Norms For The Abbreviated Barcelona Test
Norms For The Abbreviated Barcelona Test
Norms For The Abbreviated Barcelona Test
Abstract
The abbreviated Barcelona Test (a-BT) is an instrument widely used in Spain and Latin American countries for general neuropsychological
assessment. The purpose of the present study was to provide new norms for the a-BT as part of the Neuronorma project. The sample consisted
of 346 healthy controls. Overlapping cell procedure and midpoint techniques were applied to develop the normative data. Age, education, and
sex influences were studied. Results indicated that although age and education affected the score on this test, sex did not. Raw scores were
transformed to age-adjusted scaled scores (SSA) based on percentile ranks. These SSA were also converted into age–education scaled scores
using a linear regression model. Norms were presented on age–education scaled scores. Also, the a-BT cognitive profile was presented and
should prove to be clinically useful for interpretation. These co-normed data will allow clinicians to compare scores from a-BT with all the
tests included in the Neuronorma project.
Introduction
The availability of appropriate normative data is critical to the quality of neuropsychological assessment. As a result, well-
standardized tests should be chosen (Evans, 2003; Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006) because normative data are necessary for
†
Deceased.
‡
Members of the Neuronorma Study Team are given in Appendix B.
# The Author 2010. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].
doi:10.1093/arclin/acq098 Advance Access publication on 13 December 2010
M. Quintana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 26 (2011) 144–157 145
diagnostic and descriptive accuracy (Busch & Chapin, 2008). Several integrated neuropsychological batteries have been pub-
lished with the objective of improving precision in diagnosis (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004).
The Barcelona Test (BT) is a neuropsychological battery that has been standardized and validated in a Spanish population
(Peña-Casanova, 1990). It includes a series of subtests that cover a basic spectrum of the neuropsychological functions:
language, orientation, attention, praxis, visual perceptual functions, memory, and executive functions. The abbreviated form
(a-BT) was designed to evaluate cognitive deficits associated with brain damage. It deals with the most basic and sensitive
neuropsychological items of the original and has a global standard score (Guardia et al., 1997; Peña-Casanova, Guardia,
Bertran-Serra, Manero, & Jarne, 1997; Peña-Casanova, Meza, et al., 1997). It takes only 30– 45 min to administer.
The norms for this test were published in 1997 (Peña-Casanova, Guardia, et al., 1997) and were based on a sample of 341
individuals, aged 20– 80 years (M ¼ 54.80 years, SD ¼ 17.44). Five different groups concerning age and level of education
were considered, resulting in five distinct cognitive profiles (Peña-Casanova, Guardia, et al., 1997).
The Revised BT was later published with an increased normative sample (Peña-Casanova, 2005). In addition, a number of
Research Participants
Participants in this study included 356 healthy, older adults subjects. A detailed description of recruitment procedures,
sample characteristics, and other aspects of the Neuronorma project has been provided by Peña-Casanova, Blesa, and col-
leagues, (2009). Briefly, participants were independently functioning, community-dwelling, and Spanish speakers aged over
49 with no active neurological/psychiatric disorders or current medical illness that could affect cognition.
Participants were recruited from a variety of sources such as: (1) spouses of patients evaluated at the participating centers;
(2) different senior citizen activity centers; and (3) by word of mouth.
Ten participants did not complete the a-BT subtest and were excluded from the final analyses (n ¼ 346). The sample
included 140 men (40.5%) and 206 women (59.5%). The mean age was 65.04 (SD ¼ 9.38) years, with a mean education of
10.56 years (SD ¼ 5.46). The majority (97%) of study participants were right-handed. Descriptive demographic data for the
sample are presented in Table 1. Additional demographic, sociocultural, and health-related characteristics of the normative
146 M. Quintana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 26 (2011) 144–157
n Percent of total
Age group
50–56 75 21.6
57–59 49 14.4
60–62 33 9.5
63–65 18 5.2
66–68 25 7.2
69–71 49 14.4
72–74 33 9.5
75–77 30 8.6
78–80 21 6.0
.80 13 3.7
sample (family antecedents, personal antecedents, and active medical treatment) have been described in detail by
Peña-Casanova, Blesa, and colleagues (2009).
Approval to conduct the study was obtained from the Research Ethics Committee of the Municipal Institute of Medical Care
of Barcelona, Spain, and from the different participating centers. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration
of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 1997) and its subsequent amendments, and the European Union regulations concern-
ing medical research. All participants signed an informed consent before being tested and they received no financial reimburse-
ment or any other compensation.
Neuropsychological Measures
A global cognition measure, the Mini Mental Status Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) in a Spanish vali-
dated version (Blesa et al., 2001), was used to select study participants. The age- and education-adjusted cutoff in the study was
24. Functional changes were evaluated by the IDDD (Teunisse et al., 1991) in its Spanish validated version (Böhm et al., 1998).
The a-BT was administered as part of a larger battery of neuropsychological measures in the Neuronorma project
(Peña-Casanova, Blesa, et al., 2009). Testing and scoring were performed by neuropsychologists specifically trained for
this project. All the onsite neuropsychologists were licensed as psychologists and highly experienced in neuropsychological
test administration and diagnosis. Standard administration and scoring procedures were followed as outlined in the original
a-BT manual (Peña-Casanova, 1990). It consists of 41 subtests that generate 55 variables which encompass a basic spectrum
of the neuropsychological functions: language, attention, mental tracking, working memory, repetition, confrontation naming,
semantic fluency, verbal comprehension, reading, writing, praxis, visual perceptual functions, verbal memory (story), visual
memory (figures), numerical reasoning, concept formation, sustained attention, speed, and visuospatial and motor skills
(details are shown in Appendix A).
In a series of subtests, a double score is included: a “pass or fail score” (one point scored for each item passed) and a “time
score” (score adjusted to allow for delay in responding). Credit is only allowed for a correct response. The score may range
from 1 to 3 for correct items depending on the time elapsed for responding.
Statistical Analysis
Normative procedures were the same as those used in the Neuronorma project (Peña-Casanova, Blesa, et al., 2009). To sum-
marize, age groups were defined through the overlapping cell procedure described by Pauker (1988). The effect of age,
M. Quintana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 26 (2011) 144–157 147
education, and sex on raw a-BT subtest scores was studied using coefficients of correlation (r) and determination (R 2). The
a-BT subtest raw scores were converted to age-adjusted scaled scores (SSA; from 2 to 18), SSA based on percentile ranks,
to produce a normal distribution (average 10, SD 3). Linear regressions were applied to the normalized SSA on each variable
to further adjust for education and derived age- and education-adjusted scaled scores (SSAE).
The regression coefficient (b) from this analysis was taken as the basis for education adjustments, when b was significant
value. The formula outlined by Mungas, Marshall, Weldon, Haan, and Reed (1996) employed to calculate SSAE was the fol-
lowing:
In this linear regression, the criterion variable was the a-BT score and the predictor variable was years of education. This model
can be applied due to the previously created normal distribution.
Results
Correlations (r) and shared variances (R 2) of all the subtests of the a-BT with age, education, and sex are presented in
Table 2.
Age and education shared variance in the majority of the a-BT subtests. Sex differences accounted for ,2% of shared var-
iance in all subtests, except in arithmetic, indicating no need to control for this demographic variable.
It was not possible to calculate correlations and shared variances in several variables because all the subjects obtained the
same value. For example, the variance was zero in the case of subtests such as language and grammar, or forward series.
For age, shared variance (R 2) ranged between minimal (e.g., ,1% in personal orientation) and significant values (e.g.,
24.8% in digit-symbol). A similar effect occurred in the case of education. The influence of this variable was significant in
the majority of subtests, with high values as in visual memory (R 2 ¼ 30.8%), arithmetic (R 2 ¼ 39.3%), similarities (R 2 ¼
40.6%), and digit-symbol (R 2 ¼ 43.5%).
As an example, Table 3 provides the conversion of the raw scores of the story (narrative fragment) free recall to SSA. In this
table are presented the percentile ranks, the age range of each midpoint (50 – 56, 57– 59, 60– 62, 63– 65, 66– 68, 69– 71, 72– 74,
75– 77, 78– 80, and 81+), the ranges of ages contributing to each normative group, and the number of participants contributing
to each test normative estimate. The SSA have a range from 2 to 18 and a normal distribution (a mean of 10 and an SD of 3).
Due to the large number of subtests, the rest of the SSA tables are presented as Supplementary material 1.
Table 4 provides the conversion of SSA to SSAE for the same previously presented subtest, the story (narrative fragment) free
recall. To use this table, select the appropriated column corresponding to the patient’s years of education, find the patient’s SSA,
and subsequently refer to the corresponding SSAE. See Supplementary material 2 for the other subtests of the a-BT.
We observed that some subtests show a very skewed distribution. In these special subtests, a single item failure represents
impairment. These subsets are presented in Table 5. If the subject obtains the maxim score, the SSA is 18, but if the subject
makes a mistake in such case, the SSA is 2. The same pattern was observed in all age range of each midpoint. Moreover, in this
table, the raw score followed by its percentage in the normative sample is presented.
From these age – education-adjusted scaled scores, a new cognitive profile of the a-BT was designed (Table 6). This profile
includes all scaled scores (from 2 to 18) and percentile ranks. Furthermore, the classification of ability levels is shown: very
impaired, impaired, low average, average, high average, superior, and very superior.
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to provide new normative data, from a multicenter project, of the a-BT. This brief test
includes the main neuropsychological areas and only takes 30 – 45 min to administer. It is, therefore, a test situated between
screening tests and comprehensive neuropsychological batteries, such as the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
Neuropsychological Status (RBANS; Randolph, 1998) or the ADAS-Cog (Rosen, Mohs, & Davis, 1984).
It is of interest that our results indicate that not all the subtests of the a-BT show the same score distribution. Some of them
(e.g., fluency and grammar, informative content of language, orientation, forward and backward series, naming, reading, etc.)
have little or null dispersion of data. These kinds of test are, in fact, categorical or qualitative variables. That is to say, a subject
repeats well (preservation) in contrast to not repeating well (alteration). A fact had previously been pointed out when the BT
was first published (Peña-Casanova, 1990). In other cases (e.g., complex ideational sentence comprehension, pseudoword
reading and discrimination, superimposed figures, etc.), the dispersion is partial, which may affect the correct use of the
148 M. Quintana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 26 (2011) 144–157
Table 2. Correlations (r) and shared variances (R 2) of raw scores with age, years of education, and sex (NA: not applicable because shared variance was zero)
Table 3. Age-adjusted scores (SSA) for story (narrative fragment) free recall
2 ,1 3– 4 3– 4 3 –4 3 3 3 3 3– 4 3– 5 3 –5
3 1 5– 6 5 5 4– 5 4 4 4 5
4 2 5 5 5
5 3–5 7 6 6 6
6 6–10 8 7 7 7 6– 7 6 –7 6 6 6 6
7 11–18 9– 10 8– 9 8 –9 8– 9 8– 9 8 –9 7 –8 7 7
8 19–28 11– 12 10– 11 10–11 10 10 10 9 8 7
9 29–40 13 12– 13 12 11 11 11 10 9– 10 8– 9 8
10 41–59 14 14 13–14 12– 13 12– 13 12 11–12 11 10 9 –10
2 5 5 4 4 4 4 3 3 3 3 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 0 0
3 6 6 5 5 5 5 4 4 4 4 3 3 3 3 3 2 2 2 2 1 1
4 7 7 6 6 6 6 5 5 5 5 4 4 4 4 4 3 3 3 3 2 2
5 8 8 7 7 7 7 6 6 6 6 5 5 5 5 5 4 4 4 4 3 3
6 9 9 8 8 8 8 7 7 7 7 6 6 6 6 6 5 5 5 5 4 4
7 10 10 9 9 9 9 8 8 8 8 7 7 7 7 7 6 6 6 6 5 5
8 11 11 10 10 10 10 9 9 9 9 8 8 8 8 8 7 7 7 7 6 6
9 12 12 11 11 11 11 10 10 10 10 9 9 9 9 9 8 8 8 8 7 7
10 13 13 12 12 12 12 11 11 11 11 10 10 10 10 10 9 9 9 9 8 8
11 14 14 13 13 13 13 12 12 12 12 11 11 11 11 11 10 10 10 10 9 9
12 15 15 14 14 14 14 13 13 13 13 12 12 12 12 12 11 11 11 11 10 10
13 16 16 15 15 15 15 14 14 14 14 13 13 13 13 13 12 12 12 12 11 11
14 17 17 16 16 16 16 15 15 15 15 14 14 14 14 14 13 13 13 13 12 12
15 18 18 17 17 17 17 16 16 16 16 15 15 15 15 15 14 14 14 14 13 13
16 19 19 18 18 18 18 17 17 17 17 16 16 16 16 16 15 15 15 15 14 14
17 20 20 19 19 19 19 18 18 18 18 17 17 17 17 17 16 16 16 16 15 15
18 21 21 20 20 20 20 19 19 19 19 18 18 18 18 18 17 17 17 17 16 16
Notes: Education adjustment applying the following formula: SSAE ¼ SSA – (b × [education (years) – 12]), where b ¼ 0.2596. SSA ¼ age-adjusted scaled
scores; SSAE ¼ age– education-adjusted scaled scores.
Table 5. Percent distribution of raw scores in selected subtest and age-adjusted scores (SSA) for all age range of each midpoint
Subtest Raw score (%) Scale score 2, percentile range ,1 Scale score 18, percentile range .99
Subtest percentiles ,1 1 2 3 –5 6 –10 11–18 19– 28 29– 40 41–59 60–71 72– 81 82– 89 90– 94 95–97 98 99 .99
Notes: I ¼ impaired; LA ¼ low average; HA ¼ high average; T ¼ time score; compr ¼ comprehension.
M. Quintana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 26 (2011) 144–157 151
overall Neuronorma analysis method based on normal distributions. This problem also appears in other neuropsychological
tasks such as digit repetition (Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Úbeda, Quintana-Aparicio, et al., 2009) or in some subtests of the
Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (Peña-Casanova, Quintana-Aparicio, Quiñones-Úbeda, et al., 2009). To minimize
this effect, some authors suggest dealing with data in a raw score form rather than converting them into scaled scores (Lezak
et al., 2004). We certainly agree with such a proposition and recognize the statistical problems of forcing these kinds of scores
into a normal distribution. Finally, the rest of the subtests (e.g., semantic fluency, constructional praxis-drawing copy, all
measures of memory, digit-symbol, arithmetic, similarities, and block design) show a clear variability of scores.
In spite of the psychometrically different measures, given the characteristics and purposes of this normative project, we
decided to maintain the same model of statistical analysis for all the subtests. Thus, we developed normative data following
the single procedure used in the Neuronorma project.
The normative data used should be recent because when a test is re-normed, there is typically a reduction in the resulting
standard scores with the new version (Pae et al., 2005). As a result, a better performance with regard to the original sample, and
Moreover, it manages to solve the limitation of using large groups that are age-stratified with no overlapping by allowing the
comparison with more adjacent ages.
We also obtained the same metric for all the subtests which made one cognitive profile possible. In the original normative
data (Peña-Casanova, Guardia, et al., 1997), five different cognitive profiles were published. Moreover, the sample came from
different Spanish regions (for more information, see Peña-Casanova, Blesa, et al., 2009); therefore, it was a multicenter project
and thus achieved a better representation of the Spanish population. Finally, the normative data for the a-BT were developed in
conjunction with norms for all the neuropsychological tests comprising the Neuronorma test battery. This same co-norming
process had been previously used by Rilling and colleagues (2005) for older African Americans on the Mattis Dementia
Rating Scale, as part of the MOAANS project.
We anticipate that this feature of Neuronorma norms will facilitate interpretation of a-BT performance within the context of
a patient’s overall neuropsychological profile, although the a-BT is of interest on its own as a general cognitive functioning
battery. Moreover, it can be used as a complement to the Neuronorma battery, since it includes cognitive domains not assessed
Supplementary material
Funding
This study was mainly supported by a grant from the Pfizer Foundation and by the Medical Department of Pfizer, SA. Spain.
It was also supported by the Behavioral Neurology group of the Program of Neuropsychopharmacology of the Institut
Municipal d’Investigaciò Mèdica, Barcelona, Spain. Dr. Jordi Peña-Casanova has received an intensification research grant
from the CIBERNED (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas), Instituto
Carlos III (Ministry of Health and Consumer Affairs of Spain). Dr. Peña-Casanova (2008) has received the “Alzheimer
Award” from the Spanish Society of Neurology, as a leader of the Neuronorma project and as well the Neuronorma Group
(2010) have received the “Alzheimer Award” from the Spanish Society of Neurology.
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
Appendix A
1 Fluency and grammar Conversation, picture description, and narrative speech — Categorical descriptive rating scale from 0– 10
tasks nonfluent to fluent speech
2 Informative content of Conversation, picture description, and narrative speech — Informative content rating scale from no 0– 10
language tasks information to normality
3 Personal orientation Questions on: First name and last name, age, date of 7 Correct: 1 point 0– 7
birth, place of birth, relative’s names, address, and Incorrect: 0 point
occupation
4 Spatial orientation Questions on: City, neighborhood, type of place, name of 5 Correct: 1 point 0– 5
the place, and floor Incorrect: 0 point
Appendix A. (continued)
# Subtest Description NI Scoring Score
range
5 Temporal orientation Questions on: Date, day of the week, time, month, part of 6 Correct: 1 point 0– 23
the day, and year Incorrect: 0 point
For month correct: 5 points
For year correct: 10 points
For part of the day correct: 5 points
6 Digit Span Forward Repetition of digit sequences of increasing length (3–9) 7 The most large string repeated forward 0– 9
forward
7 Digit Span Backward Repetition of digit sequences of increasing length (2–8) 7 The most large string repeated backward 0– 8
backward
Appendix A. (continued)
# Subtest Description NI Scoring Score
range
34 Symbolic conventional ges- To carry out symbolic gestures for communication 5 Correct: 2 points 0– 10
tures (commands): right (commands, right limb/hand; e.g., How would you Impaired: 1 point
limb/hand pretend to. . .wave good bye, salute like a soldier, etc.) Failed: 0 point
35 Symbolic conventional ges- Items of subtest #34: commands, left limb/hand 5 Correct: 2 points 0– 10
tures (commands): left Impaired: 1 point
limb/hand Failed: 0 point
36 Symbolic conventional ges- Items of subtest #34: imitation, right limb/hand 5 Correct: 2 points 0– 10
tures (imitation): right Impaired: 1 point
limb/hand Failed: 0 point
Appendix B
Steering committee: J.P.-C., Hospital del Mar, Barcelona, Spain; R.B., Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona,
Spain; M.A., Hospital Mútua de Terrassa, Terrassa, Spain.
Principal investigators: J.P.-C., Hospital de Mar, Barcelona, Spain; R.B., Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona,
Spain; M.A., Hospital Mútua de Terrassa, Terrassa, Spain; J.L.M., Hospital Clı́nic, Barcelona, Spain; A.R., Hospital Clı́nico
Universitario, Santiago de Compostela, Spain; M.S.B., Hospital Clı́nico San Carlos, Madrid, Spain; C.A., Hospital Virgen
Arrixaca, Murcia, Spain; C.M.-P., Hospital Virgen Macarena, Sevilla, Spain; A.F.-G., Hospital Universitario La Paz,
Madrid, Spain; M.F., Hospital de Cruces, Bilbao, Spain.
Genetics substudy: Rafael Oliva, Service of Genetics, Hospital Clı́nic, Barcelona, Spain.
Neuroimaging substudy: Beatriz Gómez-Ansón, Radiology Department and IDIBAPS, Hospital Clı́nic, Barcelona, Spain.
Research Fellows: Gemma Monte, Elena Alayrach, Aitor Sainz, and Claudia Caprile, Fundació Clinic, Hospital Clinic,
References
Attix, D. K., Story, T. J., Chelune, G. J., Ball, J. D., Stutts, M. L., Hart, R. P., et al. (2009). The prediction of change: Normative neuropsychological trajectories.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 21– 38.
Baxendale, S. (2010). The Flynn effect and memory function. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 1 –5.
Blesa, R., Pujol, M., Aguilar, M., Santacruz, P., Bertran-Serra, I., Hernandez, G., et al. (2001). Clinical validity of the ‘mini-mental state’ for Spanish speaking
communities. Neuropsychologia, 39, 1150– 1157.
Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., & Roth, M. (1968). The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter
of elderly subjects. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 797–811.
Böhm, P., Peña-Casanova, J., Aguilar, M., Hernández, G., Sol, J. M., & Blesa, R. (1998). Clinical validity and utility of the Interview for Deterioration of Daily
Living in Dementia for Spanish-speaking communities. International Psychogeriatrics, 10, 261–270.
Bowman, M. L. (2002). The perfidy of percentiles. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 295–303.
Busch, R. M., & Chapin, J. S. (2008). Review of normative data for common screening measures used to evaluate cognitive functioning in elderly individuals.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22, 620– 650.
Busch, R. M., Chelune, G. J., & Suchy, Y. (2006). Using norms in neuropsychology assessment in elderly. In D. K. Attix, & K. A. Wersh-Bohmer (Eds.),
Geriatric neuropsychology: Assessment and intervention (pp. 133– 157). New York: The Guilford Press.
Bush, S. S. (2010). Determining whether or when to adopt new versions of psychological and neuropsychological tests: ethical and professional considerations.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24, 7 –16.
Cejudo-Bolı́var, J. C., Torrealba-Fernández, J., Guardia-Olmos, J., & Peña-Casanova, J. (1998). Constructive praxis to the copy: extended norms of the
Barcelona test [Praxis constructiva a la copia normas ampliadas del test Barcelona]. Neurologı́a, 13, 329–334.
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2009). Percentiles please: The case for expressing neuropsychological test scores and accompanying confidence limits as
percentiles ranks. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 193– 204.
De la Paz, M. P., Philen, R. M., Gerr, F., Letz, R., Ferrari Arroyo, F. J., Vela, L., et al. (2003). Neurological outcomes of toxic oil syndrome patients 18 years
after the epidemic. Enviormental Health Perspectives, 111, 1326–1334.
Duque, P., Páramo, M. D., Borges, M., Peral, E., Garcı́a Moreno, J. M., Balarezo, L., et al. (2003). Neuropsychological disorders in amyotrophic lateral scler-
osis. Don’t they exist or do they just go undetected? [Alteraciones neuropsicológicas en esclerosis lateral amiotrófica, existen o no se detectan?]. Revista de
Neurologı́a, 1, 3 –8.
156 M. Quintana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 26 (2011) 144–157
Evans, J. (2003). Basic concepts and principles of neuropsychological assessment. In P. W. Halligan, U. Kischka, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Handbook of clinical
neuropsychology (pp. 15– 26). New York: Oxford University Press.
Ferman, T. J., Lucas, J. A., Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Willis, F. B., Pertersen, R. C., et al. (2005). Mayo’s Older African American Normative Studies: Auditory
Verbal Learning Test norms for African American elders. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, 214– 228.
Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 29–51.
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171– 191.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189– 198.
Frutos-Alegrı́a, M. T., Moltó-Jordà, J. M., Morera-Guitart, J., Sánchez-Pérez, A., & Ferrer-Navajas, M. (2007). The neuropsychological profile of mild cog-
nitive impairment with involvement of multiple cognitive areas. The importance of amnesia in distinguishing two subtypes of patients [Perfil neuropsico-
lógico del deterioro cognitivo leve con afectación de múltiples áreas cognitivas. Importancia de la amnesia en la distinción de dos subtipos de pacientes].
Revista de Neurologı́a, 44, 455 –459.
Garcı́a-Morales, P., Gich-Fullà, J., Guardia-Olmos, J., & Peña-Casanova, J. (1998). Digit span, automatic speech and orientation: amplified norms of the
Barcelona Test [Series de dı́gitos, series automáticas y orientación: normas ampliadas del Test Barcelona]. Neurologı́a, 13, 271–276.
Peña-Casanova, J., Meza, M., Bertran-Serra, I., Manero, R. M., Espel, G., Martı́, A., et al. (1997). Shortened version of the Barcelona test (III): criterion validity
with the ADAS-Cog [Versión abreviada del test Barcelona (III): validez de criterio con el ADAS-Cog]. Neurologı́a, 12, 117– 119.
Peña-Casanova, J., Monllau, A., Böhm, P., Blesa, R., Aguilar, M., Sol, J. M., et al. (2005). Correlations between cognition and function in Alzheimer’s disease:
based on the abbreviated Barcelona Test (a-BT) [Correlación cognitivo-funcional en la demencia tipo Alzheimer: a propósito del Test Barcelona
Abreviado]. Neurologı́a, 20, 4– 8.
Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Gramunt-Fombuena, N., Aguilar, M., Casas, L., Molinuevo, J. L., et al. (2009). Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies
(NEURONORMA Project): Norms for Boston Naming Test and Token Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 343 –354.
Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Gramunt-Fombuena, N., Quintana, M., Aguilar, M., Molinuevo, J. L., et al. (2009). Spanish Multicenter Normative
Studies (Neuronorma Project): Norms for the Stroop Color-Word Interference and the Tower of London-Drexel. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24,
413–429.
Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Gramunt-Fombuena, N., Quintana-Aparicio, M., Aguilar, M., Badenes, D., et al. (2009). Spanish Multicenter
Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for Verbal Fluency Tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 395–411.
Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Quintana-Aparicio, M., Aguilar, M., Badenes, D., Molinuelo, J. L., et al. (2009). Spanish Multicenter Normative
Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for Verbal Span, Visuospatial Span, Letter and Number Sequencing, Trail Making Test, and Symbol Digit