim that physical activity, in different forms, contributes to academic achievement.
Some studies have focused on increasing levels of activity during school physical educa-
tion lessons, whilst others have introduced it as a separate programme. There is also the
phenomenon of classroom physical activity breaks. Even the most cautious commentators
acknowledge that significant increases in physical activity cause no harm to achievement in
other classroom subjects, despite the fact that they have necessarily had their time reduced
(Trudeau and Shephard 2008). Adding time to classroom-based subjects by taking time
away from physical activity programmes does not improve performance in those subjects,
and is likely to lead to a host of personal and health consequences (Bailey 2006).
Sport in Society   13

A fascinating experiment was carried in with a group of 10- and 11-year children in
Vanves, a district of Paris, France in the 1950s. The teacher-researchers ring-fenced 26% of
the school day for physical activities. Morning and afternoon, half the children exercised,
whilst a control group continued to take part in the standard two hours a week of physical
education. Despite the loss of a quarter of classroom teaching time, the academic grades of
the experimental group did not worsen, often actually improving. The teachers also reported
fewer discipline problems, greater attentiveness in class and less absenteeism. Of course,
this was a small-scale study, albeit with a control group. The findings were not published in
scientific journals, so they were not exposed to the standard quality measures. In addition,
it is always difficult to unpick the relative value and harm done by the multitude of factors
experienced by young people. Even if the reported findings are taken on face value, it is
impossible to say with any confidence that physical activity was the key factor. If its con-
clusions turn out to be valid, however, they raise important questions about the standard
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

ways in which schools organize and prioritize their responsibilities.


A number of studies have returned to the same basic question asked at Vanves: What
happens if a block of the school timetable is replaced with physical activity? The first of
these studies was conducted in Trois-Rivières, Canada, in the 1970s, and tracked more
than 500 primary-aged students for 6 years, with control groups being formed from the
classes preceding and succeeding classes in the same schools. The experimental classes had
an hour-a-day of physical activity, leading to a 14% reduction in time for the other school
lessons, while the control classes were taught for the standard 40 min of physical education
per week. According to teacher assessments, the overall educational performance of the
daily activity group was better than their less active peers. They also scored higher on a
standard intelligence test, and in mathematics examinations (although they did less well in
English tests) (Trudeau et al. 1999).
There have now been many international studies exploring the question of the relation-
ship between physical activity during the school day and educational performance, and
they have tended to come to the same conclusions: replacing a sizeable amount of time
for classroom-based subjects with physical activities does not harm performance in those
subjects, and in some circumstances seems to result in improvements in grades (Booth
et al. 2013). A second set of studies approached the matter from a different angle: How does
physical fitness affect educational performance? Physical activity and fitness are related, of
course, as fitness tends to be greatest among the most active. However, fitness is an elusive
measure as it is impossible to separate the contribution made to fitness from school-based
physical activity in school from that which takes place before and after school, over the
weekend, and during vacations. Also, children’s bodies and brains are in a state of constant
change, and older, more physically mature children tend to outperform their younger,
smaller peers, anyway (Bailey 2005). Despite these cautions and concerns, numerous studies
have found that physically fit children tend to outperform their less fit peers. According
to one meta-analysis, increased fitness was associated with better grades in mathematical,
reading and IQ scores (Fedewa and Ahn 2011). That it is these particular measures that
seem to be most associated with physical fitness is, in itself, intriguing as these are precisely
the areas that would be expected to be affected by improved brain functioning (McClelland,
Pitt, and Stein 2015).
Consider, too, a study that found that, in the words of its title, ‘Schools with Fitter Children
Achieve Better Literacy and Numeracy Results’ (Telford et al. 2012). The researchers found
14    R. Bailey

that both activity and fitness levels correlated with government tests scores, and that schools
with fitter children outperformed the others. The explanation for these outcomes offered
by the researchers was that physical activity and fitness are elements of schools that tend to
have atmospheres that are supportive of educational achievement and success, and this is
plausible. However, there may also be a supplementary account. Many studies report associa-
tions between physical activity and physical fitness and academic achievement. It would be a
mistake to view this as a disappointing finding. On the contrary, such are the complexities of
child development and educational performance that a single factor is unlikely to have more
than a weak association (Farb and Matjasko 2012). Educational outcomes are influenced by
many forces differentially across various contexts, so physical activity alone should not be
expected to have large or consistent effects on educational outcomes. However, the effects
of exercise on the brain are almost inseparable with effects of other factors such as diet and
sleep. Young people who are fit and active, well fed and well rested are more likely to have a
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

successful school career than those who are not. So, beyond the positive effects associated
with physical activity and physical fitness, they should also be understood as part of a syn-
ergistic whole that is likely to have a profound effect in shaping the educational, personal
and social lives of young people. In addition, positive effects during the early stages of life
can lead to multiplied and magnified benefits in the long term (Basch 2011).
The traditional position among many decision-makers has been that classroom-based
subject areas like mathematics and reading are of the greatest value, and that it is essential
that these areas are prioritized within schools. A second assumption is that any interfer-
ence with student success in these areas must be resisted at all costs. Consequently, many
schools have found themselves under pressure to squeeze out non-core subjects like the
arts and sport, not because they are unimportant, but because they are of a lower priority.
These practices have been reinforced by parents who fear the consequences of time away
from the most prestigious subject areas. So, physical activities, like sport and dance, are
often relegated to the positions of enjoyable, but frivolous recreations that distract from the
main business of schooling. In this context, it is hardly surprising that every study from the
last few decades of the state and status of physical activity opportunities in school – and
especially their manifestations as curriculum physical education and sport – has reported
serious cause for concern. Some school systems have little or no place for these physical
activity experiences. Others keep a place in the timetable, but their quality is undermined
by poorly trained or untrained teaching staff, inadequate facilities or a common pattern
that when time and money is limited, these are among the first subjects to be cut. There
are considerable variations between countries and states, but quality physical activity expe-
riences in their various forms often become pushed to margins, offered as a trivial escape
from the main business of the school, or squeezed out of the school timetable completely.
This is a concerning situation, as physical activities can have significantly positive effects
on a range educational outcomes, especially when delivered by properly trained staff and
in a positive learning environment.

Conclusion
Physical activity is associated with a range of health risks, both during childhood and in
later life. The pandemic spread of sedentary behaviour around the world has been identified
as a significant public health risk. Any attempt to address these problems is undermined by
Sport in Society   15

outdated and incorrect views of the relationship between the mind and the body. Nowhere
is this clearer than in education. The school is the main societal vehicle for young people’s
regular physical activity, yet evidence from many countries reveals that many children are
inactive (Hallal et al. 2012). The place of physical education and school sport within the
curriculum are perennially under threat, and this precarious position is driven by par-
ents’ and educators’ fears that time given to physical activity undermines performance in
classroom-based subjects. Yet, there is no evidence that time dedicated to physical activity
interferes with academic achievement. On the contrary, there are reasons to suppose that,
assuming appropriate provision, physical activity can contribute to success at school. Put
another way, it is simply a mistake to assume that the quantity of classroom experiences
determines academic achievement; it is the quality, not the quantity of learning that matters
most. Physical activity can enhance the quality of learning at school.
Regular physical activity promotes growth and development in youth and has multiple
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

benefits for physical, mental and cognitive health. This much seems uncontroversial, but
it may well be the case that the cluster of positive outcomes that have been identified to
be associated with physical activity are the expressions of a more fundamental effect, that
improved organic functioning. If this is true, it is hardly surprising that studies have found a
relationship between vigorous and moderate intensity physical activity and the structure and
functioning of the brain. It would also come some way to explain why young people who are
more active show improved self-esteem, greater attention, have faster cognitive processing
speed and perform better on standardized academic tests than their less active peers. Of
course, academic performance is influenced by other factors, such as parental involvement
and socioeconomic status. Nevertheless, ensuring that children and adolescents achieve at
least recommended levels of vigorous or moderate-intensity physical activity (the World
Health Organisation recommendations for physical activity state that children – 5–17 years –
should accumulate at least 60 min of moderate-to-vigorous-intensity physical activity daily,
World Health Organization 2010) suggests the improvement of factors that are supportive
of educational performance. Whether such a relationship is a causal one is still a contested
issue (Hattie and Clinton 2012; Singh et al. 2012a, 2012b). However, to step off the academic
fence for a moment, the weight of evidence, especially when measures of quality are taken
into account, makes the case for an association (even a causal relationship) persuasive.
It is too soon to develop a causal model, however, but it might be possible to sketch out
the early stages of a model. In the words of Keeves (1997, 559), ‘the model, like the hypoth-
eses, which are contained within it, can be built from accumulated evidence, intuition by
analogy, or derived from theory’. This model describes a framework for investigating the
actualisation of measurable outcomes resulting from vigorous and moderate-to-vigorous
physical activity. Presented in this way, the model offers a series of testable hypotheses,
which could act as foci for subsequent explanatory and predictive theory development.
Figure 2 offers such a tentative model. Based on the evidence reported in this article,
as well as the others gathered together in this volume, it is possible to hypothesis certain
relationships between key factors that underpin educational achievement. This assumes,
of course, the positive relationship between activity in school and performance in tests.
The findings presented in this article suggest that discussions of the importance of young
people’s physical activity need to be broadened from the conventional discourses of health
and disease (which are, of course, of great importance) to consider the contributions that
activity makes to the holistic development and functioning of the human being. Active
16    R. Bailey
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

Figure 2. A tentative model of the relationships between physical activity and educational achievement.

young people tend to perform better than their sedentary peers. They tend to perform
better at school; on tests; and during their career. It is impossible to calculate the total full
advantage offered by these improvements, but is worthwhile acknowledging that they result
from participation in activities that are mostly free, and, when presented appropriately, with
which young people will participate in happily and voluntarily. This means that the com-
mon assumption that physical activity and classroom learning are necessarily in conflict is
mistaken, and educators ought to rethink the planning and delivery of schooling radically.
There are many implications of this revolution for the understanding of the relationship
between physical activity and education. The ways in which schools are organized and
presented to young people need to change. They are outdated and inadequate since they
were designed (more than 100 years ago) with the vision of the child as passive and still,
when it is now known that the child is an active and moving learner. Based on the evidence
reported here, it can be plausibly claimed that schools need to offer a wide range of positive,
attractive physical activities to all students. Movement needs to step out of the gym and
infiltrate the whole school day.

Acknowledgements
I’d like to thank my colleagues involved with the Designed to Move initiative for support, encour-
agement and insights. Thanks are due, in particular, to Nithya Gopu, Lisa MacCallum, Will Norman,
Chuck Hillman, Shawn Arent, and Al Petitpas. I am indebted to Jennifer Leigh, Ed Cope and Tony
Macfadyen for their valuable comments of an earlier version of this paper.

Funding
This work was supported by the Nike, Inc.

References
Åberg, M. A., N. I. L. Pedersen, K. Torén, M. Svartengren, B. Bäckstrand, T. Johnsson, C. M. Cooper-
Kuhn, N. D. Åberg, M. Nilsson, and H. G. Kuhn. 2009. “Cardiovascular Fitness is Associated
with Cognition in Young Adulthood.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106 (49):
20906–20911.
Sport in Society   17

Ackerman, P. L. 2013. “Engagement and Opportunity to Learn.” In International Guide to Student


Achievement, edited by J. Hattie and E. M. Anderman, 39–41. London: Routledge.
Archambault, I., M. Janosz, J.-S. Fallu, and L. S. Pagani. 2009. “Student Engagement and Its Relationship
with Early High School Dropout.” Journal of Adolescence 32 (3): 651–670.
Attwood, G., and P. Croll. 2006. “Truancy in Secondary School Pupils: Prevalence, Trajectories and
Pupil Perspectives.” Research Papers in Education 21 (4): 467–484.
Bailey, R. P. 2005. “Physical Growth and Physical Development.” In Encyclopedia of Human
Development, edited by N. Salkind, 1000–1007. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bailey, R. P. 2006. “Physical Education and Sport in Schools: A Review of Benefits and Outcomes.”
Journal of School Health 76 (8): 397–401.
Bailey, R. P., D. Collins, P. Ford, A. MacNamara, M. Toms, and G. Pearce. 2010. Participant Development
in Sport: An Academic Review. Leeds: Sports Coach UK.
Bailey, R. P., C. Hillman, S. Arent, and A. Petitpas. 2013. “Physical Activity: An Underestimated
Investment in Human Capital.” Journal of Physical Activity and Health 10 (3): 289–308.
Basch, C. E. 2011. “Healthier Students Are Better Learners: A Missing Link in School Reforms to
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

Close the Achievement Gap.” Journal of School Health 81 (10): 593–598.


Baumeister, R. F., J. D. Campbell, J. I. Krueger, and K. D. Vohs. 2003. “Does High Self-Esteem Cause
Better Performance, Interpersonal Success, Happiness, or Healthier Lifestyles?” Psychological
Science in the Public Interest 4 (1): 1–44.
Blomfield, C. J., and B. L. Barber. 2010. “Australian Adolescents' Extracurricular Activity Participation
and Positive Development: Is the Relationship Mediated by Peer Attributes?” Australian Journal
of Educational & Developmental Psychology 10: 114–128.
Boecker, H., C. H. Hillman, L. Scheef, and H. K. Strüder, eds. 2012. Functional Neuroimaging in
Exercise and Sport Sciences. New York: Springer.
Bond, L., H. Butler, L. Thomas, J. Carlin, S. Glover, G. Bowes and G. Patton. 2007. Social and School
Connectedness in Early Secondary School as Predictors of Late Teenage Substance Use, Mental
Health, and Academic Outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health 40(4): 357-e9–357-e18.
Bonhauser, M., G. Fernandez, K. Püschel, F. Yañez, J. Montero, B. Thompson, and G. Coronado. 2005.
“Improving Physical Fitness and Emotional Well-Being in Adolescents of Low Socioeconomic
Status in Chile: Results of a School-Based Controlled Trial.” Health Promotion International 20
(2): 113–122.
Boone, E. M., and B. J. Leadbeater. 2006. “Game on: Diminishing Risks for Depressive Symptoms
in Early Adolescence through Positive Involvement in Team Sports.” Journal of Research on
Adolescence 16 (1): 79–90.
Booth, J. N., P. D. Tomporowski, J. M. Boyle, A. R. Ness, C. Joinson, S. D. Leary, and J. J. Reilly.
2013. “Associations between Executive Attention and Objectively Measured Physical Activity in
Adolescence: Findings from ALSPAC, a UK Cohort.” Mental Health and Physical Activity 6 (3):
212–219.
Bowker, A., S. Gadbois, and B. Cornock. 2003. “Sports Participation and Self-Esteem: Variations as
a Function of Gender and Gender Role Orientation.” Sex Roles 49 (1–2): 47–58.
Boyd, A., J. Golding, J. Macleod, D. A. Lawlor, A. Fraser, J. Henderson, L. Molloy, A. Ness, S. Ring,
and G. D. Smith. 2012. “Cohort Profile: The ‘Children of the 90s’ - the Index Offspring of the Avon
Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.” International Journal of Epidemiology 42 (1): 111–127.
Carroll, A., S. Houghton, K. Durkin, and J. A. Hattie. 2009. Adolescent Reputations and Risk. New
York: Springer.
Cataldo, R., J. John, L. Chandran, S. Pati, and A. Shroyer. 2013. “Impact of Physical Activity
Intervention Programs on Self-Efficacy in Youths: A Systematic Review.” ISRN Obesity 2013: 1–11.
Chaddock, L., C. H. Hillman, S. M. Buck, and N. J. Cohen. 2011. “Aerobic Fitness and Executive
Control of Relational Memory in Preadolescent Children.” Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise 43 (2): 344–349.
Chiu, L.-H. 1990. “The Relationship of Career Goal and Self-Esteem among Adolescents.” Adolescence
25 (99): 593–597.
18    R. Bailey

Côté, J., L. Strachan, and J. Fraser-Thomas. 2008. “Participation, Personal Development and
Performance through Youth Sport.” In Positive Youth Development through Sport, edited by N. L.
Holt, 34–45. London: Routledge.
Cotman, C. W., N. C. Berchtold, and L.-A. Christie. 2007. “Exercise Builds Brain Health: Key Roles
of Growth Factor Cascades and Inflammation.” Trends in Neurosciences 30 (9): 464–472.
Daniels, E., and C. Leaper. 2006. “A Longitudinal Investigation of Sport Participation, Peer Acceptance,
and Self-Esteem among Adolescent Girls and Boys.” Sex Roles 55 (11–12): 875–880.
Davis, C. L., P. D. Tomporowski, C. A. Boyle, J. L. Waller, P. H. Miller, J. A. Naglieri, and M. Gregoski.
2007. “Effects of Aerobic Exercise on Overweight Children's Cognitive Functioning: A Randomized
Controlled Trial.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 78 (5): 510–519.
Davis, C. L., P. D. Tomporowski, J. E. McDowell, B. P. Austin, P. H. Miller, N. E. Yanasak, J. D. Allison,
and J. A. Naglieri. 2011. “Exercise Improves Executive Function and Achievement and Alters
Brain Activation in Overweight Children: A Randomized, Controlled Trial.” Health Psychology
30 (1): 91–98.
Dawkins, M. P., M. M. Williams, and M. Guilbault. 2006. “Participation in School Sports: Risk or
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

Protective Factor for Drug Use among Black and White Students?” The Journal of Negro Education
75 (1): 25–33.
Denault, A.-S., and F. Poulin. 2009. “Predictors of Adolescent Participation in Organized Activities:
A Five-Year Longitudinal Study.” Journal of Research on Adolescence 19 (2): 287–311.
Doherty, J., and R. P. Bailey. 2003. Supporting Physical Development and Physical Education in the
Early Years. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Donnelly, J. E., and K. Lambourne. 2011. “Classroom-Based Physical Activity, Cognition, and
Academic Achievement.” Preventive Medicine 52: S36–S42.
Dworkin, J., and R. Larson. 2006. “Adolescents’ Negative Experiences in Organized Youth Activities.”
Journal of Youth Development 1 (3): 1–19.
Eccles, J. S., B. L. Barber, M. Stone, and J. Hunt. 2003. “Extracurricular Activities and Adolescent
Development.” Journal of Social Issues 59 (4): 865–889.
Ekeland, E., F. Heian, and K. Birger Hagen. 2005. “Can Exercise Improve Self Esteem in Children and
Young People? A Systematic Review of Randomised Controlled Trials.” British Journal of Sports
Medicine 39 (11): 792–798.
Etnier, J. L., P. M. Nowell, D. M. Landers, and B. A. Sibley. 2006. “A Meta-Regression to Examine
the Relationship between Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Performance.” Brain Research Reviews
52 (1): 119–130.
Farb, A. F., and J. L. Matjasko. 2012. “Recent Advances in Research on School-Based Extracurricular
Activities and Adolescent Development.” Developmental Review 32 (1): 1–48.
Fedewa, A. L., and S. Ahn. 2011. “The Effects of Physical Activity and Physical Fitness on Children’s
Achievement and Cognitive Outcomes.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 82 (3): 521–535.
Feldman, A. F., and J. L. Matjasko. 2005. “The Role of School-Based Extracurricular Activities in
Adolescent Development: A Comprehensive Review and Future Directions.” Review of Educational
Research 75 (2): 159–210.
Fox, K. R. 1999. “The Influence of Physical Activity on Mental Well-Being.” Public Health Nutrition
2 (3a): 411–418.
Fredricks, J. A., and J. S. Eccles. 2006. “Is Extracurricular Participation Associated with Beneficial
Outcomes?” Concurrent and Longitudinal Relations. Developmental Psychology 42 (4): 698–713.
Grant, J. D., J. F. Scherrer, M. T. Lynskey, A. Agrawal, A. E. Duncan, J. R. Haber, A. C. Heath, and
K. K. Bucholz. 2012. “Associations of Alcohol, Nicotine, Cannabis, and Drug Use/Dependence
with Educational Attainment: Evidence from Cotwin-Control Analyses.” Alcoholism: Clinical and
Experimental Research 36 (8): 1412–1420.
Habel, M. A., P. J. Dittus, C. J. De Rosa, E. Q. Chung, and P. R. Kerndt. 2010. “Daily Participation
in Sports and Students' Sexual Activity.” Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health 42 (4):
244–250.
Hallal, P. C., L. B. Andersen, F. C. Bull, R. Guthold, W. Haskell, U. Ekelund, and The Lancet Physical
Activity Series Working Group. 2012. “Global Physical Activity Levels: Surveillance Progress,
Pitfalls, and Prospects.” The Lancet 380 (9838): 247–257.
Sport in Society   19

Harber, V. J., and J. R. Sutton. 1984. “Endorphins and Exercise.” Sports Medicine 1 (2): 154–171.
Hattie, J., and J. Clinton. 2012. “Physical Activity is Not Related to Performance at School.” Archives
of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 166 (7): 678–679.
Hillman, C. H., S. M. Buck, J. R. Themanson, M. B. Pontifex, and D. M. Castelli. 2009. “Aerobic
Fitness and Cognitive Development: Event-Related Brain Potential and Task Performance Indices
of Executive Control in Preadolescent Children.” Developmental Psychology 45 (1): 114–129.
Hillman, C. H., D. M. Castelli, and S. M. Buck. 2005. “Aerobic Fitness and Neurocognitive Function in
Healthy Preadolescent Children.” Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 37 (11): 1967–1974.
Hillman, C. H., K. I. Erickson, and A. F. Kramer. 2008. “Be Smart, Exercise Your Heart: Exercise
Effects on Brain and Cognition.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9 (1): 58–65.
Hollar, D., S. E. Messiah, G. Lopez-Mitnik, T. L. Hollar, M. Almon, and A. S. Agatston. 2010. “Effect
of a Two-Year Obesity Prevention Intervention on Percentile Changes in Body Mass Index and
Academic Performance in Low-Income Elementary School Children.” American Journal of Public
Health 100 (4): 646–653.
Hughes, J. N., W. Luo, O.-M. Kwok, and L. K. Loyd. 2008. “Teacher-Student Support, Effortful
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

Engagement, and Achievement: A 3-Year Longitudinal Study.” Journal of Educational Psychology


100 (1): 1–14.
Institute of Medicine and National Research Council. 2011. The Science of Adolescent Risk-Taking.
Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Ismail, A. H. 1967. “The Effects of a Well-Organized Physical Education Programme on Intellectual
Performance.” Research in Physical Education 1 (2): 31–38.
Jacobson, L., and C. Mokher. 2009. Pathways to Boosting the Earnings of Low-Income Students
by Increasing Their Educational Attainment. Washington, DC: Hudson Institute Center for
Employment Policy.
Juvenal. Undated. “Satires.” Accessed November 19, 2015. http://www.gutenberg.us/articles/Satires_
(Juvenal)
Keays, J. J., and K. R. Allison. 1994. “The Effects of Regular Moderate to Vigorous Physical Activity
on Student Outcomes: A Review.” Canadian Journal of Public Health 86 (1): 62–65.
Keeves, J. P. 1997. “Models and Model Building.” In Educational Research, Methodology, and
Measurement: An International Handbook, edited by L. W. Anderson and S. P. Keeves, 386–394.
Cambridge: Pergamon.
Kubesch, S., L. Walk, M. Spitzer, T. Kammer, A. Lainburg, R. Heim, and K. Hille. 2009. “A 30-Minute
Physical Education Program Improves Students' Executive Attention.” Mind, Brain, and Education
3 (4): 235–242.
Laertius, D. 1925. Lives of Eminent Philosophers. Translated by R. D. Hicks. Vol. 2. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Landers, D. M., and S. M. Arent. 2001. “Physical Activity and Mental Health.” In Handbook of Sport
Psychology. 2nd ed., edited by R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas and C. M. Janelle, 740–765. New
York: Wiley.
Lardon, M. T., and J. Polich. 1996. “EEG Changes from Long-Term Physical Exercise.” Biological
Psychology 44 (1): 19–30.
Lawson, M. A., and H. A. Lawson. 2013. “New Conceptual Frameworks for Student Engagement
Research, Policy, and Practice.” Review of Educational Research 83 (3): 432–479.
Linnenbrink, E. A., and P. R. Pintrich. 2002. “Motivation as an Enabler for Academic Success.” School
Psychology Review 31 (3): 313–327.
Mahoney, J. L., and R. B. Cairns. 1997. “Do Extracurricular Activities Protect against Early School
Dropout?” Developmental Psychology 33 (2): 241–253.
Makkai, T., L. Morris, J. Sallybanks, and K. Willis. 2003. Sport, Physical Activity and Antisocial
Behaviour in Youth. Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.
Marsh, H. W. 1993. “The Effects of Participation in Sport during the Last Two Years of High School.”
Sociology of Sport Journal 10 (1): 18–43.
Maynard, B. R., C. P. Salas-Wright, M. G. Vaughn, and K. E. Peters. 2012. “Who Are Truant Youth?
Examining Distinctive Profiles of Truant Youth Using Latent Profile Analysis.” Journal of Youth
and Adolescence 41 (12): 1671–1684.
20    R. Bailey

McCarthy, W., and E. Grodsky. 2011. “Sex and School: Adolescent Sexual Intercourse and Education.”
Social Problems 58 (2): 213–234.
McClelland, E., A. Pitt, and J. Stein. 2015. “Enhanced Academic Performance Using a Novel Classroom
Physical Activity Intervention to Increase Awareness, Attention and Self-Control: Putting Embodied
Cognition into Practice.” Improving Schools 18 (1): 83–100.
Miller, K. E., M. J. Melnick, G. M. Barnes, M. P. Farrell, and D. Sabo. 2005. “Untangling the Links
among Athletic Involvement, Gender, Race, and Adolescent Academic Outcomes.” Sociology of
Sport Journal 22 (2): 178–193.
Molloy, G. N. 1989. “Chemicals, Exercise and Hyperactivity: A Short Report.” International Journal
of Disability, Development and Education 36 (1): 57–61.
Overton, W. F. 2008. “Embodiment from a Relational Perspective.” In Developmental Perspectives
on Embodiment and Consciousness, edited by W. F. Overton, U. Mueller and J. L. Newman, 1–18.
New York, NY: Erlbaum.
Pintrich, P. R., R. W. Roeser, and E. de Groot. 1994. “Classroom and Individual Differences in Early
Adolescents' Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning.” The Journal of Early Adolescence 14 (2):
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

139–161.
Pontifex, M. B., B. J. Saliba, L. B. Raine, D. L. Picchietti, and C. H. Hillman. 2013. “Exercise Improves
Behavioral, Neurocognitive, and Scholastic Performance in Children with Attention-Deficit/
Hyperactivity Disorder.” The Journal of Pediatrics 162 (3): 543–551.
Rosenberg, M., C. Schooler, C. Schoenbach, and F. Rosenberg. 1995. “Global Self-Esteem and Specific
Self-Esteem: Different Concepts, Different Outcomes.” American Sociological Review 60 (1): 141–
156.
Rosewater, A. 2009. “Learning to Play and Playing to Learn: Organized Sports and Educational
Outcomes.” Education Digest 75 (1): 50–56.
Ryan, A. M., and G. W. Ladd, eds. 2012. Peer Relationships and Adjustment at School. Charlotte, NC:
IAP.
Short, M. A., M. Gradisar, L. C. Lack, and H. R. Wright. 2013. “The Impact of Sleep on Adolescent
Depressed Mood, Alertness and Academic Performance.” Journal of Adolescence 36 (6): 1025–1033.
Singh, A. S., L. Uijtdewilligen, J. W. R. Twisk, W. van Mechelen, and M. J. M. Chinapaw. 2012a.
“Physical Activity is Not Related to Performance at School – Reply.” Archives of Pediatrics &
Adolescent Medicine 166 (7): 678–679.
Singh, A. S., L. Uijtdewilligen, J. W. R. Twisk, W. van Mechelen, and M. J. M. Chinapaw. 2012b.
“Physical Activity and Performance at School: A Systematic Review of the Literature including a
Methodological Quality Assessment.” Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 166 (1): 49–55.
Sonstroem, R. J., and S. A. Potts. 1996. “Life Adjustment Correlates of Physical Self-Concepts.”
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 28 (5): 619–625.
Strauss, R. S., D. Rodzilsky, G. Burack, and M. Colin. 2001. “Psychosocial Correlates of Physical
Activity in Healthy Children.” Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 155 (8): 897–902.
Telford, R. D., R. B. Cunningham, R. M. Telford, and W. P. Abharatna. 2012. “Schools with Fitter
Children Achieve Better Literacy and Numeracy Results: Evidence of a School Cultural Effect.”
Pediatric Exercise Science 24 (1): 45–57.
Tinto, V. 1987. Leaving College: Rethinking the Causes and Cures of Student Attrition. Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Tomporowski, P. D., C. L. Davis, P. H. Miller, and J. A. Naglieri. 2008. “Exercise and Children’s
Intelligence, Cognition, and Academic Achievement.” Educational Psychology Review 20 (2):
111–131.
Trudeau, F., L. Laurencelle, J. Tremblay, M. Rajic, and R. J. Shephard. 1999. “Daily Primary School
Physical Education: Effects on Physical Activity during Adult Life.” Medicine & Science in Sports
& Exercise 31 (1): 111–117.
Trudeau, F., and R. J. Shephard. 2008. “Physical Education, School Physical Activity, School Sports and
Academic Performance.” International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 5 (1): 10.
Veliz, P., and S. Shakib. 2012. “Interscholastic Sports Participation and School Based Delinquency:
Does Participation in Sport Foster a Positive High School Environment?” Sociological Spectrum
32 (6): 558–580.
Sport in Society   21

Weiss, M. R., and L. M. Petlichkoff. 1989. “Children's Motivation for Participation in and Withdrawal
from Sport: Identifying the Missing Links.” Pediatric Exercise Science 1 (3): 195–211.
Wentzel, K. R., and K. Caldwell. 1997. “Friendships, Peer Acceptance, and Group Membership:
Relations to Academic Achievement in Middle School.” Child Development 68 (6): 1198–1209.
Whitelaw, S., J. Teuton, J. Swift, and G. Scobie. 2010. “The Physical Activity – Mental Wellbeing
Association in Young People: A Case Study in Dealing with a Complex Public Health Topic Using
a ‘Realistic Evaluation’ Framework.” Mental Health and Physical Activity 3 (2): 61–66.
Woodward, L. J., and D. M. Fergusson. 2000. “Childhood Peer Relationship Problems and Later
Risks of Educational under-Achievement and Unemployment.” Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry 41 (2): 191–201.
World Health Organization. 2010. Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health. Geneva:
World Health Organization.
Zaff, J. F., K. A. Moore, A. Romano Papillo, and S. Williams. 2003. “Implications of Extracurricular
Activity Participation during Adolescence on Positive Outcomes.” Journal of Adolescent Research
18 (6): 599–630.
Downloaded by [University of Central Lancashire] at 15:30 28 July 2016

Zimmermann-Sloutskis, D., M. Wanner, E. Zimmermann, and B. W. Martin. 2010. “Physical Activity


Levels and Determinants of Change in Young Adults: A Longitudinal Panel Study.” International
Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 7 (2): 1–13.

View publication stats

You might also like