Indigenous Psy

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/324910165

INDIGENOUS PSYCHOLOGY: TOWARD NEW HORIZONS OF POSSIBILITY FOR


UNDERSTANDING HUMAN BEHAVIOUR

Article · May 2018

CITATIONS READS

0 344

1 author:

Eshrat Ara
Government College for Women, Srinagar
16 PUBLICATIONS   9 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Family Environment and Cognitive Distortions as Predictors of Behavioural Problems in Youth View project

Suicide Ideation: A Survey View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Eshrat Ara on 03 May 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


INDIGENOUS PSYCHOLOGY:
TOWARD NEW HORIZONS OF POSSIBILITY FOR
UNDERSTANDING HUMAN BEHAVIOUR
Eshrat Ara
Research Scholar, Department Of Psychology, University of Kashmir (India)

ABSTRACT
The world we live in today has changed in many ways that calls for a better understanding of each other, which
calls for focusing on research on indigenous psychologies, for without knowing the psychology of people in their
indigenous contexts, we cannot quite understand their worldview and why they do what they do. Indigenous
models can be developed by starting from cultural insight. India has a rich scholarly tradition and psychology
can take advantage of this cultural wealth. We need to enrich our psychological understanding of humankind by
building indigenous models. Indigenous psychology has tremendous potential to contribute to global
psychology. This paper presents a methodology for developing models from indigenous ideas, and describes
that this methodology is useful for building models, which can also serve as self-help frameworks for
practitioners, thus contributing to the world of practice.
Keywords: Indian Psychology, Indigenous Psychology, Western Psychology,

“Differences offer us choices, choices offer us freedoms, and freedoms offer us the possibility to move beyond
limited views of who we are, and what we can become, to new horizons of thought and being”. (Antony J.
Marsella)
The discipline of psychology has a unique distinction of sharing membership with natural science, life
science, social science, and human science endeavours. In Indian universities it is placed in science, arts, and
social science faculties. In true sense it has multiple identities, and engages with a wide range of methodologies
and thematic concerns. Therefore, a multidisciplinary approach in theories and methods, multivocality, and a
certain kind of hybridity have become hallmarks of psychological enquiry [1].
For a long time, mainstream psychology maintained a physicalist orientation and its research was
preoccupied with logico-positivist framework, which was emulated by behaviourists and it is still imprinted
upon the minds of students when they enter in psychology classes. Its insufficiency is now felt and, accordingly,
several alternative views have emerged. The emerging scenario is one in which disparate trends are going to co-
exist. Thus, while globalization is leading to homogenization, we also register voices of separate cultural

268 | P a g e
identities [1]. Therefore, psychologists may have to develop theories and methods that equip people to meet
challenges of life.
Psychology as a discipline has been dominated by western psychology, and the psychology of 1% of
the population of the world is imposed on the rest of the world as universal knowledge [2]. This needs to
change. The realities of our global era have resulted in an increased awareness of the diversities of people and
cultures across the world. This has led to growing efforts to understand, appreciate, and respect the diverse
psychologies that we are encountering. The challenge, therefore, is no longer to simply study these differences
using theories and methodology of cross-cultural psychology, cultural psychology, minority psychology, but
rather to approach a group’s unique and distinct construction of reality. Psychology needs to move, as a science
and profession, toward new horizons of possibility for understanding human behaviour [3].
India has a strong and variegated tradition of reflective scholarship with rich insights into human
psyche and behaviour much before the advent of modern scientific psychology. One finds impressive
experience-based accounts of mental states and activities. The goal of related deliberations was self-realization
and alleviating suffering. The inner-directed discipline to regulate self processes and fostering harmony of mind,
body, and spirit seems central to most of the Indian accounts. A holistic and integral vision has been central to it.
It may be noted that the Indian thought systems (based on religions, e.g., Hinduism, Buddhism, Jainism,
Sikhism, and Islam) are quite diverse in their orientation. Thus, the thought systems and their various
subsystems present a range of positions about human nature, behaviour, goals of life, nature of cognitive and
affective processes, and mental health and well-being. The ideas presented in them draw upon rich textual
traditions and cultural practices. Due to colonial exploitation this rich source remained neglected and an
aversion was nurtured toward them [1; 3; 4].
In traditional Indian thought, psychology was never a subject independent of metaphysics. Indian
psychology was based on introspection and observation. It was not empirical or experimental, but was based on
metaphysics [5]. While psychology became established as an empirical science in the west, in both the USA and
in Europe, by 1950, in India it remained a part of the discipline of philosophy. Following its independence in
1947 from the British rule, psychology in India moved away from its Indian roots to mimic western method and
theory. In the 1950s, the Indian as well as the global zeitgeist was filled with the spirit of national development,
and the western countries offered the gold standard for development. India had undergone hundreds of years of
colonization and needed to become strong, and the western-educated Indian leaders did not know anything
better than to emulate the west. Humanists like Gandhi did champion indigenization in both the economy and
the lifestyle, but they became the outliers, the saints who were to be venerated and worshipped, but not to be
followed by either the leaders or the masses in their daily living [3].
Mishra [6] provided a succinct analysis of the development of indigenous psychology in India and
posited that psychology like all other sciences was imported to India from the west, and psychological concepts
that did not fit western assumed etics or universals were simply considered to be anomalies. Thus, in the second

269 | P a g e
half of the twentieth century, Indian psychologists seldom attempted to derive psychological principles from
their philosophical or folk traditions. As a result, it has become largely irrelevant to the Indian populace.
The western world dominates research and knowledge creation that often leads to starting with
theoretical positions that are grounded in the western cultural contexts. Thus, starting with such a theoretical
position, leads to the pseudoetic approach in which theories are western emics. To avoid this, it is necessary to
start with insights offered by indigenous cultures with novel and useful research methodology. This approach
proposes that we start with insights from folk wisdom and classical texts in indigenous nonwestern cultures. We
should enrich these insights with anecdotal evidence, qualitative analyses, and observational data from the target
indigenous culture [3].
Psychology needs to be grounded in the cultural worldview of the society and people being
investigated, and without making such effort we cannot begin to understand human psychology. The intention is
not to call to question the existing western psychological knowledge, but to inspire a dialogue among various
indigenous psychologies, including the western psychology. We need to put a moratorium on pseudoetic
research that leads to the mindless copying of western ideas, and start paying attention to indigenous ideas in
psychology [3]. Psychological research in India can be used to exemplify the general ideas presented in this
paper.
The evolution of cross-cultural psychology has helped change this “look to the west” thinking, and
researchers are seeking local conceptualizations, insights, and understanding [3; 7; 8; 9, 10, 11, 12, 13,]. In
recent years efforts have been made to recover the sources of Indigenous wisdom [1; 14; 15; 16; 17].
The scientific worldview is being adopted in the western countries, but there is still a lot of resistance
in other cultures to a total acceptance of the scientific worldview. We find innumerable examples of how people
are comfortable using the scientific methods in chemistry, engineering, and such other domains, but when it
comes to areas where science is not able to give a definitive answer, they resort to other systems of explanations,
which are often derived from their own cultures. And these are the domains of research for social science in
general, and psychology in particular. We often find people using processes of decision-making that could not
be called rational. We can label such behaviors as superstition and argue that such behaviors or their
“unscientific” explanations would go away in time. Or, we can examine them more systematically, and learn
about people’s worldviews, what they do in different contexts, and why. Our worldview gives us faith in how
the world around us works, and faith cannot be discarded [18].
Indigenous models can be developed by starting from cultural insight. India has a rich scholarly
tradition, and psychology can take advantage of this cultural wealth. The classical texts (the Bhagavad-Gita, the
Upanishads, the Buddhist texts, the Jain texts, the Guru Granth Sahib, the Bible and the Quran) can be a source
of much psychological insight to study cognition, emotion, and behavior, and there are many other texts from
which researchers can borrow ideas (e.g., the Sufi). The rich folk wisdom should also be tapped, which could
provide a good starting point. We need to enrich our psychological understanding of humankind by building

270 | P a g e
indigenous models, especially since we now live in a forever shrinking global village. Indigenous psychology
has tremendous potential to contribute to global psychology [3; 19].
We need to appreciate research just as life is – messy. Thus research is going to be messy and may not
always fit into the experimental paradigm. Clearly, Indian Psychology is holistic and much innovation will be
needed to meet the research need of people in India [3]. The humanistic approach to research fits naturally with
Indian Psychology in contrast to the scientific approach. It should be noted that the meaning of episteme in
ancient Greek was “knowledge,” whereas in Modern Greek it means “science” [20]. The epistemology of Indian
Psychology and philosophy merge with the general Indian worldview of knowledge, truth, and belief about
making sense of the self and the world [1; 3].
It is apparent that the Western psychology has focused on individual’s goals, goal achievement, and the
need for achievement. Indigenous Indian psychology, on the other hand, as a consequence of the Indian
worldview, has focused on self and its interactions with the world through desires, controlling desires, and
attaining personal peace. In indigenous Indian psychology, therefore, tremendous emphasis is placed on how to
deal with, even eliminate, desires, whereas we find that in western cultures following one’s desires (e.g., doing
one’s own thing and doing what, one likes to do) is greatly emphasized. Thus, the Indian worldview leads to
building psychological models that are quite different from what we have in the west [3; 7; 8; 9; 10; 11; 14; 17;
21; 22; 23; 24].
It should be noted that, although counterintuitive, fluency in English language is a major disadvantage
that Indian and other researchers face. Since most Indian researchers are fluent in English, they think in English,
and much of the western literature, therefore, makes sense to them. This gets further compounded by the desire
to succeed by publishing in international journals, which require building on the western ideas. Thus, they never
pause to think if the concepts would make sense to the masses. It will help if psychology students were required
to study the classic texts and folk literature to develop sensitivity to indigenous ideas. Research, in indigenous
psychology, calls for adopting a diversity of methodologies, beyond the experimental method favored by
western psychology and social sciences [3; 25). Thus, research in indigenous psychology is likely to provide
new paradigms and models that cannot be developed following the western research tradition.
Marsella [19] entreated researchers to replace the western cultural traditions by more encompassing
multicultural traditions and reiterated the need to emphasize the cultural determinants of human behavior, which
has been discussed in the literature [6; 25; 27; 28]. He recommended the systems orientation and noted that
many indigenous psychologies are well equipped to deal with ascending dimensions of behavioral contexts,
from individual to family to society to nature to spirituality. He further proposed that qualitative research
including such methods as narrative accounts, discourse analysis, and ethnographic analysis should be
encouraged [3].
Paranjpe [16] showed that many processes in Indian Psychology require observation and analysis of the
subjective and within-person information. On the other hand, in western psychology, the focus is primarily on

271 | P a g e
the study of the other, and thus the empirical paradigm that lends itself to the observation of the other is more
appropriate. Sociology of knowledge creation in India is different from that in the west. In the west, research
culture is tighter and allows little or no freedom to researchers to deviate from what is considered standard
practices [3]. On the other hand, the research culture is much looser in India as people are more open to
accommodating variation in conceptualization and method. Clearly, science is tight, whereas human experience
is loose, and this gets further reflected in Indians having a holistic approach to research, whereas in the west,
people value research broken into small pieces leading to testing one hypothesis at a time. So it appears that
there are major differences between western and Indian research enterprises, much like their cultures [8; 10; 12].
Second, there was clear shift toward non-experimental research as seen in the first 15 years of Sinha’s career
[29].
The issue is simply not about method, experimental or otherwise, but about research questions. The
questions asked by Indian psychologists, if they keep close to the reality of Indian society, are likely to be
different from those in the west. Methodology should follow the demand of the research questions, rather than
researchers manufacture questions that fit the experimental methodology. Thus, the message for the Indian
Psychology is quite clear: Address research questions that are grounded in the Indian milieu, using methods that
make sense to address the research problem, rather than fit into the 2 × 2 designs to fit in the western journals
[3].
Engagement with broader theoretical frameworks and using innovative methodology are helping to
realize the unexplored potential of psychology. In this effort the discipline is becoming “glocal” by responding
to the aspects of local reality and global concerns [1; 30].
CONCLUSION
India and Indian people have lived for thousands of years without western psychology and can do so
today and in the future. This is not a call for rejection of western psychology. It is a call to get strong in one’s
indigenous worldview to be able to deal with ideas from other cultures with strength, rather than by constantly
apologizing for what may be the strength of one’s culture as its weakness as viewed from the western
perspective. Once such an Indian Psychology is developed in its own right, cross-cultural psychology and
comparative work can begin. Standing on its own foundation, and thus existing in its own right, is necessary for
cultural or indigenous psychologies to develop.
The indigenous models have been tested over thousands of years by practitioners and thus it has much
empirical validity, which can be further tested by individuals in their own experience. If a model makes sense
when one reflects on his or her own personal experience, and if it helps the person change a behavior that is of
concern to him or her, then that is the best practical use of a theoretical model. Perhaps that is why Kurt Lewin
said, “There is nothing as practical as a good theory.”

272 | P a g e
In order to make sense of the social and psychological reality we need to be culturally sensitive,
enhance our conceptual repertoire and be willing to go for a more open-ended methodological strategy toward
new horizons of understanding human behaviour.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I would like to thank my mentor (Dr. Showkat Ahmad Shah) for his guidance.

REFERENCES
[1] G. Misra, Psychology in India volume 4: Theoretical and methodological developments (Indian Council of
Social Science Research, 2011).
[2] H.C. Triandis, Culture and social behavior (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1994).
[3] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Spirituality and Indian Psychology: Lessons from the Bhagavad-Gita (Springer, 2011).
[4] J. Sinha, Indian psychology, volume 1 and 2 (London, UK: Kegan Paul, 1993).
[5] R.A. Baron, and G. Misra, Psychology: Indian subcontinent edition (Pearson India Education Services Pvt.
Ltd, 2016).
[6 ] K. Gergen, A. Gulerce, A. Lock, and G. Mishra, Psychological science in cultural context, American
Psychologist, 51, 1996, 496–503.
[7] Z.A. Ansari, Quranic Concepts of Human Psyche (New Delhi: Adam Publishers and Distributers, 2006).

[8] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Culture’s influence on creativity: The case of Indian spirituality, International Journal of
Intercultural Relations, 27 (1), 2003, 1–22.
[9] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Bridging science and spirituality: Challenges for Yoga, Proc. 14th International Conf. on
Frontiers of Yoga Research and Applications, Bangalore, India, 2003
[10] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Science of culture and culture of science: Worldview and choice of conceptual models and
methodology, The Social Engineer, 11 (2), 2008, 26–43.
[11] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Globalization and indigenous cultures: Homogenization or differentiation? International
Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32 (4), 2008, 305–317.
[12] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Methodology for building psychological models from scriptures: Contributions of Indian
psychology to indigenous and global psychologies, Psychology and Developing Societies, 22 (1), 2010, 49–93.
[13] D.P.S. Bhawuk, Epistemology and ontology of Indian psychology: A synthesis of theory, method, and
practice. Psychology and Developing Societies, 22 (1), 2010, 157–190.
[14] A.H. Alawi, The Quranic concept of mental health. Proc. 7th Conf. of Pakistan Psychological Association
on Quranic Concepts of Human Psyche, April 1988.
[15] A.K. Dalal, and G. Misra, The core and context of Indian psychology, Psychology and Developing
Societies, 22 (1), 2010.
[16] A. Paranjpe, Theories of self and cognition: Indian psychological perspectives, Psychology and Developing
Societies, 22 (1), 2010.
273 | P a g e
[17] Saeeduzzafar, and A. Aijaz, Stress management: An Islamic approach, Psychological Studies, 53, 2008,
175-180.
[18] M. McConnell, Faith can help you heal, The Reader’s Digest, October 1998, 109–113.
[19] A.J. Marsella, Toward a “Global-Community Psychology,” American Psychologist, 53(12), 1998, 1282–
1291.
[20] M. Foucault, The order of things: An archaeology of the human sciences (Brunner-Routledge, 2002).
[21] H. Benson, Yoga for drug abuse. New England Journal of Medicine, 281, 1969, 145–152.
[22] H. Benson, Transcendental meditation – science or cult? Journal of the American Medical Association, 227
(7), 1974, 807.
[23] R.K. Wallace, The physiological effects of transcendental meditation. Science, 167, 1970, 1751–1754.
[24] R.K., Wallace, and H. Benson, The philosophy of meditation, Scientific American, 1972, 84–90.
[25] M.Q. Patton, Qualitative research and evaluation methods (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2002).
[26] K. Gergen, Exploring the postmodern: Perils or potentials, American Psychologist, 49, 1994, 412–416.
[27] K. Gergen, Psychological science in postmodern context, American Psychologist, 56 (10), 2001, 803–813.
[28] K. Pawlik, The psychological dimension of global change, International Journal of Psychology, 20, 1991.
[29] J.B.P. Sinha, Living and doing psychology, Psychology and Developing Societies, 22 (1), 2010.
[30] R.C. Tripathi, In search of a glocal psychology, In G. Misra (Ed.), Handbook of psychology India (pp. 270-
373), (New Delhi: Oxford University Press, 2010)

274 | P a g e

View publication stats

You might also like