0% found this document useful (0 votes)
60 views15 pages

Learning in Education: January 2003

This document summarizes three theories of learning - behaviorism, cognitivism, and social theories - and how they relate to educational practice. Behaviorism views learning as a change in observable behavior due to environmental reinforcement or punishment. In education, this translates to teachers shaping student behavior through rewards and consequences. Cognitivism sees learning as the development of mental representations and understanding through meaning-making. Social theories emphasize that learning occurs through participation in social and cultural contexts. Different educational approaches make assumptions about learning aligned with these theoretical perspectives.

Uploaded by

Mutu King
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
60 views15 pages

Learning in Education: January 2003

This document summarizes three theories of learning - behaviorism, cognitivism, and social theories - and how they relate to educational practice. Behaviorism views learning as a change in observable behavior due to environmental reinforcement or punishment. In education, this translates to teachers shaping student behavior through rewards and consequences. Cognitivism sees learning as the development of mental representations and understanding through meaning-making. Social theories emphasize that learning occurs through participation in social and cultural contexts. Different educational approaches make assumptions about learning aligned with these theoretical perspectives.

Uploaded by

Mutu King
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/304049716

Learning in Education

Article · January 2003

CITATIONS READS
3 3,395

1 author:

Stanton Wortham
Boston College, USA
134 PUBLICATIONS   2,887 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Shirley, D., Wortham, S., & Kim, D. (2020). The Quest for a Purpose to Encompass the Highest Moral Values: Introduction to the Special Issue. ECNU Review of Education, 3(3),
399-405. View project

Discourse and Education View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Stanton Wortham on 27 November 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


University of Pennsylvania
ScholarlyCommons
GSE Publications Graduate School of Education

1-1-2003

Learning in Education
Stanton Wortham
University of Pennsylvania, [email protected]

Postprint version. Published in Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science Volume 1, Edited by Lynn Nadel (New York: Nature Publishing Group, 2003), Article
563, pages 1079-1082.

This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. http://repository.upenn.edu/gse_pubs/82


For more information, please contact [email protected].
Learning in Education
Abstract
Learning takes place in many settings, but educational institutions foster both breadth and depth of learning.
Different types of teaching make very different assumptions about what learning is.

Keywords
cognitive mediation, social context, teaching, schools, human nature

Disciplines
Education

Comments
Postprint version. Published in Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science Volume 1, Edited by Lynn Nadel (New York:
Nature Publishing Group, 2003), Article 563, pages 1079-1082.

This book chapter is available at ScholarlyCommons: http://repository.upenn.edu/gse_pubs/82


Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science

Article 563: Learning in Education

Stanton Wortham
Associate Professor and Chair
Educational Leadership Division
Graduate School of Education
University of Pennsylvania

3700 Walnut Street


Philadelphia, PA 19104-6216
USA
215 898 6307; FAX 215 898 4399
[email protected]

1
Learning in Education
Introduction
Behavior
Mind
Society

[Keywords: cognitive mediation, social context, teaching, schools, human nature]

Learning takes place in many settings, but educational institutions foster both breadth and

depth of learning. Different types of teaching make very different assumptions about

what learning is.

Introduction

Theories of learning have been applied most often in educational institutions. The

relationship between cognitive science and education has benefited both scientists and

practitioners. Scientists have used educational settings to develop and test their theories,

and practitioners have used new knowledge about learning to design more effective

education.

Broadly conceived, education is the process of continuing the human species. All

humans are born immature, without the knowledge and skills they will need to

function⎯without language, without knowing how to use complex tools, etc. The

species continues because adults communicate knowledge and skills to the next

generation. This intergenerational transfer allows future generations to build on prior

accomplishments.

2
Thus all humans teach. Whether they realize it or not, all teachers act as if some

theory of learning is true. Particular ways of teaching make assumptions about what

learning is. Furthermore, theories of learning themselves rest on conceptions of human

nature. Different accounts of how people learn assume different things about what

people are essentially like.

This chapter describes three broad theories of learning⎯together with the

conceptions of human nature underlying these theories⎯and the types of educational

practice that have been built on these theories. The chapter has two purposes. First, it is

important to recognize the theories of learning and conceptions of human nature that

underlie various types of schooling. The chapter describes how typical teacher and

student behavior makes assumptions about how learning happens. Second, as theories of

learning have developed, we have learned that earlier theories were too simple. The

chapter describes how more complex accounts of learning and human nature are needed

to guide educational practice.

Behavior

Theories of learning that focus on behavior are called “behaviorist.” Behaviorists

argue that humans should not consider themselves special. Copernicus showed that the

earth was not the center of the solar system, and Darwin showed that humans were not

qualitatively different from animals. Behaviorists further puncture our sense of

superiority, arguing that humans do not have free will to act as they choose. “A person

does not act upon the world,” B. F. Skinner said, “the world acts upon him.” On this

theory, the environment shapes people’s behavior through reinforcement. Just as Darwin

3
showed that organisms appear designed by a creator to fit their niche, even though

adaptation is in fact a result of random variation and natural selection over time,

behaviorists show that humans appear to reflect and choose their actions, while in fact

their behavior has been shaped by reinforcement.

To learn, then, is to change one’s behavior in response to reinforcement. This

account of learning contains three central elements: behaviors by the organism,

conditions present in the environment, and consequences that follow from various

behaviors. People, like other animals, will generate various behaviors in a new situation.

Some of these behaviors will result in positive consequences, while others will not.

People learn to respond more often with behaviors that were reinforced positively in a

given situation.

Behaviorist education

On a behaviorist account, teaching is the systematic shaping of a student’s

behavior. The teacher has control and students are raw material to be shaped. Teachers

arrange reinforcements so that students come to behave as teachers want them to.

Scientists have successfully taught pigeons to play ping pong, for instance, by designing

a long series of intermediate skills that lead from natural pigeon behavior to ping pong.

They reinforce the pigeons for doing each of these intermediate skills, in turn, until the

pigeons produce the target behavior. Similarly, teachers of human students should define

the target behaviors, design a path of intermediate behaviors from what students can

already do, then reinforce students at each step until they produce the target behavior.

4
Behaviorists have designed “teaching machines” that dispense rewards as students

accomplish pre-specified tasks. One famous picture shows a small boy playing a piano,

with a candy dispenser on top. Although these pictures now look outdated, many

practices in today’s schools presuppose a behaviorist account of learning. Discipline

systems almost always rely on rewards and punishments to shape students’ behavior.

Grades are used as reinforcers. And many classroom practices, from worksheets to

testing, involve teachers rewarding students for producing desired behavior.

Research in cognitive science over the last fifty years has shown that behaviorism

is not an adequate theory of learning. People often act because they value activities

intrinsically, not for external reinforcement. As described in the next section, people also

develop complex representations of the world and reflect on their actions in a way that

behaviorists denied. Why, then, do students and teachers so often act as if behavorism

were true?

Because it works. If you have control over effective reinforcers, you can shape

people’s behavior. Behaviorism is not false. It is true, but it is not the whole truth.

Under certain circumstances, people do learn just like animals. The question is whether

we should create more circumstances that encourage people to learn this way. Cognitive

scientists claim that we should not, because humans have the potential to learn in non-

behaviorist ways, and because students can develop deeper knowledge when encouraged

to learn differently.

5
Mind

Theories of learning that focus on mental representations are called “cognitivist.”

Cognitive approaches to learning see humans as actively making sense of the

environment. People develop mental models of the world and act on the basis of these

models, not simply in response to reinforcements. When people encounter a new

situation, they assimilate it to their own pre-existing models of the world. Learning

involves expanding those mental models, in order to make them more accurate.

This account of learning distinguishes between genuine understanding and merely

producing the right behavior. People often just parrot the right answer without

understanding it, just as pigeons can play ping pong without understanding what they are

doing. True learning involves a deeper grasp of the subject matter, such that people’s

mental models line up with the world. Furthermore, people cannot be forced to learn.

True learning requires a change in people’s internal models, and learners must change

these models themselves.

Cognitive scientists have described various structures and processes that underlie

learning. There seem to be some universal constraints, which presuppose people to

certain broad types of mental models. Particular domains of knowledge are also

organized in distinct ways, to facilitate learning. And individuals sometimes vary in the

types of structures that they operate most effectively with. For instance, there are

different learning styles—some people learn most effectively through verbal

explanations, while others learn more effectively through visual diagrams, and so on.

6
Cognitivist education

From this perspective, learners need to develop deeper understandings, not just

produce the right behaviors. Deeper understandings cannot be imposed on students,

because they must construct their own mental models. So teachers do not shape students,

nor do they deliver correct answers. Teachers should develop educational environments

that push students to broaden and deepen their own models, thus opening up areas of the

world that students have not thought about. After teachers have set up rich educational

environments, ones that contain puzzles designed to provoke students to reflect, then they

must allow students to explore. Teachers can challenge students, by pointing out

contradictions in their beliefs, but students themselves must recognize the puzzles and

work to solve them. Teachers can explain, but if students can only repeat a teacher’s

explanation then they have not truly learned. Students themselves must integrate new

experience with their own developing mental models.

Assessment is a bigger challenge for cognitivist educators than behaviorist ones.

Behaviorists pre-define the educational objective, and they assess whether students

produce the desired behavior. Genuine cognitive learning, in contrast, takes place

internally. Teachers can infer about students’ understandings, but they do not want to

encourage rote learning by using simple tests. Instead of assessing whether students get

the right answers, cognitivist educators try to assess underlying thought processes by

examining how students reached certain answers.

Cognitivist theories of learning are more widely accepted than behaviorist ones.

Nonetheless, there is less cognitivist teaching than behaviorist teaching in our schools.

This happens partly because cognitivist education is difficult for both teachers and

7
students. Because they are responsible for students’ learning, it is hard for teachers to let

students pursue their own ideas much of the time. Students also find it easier to write

down what the teacher says, instead of developing their own accounts. This sort of

resistance can be overcome, and many teachers do successfully encourage students to

develop their own deeper understandings. But behaviorist practice has been harder to

overcome than behaviorist theory.

Society

Theories of learning that go beyond mental representations to include social

practices are called “social cognitivist.” Cognitivist learners are autonomous, developing

models themselves to make sense of the environment. Recent theories present the

learner, instead, as a participant in social activities. Learning, on this account, is a

transformation of participation in activity, not primarily the creation of mental models.

Instead of simply developing their own representations, people become increasingly

competent participants in the intellectual lives of those around them.

From this point of view, people learn as they more competently use tools to

facilitate thought and action. Adults incorporate learners into their activity by teaching

them how to use certain cognitive tools. Some of these tools are mental, like mnemonic

devices. Others are objects, like maps. But learners do not have to construct them alone,

because these tools have already been developed and can be borrowed from others.

Any theory of learning presupposes a “unit of analysis.” This is the smallest unit

that preserves essential behavior of the whole. In order to study the behavior of water,

for example, one must understand the molecular level. Studying hydrogen and oxygen

8
atoms separately will not allow one fully to understand the behavior of water. Similarly,

one cannot fully understand learning solely by studying individuals’ mental

representations. Individual cognitions are essential, as hydrogen atoms are essential to

water, but learning itself depends on a larger unit: a social activity, which includes

individuals’ mental representations, various cognitive tools, and others’ knowledge and

skill, all of which together allow learning.

Unlike behaviorists, and like cognitivists, social cognitivists describe how

cognitive structures and processes mediate between the environment and people’s

actions. But social cognitivists emphasize that these mediating structures go beyond

individuals’ mental models to include tools and other aspects of social activities.

Although some activities (like conventional tests) do require individuals to think in

isolation with limited tools, a full account of learning must analyze social activities in

addition to mental representations.

Social cognitivist education

In a social cognitivist approach, both teacher and student are active. Instead of

relying primarily on students’ own exploration and model-building, the teacher acts as a

competent practitioner of the activity being taught and brings tools for students to use.

Teachers guide students as they begin to participate in the activity. This guidance allows

students to do tasks that they would not be able to perform on their own. Students act

like apprentices, at first doing minor parts of the task while observing others, then taking

on increasing responsibility.

9
Teachers should design more naturalistic or “authentic” activities for students to

participate in, where the goal is competent participation in real activity. Many medical

schools, for instance, now use “problem-based learning”—in which groups of beginning

students are given real, complex cases and asked to diagnose the problem. They must

consult more expert practitioners, do research on relevant topics, and develop alternative

diagnoses to present in class. Students thus learn how to participate in the practice of

medical diagnosis, and they learn the relevant facts along the way.

From this perspective, testing is unnatural. If students must learn to participate

competently in real activities, teachers should not test whether they can solve problems

by themselves out of context. And because learning most often involves participating

with others to accomplish a task, students should not be tested alone. Students should

instead be asked to exhibit their mastery by participating competently in naturalistic

activities.

Like behaviorism, pure cognitivism is only partly true. Just as people are often

manipulated by reinforcements, people often rely primarily on their own mental models

to understand the environment. But if our educational goal is to help young people build

on the knowledge and skills that have been developed by previous generations, we should

treat them neither as animals to be shaped nor as lone thinkers. We must help them grow

into and expand the activities that make us human. This will require educational

practices based on more complex accounts of learning.

10
Further Reading

Anderson J, Reder L, and Simon H (1996) Situated Learning and Education. Educational

Researcher, 25, 5-11.

Duckworth E. (1987) “The Having of Wonderful Ideas” and Other Essays on Teaching

and Learning. New York: Teachers College Press.

Engeström Y, Miettinen R, and Puramäki R (1999) Perspectives on Activity Theory.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gardner H (1999) Intelligence Reframed. New York: Basic Books.

Greeno J (1997) On Claims that Answer the Wrong Questions. Educational Researcher,

26, 5-17.

Hutchins E (1995) Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Lave J, and Wenger E (1991) Situated Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University

Press.

McGilly K (1994) Classroom Lessons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Piaget J. (1967) Six Psychological Studies. New York: Random House.

Renninger K (1998) Developmental Psychology and Instruction. In I Siegel and K

Renninger Child Psychology and Practice. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Rogoff B, Turkanis C, and Bartlett L (2001) Learning Together. Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Schwartz B (1985) The Battle for Human Nature. New York: Norton.

Skinner, BF (1968) The Technology of Teaching. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

Vygotsky L (1978) Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wertsch J (1998) Mind as Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

11
Glossary

Authentic assessment—Assessing students’ knowledge while they participate in more

naturalistic activities, instead of tests.

Cognitive mediation—The process in which people’s models of the world shape their

understandings of and reactions to it.

Cognitive tools—Objects like maps, words, or mnemoric devices that people rely on to

facilitate thinking.

Genuine understanding—When a learner goes beyond getting the right answer and

develops a representation that more accurately reflects the world.

Operant conditioning—Shaping a learner’s behavior by selectively reinforcing his or her

responses to particular situations.

Situated cognition—The dependence of cognitive accomplishments on non-mental

components like cognitive tools and other aspects of social activity.

12

View publication stats

You might also like