Gender Schema Theory and Its Implication
Gender Schema Theory and Its Implication
Gender Schema Theory and Its Implication
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/3173685?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Signs
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Gender Schema Theory and Its
Implications for Child Development:
Raising Gender-aschematic Children
in a Gender-schematic Society
Psychoanalytic Theory
The first psychologist to ask how male and female are transmuted
into masculine and feminine was Freud. Accordingly, in the past virtually
598
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 599
3. E.g., Nancy Chodorow, The Reproduction of Mothering: Psychoanalysis and the Sociology
of Gender (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1978); Gayle Rubin, "The Traffic in
Women: Notes on the 'Political Economy' of Sex," in Toward an Anthropology of Women, ed.
Rayna Reiter (New York: Monthly Review Press, 1975), pp. 157-210.
4. Lawrence Kohlberg, "A Cognitive-Developmental Analysis of Children's Sex-Role
Concepts and Attitudes," in The Development of Sex Differences, ed. Eleanor E. Maccoby
(Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1966), pp. 82-173; Maureen J. McConaghy,
"Gender Permanence and the Genital Basis of Gender: Stages in the Development of
Constancy of Gender Identity," Child Development 50, no. 4 (December 1979): 1223-26.
5. Eleanor E. Maccoby and Carol N. Jacklin, The Psychology of Sex Differences (Stanford,
Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1974).
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
600 Bem Gender Schema Theory
Although social learning theory can account for the young child's
acquiring a number of particular behaviors that are stereotyped by the
culture as sex appropriate, it treats the child as the relatively passive
recipient of environmental forces rather than as an active agent striving
to organize and thereby to comprehend the social world. This view of
the passive child is inconsistent with the common observation that chil-
dren themselves frequently construct and enforce their own version of
society's gender rules. It is also inconsistent with the fact that the flexibil-
ity with which children interpret society's gender rules varies predictably
with age. In one study, for example, 73 percent of the four-year-olds
and 80 percent of the nine-year-olds believed-quite flexibly-that there
should be no sexual restrictions on one's choice of occupation. Between
those ages, however, children held more rigid opinions, with the middle
children being the least flexible of all. Thus, only 33 percent of the
five-year-olds, 10 percent of the six-year-olds, 11 percent of the seven-
year-olds, and 44 percent of the eight-year-olds believed there should be
no sexual restrictions on one's choice of occupation.7
This particular developmental pattern is not unique to the child's
interpretation of gender rules. Even in a domain as far removed from
gender as syntax, children first learn certain correct grammatical forms
through reinforcement and modeling. As they get a bit older, however,
they begin to construct their own grammatical rules on the basis of what
they hear spoken around them, and they are able only later still to allow
6. Walte- Mischel, "Sex-'yping and Socialization," in Carmichael's Manual of Child
Psychology, ed. Paul H. Mussen, 2 vols. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1970), 2:3-72.
7. William Damon, The Social World of the Child (San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1977).
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 601
for exceptions to those rules. Thus, only the youngest and the oldest
children say "ran"; children in between say "runned."8 What all of this
implies, of course, is that the child is passive in neither domain. Rather,
she or he is actively constructing rules to organize-and thereby to
comprehend-the vast array of information in his or her world.
Cognitive-Developmental Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
602 Bem Gender Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 603
Gender-schematic Processing
Gender schema theory begins with the observation that the devel-
oping child invariably learns his or her society's cultural definitions of
femaleness and maleness. In most societies, these definitions comprise a
diverse and sprawling network of sex-linked associations encompassing
not only those features directly related to female and male persons-
such as anatomy, reproductive function, division of labor, and personal-
ity attributes-but also features more remotely or metaphorically related
to sex, such as the angularity or roundedness of an abstract shape and
the periodicity of the moon. Indeed, no other dichotomy in human
experience appears to have as many entities linked to it as does the
distinction between female and male.
But there is more. Gender schema theory proposes that, in addition
to learning such content-specific information about gender, the child
also learns to invoke this heterogeneous network of sex-related associa-
tions in order to evaluate and assimilate new information. The child, in
short, learns to encode and to organize information in terms of an
evolving gender schema.
A schema is a cognitive structure, a network of associations that
organizes and guides an individual's perception. A schema functions as
an anticipatory structure, a readiness to search for and to assimilate
incoming information in schema-relevant terms. Schematic information
Psychological Review 88, no. 4 (July 1981): 354-64; and "Gender Schema Theory and
Self-Schema Theory Compared: A Comment on Markus, Crane, Bernstein, and Siladi's
'Self-Schemas and Gender,' "Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43, no. 6 (December
1982): 1192-94.
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
604 Bem Gender Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 605
15. Jerome Kagan, "Acquisition and Significance of Sex Typing and Sex Role Iden-
tity," in Review of Child Development Research, ed. Martin L. Hoffman and Lois W. Hoffman
(New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1964), 1:137-67.
16. Susan M. Andersen and Sandra L. Bem, "Sex Typing and Androgyny in Dyadic
Interaction: Individual Differences in Responsiveness to Physical Attractiveness," Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 41, no. 1 (July 1981): 74-86; Bem, "Gender Schema
Theory"; Kay Deaux and Brenda Major, "Sex-related Patterns in the Unit of Perception,"
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 3, no. 2 (Spring 1977): 297-300; Brenda Girvin,
"The Nature of Being Schematic: Sex-Role Self-Schemas and Differential Processing of
Masculine and Feminine Information" (Ph.D. diss., Stanford University, 1978); Robert V.
Kail and Laura E. Levine, "Encoding Processes and Sex-Role Preferences," Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology 21, no. 2 (April 1976): 256-63; Lynn S. Liben and Margaret L.
Signorella, "Gender-related Schemata and Constructive Memory in Children," Child Devel-
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
606 Bem
Gender Schema Theory
opment 51, no. 1 (March 1980): 11-18; Richard Lippa, "Androgyny, Sex Typing, and the
Perception of Masculinity-Femininity in Handwriting,"Journal of Research in Personality 11,
no. 1 (March 1977): 21-37; Hazel Markus et al., "Self-Schemas and Gender," Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 42, no. 1 (January 1982): 38-50; Shelley E. Taylor and
Hsiao-Ti Falcone, "Cognitive Bases of Stereotyping: The Relationship between Categori-
zation and Prejudice," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 8, no. 3 (September 1982):
426-32.
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 607
Gender schema theory predicts and the results of this study confirm
that sex-typed subjects are significantly faster than non-sex-typed sub-
jects when endorsing sex-appropriate attributes and when rejecting
sex-inappropriate attributes. These results suggest that when deciding
whether a particular attribute is or is not self-descriptive, sex-typed indi-
viduals do not bother to go through a time-consuming process of re-
cruiting behavioral evidence from memory and judging whether the
evidence warrants an affirmative answer-which is presumably what
non-sex-typed individuals do. Rather, sex-typed individuals "look up"
the attribute in the gender schema. If the attribute is sex appropriate,
they quickly say yes; if the attribute is sex inappropriate, they quickly say
no. Occasionally, of course, even sex-typed individuals must admit to
possessing an attribute that is sex inappropriate or to lacking an attribute
that is sex appropriate. On these occasions, they are significantly slower
than non-sex-typed individuals. This pattern of rapid delivery of
gender-consistent self-descriptions and slow delivery of gender-
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
608 Bem Gender Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 609
observing, for example, that what parents, teachers, and peers consider
to be appropriate behavior varies as a function of sex; that toys, clothing,
occupations, hobbies, the domestic division of labor-even pronouns-
all vary as a function of sex.
Gender schema theory thus implies that children would be far less
likely to become gender schematic and hence sex typed if the society
were to limit the associative network linked to sex and to temper its
insistence on the functional importance of the gender dichotomy. Ironi-
cally, even though our society has become sensitive to negative sex
stereotypes and has begun to expunge them from the media and from
children's literature, it remains blind to its gratuitous emphasis on the
gender dichotomy itself. In elementary schools, for example, boys and
girls line up separately or alternately; they learn songs in which the
fingers are "ladies" and the thumbs are "men"; they see boy and girl
paper-doll silhouettes alternately placed on the days of the month in
order to learn about the calendar. Children, it will be noted, are not
lined up separately or alternately as blacks and whites; fingers are not
"whites" and thumbs "blacks"; black and white dolls do not alternately
mark the days of the calendar. Our society seeks to deemphasize racial
distinctions but continues to exaggerate sexual distinctions.
Because of the role that sex plays in reproduction, perhaps no soci-
ety could ever be as indifferent to sex in its cultural arrangements as it
could be to, say, eye color, thereby giving the gender schema a sociologi-
cally based priority over many other categories. For the same reason, it
may even be, as noted earlier, that sex has evolved to be a basic category
of perception for our species, thereby giving the gender schema a
biologically based priority as well. Be that as it may, however, gender
schema theory claims that society's ubiquitous insistence on the func-
tional importance of the gender dichotomy must necessarily render it
even more cognitively available-and available in more remotely rele-
vant contexts-than it would be otherwise.
It should be noted that gender schema theory's claims about the
antecedents of gender-schematic processing have not yet been tested
empirically. Hence it is not possible at this point to state whether indi-
vidual differences in gender-schematic processing do, in fact, derive
from differences in the emphasis placed on gender dichotomy in indi-
viduals' socialization histories, or to describe concretely the particular
kinds of socialization histories that enhance or diminish gender-
schematic processing. Nevertheless, I should like to set forth a number
of plausible strategies that are consistent with gender schema theory for
raising a gender-aschematic child in the midst of a gender-schematic
society.
This discussion will, by necessity, be highly speculative. Even so, it
will serve to clarify gender schema theory's view of exactly how gender-
schematic processing is learned and how something else might be
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
610 Bern Gender Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 611
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
612 Bem Gender Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 613
20. Stephanie Waxman, What Is a Girl? What Is a Boy? (Culver City, Calif.: Peace Press,
1976).
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
614 Bem Gender Schema Theory
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Signs Summer 1983 615
parents will not and should not be satisfied to pretend that they think all
ideas-particularly those about gender-are equally valid. At some
point, they will feel compelled to declare that the view of women and
men conveyed by fairy tales, by the mass media-and by the next-door
neighbors-is not only different, but wrong. It is time to teach one's
children about sexism.
Moreover, it is only by giving children a sexism schema, a coherent
and organized understanding of the historical roots and the con-
temporaneous consequences of sex discrimination, that they will truly be
able to comprehend why the sexes appear to be so different in our
society: why, for example, there has never been a female president of the
United States; why fathers do not stay home with their children; and
why so many people believe these sex differences to be the natural con-
sequence of biology. The child who has developed a readiness to encode
and to organize information in terms of an evolving sexism schema is a
child who is prepared to oppose actively the gender-related constraints
that those with a gender schema will inevitably seek to impose.
The development of a sexism schema is nicely illustrated by our
daughter Emily's response to Norma Klein's book Girls Can Be Anything.21
One of the characters is Adam Sobel, who insists that "girls are always
nurses and boys are always doctors" and that "girls can't be pilots, . . .
they have to be stewardesses." After reading this book, our daughter,
then age four, spontaneously began to label with contempt anyone who
voiced stereotyped beliefs about gender an "Adam Sobel." Adam Sobel
thus became for her the nucleus of an envolving sexism schema, a
schema that enables her now to perceive-and also to become morally
outraged by and to oppose-whatever sex discrimination she meets in
daily life.
As feminist parents, we wish it could have been possible to raise our
children with neither a gender schema nor a sexism schema. At this
historical moment, however, that is not an option. Rather we must
choose either to have our children become gender schematic and hence
sex typed, or to have our children become sexism schematic and hence
feminists. We have chosen the latter.
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
616 Bem Gender Schema Theory
L. Ber, Wendy Martyna, and Carol Watson, "Sex-Typing and Androgyny: Further Ex-
plorations of the Expressive Domain," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34, no. 5
(November 1976): 1016-23; Sandra L. Bern, "Beyond Androgyny: Some Presumptuous
Pr-escl-iptions for a Liberated Sexual Identity," in The Futu-e of Women: Issues in Psychology,
ed. Julia Sherman and Florence Denmark (New York: Psychological Dimensions, Inc.,
1978), pp. 1-23; Sandra L. Bern and Ellen Lenney, "Sex-Typing and the Avoidance of
Cross-Sex Behavior,"Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 33, no. 1 (Janua-y 1976):
48-54.
This content downloaded from 140.123.217.91 on Sun, 17 Apr 2016 08:18:26 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms