Introducing Criminology
Introducing Criminology
Introducing Criminology
Introducing criminology
i
Introducing Criminology
ii
Contents
Introducing criminology
Clive Coleman
Clive Norris
First published by Willan Publishing 2000
This edition published by Routledge 2011
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
270 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016
The right of Clive Coleman and Clive Norris to be identified as the authors of this Work has
been asserted by them in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988
All rights reserved; no part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval;
system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying,
recording or otherwise without the prior written permission of the Publishers or a licence
permitting copying in the UK issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, 90 Tottenham
Court Road, London W1P 9HE.
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.
iv
Contents
Contents
Preface viii
Introduction 1
v
Introducing Criminology
Psychological approaches 31
Psychoanalytic approaches 33
Learning theories 35
Cognitive approaches 41
The revival of approaches locating the sources of crime in the
individual 50
Conclusion 54
vi
Contents
Bibliography 180
Index 198
vii
Introducing Criminology
viii
Crime, the criminal and criminology
Preface
ix
Introducing Criminology
x
Introduction
Introduction
Crime and criminology were two of the growth industries of the second
half of the twentieth century. Crime and how to respond to it are major
issues of today. Many universities now offer degree courses in crimin-
ology, and it is often taken as an element in other degree programmes. In
addition, there is significant interest on the part of the general public.
There seemed to be a need for a relatively short book that would welcome
people to the subject, being suitable for those who were just interested in
it, those thinking of taking it up, or those who have already embarked on a
course of study. There are plenty of big American-style textbooks that
attempt a comprehensive coverage. This book does not try to do this. The
first three chapters are broadly based, taking an overview of some major
themes and approaches. The second half of the book provides a more
detailed insight into the way in which criminologists go about their
business by looking in depth at three topics, selected to illustrate work that
has been done in different areas of criminology. We like to think that what
we are doing is rather like inviting readers to a party, at which they will
meet some key figures in the subject, eavesdrop on some of their conversa-
tions and arguments, and learn how they go about their work in terms of
actual research.
This book, then, invites the reader to a party, given to celebrate the
birthday of criminology, although there is some dispute about the date of
this. Many of the guests will have claims to be called criminologists. They
may surprise you by the diversity in their backgrounds and interests.
Among the guests you will meet: an Italian Professor with a fascination
with your bodily measurements, for whom size is important, and who
claims to be the father of criminology; a rather challenging person who
tells you that it is necessary to change the nature of our greedy capitalist
society before we can make real progress in dealing with crime; someone
1
Introducing Criminology
who wants to improve the conditions in our prisons and limit their use to
the smallest number of cases; a civil servant who works for a big
government department, which is concerned with the running of the
criminal justice system. Finally, a rather troublesome gatecrasher, who
regrets that criminology was born at all. Still want to come? Then read on...
Parties can be fun; they can also be tense but revealing occasions. The
world of criminology is not so very different from this. As we shall see,
criminologists seem to form themselves into identifiable groupings, which
do not always communicate easily with each other, almost as if they speak
different languages. Indeed, they often do speak different languages, and
criminology can develop in different ways in different countries (see Nye
1984 on the conflict between the ‘French’ and ‘Italian’ schools). The
occasional skirmish is to be observed, the reasons for which are not always
clear to outsiders. When criminologists are gathered together, it is not all
sweetness and light, for big questions are asked by them, to which there
are no easy answers. Since criminology raises issues about human nature
and social order, and connects with the world of human suffering, it deals
with questions that matter to people’s lives. It is therefore not surprising
that heat is often generated.
Birthday parties are often occasions for reminiscence. Guests tell stories
that illustrate the character of the person whose big day is being
celebrated. Some of the older guests are able to recall the very early history
of the person, and even how they were first contemplated (or not) and
conceived. This book will begin by asking such basic questions about the
character of criminology: what is it about? It will attempt to trace its
history and origins from the earliest times, when it may have been a mere
twinkle in someone’s eye. Who were the parents, and what were they like?
What impact did they have on the development of their offspring? Did
others have an influence on the development of criminology in ways that
the parents did not contemplate? Has there been dispute over the
parentage of the subject, and over the way in which its life and career
should develop?
Indeed, we may even find those at the party who, far from celebrating
the birth and continued existence of criminology, are essentially critical of
its fundamental character and concerns: has it spent much of its time on
the ‘wrong’ subject matter and asking the ‘wrong’ questions? Does it need
to change in a radical way or even be abolished altogether? The basic
nature of criminology, its origins and its subject matter are the focus of the
first chapter of this book.
Parties are also occasions where games are often played. What kinds of
games are played by criminologists? In chapter 2 we begin to look at a
game which has consistently been played by criminologists over the years:
‘spot the difference’ between offenders and non-offenders. The intention
has been to discover how offenders and non-offenders differ from each
2
Introduction
3
Introducing Criminology
criminologist. Surely this type of offender is a good candidate for ‘spot the
difference’? If theories of crime in general are difficult to construct when
crime is such a varied category, ranging from the theft of milk from
doorsteps to serial rape, surely such a specific crime as this should be a
good candidate for constructing a criminological theory? We therefore
give the experts pencils and paper, and ask them to come up with their
theoretical ‘profiles’ of the serial killer. Which, if any, is the most
convincing in terms of contributing to our understanding – that produced
by biology, psychology or sociology? The chapter serves as an illustration
of some of the procedures and problems involved in constructing a theory
of a particular type of crime.
While for many years the quest to understand and explain crime has
been a major preoccupation of criminology, a rather more recent focus has
been the workings of the criminal justice system. Rather than attempting
to give an overview of this area, we take an example of one important
agency within it – the police. Often seen as playing a key role as gate-
keepers in the criminal justice process, placed as they are at the entry point
of the formal process, the police are also the agency with which most
readers are likely to be familiar. Even if readers have not had their collars
felt, they are likely to have had some contact with the police, and to have
consumed a rich diet of representations of the police and policing through
the mass media.
We focus on some key issues that have preoccupied criminologists in
their study of the police. Firstly, what is their role? Are they solely respon-
sible for policing, or are other agencies involved? What do the police
actually do? Is their main role best seen as the control of crime, as is often
suggested? Secondly, we introduce two concepts that are important in the
study of criminal justice: discretion and discrimination, which we discuss
in the context of the police and policing. What is discretion and why does
it exist? Does police discretion result in discrimination against certain
groups in the population? Here we introduce the concept of police culture,
which has often been used to explain the way in which police discretion is
exercised. Finally, we outline a framework which has been commonly
used in the study of criminal justice: the distinction between due process
and crime control models of criminal justice. We discuss these models in
relation to policing, and ask how police practice in violation of ‘due
process’ might come about.
Another area that has received increasing attention in recent years is
crime prevention. Our final main chapter provides a brief introduction to
this area, before examining in depth a highly topical example of crime
prevention activity – closed circuit television (CCTV) systems. After
examining the reasons behind the popularity and growth of CCTV, we
look at a key question that criminologists are often expected to answer
about such initiatives: does it reduce crime? As an illustration of the
4
Introduction
5
Introducing Criminology
Chapter 1
Having invited you to the party, it is now time to find out more about the
guests and their activities. The starting point is to introduce you to two key
terms: crime and the criminal. Although the meaning of these terms may
initially seem obvious, there have long been arguments about the defini-
tion of them. These arguments are important, for they raise key issues
about criminology and its scope, and the way in which societies identify
and respond to actions as criminal. This leads into a consideration of the
nature and focus of criminology itself. There are different ideas about its
nature and aims, which will be explained. The final part of the chapter
attempts to contribute to our understanding of criminology by taking a
brief overview of its history, particularly in its early days. When was it
born, and do those early years help to explain its nature and development
in the years to come?
have been convicted of crime and those who have not. ... The criminologist
is therefore quite justified in making the convict population the subject of
his studies as he does’ (Michael and Adler 1933: 3). In practice, for many
years criminologists conducted studies of prisoners; the prison provided a
convenient captive sample for researchers and a context within which
their specialism could develop with a particular focus – the individual
criminal. However, prisoners are only a sample of those convicted of
crime, who in their turn are only a sample of those who have broken the
criminal law; it cannot be assumed that such samples are representative of
all law-breakers.
10
Crime, the criminal and criminology
becoming subservient to that particular state, the status quo, and the
interests it serves. For example, consider the position of a criminologist in
Nazi Germany of the 1930s, where certain groups in the population had
their human rights attacked, with the support of the state and its legal
apparatus. From the human rights perspective, the state can be seen as a
perpetrator of crime, rather than simply the authority that defines crime.
For the Schwendingers the role of the criminologist should be to engage in
the stuggle for those basic human rights so that criminologists become
guardians of those rights rather than defenders of the current order of
things (consider again the predicament of our criminologist in Nazi
Germany). What the human rights approach shares in common with that
of some earlier writers is the attempt to identify certain basic standards,
any deviation from which is ‘crime‘.
11
Introducing Criminology
12
Crime, the criminal and criminology
What is criminology?
• the attempt to describe and analyse the extent, nature, and distribution
of the various forms of ‘crime’ and ‘offenders’.
• the analysis of the ‘causes’ of crime, including the attempt to develop
theories which will help us to explain and understand it.
• the study of the formulation of criminal laws. Although this area is
often said to be part of the sociology of law rather than criminology
itself, our discussion above indicates the value of such study to
criminology.
13
Introducing Criminology
A multi-disciplinary discipline?
Criminology is often described as a multi-disciplinary enterprise (Garland
1997: 19; Lanier and Henry 1998: 5). What this means is that ‘criminolo-
gists draw upon a variety of other disciplines, most notably sociology,
psychology, psychiatry, law, history, and anthropology – indeed, one of the
major dynamics of modern criminology is the incessant raiding of other
disciplines or ideologies for new ideas ...’ (Garland 1997: 19). To these
disciplines could be added those of biology, geography, economics, and
political science, all of which have made forays into the subject or have
been raided in the way that Garland suggests. Two points follow from this.
One is that this promiscuity – the frequent intercourse with a variety of
partners – can make life both interesting and difficult. Variety may be the
spice of life, but each of these disciplines tends to have its own assump-
tions, basic ideas and concepts, a bank of shared knowledge, a particular
focus of inquiry, favoured methods of investigation, and a view of the
world which leads to particular kinds of policy implications. Disciplines
are also internally diverse, harbouring different perspectives within them
(see, for example, the various approaches in psychology and sociology
discussed in Chapters 2 and 3). All of this makes for a very diverse but
intellectually challenging subject. Few criminologists are equally at ease
with all of these disciplines.
The second point concerns criminology’s claim to be a discipline at all.
Is the enterprise so parasitic upon those other disciplines that it has no
right to be called one itself? Is it merely the application of a number of
other disciplines to a particular subject matter – one largely defined by the
state as a problem? Is it better seen as a field of study, rather than a
discipline as such? (Lea 1998)
While it has often been argued that criminology is not, or should not be
seen as, a discipline at all, others have taken the view that the existence of
a discipline is not merely about such matters as having a distinctive and
defensible subject matter or methods of enquiry, but also about institu-
tional foundations and structures:
Garland’s argument is that the reasons for the existence of criminology are
to be found in particular historical circumstances, and that its coming into
being was by no means inevitable. It is also a history that tells us a great
deal about the nature of criminology today. For one thing, this may help us
to understand why many working on studies of crime and justice do not
adopt this disciplinary identity, do not necessarily belong to the
professional associations or publish in the specialist journals.
16
Crime, the criminal and criminology
17
Introducing Criminology
Human nature
People are seen as self-interested, rational creatures (able to weigh up
competing courses of action), able to make and act in accordance with
personal choices (often described as ‘free will‘).
Causes of crime
Since people are normally rational creatures, it seems to follow that either
the pleasure or gain from the crime outweighs the pain of punishment
associated with it, or that people are making irrational decisions for some
reason (e.g. the institutions of law or education may be sending out
‘wrong’ or confusing messages, so that people do not have the appropriate
information to make rational decisions).
18
Crime, the criminal and criminology
Other headings for comparison are often included. Such frameworks are
invaluable in getting a grasp of the various perspectives and their basic
concepts. There are, however, one or two limitations. First, such outlines
are the product of a great deal of simplification; complexity and variety is
often lost. We must not expect all of the thinkers who are grouped together
under such labels to subscribe to identical ideas. Furthermore, the attempt
to provide satisfying contrasts between classicism and its opponent in the
nineteenth century (positivism) sometimes goes too far. For example,
Beirne (1993) has argued that, contrary to popular belief, Beccaria (the
most frequently given example of a ‘classical’ thinker), did not subscribe to
a view of humans as endowed with ‘free will’ and ‘rational choice’, but
was concerned with ‘the application to crime and penal strategies of the
“science of man,” a deterministic discourse implicitly at odds with the
conventional assumption about ... “classical criminology“ ‘ (Beirne 1993:
5). Beirne suggests that Beccaria was involved in a very rudimentary
attempt to forge some key concepts of an embryonic criminology. These
concepts include “crime,” “criminal,” and “causes of crime” ‘ (Beirne 1993:
44). Perhaps unborn as yet, but the embryo was in place?
There is little doubt that the ideas of the classical school were very
influential in the field of law, criminal justice and penal strategies.
Criminal justice systems, especially those of western societies, often
incorporated elements of the classical programme. Putting such principles
into practice raised some problems, however. One concerned the assump-
tion that all people were equal before the law and able to make equally
rational decisions. What about, for example, children and those suffering
from mental illness? Pure classicism, with its focus upon the crime rather
than the offender, seemed to be oblivious to the differences between
individuals which might influence notions of their culpability for a crime.
Revisions to the classical scheme were therefore made in various legal
systems, such as provision for the consideration of ‘mitigating circum-
stances’ (elements from the history and current situation of the person),
that could be seen as affecting the operation of free will and rationality on
the part of the individual. The resulting amalgam of classical and other
principles is often referred to as neo-classicism.
A second main problem concerns the nature of society. Classical theory
urges that all are equal before the law, but how is this to work when society
itself is characterised by major inequalities? First, we might ask whether
inequalities in the wider society are themselves involved in the origins of
crime – a possibility that is not fully considered by the framework.
19
Introducing Criminology
Quetelet came to the conclusion that the causes of crime were to be found
in aspects of social organisation, and that it was up to ‘legislators’ to
identify and remove them as far as possible. In their attempt to use
methods modelled on the natural sciences, and to identify causes of crime
20
Crime, the criminal and criminology
that could be stated in the same manner as the laws of those sciences, the
moral statisticians have some claim to have been founders of a ‘scientific’
study of crime. However, they had no conception of a scientific specialism
that could be separated off from other areas of study. The accolade of
founder of ‘scientific’ criminology has usually been awarded to Cesare
Lombroso. Very different from the moral statisticians, Lombroso’s focus
was upon the individual criminal, whom he thought could be the object of
study for a new discipline.
• The idea that people were endowed with free will was mocked as being
untenable. Human behaviour was determined by a range of factors.
21
Introducing Criminology
What was remarkable was how, from such initial ideas, there grew an
international movement, calling itself criminology, in order to distinguish
its broader vision from the criminal anthropology of Lombroso himself,
the specific notions of which were soon discredited. Garland (1985, 1997)
suggests that among the reasons behind its popularity and growth
towards the end of the nineteenth century are the following:
23
Introducing Criminology
Conclusion
It seems agreed that the infant discipline was with us at least by the fourth
quarter of the nineteenth century, but there has been some confusion over
both its birthday and parentage. Many commentators have suggested that
Lombroso was the father of the infant, a sickly child who was nursed into
relative health by others. But the child needed a name. Lombroso’s own
preference was eventually brushed aside – he was increasingly seen as a
suspect guardian for the infant discipline – and the name criminology
emerged, probably, it now seems, the idea of one of its godfathers,
Garafalo in 1885 (Beirne 1993: 235). The kind of academic enterprise
favoured by the Italian positive school was certainly the first to bear the
name of criminology. While the classical school, the moral statisticians and
the practical efforts of psychiatrists and prison doctors had already
produced offspring with rather different perspectives, which still have
some resonance in contemporary criminology, Lombroso was certainly
influential in moulding the character of a criminology at the end of the
nineteenth century as a separate scientific discipline with a particular
focus on the criminal.
There is one history of criminology which portrays it as a succession of
perspectives, usually beginning with the classical school, through the
work of the moral statisticians, to the work of Lombroso and his col-
leagues and beyond. This view suggests that there has been a long
connected sequence of reflections on a given topic. In a loose definition of
the subject, these are all forms of ‘criminology’. Alternatively, criminology
proper is seen as emerging as these reflections shift from the speculative to
the scientific. For others, such as Garland (1997), these histories will not do
at all. For him, the development of criminology as a specialised form of
enquiry, engaged in the scientific study of ‘the criminal’, an object of study
that was not given but constructed in a particular way, was the product of
particular historical circumstances. The study of crime and justice could
have been continued by a wide range of parties, from different disciplines
and agencies, without the formation of a criminology as such (the
argument that a separate discipline of criminology is neither necessary nor
desirable is still to be heard). Instead, the idea of a specialist discipline,
focused on the study of the criminal, emerged in Europe at the end of the
nineteenth century. Although based on a notion of the criminal that was to
be discredited, this did not stop its rapid development.
24
Crime, the criminal and criminology
25
Introducing Criminology
Chapter 2
Since the early days of their discipline, many criminologists have played
the game of ‘spot the difference’ between offenders and non-offenders. If
differences could be found, it was thought that these would hold the key
to developing a theory (a systematic account of the causes) of crime, as
well as opening up the possibility of an intervention before a crime is com-
mitted. We have seen how Lombroso became famous, and then infamous,
for his attempts to find biological differences between the two groups.
Although Lomboro’s specific ideas were largely discredited, this did not
stop the attempt to continue the broad tradition of searching for biological
differences in the twentieth century. Others have looked for differences in
terms of psychology; here the differences sought may be, for example, in
terms of types of personality, one or more of which may be suggested as
having a propensity to crime in general, or to a certain kind of crime.
In their extreme form, approaches which concentrate on characteristics
of the person (whether they are biological or psychological in their focus)
are what Albert Cohen (1966: 43) called ‘kinds of people theories’ of crime
or deviance. They lead to the question ‘How did they get that way?’ The
answer to this may be, for example, in terms of heredity, or some accident
resulting in brain damage, or a distinctive form of personality develop-
ment or learning. At the other extreme, there are those approaches that
seem to suggest that people who commit crime are not special kinds of
people at all, but that we should look to the situation or circumstances in
which people are located in order to explain crime. The key differences to
spot, such as various kinds of stresses or frustrations, the presence of
significant opportunities to commit crime, pressure to conform to group
expectations, or the absence of effective sanctions to deter crime, lie in the
context, rather than in the person. Sociological approaches to crime, which
are considered in the next chapter, frequently take this view.
26
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
Biological approaches
Physical characteristics and crime
There has been an enduring search for the causes of crime from the
discipline of biology in the twentieth century. First, there has been a
continuing interest in the possibility of a link between physical charac-
teristics and crime. Researchers have therefore continued to measure the
bodily characteristics of ‘criminals’ (usually convicted persons) and
compared them with control groups of the ‘normal’ population (on the
assumption, of course, that these would be largely people who had not
committed crime). Hooton’s (1939) study along these lines was very much
like Lombroso’s early work in its notion that criminals were an ‘inferior’
type of person who could be identified by certain measurable bodily
characteristics. Others introduced the idea of body types (somatotypes),
which were thought to be linked to temperament (Kretschmer 1936), and
then to the propensity to commit crime (Sheldon 1949). A number of
studies which explored these ideas came up with the finding that the
‘criminal’ group tended to cluster within a certain body type – in the
mesomorphic grouping, the lean and muscular type said to have an
assertive temperament. Such a correlation (a statistical association
between body type and crime) is open to rather different interpretations,
27
Introducing Criminology
Twin studies
There was also an attempt to explore the genetic contribution to crime.
This was done in two main ways: twin studies and adoption studies. Twin
studies are a classic way of attempting to assess the relative contribution of
heredity and environment to a trait or behaviour. There are two sorts of
twins: monozygotic and dizygotic. Monozygotic twins (commonly
referred to as identical twins) come from the same egg and share the same
genetic makeup. Dizygotic twins, on the other hand, are from different
eggs, fertilised in the womb at about the same time. They share less of their
genetic makeup with each other. It is known that certain traits (such as hair
or eye colour) tend to be shared by monozygotic twins, but not so
frequently by dizygotic twins. The reason for this is that such traits are
under the control of the genetic make-up, which monozygotic twins have
in common, thus creating a high degree of correspondence (called
concordance) for such traits.
If it could be shown that there are higher concordance rates for criminal-
ity in monozygotic twins than in dizygotic twins, wouldn’t this suggest
that there was a strong genetic component to crime? A number of studies
conducted over the years, some of them much more sophisticated than
others, have come up with the finding that the concordance rates for crime
(usually measured by official convictions) are higher for identical twins
than those for other twin pairs (see, for example, the overview in Raine
1993). Contemporary researchers do not claim that there is a ‘crime gene’,
but this evidence does lead some to suggest that a genetic factor is
influencing the propensity to crime. Again, this is a correlation, and an
interesting one at that. Alternative interpretations have been suggested,
such as that identical twins may be more alike in this way and in other
forms of behaviour because, on the whole, they may be treated more alike
in their upbringing. That is, the explanation for the higher concordance
could be environmental rather than genetic. Researchers in this tradition
have sometimes conducted a slightly different type of study in order to
provide more evidence.
Studies of adoptees
Studies of adoptees track the development of siblings who have been
separated at an early age. Identical twins would be ideal for such research,
but separations of these are unusual and such studies are therefore rare.
Siblings share a genetic make-up to a significant degree, but are thus
exposed to different environments. The researchers then examine the
28
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
Although this list may seem rather a rag-bag of topics, recent research has
been more clearly directed to explore the role of such processes in creating
differences in personality, learning, motivation and the processing of
information and thus in impulsivity, aggression, emotional responsive-
ness and stimulus-seeking behaviour, with a particular focus upon the
‘psychopathic personality’ (see chapter 4) and the notion that this may
have a biological basis.
29
Introducing Criminology
A recent book on this area of study ends with what Rutter et al (1998:
127) describe as ‘over-the-top’ claims:
30
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
Psychological approaches
31
Introducing Criminology
prison doctors we have just encountered, until the very influential work
by Cyril Burt (1925), published as The Young Delinquent. This work, based
on extensive statistical analysis of the characteristics and background of
400 schoolchildren, took the view that delinquency was the outcome of
various combinations of factors, from a long list of possibilities, operating
in each case (a multi-factor approach). He concluded that poor discipline,
poor relationships in the family, and particular types of temperament were
especially important.
Reflecting a rather different tradition was the establishment of the
Institute for the Study and Treatment of Delinquency (ISTD) in 1931, and
its associated Psychopathic Clinic in 1933 (later renamed the Portman
Clinic). What was significant about these developments was the influence
within them of psychoanalytic ideas. Psychoanalysis represents one par-
ticular perspective within psychology which by no means has been
subscribed to by all psychologists. It is therefore time to briefly survey the
major perspectives in the discipline which have attempted to aid the
understanding of crime. As ever, the divisions between them are never
entirely clear cut, and there is considerable diversity within each. What is
presented here is a little like a set of quick sketches of some old friends.
Atkinson et al (1993: 7) suggest that there are five major perspectives in
modern psychology. Any topic in the discipline, such as learning or crime,
can be approached by one or more of these.
• The biological perspective attempts to understand behaviour by
reference to electrical and chemical processes taking place within the
body, especially in the brain and nervous system. We have already
encountered this approach in our consideration of the biological level of
analysis above.
• The psychoanalytic approach takes the view that people have a
complex inner mental life, much of which takes place at an unconscious
level, and which holds the key to understanding behaviour. Pheno-
mena as diverse as dreams and emotional problems can have deeper
meanings which can be uncovered by the analyst.
• The behavioural perspective, most associated with the work of
Skinner. The focus is primarily upon overt behaviour, its observable
antecedents and consequences, rather than upon internal processes, as
is the case with the first two approaches. Few psychologists today
would describe themselves as behaviourists, but the influence of the
approach has been considerable.
• The cognitive approach has developed strongly in recent years, and
was partly a response to the narrowness of the behaviourist approach.
It is explicitly concerned with mental processes, such as perception,
memory, decision-making and problem-solving.
32
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
Psychoanalytic approaches
Although these approaches trace their ancestry to Freud, we should not
expect those under this heading always to agree with him, or with each
other. Certain common ideas can be identified which seem relevant in the
context of crime.
33
Introducing Criminology
34
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
We shall clarify the last point made a little later. The view expressed seems
a little different from that when Rutter and Giller (1983: 257) concluded
some years ago that psychoanalytic theories ‘have not proved to be
particularly useful either in furthering our understanding of crime or in
devising effective methods of intervention’.
Learning theories
Differential association
One of the earliest attempts to view crime as a product of learning was
developed by the famous criminologist, Edwin Sutherland, whom we
35
Introducing Criminology
Classical conditioning
The name of Ivan Pavlov will always be remembered in connection with
his dogs. Pavlov conducted one of the most famous experiments into the
way in which organisms learn. The salivation of dogs was measured, and,
as you might expect, the dogs would begin to salivate on the appearance
36
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
37
Introducing Criminology
38
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
reinforcement for approved conduct are important but are ignored in the
framework (Feldman 1993: 169). Hollin (1989: 58) makes the point that
Eysenck’s approach to the study of personality is only one of several
approaches, and has been criticised (Mischel 1968), and that other
personality dimensions may prove to be linked to criminality. Although
Feldman (1993: 169) concludes that the approach is likely to play a role in
the explanation of criminal conduct, it is not comprehensive in its
approach, ignoring such items as the role of instigating events and many
social factors in explaining crime. The theory of Trasler (1962) was an
attempt to incorporate a range of social factors, such as child-rearing
practices, in a similar approach. Overall, there is clearly some diversity of
opinion on the theory. While some appear to see some merit in the
approach, others are more critical, such as Blackburn (1993: 127) who
concludes that it ‘is not well supported’.
Operant learning
This perspective upon learning originates from the work of B.F. Skinner,
the famous psychologist associated with that approach in psychology
called behaviourism. In this view, behaviour can be seen to operate on the
surrounding environment to produce consequences which may be
rewarding or aversive for the individual concerned. In the first case the
behaviour is said to be reinforced and will tend to be repeated, whereas in
the second the behaviour is said to be punished and will become less
frequent. Cues in the environment signal to the individual when particular
behaviours are likely to be reinforced or punished in these terms. The
individual thus has a learning history which explains the ‘acquisition’ of
particular patterns of behaviour.
Crime has been analysed in these terms as an operant behaviour,
with the criminal to be understood in terms of a particular background
of reinforcement and punishment experiences. Jeffery (1965) attempted
to reformulate differential association into a theory of differential
reinforcement, as did Burgess and Akers (1966: 137), for whom criminal
behaviour is said to be ‘learned according to the principles of operant
conditioning’. For some critics, these attempts to improve on differential
association were hardly an improvement. For Taylor, Walton and Young
(1973: 133) such an approach may be well founded on studies of rats (the
original Skinner studies were also conducted with pigeons), but human
purpose, choice, and the development of what people find rewarding is
actually much more complex than this framework allows. For many
psychologists, the focus on events external to the individual produced an
unduly restricted analysis, neglecting the role of processes within the
individual, processes that were the focus of a developing cognitive
psychology.
39
Introducing Criminology
Overview
Learning theories have received a considerable amount of attention over
the years from psychologists and sociologists in the quest to understand
crime. In terms of ‘spot the difference’, they suggest that criminal be-
haviour is to be understood by reference to differences in the learning
experiences of individuals. However, while some suggest that pre-existing
differences between individuals (e.g. in their biology or personality) are
40
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
Cognitive approaches
The criminal personality?
In 1976, Yochelson and Samenow made a remarkable claim – to have
discovered the distinctive thinking patterns of the criminal, with styles
and ‘errors’ of their very own. Over 40 such errors of thinking are
described, including pervasive fearfulness, especially of a state in which
the individual feels worthless, the ‘power thrust’ – a need for power and
control, ‘fragmentation’ – inconsistencies in thinking, a failure to empa-
thise with others, perceiving themselves as victims, poor decision-making,
and extensive fantasies of antisocial acts.
Although this study is clearly cognitive in its focus upon thinking
processes, it is open to considerable criticism. The study is based on a
sample of 240 male offenders, many of whom had been committed to a
psychiatric hospital for assessment, or because they had been found ‘not
guilty by reason of insanity’. This is hardly a representative sample of
offenders to make generalisations about the ‘criminal mind’. Second, no
attempt was made to compare this sample with a comparison group of
non-offenders, to see how distinctive the findings were. Third, the
41
Introducing Criminology
Hollin (1989: 53) concludes that a ‘direct causal link between moral
functioning and criminal behaviour remains to be established’. Research
continues, and if elements of the theory survive the test, they will certainly
need to be complemented by other factors and approaches, such as aspects
of situations which are known to influence behaviour in the ‘real’ world.
Intelligence It has long been pointed out that samples of offenders score
lower on average than non-offenders on measures of intelligence. This
finding was often treated with scepticism or even dismissed. Critics
pointed out that the studies producing this finding were conducted with
officially convicted offenders, who might be a biased sample, and even
surmised that less intelligent offenders would be more likely to get caught.
More recent studies, which take class and race into account, and use self-
reported rather than simply official measures of offending, still find a
small but significant correlation, especially for verbal ability (Blackburn
1993: 187). This finding is open to a number of interpretations, such as that
low intelligence affects the ability to think in an abstract way, foresee the
consequences of actions and appreciate the feelings of victims – all
cognitive processes (Farrington 1997: 386). Another is that its effect is via
low school attainment (which is also a predictor of offending), which may
set in train a process of disengagement from school, truancy, and
43
Introducing Criminology
44
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
Values, beliefs and attitudes Perhaps offenders differ from others in their
values, beliefs and attitudes to an extent which would make their
offending easier to understand? This would be consistent with various
learning theories, and also with sociological subcultural theories, which
suggest that delinquents hold alternative sets of norms and values, which
result in law-breaking conduct (see, for example, Cohen 1955, for whom
the delinquent subculture was the opposite of middle-class values). This is
a huge topic, and what research has been conducted has mainly been with
reference to juveniles. With gang delinquents, there seems to be some
evidence of differences in values, beliefs and attitudes, but the similarities
to conventional norms and values are more striking (see, for example,
Short and Strodtbeck 1965).
Matza (1964) came to the rescue, pointing out that delinquency was
mostly episodic in nature, so that various theories which saw delinquents
as having been programmed into an alternative set of norms and values
would predict too much delinquency. For him, delinquents largely
subscribe to conventional values, and his account stresses the situational,
interactional context of delinquency, suggesting that individuals drift into
delinquency, partly through the episodic release from the moral bind of
law through techniques of neutralisation. These are invocations of extenuat-
ing circumstances for breaking rules that are claimed to neutralise the guilt
that would otherwise be felt in breaking the law, such as a denial of
responsibility (‘I couldn’t help it’), or appeal to higher loyalties (‘I know
it’s wrong, but I can’t let down my mates’). Since these are essentially
situational processes, it has proved difficult to test them out, as some have
tried to do, by the use of questionnaires and interviews away from those
situations, and it remains unclear to what extent these techniques precede
acts or are rationalisations/excuses after the event.
This material is primarily sociological, but similar concerns can be
found in psychology. For example, in Bandura’s (1983) theory of
aggression, personal and social standards of behaviour can be neutralised
by ‘cognitive distortions’ such as dehumanising or blaming the victim.
Abel et al (1984) found seven types of such distortion among child
molesters (such as ‘children don’t tell because they enjoy the sex’).
45
Introducing Criminology
Skills that relate to relationships with other people There has also been some
interest in those skills that relate to relationships with other people. Role-
taking – seeing things from the point of view of others – is a key element in
Kohlberg’s theory outlined above, and research studies indicate that
offenders have a tendency to see things from their own viewpoint
(egocentrism) rather than that of other people (Hollin 1989: 49; Blackburn
1993: 205), but also that skills in role-taking might be employed in certain
sorts of manipulative offending behaviour – a further reminder of the
varied nature of crime and offenders. Findings are rather inconsistent on
empathy – the capacity to appreciate the feelings of others and, perhaps, to
respond to them – partly, perhaps, because there are a number of different
concepts and measures of this (Blackburn 1993: 205).
Interpersonal problem-solving skills have also been investigated, with
some evidence emerging that offenders are less thorough and capable in
such problem situations, suggesting that they may act in real situations on
the basis of limited or misleading information (Blackburn 1993: 207).
Rutter, Giller and Hagell (1998: 151–2) draw attention to recent research
studies which suggest that aggressive individuals have a distorted way of
processing information in social encounters, including wrongly perceiv-
ing negative intentions in others, misinterpreting social interactions and
concentrating on the aggressive behaviour of others: this may be the result
of their own past experiences. Finally, there is the linked area of social
skills. It is often assumed that offenders are deficient in such skills, and
indeed, training in such skills is often used in the treatment of offenders.
Unfortunately, Blackburn (1993: 207) suggests that the evidence on this
score is limited, partly because of the lack of agreement on the definition,
measurement and identification of such skills, and concluding that delin-
quents are probably varied in this respect.
46
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
47
Introducing Criminology
48
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
approach, Cornish and Clarke do not assume full rationality, and accept
that offences differ according to the extent of cognitive processing
involved. The approach does, however, generally focus our attention on
certain aspects of decision-making rather than others.
A related point is that although this approach may be helpful in looking
at many property crimes, such as burglary, it is of little value with offences
like murder, rape, and assault (Trasler 1986). Cornish and Clarke (1986)
reply that violence is still chosen by the individual, and decisions,
however rudimentary, are made about the victim, the location and timing,
although they accept that ‘special motivational theories’ may be included
in accounts of some crimes.
These first two points indicate that the approach is more a perspective
into which a variety of ideas and findings can be incorporated, rather than
a specific theory in the traditional sense. It tells us where to look, rather
than what to expect when we get there.
The third point, also made by Trasler (1986) concerns the crime-specific
focus. Since much crime is committed by a smallish group of offenders
who do not specialise much, is this focus the best strategy? Cornish and
Clarke deny that their framework assumes offenders tend to specialise,
but concede that it may not be sensible to construct separate involvement
models for those crimes which are largely perpetrated by the same sorts of
offenders.
The fourth and final point concerns the narrow focus engendered by the
approach. Wright and Decker (1994) conclude from their study that the
approach fails to take account of the wider social and cultural context
within which decisions are made; for example, street life culture, which
stressed such features as self-reliance and spontaneity, had a considerable
impact on their burglars’ decisions, such as ill-gotten gains being spent on
drugs, alcohol and status-enhancing goods. Although a wider perspective
is allowable in the involvement models, this seems to fade from view in
practice, where the concern is very much with situational elements that,
hopefully, can be manipulated by immediate policy measures.
The dominance of policy relevance and situational factors leads to a
neglect of wider social, economic and political contexts, which may be
crucial in understanding crime and crime rates but which are deemed to
be of little practical use. This is hardly surprising, given that these studies
grew out of research sponsored by the Home Office in the wake of its
concern with crime prevention; they have been designated by Jock Young
(1994), rather unsympathetically, as examples of the ‘new administrative
criminology’, tied to state interests in controlling and reducing crime, if
possible by methods that are cheap, practicable and do not disturb the
status quo. Few civil servants or politicians in office welcome research that
calls for fundamental social change to prevent crime. Management of the
crime problem takes priority over the understanding of underlying
49
Introducing Criminology
Rutter, Giller and Hagell (1998) note that research focusing on individual
characteristics had become unfashionable by the 1970s. Self-report studies
had revealed that delinquent acts were so widespread, it seemed unlikely
that theories stressing individual differences would be useful in
accounting for them. Sociological approaches, which we consider in the
next chapter, were also much in vogue. It seems fair to say that the 1980s
and 1990s have seen a resurgence of interest in theories and research on
crime which locate the causes within the individual. This point has been
made by a number of commentators, including Lilly, Cullen and Ball
(1995: 196) and Rutter, Giller and Hagell (1998: 127). The reasons given for
this revival by the two sets of authors are, however, very different. The
revival of interest in individualistic theories, according to Rutter, Giller
and Hagell, is due to five factors:
50
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
51
Introducing Criminology
both countries. While Britain saw the emergence of what Young calls the
new administrative criminology with its focus on situational crime
prevention and rational choice, he sees something rather different as the
‘establishment’ criminology in the USA, a creature he calls ‘right realism’.
This is based largely on the work of James Q Wilson, author of the best-
selling Thinking About Crime (1975), and an advisor to the Reagan
administration, and co-author of another highly influential book (Wilson
and Herrnstein 1985). The latter work embodies some of the ideas we have
discussed in this chapter, and its popularity is consistent with the
increasing attention paid to individual differences in recent years.
Wilson and Herrnstein are said to be ‘realist’ by Young in the sense that
they accept that crime is a real problem in American society, that it had
grown rapidly over the years and needed to be taken seriously in terms of
explanation and policy. This contrasted with those who seemed to suggest
that concern about crime had been inflated by misleading statistics and the
mass media. Unlike the exponents of the new administrative criminology
in Britain, who had turned away from traditional theories of crime, Wilson
and Herrnstein put forward a general theory of criminal behaviour – or at
least of serious ‘street’ and ‘predatory’ crime. It is a theory based on the
principles of behavioural psychology, including the work of Eysenck. It
recognises that crime can be understood in terms of the immediate
circumstances in which it occurs or in terms of enduring dispositions of
the individual. The main elements can be summarised as follows:
• Choice: people choose actions in terms of the gains and losses that are
associated with various alternatives. We therefore need to understand
the way in which people evaluate those gains and losses.
• Conditioning: both classical conditioning, involving the autonomic
nervous system as in Eysenck’s theory, and operant conditioning have a
key role to play. The susceptibility of the individual, and the effective-
ness of the conditioning are therefore important factors. Some people
are so resistant to classical conditioning that they are deficient in
‘conscience’.
• Impulsiveness: some people have difficulty in seeing the likely future
consequences of their behaviour, or discount those they can foresee to
such an extent that they are resistant to the operant conditioning that
might result in them choosing ‘non-crime’. The rewards of crime are
generally in the present, whereas the costs often occur in the future,
whereas the opposite is the case for ‘non-crime’.
• Equity and inequity: people evaluate situations in terms of the gains
being received by people (e.g. income or material goods) relative to the
contributions made (e.g. amount of work, degree of skill). Individuals
differ in their evaluations of what is seen as fair or unfair because of a
52
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
The theory has been subjected to criticism. Lilly, Cullen and Ball (1995), for
example, mention the lack of clarity in the basic concepts, the selective use
of evidence, concentrating on that which supports the theory, and the
neglect of offending such as white-collar crime, which arguably can often
53
Introducing Criminology
be seen as predatory. It is, however, the wider implications of the work that
they find most disturbing – the idea that biological differences between
individuals (mainly among the poor, who seem to specialise in the type of
crime upon which the theory concentrates), are the root of much crime, has
particular implications about human nature and what policies should be
implemented to reduce crime. This is a further example of resistance by
sociologists to the biological approach to explaining crime (which is only
one element in this theory) that we mentioned earlier in the chapter.
Conclusion
Playing the game of ‘spot the difference’ between offenders and non-
offenders has proved more difficult than we might have first thought.
Along the way, however, we have learned some lessons. First, there is the
‘heterogeneity of the offender population’ – that offenders and crime are
varied in nature, so that we should not expect all offenders to be alike in
certain respects, such as in their biological or cognitive functioning. We
should therefore be sensitive to different types of crime and offender –
from the serial sex offender through to the juvenile shoplifter, from the
drugs dealer to the violent offender. Some might suggest that this
diversity makes the task a fruitless one – that the only thing such persons
and acts share is the fact that they are defined as criminal. Others have
dedicated themselves to the game with renewed enthusiasm in recent
years, determined to make a go of it by using more specific targets in their
studies. Rutter, Giller and Hagell (1998: 166) suggest that individual
characteristics are more likely to affect antisocial behaviour which begins
early in childhood and carries on during the life of the individual, rather
than the more common variety which seems to be limited to the adolescent
phase.
Second, there is the plethora of possibly overlapping ‘variables’ which
have been investigated in relation to offending, which have often been
conceptualised and measured in a variety of ways. For example, are
impulsivity and hyperactivity part of a single syndrome or separate
factors? Rutter, Giller and Hagell (1998: 149), in their recent review, rate
hyperactivity (or inattention) as having the most robust association with
antisocial behaviour which begins early in childhood and tends to persist
into adulthood, but say it is unclear whether this is separate risk factor or
part of a broader one with impulsivity and ‘cognitive impairment’. They
conclude the best-supported risk factors are ‘hyperactivity, cognitive
impairment (especially with respect to verbal and executive planning
skills), temperamental features (especially impulsivity, sensation seeking,
lack of control, and aggressivity), and a bias in social cognitive information
processing’ (Rutter, Giller and Hagell 1998: 166).
54
Offenders and non-offenders: spot the difference?
55
Introducing Criminology
Chapter 3
A major focus of the last chapter was the attempt to identify the causes of
crime, primarily through the approaches of biology and psychology. At
first, the discipline of sociology continued to play this traditional game but
looked for causes elsewhere, in the social contexts in which individuals
and groups were located – the sociological version of ‘spot the difference’.
Either the approaches considered in the last chapter were looking for those
causes in the wrong place, or at least those approaches needed to be
supplemented with a broader vision. We shall see how, eventually, some
sociologists turned away from the search for causes, and began exploring
other avenues. We look first at the approach which looked for the origins
of crime in the urban environment, before reviewing some other major
perspectives that have influenced criminology over the course of the
twentieth century. Although many of these perspectives are sociological,
others, such as feminism and postmodernism, cannot be neatly identified
with any traditional discipline of this kind; their ideas, however, have
implications for both sociology and criminology.
Environmental criminology
56
A broader vision of crime
of Chicago divided into concentric zones, radiating from the city centre to
the outer commuter zone. They found that rates of delinquency declined
as you moved out from the centre, a pattern that was also found in other
American cities of the time and which came to be known as the zonal
hypothesis. There was a particular focus upon one zone, the zone in
transition, an area of the city characterised by low rents and physical
deterioration adjacent to the city centre, where there was a concentration
of delinquents. This concentration was found to persist over long periods
of time, despite the fact that the composition of the population living in the
area had changed frequently over time, since it was the area where the
various new immigrant groups tended to live until they could afford to
move elsewhere. This diverse and rapidly changing population, they
argued, led to social disorganization – an absence of stable or common
standards and a breakdown in community institutions – and a resulting
failure to socialise or control children effectively. Social disorganization
was seen as a key explanation for high rates of delinquency in such areas.
To the zonal hypothesis and social disorganization can be added a third
key idea – cultural transmission. Shaw and McKay suggested that in such
areas of the city, delinquent traditions could become established and
passed on in play groups and gangs. Such ideas were taken up and
developed by subcultural theory and by Sutherland in his theory of
differential association. Overall, the Chicagoan perspective can be seen as
being very different from many of those considered in the last chapter. The
focus in understanding crime was not on the characteristics of individuals,
but on the social circumstances brought about by rapid change in certain
parts of the city. The policy implications involved the physical renewal of
such areas and the attempt to redress social disorganization by com-
munity development.
More recent studies of the distribution of offender residence in the
Chicago tradition have failed to confirm the universality of the original
zonal distribution. In England, for example, planned development and the
location of council housing on the periphery of cities, thus relocating
higher risk populations away from the inner city, have led to more
complex patterns (Morris 1957; Baldwin and Bottoms 1976). Even recent
study of Chicago itself (Bursik 1986) has found the original model no
longer applies.
The work of the Chicago School established a tradition of research into
the spatial distribution of offenders, but more recent studies have also been
concerned with the spatial distribution of offences (the two need to be
carefully distinguished – see below). Although the Chicago School
advocated the use of other sources of data, including participant observa-
tion, in the study of patterns of social life in the city, much of the early work
conducted on spatial distributions has been criticised for its failure to treat
official statistics of crime and offenders with caution, for these may be
57
Introducing Criminology
58
A broader vision of crime
move out (see, for example, Damer 1974), thus contributing to the
neighbourhood’s decline.
59
Introducing Criminology
60
A broader vision of crime
accounts and very patchy agency records, which are hardly a good basis
on which to come to conclusions about the extent, nature, impact and
trends in white-collar crime. Researching the relatively powerful is often
difficult enough in any case: Sutherland’s (1949) key text was originally
published in a ‘cut’ version, because of the inflammatory nature of some of
its findings. Little wonder, perhaps, that much work has involved
attempting to demonstrate the seriousness of, and harm created by, such
actions (Nelken 1997: 893), rather than the kind of work which has
characterised ‘ordinary’ crime.
Those who have looked for explanations of white-collar crime have
revisited the old questions: is it so different from other types of crime to
merit its own theoretical approach, or should our theories of crime be able
to accommodate it? Are explanations based on individual characteristics
or structural considerations more appropriate, or some combination of the
two? Finally, in terms of the response to white-collar crime, should it be
treated (in terms of, for example, policing, prosecution and punishment)
in the same way as ordinary crime, or is it so different that different
treatment can be justified? The persistence of such basic questions
underlines Nelken’s (1997: 895) point that ‘ambiguity about the nature of
white-collar crime and the best way to respond to it, form an essential key
to the topic’.
Slapper and Tombs (1999: 1) suggest that Sutherland’s ‘efforts to
redirect the energies of academic criminology were, in the immediate term
at least, a failure’. There has, however, been a clear growth in interest in the
topic in more recent times, spurred on, perhaps, by the increasing concerns
about the insidious activities of professionals and organizations, which
have the capacity to affect so many lives, in local, national and,
increasingly, global contexts. The environmental movement is but one
example of this, and we now hear calls for a ‘green’ criminology, focusing
on environmental violations and their control (South 1998). There can,
however, be a tendency for the topic to become a ‘specialism’ within
criminology, rather than being placed at the heart of research and teaching.
As such, it can be somewhat marginalized, the more so because of its
‘ambiguous’ character and its capacity, even today, to raise awkward
questions.
61
Introducing Criminology
62
A broader vision of crime
Merton (1957: 157) thus speaks of a ‘strain toward anomie’ where the
culture and the social structure do not mesh together, with anomie itself
seen as normlessness, a breakdown of the norms, ‘a breakdown of the
regulatory structure’ in which, at the extreme, ‘calculations of personal
advantage and fear of punishment are the only regulating agencies’. This
idea of strain has resulted in commentators calling this an example of a
sociological strain theory.
Despite its success, Merton’s theory has received much criticism over
the years. Much of it has suggested that the theory is incomplete and
undeveloped as a theory of deviant behaviour – a limitation that Merton
has readily accepted since the earliest statements of the approach (Merton
1938: 682). For example, it has often been said that it fails fully to account
for different outcomes: why do some still conform when experiencing
acutely the disjunction? how do we account for different kinds of deviant
behaviour adopted? Albert Cohen (1965) pointed out that the framework
was individualistic, neglecting group processes and their role in deviance,
and ignored the role of the social reaction to deviance in shaping it.
Cloward and Ohlin (1960) maintained that not only were legitimate
opportunities unequally distributed in society, so too were illegitimate
opportunities, making a difference in what kind of deviance, if any, the
individual might turn to when subjected to strain. A further comment was
that Merton perhaps gave undue weight to social class in his explanation.
After all, any theory of rates of deviant behaviour might be expected to be
able to account for the more dramatic variations in rates according to
gender and age, but Merton is silent on these issues.
These and other criticisms failed to silence the theory, which, although
subject to the changing fads and foibles in criminology and sociology,
continued to appear in various forms over the years. Steven Box (1983)
adapted Merton’s concept of anomie for the study of corporate crime,
arguing that such organizations are profit-seeking institutions in an
uncertain environment, leading to the exploration of alternative, and
perhaps illegal, means that will reduce that uncertainty. A similar
argument is used by Passas (1990), also in relation to corporate deviance.
Market capitalism, which promotes the pursuit of profit by the most
technically efficient means, while encouraging appetites and consumption
irrespective of the capacity for their fulfilment, does appear to be a recipe
for the strain toward anomie in the way that Merton suggested.
Messner and Rosenfeld (1994) in their ‘institutional anomie theory’
broadly follow Merton’s framework in accounting for high rates of crime
in the USA, adding that economic institutions are so dominant in that
society that people are less socialized or constrained by values, commit-
63
Introducing Criminology
ments and beliefs other than those of the market place. Agnew (1985), on
the other hand, has pressed the claims for a strain theory which is more
social psychological in approach, arguing that ‘negative relationships’ of
various kinds create negative emotions in the person. Whether crime and
delinquency or some other adaptation results from this strain depends on
a variety of constraints. Some evidence seems to suggest that delinquent
behaviours are associated with various negative relationships and life
events (Agnew and White 1992).
Lea and Young (1984), in what they call their ‘left realist’ approach,
stressed the role of relative deprivation in creating crime – a level of
perceived unfairness in one’s allocation of resources. More recently, Jock
Young (1999) has argued that the rise in crime in the second half of the
twentieth century is due to the combination of relative deprivation and
rising individualism, in a society that has become more socially exclusion-
ary, while culturally inclusionary (through the mass media) – thus
fostering expectations that the society cannot satisfy in significant groups
in the population, precisely what Merton had in mind.
In the global context, Rock (1997: 239) suggests that the concept of
anomie, in the sense of chronic deregulation, is appropriate to a number of
places around the world which are tottering on the brink of chaos and
lawlessness, where the state is struggling or has withdrawn from the battle
to maintain order. Merton’s account of anomie, which was developed
within the context of the USA at a particular time, will not necessarily
apply there. Finally, Merton’s theory became a major element in some
subcultural theories of delinquency, to which we now turn.
64
A broader vision of crime
65
Introducing Criminology
(1974) study in Liverpool, stealing car radios was a way of buying yourself
a ‘good time’, again a response to limited leisure opportunities. Although
the original theories were found wanting, a great deal was learned about
delinquency in the process of testing them out.
These theories, which attempted to tie together delinquency, sub-
culture, gangs and the social predicament of the working-class boy, were
eventually judged a failure, even though they had a major impact on social
policy in the USA in the 1960s. Gangs continue to be studied, especially in
the USA (see, for example, Huff 1996), although the lesson seems to be not
to make too many assumptions about their nature and development
without research evidence. Muncie (1999: 164) suggests that:
Subcultural theory did not wither and die, but reappeared in new guises.
As we shall see, that group of criminologists called the ‘left realists’ draw
extensively on subcultural theory. Another major development was the
work produced on the range of youth leisure styles (or subcultures) which
appeared in Britain from the 1950s onwards: teds, mods, rockers,
skinheads, punks, rude boys, and so on (for a brief overview, see Muncie
1999). One of the best known books on the subject suggested that such
subcultures could be seen as forms of ‘resistance through rituals’ (Hall and
Jefferson 1976): that such youth styles were coded responses to the
contradictions and conflicts faced by (especially working-class male)
youth within the social, economic and historical context of post-war
Britain. Such forms of ‘resistance’ were commonly seen as ‘imaginary’,
symbolic or ‘magical’ rather than as real solutions to the problems faced by
such groups.
Such approaches came under attack for their neglect of girls, especially
in the earlier work, the overplaying of the political significance of such
subcultures and the underplaying of their commercial aspects, and the
concentration on the radical and spectacular aspects as opposed to their
conservative and unremarkable features (Muncie 1999: 185–192).
Nevertheless, the structure of subcultural theory can still be clearly
seen: subcultures as collective responses to problems arising from the
wider social context for particular groups of people. There were, however,
clear differences from the earlier American theories; for example, these
66
A broader vision of crime
Control theory
67
Introducing Criminology
69
Introducing Criminology
70
A broader vision of crime
71
Introducing Criminology
72
A broader vision of crime
appears ‘more sinned against than sinning’ (Gouldner 1975: 38). This may
romanticise the deviant, diverting attention from the original action and
the suffering created for any ‘real’ victims, who seem to have been
forgotten.
Radical criminologies
73
Introducing Criminology
Despite some omissions and controversial claims, the book has remained
an influence ever since, both as a rallying call for radical criminology and
an agenda for criminological study (see Walton and Young 1998). Few can
claim the capacity to cover all the required elements, but Hall et al’s (1978)
study of the social reaction to law and order issues in the 1970s, which
argues that moral panics do not occur randomly, but are related to
fundamental economic, political and social crises in society, is often given
as an example of an attempt to cover certain aspects of the agenda in the
way suggested, in particular to provide a political economy of social
reaction. In Taylor et al’s (1975) next book the emphasis was more firmly
upon the need for a materialist (Marxist) analysis of crime and law in
relation to capitalist society. Some of the comprehensiveness of the earlier
74
A broader vision of crime
framework seemed to have been lost, and the move towards Marxism was
typical of radical writings during the 1970s. At the end of the decade, the
National Deviancy Conference (1979) published a collection of papers
with the symptomatic subtitle, From Deviancy Theory to Marxism.
Gradually, criticisms began to accumulate, especially of radical crimi-
nology in its more exclusively Marxist guise. One of the first came from
within Marxism itself, when Hirst (1975) suggested that the quest for a
Marxist criminology was a misguided one. Marx himself had very little to
say about crime in his writings. This was not an oversight, for crime is not
a particularly appropriate topic for Marxist analysis and peripheral to that
framework. In Hirst’s view, a Marxist criminology was just about as viable
as a Marxist analysis of sport. To be fair, Marxism was able to provide
answers (perhaps flawed or incomplete) to questions about the political
economy of social control, the state and its structures of law, but it was less
equipped to deal with the details of crime, except perhaps to suggest in a
rather simplistic way that it represented a form of rebellion against the
capitalist order, a means of survival, or an aspect of the class struggle. At
the very least, the approach needed to be supplemented by other
perspectives, but radical criminologists of this grouping were rarely
interested in empirical research on crime itself, with their sights firmly
fixed elsewhere.
A further criticism singled out what it saw as the undue utopianism of
the approach. If only a transformation of the social order is worthwhile,
with piecemeal reform seen as pointless, this may lead to a self-imposed
exclusion from important policy debates about issues that have real
consequences for people’s lives. Such an outcome also has the effect of
leaving the field open to one’s political opponents, in this case those from
the right. Indeed, during the late 1970s and early 1980s, ‘New Right’
conservatism in Britain and the USA seemed to have made the law and
order issue its own, with socialism appearing to offer few ‘realistic’
proposals about how to tackle rising crime. Jock Young (1986) has since
argued that the approach (with which he was previously associated) failed
to take crime and people’s fears of it seriously, including the real harm
inflicted by poor people on other poor people. This could hardly be
regarded as a redistribution of property, rebellion or aspect of the class
struggle. An overly romantic image of the criminal was conveyed, seen as
an embryonic class warrior, a present-day Robin Hood, redistributing
private property, with victims and the harm they experience sorely
neglected. It was in this context that Young and his colleagues argued for a
more ‘realistic’ approach from the left, that would take crime and its
victims seriously: left realism (see below p.79–82).
Further blows to the notion that a Marxist analysis would provide the
answers to crime and other problems of contemporary life occurred in the
form of the collapse of Marxist-inspired states and faith in its principles as
75
Introducing Criminology
Feminist perspectives
76
A broader vision of crime
the way in which men far outnumber women in their rates of offending,
gender was nevertheless given relatively little attention, even by the
emerging radical criminology. Following this critique, a number of studies
began to appear, as if to fill the gaps and deficiencies that had been
identified. This work focused on a number of topics and areas, such as:
Such studies, of which only a few examples have been given, were often
conducted from a broadly feminist viewpoint, leading some to suggest
that a feminist criminology had emerged (for far more comprehensive
overviews, see Heidensohn 1985, 1996; Walklate 1995). As the title of
Gelsthorpe and Morris’ (1990) book indicated, however, it was more
accurate to speak of feminist perspectives in criminology, for there were a
number of differences in approach. It would be fair to say that, in the early
days at least, feminists had a broad concern with exposing and working to
free women from a broad range of limitations and oppressions that they
experienced. The overall quest was in this sense a gendered one, that
sought to emancipate women from those problems. In criminology and
criminal justice, this might range from correcting the way in which women
and gender had been ill-served by criminology, to attempting to rectify the
way in which rape victims are treated by the criminal justice system.
There were, however, clearly important differences within feminism
itself which manifested themselves in different emphases and approaches
towards crime and criminal justice. There are a number of ways in which
77
Introducing Criminology
78
A broader vision of crime
Left realism
79
Introducing Criminology
80
A broader vision of crime
82
A broader vision of crime
83
Introducing Criminology
84
A broader vision of crime
Conclusion
85
Introducing Criminology
If it is the case that ‘crime’ is a status accorded to some acts and not
others at some times and not others and in some places and not
others, and that those acts are engaged in to some degree by most
people at least some of the time, then it is highly questionable
whether there is anything special that needs to be explained about
criminal acts.
(Hester and Eglin 1992: 269)
The above statement is worth careful study; it is one version of a view that
has been advanced since the 1960s. Although it is true that the quest for the
causes of crime went into some decline in the 1970s, there has been a
revival of interest in the topic in recent years, including from the left
realists, who see crime as a problem that needs to be taken seriously in
terms of explanation and the need to do something about it on a social
level. As in the past, the understanding of causes is often taken to assist
in the development of crime control policies.
Although Henry and Milovanovic (1996: 152) suggest that sceptical
postmodernist thought denies the possibility of causal statements, they
find this ‘too extreme’ and their own ‘affirmative postmodernism’
develops its own approach to causality. We find, therefore, a diverse and
rather confusing picture among these broader perspectives in crimi-
nology; for example, if the prime focus of criminology is the study of
crime, there is no clear agreement about the appropriate questions to ask
about it. This diversity and dissensus is one thing that makes criminology
such a challenging subject. This chapter has attempted to provide a guide
to some of the major differences and disagreements.
86
Thinking seriously about serial killers
Chapter 4
87
Introducing Criminology
88
Thinking seriously about serial killers
another felony (i.e. another criminal offence). This would be better left out
of the definition; they admit that we do not have the information on how
many spree murders are connected to other offences, which suggests that
this should not be considered a defining characteristic in the present state
of knowledge.
The suggested dividing line between mass and spree murder is by no
means clear cut. Consider the case of Michael Ryan, who shot and killed
one victim, drove to a petrol station and shot at the attendant, and then
drove to the town of Hungerford, where he killed fifteen people before
shooting himself. Is this a mass or spree murder? Some feel that the
concept of spree murder should be omitted altogether as an unnecessary
complication (Hickey, 1997). Holmes and Holmes (1998) also suggest that
both spree and mass murder, unlike serial murders, have no ‘emotional
cooling off period’ between killings. Again, it may be unwise to imply that
such a cooling off cannot happen within an arbitrarily chosen thirty-day
period, and will always happen in any longer period. We must remember
that such definitions and distinctions are produced by the commentator
and are not ‘natural’ categories; as such, they are more or less useful
depending on how helpful they in achieving what their purpose, such as
staking out a field of study in a clear and helpful way.
According to Holmes and Holmes (1998) the third type of multiple
murder, serial murder, involves three or more victims, who are killed over a
time period of more than thirty days, with a ‘significant cooling-off period’
between the killings. Examples from England that fit this definition spring
readily to mind: Peter Sutcliffe, the ‘Yorkshire Ripper’, known to have
killed thirteen women before capture in 1980; Dennis Nilsen, who is
believed to have killed up to sixteen people in the early 1980s; Fred West,
who was accused of twelve murders before committing suicide in prison
before he could be brought to trial, and his wife Rosemary, who was
convicted in 1995 of murdering ten women and girls; Harold Shipman, the
family doctor, who in January 2000 was convicted of the murder of fifteen
mostly elderly patients and, according to the Manchester coroner, may
have been responsible for many more. In the light of this brief list of some
of the best known cases, consider the extensive definition offered by Egger
(1990: 4):
89
Introducing Criminology
Such a definition has the advantage of not building in too many assump-
tions at an early stage.
We have already made the point that Egger’s (1990) attempt at a
definition is nevertheless useful in identifying some common character-
istics which have emerged from research on serial murder. It is important
to identify these, for they can have major implications. For example, if
serial murders tend to be ones in which there is no prior relationship
between offender and victim, this contrasts with much ‘ordinary murder’.
90
Thinking seriously about serial killers
91
Introducing Criminology
• there are about 5,000 murder victims where the offender is unknown in
the USA each year;
• between a quarter and two-thirds of these, they suggest, are victims of
serial killers;
• there are a number of undetected victims, especially among those
children who go missing every year.
They suggest that the typical number of victims for each killer is 10–12.
Dividing their estimated figure for total victims of serial murder by this
number for each killer (uncharacteristically using the lower estimates this
time!), 3,500 divided by 10, gives them a figure of 350 serial killers at large
in the USA at any one time – an alarming prospect indeed.
The assumptions on which these estimates are based have been subject
to considerable scepticism, especially the notion that such a large
proportion of murders where the offender is unknown and many recorded
cases of missing children are necessarily attributable to serial killings
(Kiger 1998). Fox (1990) has described these estimates as ‘preposterous’
and probably ten times too large. At least the basis for the estimates are
made clear and open to some inspection. Norris (1988: 19) says that the
FBI’s estimate of active serial killers ‘at large and unidentified’ is 500, but
the basis for this figure is not revealed. More sober estimates have been
given for the number of active offenders at any one time, such as 35 by
Levin and Fox (1985), while Ressler et al (1988) estimate the number of
multiple murderers (a broader category) to lie between the low 30s to over
100.
Gresswell and Hollin(1994: 6) attempt a similar exercise for England
and Wales:
As they imply, however, the actual figure will be affected by such matters
as police efficiency, the amount of time taken to detect the offenders and
the balance between the different types of multiple murderer (mass and
spree murderers are typically at large for short periods, compared with
serial murderers).
It is hard to disagree with Kiger’s (1999: 37) conclusion that ‘there is no
92
Thinking seriously about serial killers
doubt that the nature of this phenomenon makes estimation difficult and
any absolute numbers representing a reliable reflection of the scope of the
problem, impossible’. However, the range of the estimates is great enough
to be interesting in itself, and some of the reasons for this are clear, such as
the data sources used, the assumptions made, and the definition of serial
murder being used (for example, the number of victims being used as a
defining criterion can have a considerable impact upon such estimates).
Later in the chapter, we explore the possibility that it is also tempting for
some vested interests to maximize the scale and impact of a problem with
which they are concerned.
A growing menace?
Criminologists are not only interested in the extent of crime, but also in
changes in that extent over time (trends). There seems be a prevailing idea
that serial murders have been on the increase in the USA in recent decades.
Using a combination of the existing literature and press reports, Leyton
(1989) constructs a table to convey the recorded instances of multiple
murderers in the USA, between 1920 and 1984, and summarises the main
trends in the following way:
93
Introducing Criminology
1940–9: 3 (18); 1950–9: 1 (7); 1960–9: 3 (13); 1970–9: 5 (60); 1980–9: 2 (11)
(Gresswell and Hollin 1994: 7).
The authors warn us that such figures are likely to underestimate the
extent of serial murder. The Home Office figures only include those cases
where victims have been clearly identified and attributed to a particular
offender. A less restrictive definition of a serial murderer (such as one
involving three victims) would also give a less rosy picture on numbers.
However, such data, despite their limitations, do give us some indication
of broad trends over time, and hardly suggest that there has been a recent
epidemic of serial murder in England and Wales.
Once criminologists have charted as well as they can the extent of the
phenomenon they are studying, they commonly then attempt to develop
their understanding of it. They attempt to describe the common charac-
teristics, making generalisations about it. When this is being done, it may
be found that instances cluster into certain key groupings or types. When a
94
Thinking seriously about serial killers
Visionary
These killers are said to be responding to inner voices or hallucinations.
The break with reality is such that these individuals are often said to be
‘psychotic’. Key features are said to typify their murders, such as the
victims being randomly selected strangers, the murder spontaneous and
disorganised, and the location concentrated in certain areas.
95
Introducing Criminology
Missionary
This type has decided to rid the locality or the world of particular groups
he deems undesirable, such as drug dealers, drug users, prostitutes or
homosexuals. Although such individuals may be seen as being
characterised by a fixation, they are not normally considered psychotic.
Their victims are randomly selected strangers from specific types of
person, the act planned and organised in concentrated locations.
Hedonistic
This is a mixed group with three sub-types. Lust murderers are those for
whom violence and sex are intimately linked, and sexual practices and
mutilations may follow after the death of the victim. Thrill killers are
similar, but the victim is kept alive as long as possible in order to relish
their suffering. Comfort killers are those who kill for financial or other
material gain.
Power/control type
This killer is motivated by the desire to have, quite literally, the power of
life and death over the victim. Although sex may occur, this is not the
primary motivation.
A separate typology has been developed for female serial killers (Holmes
and Holmes 1998), although there is a considerable overlap with the
typology for males. The visionary corresponds to the male type, and the
comfort murderess, who kills for ‘creature comfort’ reasons, corresponds to
the male hedonistic/comfort type. A frequently cited case of this type is
that of Dorothea Puente, who was charged with nine murders in
California in 1988. She kept a rooming-house and was charged with
poisoning some of her guests in order to benefit from their social security
cheques. The hedonistic type corresponds broadly with the other male
hedonistic types, but sex is less likely to figure. The fourth type is the power
seeker, for whom murder is ‘a way to attain a sense of power’ (Holmes and
Holmes 1998: 45). Although this sounds very much like the male power/
control type, in this case power and attention is sought not over the victim
as such, but in relation to other people. An example is the mother who puts
her child into life-threatening situations and eventually kills the child, thus
gaining power over, and recognition from others. The fifth and final type
also represents a departure from the male typology – the disciple mur-
derer, who acts under the influence or orders of the leader of a group.
Holmes and Holmes note that female serial killers are relatively un-
common, and that most of them will fall into the comfort type.
How do we judge such typologies? The first point to make is that they
can only be judged in relation to some purpose or objective, which should
be clearly specified before the typology is constructed. How good is the
96
Thinking seriously about serial killers
typology in achieving that objective? We have already made the point that
the typology developed by Ressler et al (1988) was primarily to assist in the
detection of offenders. As such, it should be judged in that light. Seltzer
(1998: 131) comments,
although the types of ‘organised’ and ‘disorganised’ killers are
directly at odds, it has become routine to find apprehended killers
designated as ‘mixed’ types. ...Despite the profilers’ high profile in
the media, however, there remains a basic disagreement about what
contribution this technique has made.
By contrast, the primary objective of the Holmes and DeBurger (1988) and
Holmes and Holmes (1998) typologies is to reveal the patterns of
motivation of serial killers. These typologies are based on particular
psychological ideas about the origins of this particular form of behaviour.
If these ideas are accurate, they may contribute to a theory of serial murder
that will help us to understand and explain it.
The second point is that typologies are intended to help us to make
sense of a wealth of detailed information. They attempt to impose order, to
allow us to see patterns rather than simply a wealth of individual detail.
They are thus an attempt to go beyond the seemingly endless description
of individual cases, to group instances together, to see common features
between them. All typologies must strike some balance between simplicity
and complexity. The simpler they are, the more readily understandable
they usually are, but they may not do sufficient justice to the phenomena;
they may impose over-simplistic patterns on complex reality. The more
complex they are, the more they can deal with differences and variation in
the social world, but the less clear and readily understandable they may
be. The typology of Hickey (1986, 1997) is clearly one of the most simple
typologies, whereas that of Holmes and Holmes (1998) is one of the most
complex. Typologies will always have limitations, precisely because they
must balance competing demands of simplicity/clarity on one hand and
complexity/depth on the other. Wherever the balance is struck, something
will be lost.
The third point to make is that typologies in criminology must decide
on the primary focus in terms of, for example, the crime, the offender or
perhaps the victim. Most of the typologies in the area of serial murder
focus on the offender. That of Holmes and DeBurger (1988), for example,
had this focus, and more precisely, looks for motivational patterns. Such
motivational patterns of offenders do not provide a complete explanation
of events. Other factors may be important in accounting for these. The
behaviour of the victim and situational factors, for example, may be very
important in explaining actual outcomes (for example, some victims
selected may die, others not), a general point which has received much
attention in criminology in recent years.
97
Introducing Criminology
98
Thinking seriously about serial killers
99
Introducing Criminology
Genetic factors
The suggestion has been made that the possession of an extra male (Y)
chromosome in the genetic make-up of some individuals (what is called
the XYY syndrome) may predispose them to violence, or even to serial
murder. Clear evidence for a causal link between the XYY pattern and
criminal behaviour has always been lacking (Mednick and Volavka 1980)
and the suggested link with serial murder seems to have been based on
one or two examples of serial killers who have had, or are thought to have
had, this genetic abnormality. Arthur Shawcross, who was arrested in
New York State in 1990 and confessed to the murder of eleven women,
having already served a prison sentence for one of two earlier killings, is
reported to have had this extra chromosome (Hickey 1997: 48). Just as one
swallow does not make a summer, one case does not make a generalisa-
tion.
Head trauma
The case of Shawcross brings to our attention a second possible biological
factor: head trauma. Shawcross had suffered head injuries as a child and as
an adult. ‘Considering that abuse is a common theme in the childhoods of
serial killers, we must also be concerned with those who received head
trauma. While head trauma may not directly cause violent behaviour, the
persistent correlation must not be ignored’ (Hickey 1997: 46). Again, there
are general lessons for the criminologist. It must first be demonstrated that
serial killers are more likely to have suffered head trauma than other
people who are in other ways like the killer group (a ‘matched’ compari-
son). Even then, a correlation does not necessarily amount to causation.
For instance, alternative explanations for the correlation need to be
discounted, e.g. that the association is produced by a third factor, such as
cold, uncaring parenting leading to both physical abuse of a child and a
psychological make-up conducive to victimising others. As we saw in
chapter 2, establishing causal generalisations is more difficult than we
might think.
100
Thinking seriously about serial killers
101
Introducing Criminology
Psychosis
The first possibility to consider is that serial killers are suffering from a
form of psychosis, the symptoms of which are said to be one or more of the
following: ‘delusions, hallucinations, disorganized speech, or grossly
disorganized or catatonic behaviour’ (Hickey 1997: 53). It is all too easy to
point to examples that seem to fit this category. Joseph Kallinger claimed
to be haunted by a large floating, tentacled head called Charlie, telling him
to kill people and mutilate their genitals. The obedient Kallinger duly
obliged, murdering his own son and others in the local community.
Psychotic episodes or states are said to be brought on by ‘physiological
malfunctioning, environmental stressors, or substance use’ (Hickey 1997:
53). Although fitting well into the type of ‘visionary’ killers outlined
earlier, it seems that ‘serial killers are rarely found to be suffering from
psychotic states’ (Hickey 1997: 54), according to one author who has been
working in the field for a number of years.
Dissociative disorders
The second possibility to consider is the dissociative disorders. Dissocia-
tion refers to:
103
Introducing Criminology
fantasies are focused upon the things which cannot, at first, be done in real
life. At first, they may focus on being a hero, being the kind of person to be
admired; as time goes by and these fantasies are not fulfilled, they may
come to focus upon revenge, or sex coupled with violence. The person is
leading a kind of double life; one real, one fantasy: a struggle between the
parts is experienced. The individual may eventually find himself in a
situation that corresponds to the fantasy and ‘automatically carrying out
an act he has practised so many, many times in his mind’ (Carlisle 1998:
93). He may initially dissociate the crime, and vow that it will never
happen again. But the wish to feel such power and control becomes so
strong that eventually another murder is committed. The individual has
become what he has always feared; a fundamental shift of identity has
occurred. Guilt is compartmentalised so that it is not consciously
experienced. His secret, ‘dark side’ is experienced as having gained the
upper hand over his very being. In order to neutralise the strong
self-hate that may be engendered, he may even begin to idealise what he
has become. Carlisle (1998: 99) claims that while this model may not fit
every serial killer, it will fit most cases. Holmes and Holmes (1998: 72)
suggest it is a theory of ‘at least the sexually motivated and power-
motivated serialists’. Although it leaves a number of questions un-
answered, it does point to some important themes we shall encounter
elsewhere.
Psychopathy
Perhaps the concept most tempting to apply to the serial killer is that of
psychopathy. Henderson (1939: 19) described it as ‘a true illness for which
we have no specific explanation’. Cleckley (1976) listed sixteen
characteristics of psychopaths, and building on this, Hare (1991) has
developed a psychopathy checklist which includes such items as:
• glibness/superficial charm;
• conning, manipulative behaviour;
• lack of remorse or guilt;
• callousness/lack of empathy.
Hickey (1997: 65) states that although most psychopaths are not violent, a
common feature is their ‘constant need to be in control of their social and
physical environment. When this control is challenged, the psychopath
can be moved to violent behaviour’. He also notes that the attraction of the
term lies in the ‘catch-all’ nature of the label, apparently able to
accommodate even killers who had seemed to be Mr Nice Guy on first
impressions. But one drawback is that ‘researchers and clinicians alike
have yet to arrive at a consensus as to the proper definition of the term’
(Hickey 1997: 66). We might add that, even if it may be a useful descriptive
104
Thinking seriously about serial killers
Multiple-factor explanations
Perhaps the most illuminating avenue at present is provided by those who
try to take an overview of the main research findings, constructing a
model which is divided into a series of stages. It is clear that the explana-
tion of serial murder is very complex, and not to be achieved by a focus on
one concept or factor. Instead, any theory of such a rare phenomenon is
likely to include a wide range of elements, which on their own or even in
combination with some of the others, are likely to have been experienced
by a large number of persons without them becoming serial murderers. In
addition, the research evidence is not substantial in any case, because of
the rarity of serial murder and the various difficulties involved in conduct-
ing research on it. As such, theoretical models or frameworks must be
regarded as highly provisional. Gresswell and Hollin (1994) provide
one such model summarising the research on multiple murder, and
divided into three main stages: predisposing factors; maintaining factors;
situational factors and triggers. It provides a good illustration of how
psychological criminologists might approach the explanation of such a
crime.
105
Introducing Criminology
106
Thinking seriously about serial killers
alcohol and other drugs, pornography, or books on the occult; these are
not suggested as causal factors as such, but can lower inhibitions and feed
fantasies, which are said to be a ‘critical component in the psychological
development of the serial killer’ and ‘likely to be found in the minds of
most, if not all...’ (Hickey 1997: 91).
The fantasies increasingly involve violence, often sexual, and control
over the victim, and eventually the individual attempts to replicate them.
This is never fully achievable, but each attack may provide material for
new fantasies. Elements of the original childhood trauma may surface in
the ordeal to which the victim is subject. When a sense of control has been
reached, the victim may be slaughtered, and the individual feels a
temporary sense of equilibrium that has been missing in his life. If the
quest for fantasy fulfilment has become all-consuming, or the feelings of
low self-esteem and rejection are precipitated by events (trauma
reinforcement), the process may begin again.
107
Introducing Criminology
sis on individual pathology (i.e. what went wrong with the individual)
fully justified. Sociologists, however, may argue that such a focus upon
individual pathology neglects the social and cultural context within which
these crimes are located. The famous sociologist Emile Durkheim (1952,
originally published in 1897) conducted a study of suicide to demonstrate
the helpfulness of a sociological perspective with a similarly highly
individual phenomenon. Corresponding to their rather different focus,
sociologists tend to ask rather different sorts of questions. In the case of
serial murder, the following might be three examples.
We look initially at the first two questions. Then we move on to look at the
third.
108
Thinking seriously about serial killers
109
Introducing Criminology
110
Thinking seriously about serial killers
exempt from child abuse. Clearly, more than this is required to account for
gender differences in serial killing. Cameron and Frazer (1987), in their
discussion of sexual murder (by men) from a feminist perspective, see
‘transcendence’ – the wish to attain some kind of immortality by an extra-
ordinary action – as a reason for male predominance in this crime. Such men
are extreme products of an exaggerated culture of masculinity, for whom
aggression and male sexuality have become inextricably linked. Such men
cannot be understood apart from that wider culture which, in one way or
another, affects all our lives. Jane Caputi (1987) goes further, suggesting that
the twentieth century is characterised by a new form of genocide – sexual
murder as the ultimate expression of sexuality as a form of (male) power. It
is certainly undeniable that masculinity – or at least certain forms of it – may
be partly responsible for some very undesirable outcomes.
111
Introducing Criminology
In his view, the BSU personnel knew how to work an audience and to
cultivate the media, with the master-stroke of allowing the filming of The
Silence of the Lambs to take place at Quantico.
The mass media are the second major agency with vested interests in
the serial murder issue. It has become all too clear that the issue sells
newspapers, books and magazines, raises TV ratings and fills cinema
seats. According to Jenkins (1994: 221), ‘serial murder represents for the
media the perfect social problem’, fulfilling as it does the criteria for a
newsworthy story: it invokes an emotional response; there is drama in the
battle between good and evil; it involves a real harm and a potential threat
for readers and viewers, from the ‘quiet, apparently harmless person
down the street’ (as killers are often subsequently described by
neighbours); excitement often accompanies the hunt for the offender;
shock value will be contained in the unusual violent or sexual activity;
offenders often acquire a celebrity status so that coverage can be personal-
ised; it often comes with ready-made attention-grabbing features and
powerful visual images. Fiction and non-fiction alike (the distinction
between the two is by no means clear cut according to this analysis)
contribute to forming the prevailing images and ‘monster’ stereotypes,
with a heavy reliance on the material provided by the accredited agencies,
such as the BSU.
Killers themselves often play an important role in this process, and have
an intricate relationship with the media. Serial murderers often appear to
be avid consumers of media accounts of earlier cases, from daily
newspapers through to academic psychological discussions. It seems
highly likely that they will be influenced in some way by these sources,
‘which may go far in explaining why killers in different eras tend to
reproduce explanations of their offense that closely mimic the prevailing
ideological perspective of their day or even of the particular investigator to
whom they are confessing’ (Jenkins 1994: 224).
What killers say about their deeds may also be affected by the kind of
outcome they may be seeking, such as an insanity defence, or to appear in
the Guinness Book of Records: for example, some offenders have confessed
to murders they could not have committed. If the psychopathic
personality type is frequently to be found amongst such offenders, we
should remember their propensity to manipulate and to say whatever
might please the listener, whether investigator or researcher. For all these
112
Thinking seriously about serial killers
113
Introducing Criminology
His attempt to trace some of the elements of this culture to Germanic and
Norse myths is one highly individual (and sometimes strained) example
of the focus upon representations of violence and murder. Cultural
representations may not only have an influence on the nature and extent of
serial murder, but they may help to structure the data collected by
researchers (such as the way in which killers attempt to represent their
actions in interviews) in a very significant way. However, such an
approach still leaves us with the question of why particular individuals
find themselves taking on the role of serial killer. Giving an answer to this
question is rather more difficult than we might have thought.
Post-mortem
114
Thinking seriously about serial killers
The two broad approaches outlined here are not necessarily contradictory
or mutually exclusive. Much of the literature on serial murder attempts
some combination of them. However, some would argue that, with serial
murder, and perhaps with certain other kinds of crime, the second broad
approach is currently more likely to add to our understanding than the
first.
Finally, we have encountered another perspective on serial murder,
which does not take its primary task as attempting to understand why a
given type of crime occurs. Instead, it looks at the way in which a
particular problem or crime has been socially constructed, and the various
sources of that particular construction. The focus here is on representa-
tions of serial murder, and the notion that such representations can be
deconstructed to reveal the processes and interests that contributed to
them. This illustrates perspectives that have been introduced earlier in this
book; the implication is that we should not simply accept the way in which
a problem has been defined and the seemingly authoritative ‘knowledge’
presented to us, but to examine them in a questioning frame of mind. This
can tell us something about our society, its culture and the way in which it
defines and reponds to crime and the offender.
115
Introducing Criminology
Chapter 5
Introduction
The police are by far the most visible institution of the criminal justice
system. While most people will have had little or no contact with prisons,
probation or the courts, there must be few who by the age of majority have
not come into personal contact of one kind or another with the police.
Indeed, for the most part, the police want to be seen. Their distinctive
uniforms with helmets fashioned to elevate their stature, and their clearly
marked patrol cars equipped with sirens and flashing blue lights are
clearly designed to draw attention to themselves. This is, of course, no
accident; the police are the visible presence of the state in civil society.
From the establishment in London of the New Police in 1829 by Sir
Robert Peel, visibility was the order of the day. There were originally to be
no detectives who could move freely among the citizenry in plain clothes,
for this would resemble a continental model of policing which, as one
parliamentary committee of the time declared, ‘would be odious and
repulsive’ to the British idea of liberty. Nor were the police to be attired in
the fashion of the military, for one of the prime justifications for the setting
up of the New Police was the failure of the army to maintain order without
recourse to gross and excessive force. This had occurred with some
frequency in the preceding fifty years and most tragically at Peterloo,
when in 1819 a demonstration was set upon by the army and hundreds
were injured and eleven people killed.
When, therefore, on 29 September 1829 the citizens of London beheld
the spectacle of groups men being marched to their beats dressed in ‘blue-
tail coat, blue trousers ... and a glazed black top-hat strengthened with a
thick leather crown’ (Critchley 1978: 51), they were witnessing the birth of
a new institution. It was an institution that was symbolically differentiated
116
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
from both the military and continental models of policing through its
distinctive uniform (modelled on civilian business dress of the day), and
effectively differentiated by its mandate, which was first and foremost
preventative. This was to be achieved by providing a visible deterrent to
acts of crime and disorder. As the new Commissioners wrote in their first
force instruction: ‘It should be understood that at the outset the principal
object to be obtained is the prevention of crime.’
The public visibility of the police can be contrasted with the relatively
low visibility of the police in the criminological endeavour. Traditional
criminology largely ignored the police: Ferri’s book on The Positive School
of Criminology, published in 1906, hardly mentions them; sixty years later
Mannheim’s (1965) major 800-page, two-volume work Comparative
Criminology had no discussion of the police institution. Even as late as
1979, Holdaway (1979: 1) could write: ‘the relative dearth of research into
the British police has achieved the status of a cliché amongst sociologists.
The British police have remained largely hidden from sociological gaze.’
Criminological theory was essentially concerned with why people
broke the law and neither an analysis of law nor its enforcement seemed
relevant to this discussion. It was assumed that the relationship between
police and the law was straightforward: the police simply enforced the
law. Criminology paid little attention to how and why criminal sanctions
came to be invoked by the police against certain types of behaviours and
not others. Nor did it ask whether sanctions were applied equally to all
individuals engaged in similar types of infraction. In fact, even when this
problem was raised by authors such as Merton (1938) or Morris (1957), it
was asserted that police records and statistics based on them, despite their
limitations, did adequately represent the distribution of crime – in
particular, that crime was concentrated among the lower classes. Attention
was not, therefore, focused upon police organisation or the practice of
enforcement.
In the formative and middle years of the development of criminology,
the police institution was thus either ignored or seen as non-problematic.
However, all this was to change with the publication of Howard Becker’s
Outsiders: Studies in the Sociology of Deviance (1963), which was to become a
key text in the emergent interactionist and labelling approaches to the
study of crime and deviancy. Becker’s simple and even trite assertion that
deviant behaviour is that which is so labelled (see chapter 3) heralded a
decisive shift in the criminological agenda, both in scope and method.
The crucial role of the police in identifying and labelling persons and
actions as deviant was increasingly recognised by criminologists. What
had previously been taken for granted was now up for debate and
examination. In particular, Becker and others expanded the criminological
gaze to include in its focus not just law-breakers, but law-makers and law
enforcers as well. The process of law creation was important to Becker and
117
Introducing Criminology
the interactionist school because it raised questions about why only some
deviant acts were criminalized. This, in turn, raised questions about how
some social groups were able to assert their power to make criminal the
activities of others. The processes of enforcement were seen as important
because Becker and others did not assume a simple translation of the ‘law
in the books’ into the ‘law in action’. The rate of criminal behaviour in a
society was to be understood from the interactionist perspective as the
product of actions of persons within the criminal justice system who
defined, classified and recorded those behaviours as criminal – and these
activities were themselves worth of study.
For the interactionists, it was not the quality of the act that was
important to understanding deviancy, but the label applied to the act. This
means that they were not interested, as traditional criminology had been,
in the constitutional, psychological or physiological characteristics of
offenders, but in the processes leading to a person being officially defined
and labelled as deviant. The focus of criminological interest was thus shift-
ing away from offenders and deviants per se towards the criminal justice
system itself, its component parts and its processes. Given that the police
are the most important gatekeepers of the criminal justice system and
therefore highly influential in determining who is officially labelled as
criminal, criminological research on the police and policing assumed a
new importance. And for the interactionists, this required a different
methodology – not studies of crime rates and aggregate statistics but,
ideally, detailed and in-depth observational studies of the actual practice
of policing.
In the 1960s and 1970s then, there was a gradual opening up of the
police institution to criminological and sociological scrutiny, often in-
formed by the concerns of labelling theorists and interactionists. As Reiner
has noted,
The most characteristic type of work was close participant observa-
tion of police patrol work, primarily concerned with laying bare the
occupational culture of operational policing...in order to describe
and account for the rules and meanings that constitute it
(Reiner 1992b: 441)
However, once Pandora’s box was opened, these early concerns gave way
to new issues. The police became interesting in their own right, and in the
1980s in Britain the issues of public order (signalled largely by widespread
riots in inner city areas), police efficiency and effectiveness (given promi-
nence by the Conservative government’s concern with ‘value for money’
and limiting overall public expenditure) and miscarriages of justice (many
of which were shown, in part at least, to have emanated from poor or
corrupt policing methods), all gave rise to the police institution being
subject to an intense criminological gaze.
118
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
The word ‘police’ is derived from the Greek word ‘polis’, meaning city. As
Johnston (1992: 4) notes, before the eighteenth century, in English the word
‘police’ was used to refer to the general governance and administrative
regulation of the city. Thus the activity of policing embraced the whole
range of functions necessary for the maintenance of civic society, and if
this broader definition were still in currency today, it would include the
activities of tax inspectors, environmental health officers and school-cross-
ing patrols. Indeed, until the 1970s, the police in most parts of England and
Wales retained responsibility for such matters as taxi-licensing, dog-
licensing, diseases of animals and animal movement orders and abattoir
inspections. However, as Johnston (1992: 4) argues:
What this means is that when we use the terms ‘to police’ or ‘policing’ we
tend to equate them with what the police currently do, rather than with
their broader historical meaning. Indeed the most contemporary defini-
tion of ‘police’ in the Oxford English Dictionary reflects this shift, since
both the personnel and function are conflated in the same definition:
The civil force which is entrusted with the duty of maintaining public
order, enforcing regulations for the punishment or prevention of
breaches of the law and detecting crime. (OED)
119
Introducing Criminology
In the early 1960s the Royal Commission on the Police reviewed these
aims for the first time in 130 years and although they extended the roles
somewhat to take into account the modern circumstances of the police, the
prime responsibilities of the police were still defined as being:
• to maintain law and order and to protect persons and property.
• to prevent crime.
• to be responsible for the detection of criminals.
120
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
The purpose of the police service is to uphold the law fairly and
firmly; to prevent crime; to pursue and bring to justice those who
break the law; to keep the Queen’s Peace; to protect, help and
reassure the community; and to be seen to do all this with integrity,
common sense and sound judgement.
(quoted in Morgan and Newburn 1997: 77)
We can see that over the course of over 170 years there is a clear continuity
in these functionally based definitions of the police: the prevention or
crime, the detection of crime, the enforcement of law, and the maintenance
of order remain at their heart. For some this represents the genius of the
original formulation. However, there has been a growing dissatisfaction
amongst criminologists with defining the police by a set of idealised roles
which, some argue, are largely unattainable. To examine the source of this
dissatisfaction, we intend to examine the extent to which the police are
able to fulfil these functions
The list could go on. Name any source of human conflict or emergency
situation and the police have undoubtedly been called to deal with it. In
the light of this, Egon Bittner (1974) suggests that the key function of the
police is to deal with ‘something-that-ought-not-to-be-happening-and-
about-which-something-ought-to-be-done-now’. What is important is
that it is largely the public, not the police, who define which situations
require police intervention.
Although much police work is not directly related to law enforcement
and crime control, it is often argued that the presence of patrols on the
street has a deterrent and preventative function. A study conducted some
years ago found that some 55 per cent of patrol officers’ time was spent on
random and uncommitted patrol (Comrie and Kings, 1974). Studies of
both car and foot patrols tended to question the deterrent effect of
patrolling. Both British and American data indicated that, although some
patrol presence is necessary to deter potential offenders, the precise
number of foot or car patrols made very little difference; it is only when
patrols were removed completely that reported crime increases (Bright
1969; Kelling et al 1974).
This was hardly surprising, since foot patrol officers are only as
effective as far as their eyes can see and ears can hear and, for that matter,
so are car patrol officers. As Clarke and Hough (1984: 7) pointed out in
their review on the literature on police effectiveness:
123
Introducing Criminology
study found that over 60 per cent of callers had delayed at least five
minutes before calling the police; Beick and Kessler’s (1977) study in the
USA found that over 50 per cent of people took between twenty and forty
minutes before calling the police, with victims often discussing with
relatives and friends what action they should take before calling. Quite
clearly, rapid response is severely limited in its effectiveness by the delay
between incident and reporting.
Contrary to the popular mythology surrounding police investigations
which portrays them as involving painstaking detective work, piecing
together disparate clues to put a name to an unsolved crime, the police are
highly dependent on victims and witnesses for the identification of
offenders. Burrows and Tarling’s (1982) study of records drawn from three
metropolitan police forces illustrates that the police are responsible for
detecting either directly or indirectly only about 15 per cent of recorded
crime. There now seems to be a general consensus of research findings that
the public is primarily responsible for the detection of between 83 and 85
per cent of cleared-up offences (Steer, 1980; Mawby, 1979; Bottomley and
Coleman, 1981).
In a recent review of detective effectiveness, Bayley concluded that
detectives:
These generalisations are not necessarily true for serious crime, but with
most other offences, if the offender is apprehended, it is usually because
they are caught red-handed or because a victim or witness can give
specific information on the person who committed the offence. When the
police do not have such information, the probability of detecting offenders
is very low (Loveday 1996). Perhaps this is best illustrated by the case of
the Yorkshire Ripper manhunt which, by July 1979, months before he was
caught, had involved 500 police officers, 250,000 officer hours and had cost
over three million pounds (Nicholson 1979). More recently, there has been
a massive police effort in trying to establish the identity of the killer of Jill
Dando, the presenter of BBC’s Crimewatch programme, who was gunned
down in the street by an unknown attacker in 1999. Forensic evidence, and
124
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
Paul Rock
Rock (1973) argues that there is indeed a functional unity to all the various
police activities in that they are related to the primary role of being
‘responsible for the boundary patrolling tasks of a system of social control.
They control those deviancies which are proscribed both by external law-
giving institutions and by their own law-interpreting behaviour’ (Rock
1973: 174).
To perform this primary role, there are subsidiary functions which
facilitate its implementation and these are what Rock (1973: 183) terms ‘the
incidental and unintended consequences of police work’. Thus, the range
of non-crime-related tasks that the police find themselves involved in are a
necessary price to pay for the police ‘to continue to function as effective
agents of control’ (Rock 1973: 184). The involvement in these subsidiary
tasks is a mechanism which enables the police to perform their primary
control function without undue recourse to coercive measures, by
cloaking the office in a shroud of legitimacy. Further, such interventions
enable informal contacts with the community, resulting in an increased
information flow about other, more serious matters. Thus, the ‘coercive
role style is tempered by a benign complexion’ (Rock 1973: 184); coercive
power is transposed into legitimate authority by the diversity of tasks that
the police are prepared to engage in.
Although he regards non-crime tasks as subsidiary, merely involved
in generating legitimacy to facilitate other primary tasks, Rock does alert
us to the danger of seeing the police solely in terms of the enforcement,
125
Introducing Criminology
The public, therefore, only saw the police pulling people over,
standing over prone suspects and in other tense confrontational
situations. It is not surprising that members of the community
viewed the LAPD with fear and a degree of hostility.
(quoted in Loveday, 1996: 97)
By making legitimacy the concept that unifies the diverse police functions
with the core mandate, Rock has identified an important element of British
and American police traditions. However, he still sees policing as being
defined primarily by its law enforcement functions and this, as we have
argued, is inappropriate. Egon Bittner in his book The Functions of the Police
126
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
Egon Bittner
Bittner recognised that while other public and private officials had the
right to use force to achieve their ends, these were strictly limited to their
specific spheres of competence. Prison officers could only use force inside
a prison, mental health officials on those in their care, parents on their own
children, and so on. The police, on the other hand, have the right to use
force against any person across the whole domain of a domestic territory
and for a whole range of purposes. Thus he wrote: ‘the role of the police is
best understood as a mechanism for the distribution of non-negotiable
coercive force, employed in accordance with the dictates of an intuitive
grasp of situational exigencies’ (Bittner 1978: 33).
At first reading, this seems a remarkably obscure way of defining the
police: it certainly does not trip off the tongue, but that is because it is
trying to move beyond the specific to find a level of generality that would
make it universally applicable. If you read it again slowly, you will see that
there are three principal elements to the definition.
First, the police are a mechanism for the distribution of non-negotiable
coercive force; by saying this Bittner is drawing attention to the fact that
modern states have attempted to limit the use of coercive force by private
citizens, and have delegated this function to the police. The police thereby
become the mechanism for its distribution by deciding when, where and
against whom it is used.
Second, the police use non-negotiable coercive force. By non-negotiable
Bittner means that if a police officer decides to use force in a situation it is
difficult for anyone to legitimately challenge him or her; true the officer
might face a complaint or criminal prosecution later on, but at the time
they do not have to brook arguments or opposition – in that sense it is non-
negotiable.
Third, this force is employed in accordance with the dictates of an intuitive
grasp of situational exigencies. By this Bittner is noting that the justification
for the use of force, as such, cannot be entirely derived from any external
prescription. True, the law specifies the legitimate police use of force (in
England and Wales s.3 of the Criminal Justice Act 1967 prescribes the
circumstances and degree of force which can lawfully be used), but it
cannot say exactly when and where it may be used: that can only be
decided in the light of a police officer’s particular reading of a particular
situation at a particular time.
As Bittner notes, the utility of this definition is that it lends ‘homo-
geneity to such diverse procedures as catching a criminal, driving the
127
Introducing Criminology
Richard Ericson
Ericson attempts address this criticism by proposing a definition which
specifies the function of policing. As we have seen, most incidents that the
police involve themselves in do not end up with a crime report being filed,
nor do the police have much effect on either the prevention or detection of
crime. In the light of this, Ericson proposes that:
128
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
129
Introducing Criminology
Discretion
What is police discretion? Drawing on Davis’s (1969) seminal, but general
definition of discretion, Klockars proposed the following:
We can note three salient points about this definition. First, Klockars does
not use the word ‘law’, but the words ‘effective limits’; this is because even
where the law may insist on a course of action (in America there are ‘full
130
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
Resources
Given that the police have finite resources, they have to make choices
about how to deploy them and this inevitably means prioritising some
activities over others. Moreover, if the police were rigorously to enforce
some high volume offences like speeding, the capacity of others within the
criminal justice system to process them, such as the Crown Prosecution
Service and the courts, would soon be exceeded. As already noted, how-
ever, the scope for discretion in setting priorities has been lessened by the
Police and Magistrates Courts Act 1994.
Interpretational latitude
The law as it is written in the books purports to be clear and unambiguous,
but the messy reality of life rarely falls easily into the neat categories of the
law. All criminal statutes in England and Wales say that ‘a police constable
may arrest...’. Police officers have to interpret what they find to find a fit
between the events and the law, and they need to temper the law with
notions such as justice/fairness and appropriateness. As Klockars (1985:
98) notes, the law ‘overreaches’ itself and criminalizes more than it
intends. For instance, would the interests of justice be served by prosecut-
ing a surgeon for travelling at ten miles an hour over the speed limit, on
her way to perform life saving surgery, or the driver of a vehicle which due
to his own carelessness had swerved off the road resulting in the death of
his wife and daughter? Probably not.
Legitimacy
Full enforcement would lead to an undermining of the basis for police
legitimacy. One of the advantages of under-enforcement is that the police
can build up credit with a citizenry who, in acknowledgement that they
have been dealt with more favourably in the past, are more likely to
cooperate with police requests for help in the future. ‘Policing by consent’
is a key concept in this context. At the extreme, some have argued that full
enforcement would completely undermine the social fabric: ‘Any society
that committed the energy, resources and personnel to root out and punish
all wrongdoers would set off enough mass paranoia, violent conflict and
savage repression to become a charnel house, and pass into oblivion’
(Blumberg, 1970, cited in Rock, 1973: 179).
132
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
Efficiency
A policy of full enforcement would not allow the police to differentiate
between important and trivial crimes, and between cases where some
productive outcome can be achieved and those where this is unlikely.
Where resources are finite (see above), decisions need to be made about
the most efficient or effective use of them. Full enforcement would also
make it impossible for the police to ‘trade’ the threat of sanction for
information. One of the key investigative strategies, especially in ‘victim-
less’ crimes, of letting the little fish go, in exchange for information so that
bigger fish can be caught, would be unworkable (see Dunnighan and
Norris 1999).
Criminologists have, then, recognised the inevitability of discretion,
that police work is only partially concerned with law enforcement, and
even when there are clear infractions of law, there is no guarantee that
arrest and prosecution will follow. This realisation has had two major
implications for criminology.
First, that recorded crime statistics cannot be treated as an accurate or
reliable measure of the nature, distribution and extent of criminality. As
Kituse and Cicourel noted, crime rates should be ‘viewed as indices of
organisational processes rather than as indices of the incidence of certain
forms of behaviour’ (Kitsuse and Cicourel 1963: 135). This insight led to a
number of important studies documenting how crime statistics were a
social construction (Bottomley and Coleman 1981; McCabe and Sutcliffe
1978; Young 1991), and the development of alternative measures of crime
and victimisation which are not reliant on police processing, such as self-
report studies and victimisation surveys (see Coleman and Moynihan
1996 for a review).
Second, if the law does not provide the full answer as to what police
officers should do in a situation, attention shifts to what else influences
their decisions. As Klockars points out, this issue strikes at the heart of
democratic governance, with its emphasis on ‘the rule of law’. As he
writes:
For this reason, much criminological attention has been focused on what,
given such discretion, shapes police decision-making and the extent to
133
Introducing Criminology
which this leads to policing which is discriminatory, in the sense that the
law is unevenly applied in relation to different social groups in a way that
is ‘unjustified by legally relevant factors’ (Reiner 1992a: 162).
Discrimination
There is little doubt about the existence of what Reiner calls differentiation
– that the exercise of police powers falls disproportionately upon some
groups rather than others. Police enforcement activity largely falls upon
the young rather than the old, the poor rather than the affluent, men
rather than women, the urban rather than the rural, public rather than the
private domains, and so on. Whether and to what extent differentiation
involves discrimination are more difficult questions to answer. For
illustrative purposes, we will briefly concentrate on two areas: the ‘over-
policing’ of black people and the ‘under-policing’ of incidents of domestic
violence.
Stops: studies have consistently found that black people are more likely to
be stopped than whites, with estimates ranging from between two and
four times what one would expect on the basis of their presence in the
population. Not only is the rate of stops greater, but a black person is more
likely to be repeatedly stopped during the course of a year (Willis 1983, PSI
1983, Skogan 1994, Norris et al 1992, Macpherson 1999).
134
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
135
Introducing Criminology
In a similar way to that in which the over-policing of black people has been
attributed to the values and beliefs of officers transmitted through the
occupational culture, so too has the under-enforcement of laws relating to
domestic violence. Again some commentators note the prevalence of
sexist language, stereotypical attitudes towards women in general, the
marginalisation and harassment of women police officers, and a general-
ised belief in the inappropriateness of police intervention in domestic
affairs, consigning it to the status of ‘rubbish’ work’. From this it is argued
that it is easy to see how police discretion produces a pattern of under-
enforcement; again it is seen as a result of prejudiced attitudes and values
contained within the occupational culture (Edwards 1989, Stanko 1985).
There have been concerted attempts to change police policy and attitudes
in this area in the last decade or so since these studies were conducted.
There are, however, problems with academic accounts and policy
initiatives which see a simple translation of attitudes into behaviour. For
instance, Hoyle’s (1998) study of the policing of domestic violence found
that a general open-ended question to police officers about domestic
disputes elicited a whole range of negative responses, but these
generalised attitudes were often belied by how they said they handled
specific disputes. As she reported:
137
Introducing Criminology
This gap between attitudes and behaviour was similar to that reported in
relation to ‘race’ some years ago by the PSI study Police and People in
London (1983):
Our first impression ... was that racialist language and racial
prejudice were prominent and pervasive and that many individual
officers and also whole groups were preoccupied with ethnic
differences. At the same time, on accompanying these officers as they
went about their work, we found that their relations with black and
brown people were often relaxed and friendly ... we are fairly
confident that there is no widespread tendency for black or Asian
people to be given greatly inferior treatment by the police.
(PSI, 1983: 109 and 128)
This possibility of a gap between what officers say and what they do
undermines the simplistic use of a concept of occupational culture
(usually based on what officers say) as guide to, and explanation for,
actual police behaviour (usually that considered undesirable)
A number of criminologists (Holdaway 1983; Norris 1989; Hoyle 1998;
Manning 1977; Waddington 1999) have adopted a more complex view of
police culture, which recognises that while police culture may have
negative and undesirable traits, in itself that culture has to be understood
as embracing far more than just negative attitudes. Police culture may still
be used to help explain the difference between law in action and the law in
the books, but that culture is seen as arising from the common problems
that officers face in the course of their work. It not only contains attitudes
and beliefs, but a stock of recipe knowledge on how to achieve the policing
task: a set of working rules for managing the vicissitudes of the job
(Manning 1982). And here it should be remembered that the job is not
primarily about the prevention of crime or the enforcement of laws but
about reproducing order by providing authoritative intervention and
symbolic justice. As Chatterton (1983: 208) has argued:
Not only is there law which defines the range and nature of criminal
offences (such as murder or theft) – substantive criminal law – but there is
also law which attempts to regulate the treatment of those suspected of
crime – the law of criminal procedure. There are legal rules which govern
139
Introducing Criminology
such procedures as stop and search, search of premises, arrest, and the
detention and questioning of suspects. For example, the Police and
Criminal Evidence Act 1984 and its associated Codes of Practice were a
major attempt to provide for police and suspects a set of procedures in
these areas that would be clear, workable, fair, open and accountable.
There are also a whole host of subsidiary rules governing police practice
set down in non-statutory guidelines and force standing orders. Such
rules, it is often asserted, ensure that police powers are not exercised
arbitrarily, but constrained by the ‘due process of law’. As Sir Robert Mark,
the former commissioner of the Metropolitan Police, expressed it, ‘The fact
the British Police are answerable to law, that we act on behalf of the
community and not under the mantle of government, makes us the least
powerful, the most accountable and therefore the most acceptable police
in the world’ (Mark 1977: 56).
One of the most enduring yardsticks for evaluating the operation of
legal rules and the criminal justice system has been the model developed
by Herbert Packer (1968; see also King 1981) in the late 1960s. Packer
argued that systems of criminal justice can be usefully examined to see
what extent they correspond with two theoretical models: due process and
crime control. The models are ‘ideal’ types, each at either end of a
continuum; we would be unlikely to find a pure due process or crime
control model in reality. Let us briefly examine each in turn.
Due process
For Packer, a due process model emphasises the following.
• Effective restraints on the use of arbitrary power: for instance, the police
may only arrest and detain on the basis of evidence, rather than on
personal whim or prejudice, and there must be effective means of
reviewing these decisions
140
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
In the context of policing, the due process model sees the inviolability of
legal rules governing police powers as essential. The rules are there to
protect suspects against arbitrary power and when the rules are broken
cases should be dismissed, since otherwise this might lead to error and the
chance of a wrongful conviction.
Packer likened the due process model to an obstacle course which
guarantees that only those most likely to have committed a crime will
proceed to the next stage. Any weakness or impropriety at an earlier stage
will ensure that a case is discontinued. If it is not, there is a chance that the
innocent will be found guilty and, above all, the due process model seeks
to protect against this eventuality.
Crime control
In the crime control model, the following are examples of features that
would be emphasised:
• Disregard of legal controls: departure from legal propriety is seen as
inevitable. Formal legal rules get in the way of convicting the guilty and
therefore police malpractice is tolerated and even condoned.
• Implicit presumption of guilt: the police are seen as acting in good faith,
so that when they arrest and charge someone it is because they are
guilty. In this case there is little necessity for the courts to thoroughly
review cases before them; their job is merely to rubber stamp the
decision.
• High conviction rate: under a crime control system a high conviction
rate is necessary because if the courts were to acquit too many people
this would undermine the deterrent effect of the criminal law, and the
faith in police efficiency at bringing the right people to justice.
Whereas the primary function of the due process model is the acquittal
of the innocent, the crime control model prioritises the punishment of the
guilty.
The enduring significance of the two models has been in the realisation
that while policing is presented as being in line with due process values,
this rhetoric is undermined by the operational reality of police practice.
141
Introducing Criminology
Fabrication of evidence
The findings of the Independent Inquiry into the working practices of the
West Midlands Serious Crime Squad (Kaye 1991) revealed that in every
one of the 67 cases reviewed it had been alleged that officers fabricated
incriminating evidence. In 23 of these cases charges were dropped, the
judge directed acquittal, the jury found the defendants innocent or the
conviction was quashed on appeal (Kaye 1991: 72–73).
How can we explain this subversion of due process values? One answer
to the question is that such acts are the result of the aberrant behaviour
of individual officers who had gone off the rails. From this ‘rotten apple’,
perspective it is argued that any organisation employing tens of
thousands of people is bound to contain the occasional maverick. How-
ever good the recruitment, training, and supervisory systems, occasion-
ally someone will slip through the net. There are two fundamental
problems with such a position.
142
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
But this perhaps does not go far enough, for it is not just individual police
officers and the police organisation which can subvert due process, but the
law itself.
143
Introducing Criminology
As McBarnet (1979, 1981) has argued, in practice the law often favours
crime control rather than due process. Thus, while many police research-
ers have described the rule-breaking and illegality of the rank and file
practices, McBarnet carefully illustrates how crime control practices were
enshrined in the law, particularly ad hoc case law, in Scotland of the 1970s
(McBarnet 1981: ch 3). The consequence was that:
the police are in a sense the ‘fall guys’ of the criminal justice system,
taking the blame for any injustices in the operation of law, both in
theory (in the assumption ... that they break the rules) and indeed, in
the law. ... The law on criminal procedure in its current form does not
so much set a standard of legality from which the police deviate as
provide a license to ignore it.
(McBarnet, 1979: 39)
144
Policing and the police: key issues in criminal justice
Conclusions
• The police should not be equated with policing, an activity that can be
performed by a number of agencies. The role of the police is better
thought of as the reproduction of order by authoritative intervention
and symbolic justice, rather than in terms of crime control.
• Given the constraints, police discretion is inevitable and even desirable.
Police powers fall disproportionately on certain groups in the popula-
tion. Although the situation is complex, it appears that some part of this
is discriminatory. It is misleading simply to attribute such discrimina-
tion to individual attitudes or ‘police culture’.
• While criminal justice processes are usually presented in terms of ‘due
process’ values, certain police practices have been shown to violate
such values, and to be more consistent with a ‘crime control model’.
Rather than being understood in terms of ‘rotten apples’, examples of
police malpractice in violation of due process values seem to be related
to the organisational context in which they occur, and to the wider legal
framework and the adversarial system within which they are located.
145
Introducing Criminology
Chapter 6
Crime prevention
There are many ways to prevent crime and criminologists have sought to
delineate and differentiate the main strategies involved in crime preven-
tion. Although there is a variety of classifications (see Hughes 1998 and
Pease 1997), for our purposes the typology developed by Weiss (1987) is
most useful. Weiss identifies three main types of crime prevention activity:
primary, secondary and tertiary; we will examine each in turn.
Primary crime prevention is focussed on the offence rather than the offender,
and is often associated with situational crime prevention strategies which
focus on the immediate and localised context of the offence. At its
simplest, primary prevention may involve target hardening or removal.
Target removal strategies can include such measures as the switch from
public telephones that require coins to ones accepting only telephone
cards. Hardening strategies might include fitting metal grilles to shop
146
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
147
Introducing Criminology
148
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
149
Introducing Criminology
that there were only two local authority schemes in operation in 1987. By
1994 the Home Office indicated there were 79 town centre schemes (Home
Office, 1994); by 1996 all the major cities with a population over 500,000
boasted city centre schemes, and there were in excess of 200 police and
local authority schemes operating in high streets and smaller towns
(Norris et al 1998: 255); by 1998 this had risen to at least 440 town centre
schemes (Goodwin et al 1998).
In financial terms, between 1994 and 1997, central government made
available £37 million to fund the introduction of CCTV schemes, but on the
condition that local authorities and private business also contributed an
equal amount. Indeed Goodwin et al calculated that some 78 per cent of
the Home Office crime prevention budget was committed to CCTV during
this time. Moreover, the trend does not appear to be slowing down, as in
1999 it was announced by central government that a further £170 million
would be made available to fund the introduction of new CCTV schemes
or to extend existing ones in town centres, car parks, residential
communities and other crime ‘hot spots’ (Painter and Tilley 1999: 2).
It may be thought that the rush to install CCTV during the 1990s was
based on a firm foundation of supporting research evidence to show that it
was effective. This was not so. CCTV was introduced in town centres, and
the government funded its expansion, prior to conducting any systematic
evaluation of its effectiveness in reducing crime in such locations. What
evidence did exist prior to 1994 came from small-scale evaluations on
systems in car parks (Poyner 1992), buses (Poyner 1988), housing estates
(Musheno et al 1978), football stadia (Hancox and Morgan 1975), and the
London Underground (Burrows 1979). As Short and Ditton note, the
results of these independent and competently conducted evaluations were
‘fairly contradictory regarding the effectiveness of CCTV as a crime
prevention method’ (Short and Ditton 1995: 11), with some initiatives
showing no effect (Musheno et al 1979), others suggesting high levels of
displacement, rather than an overall reduction (Burrows 1979), and others
showing clear reductions (Poyner 1988 and 1992).
However, between 1988 and 1992 the Conservative Government had
witnessed an almost unprecedented growth in recorded crime from just
under four million offences to just under six million; for the party of ‘law
and order’, this represented a major policy failure and a potential electoral
disaster. In early 1993 however, CCTV was thrust into the limelight by two
events. First, there was the tragic killing of the two-year-old toddler, Jamie
Bulger, as his abduction by two ten-year-olds was caught on camera.
Second, the two men responsible for planting a bomb outside the Harrods
department store in London were subsequently identfied from CCTV
footage. In the glare of national publicity to which CCTV was subjected in
the wake of the Bulger case, the few schemes that did exist boasted major
benefits from its introduction. It is hardly suprising that the beleaguered
150
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
Home Secretary should jump at this ‘silver bullet’ which, through its very
public introduction onto city streets, would visibly show the government
was doing something to stem the inexorable rise in recorded crime. As he
stated in May 1995:
However, the evidence for the Home Secretary’s belief in the ‘wonderful’
technology was not that of the professional and independent evaluator
but from ‘post hoc shoestring efforts by the untrained and self-interested
practitioner’ (Pawson and Tilley 1994). And as Pease has recently
observed, ‘for those exercising stewardship of public money, good
evidence about effects should be necessary before public money is spent,
although one is tempted to ask where rigorous standards went in the
headlong rush to CCTV deployment’ (Pease 1999: 53). Thus even though
hundreds of millions of pounds have been spent on CCTV over the last
decade, by business, local communities and central government, there are
still major questions about its effectiveness.
At first sight this would appear to be a simple question to ask and many
people probably think that they already know the answer. Indeed,
representatives of the police, politicians of all persuasions, and members
of the CCTV industry have consistently answered the question with a
resounding ‘yes’, and this belief has been endorsed by largely uncritical
and complacent media coverage (Norris and Armstrong 1999: chapter 4).
However, one of the key remits of any academic discipline is to challenge
taken-for-granted assumptions, and criminology has an obligation to
subject crime and its control to critical scrutiny. The first task in the process
is to analyse apparently simple questions and lay bare their underlying
assumptions in the hope that we might be able to answer them more
satisfactorily. To answer the question, ‘does CCTV reduce crime?’, it is
necessary to consider three crucial elements contained within it:
• what is meant by CCTV?
• what is meant by crime and how can it be measured?
• what is meant by a reduction in crime and what would constitute
satisfactory evidence of a reduction occurring?
151
Introducing Criminology
identities of those under the camera’s gaze. Moreover, CCTV has been
deployed in a variety of institutional settings, from schools to hospitals, to
football stadia and transport systems. In a recent initiative in Hull, CCTV
cameras are to be deployed in the back of taxi-cabs, in order to deter
loutish behaviour and fare evaders.
So, if we want to answer the question, ‘does CCTV reduce crime?’, we
need to be clear about what CCTV is. Clearly there is a huge variation in
technological and organisational arrangements between systems, and this
has important consequences for our ability to generalise about the
effectiveness of CCTV. This relates to what evaluators term the problem of
external validity. In the main, when researchers carry out a study they aim
to make claims that go beyond the single case of their experiment or study
so as to be able to generalise their findings to a wider community. Even if
researchers themselves are often wary about the applicability of their
findings to different settings, others may be much less hesitant.
As Jupp notes researchers have distinguished two types of external
validity, population validity and ecological validity. Population validity
refers to the extent to which the findings can be generalised to the wider
population, and ecological validity refers to the extent to which the
findings can be generalised to other contexts and settings (Jupp 1989: 55).
For instance, if a study showed that the introduction of CCTV reduced
crime by 16 per cent, we cannot simply assume that a different CCTV
system, deployed in a different place, targeting a different crime problem,
with a system of different technological sophistication would have the
same impact.
were reported (Mirrlees-Black et al 1998: 19). And we can see from Table 6.1
that not all of these were actually recorded by the police. Overall the BCS
suggests that only about half (54%) of all such offences reported to the
police are recorded, which means that the recorded crime figures only
account for a quarter (24%) of the underlying crime rate, although this
ranges from 84% in the case of motor vehicle theft to 10% for theft from the
person.
This has some highly significant implications for measuring changes to
the crime rate. This would be of little concern if there was an invariant
relationship over time between reporting and recording rates but this is
not the case. According to the BCS, between 1981 and 1997 the overall rate
of reporting BCS offences to the police has fluctuated between 36% and
49%, and between the years 1991, 1993 and 1995 the rate of reporting for
burglary from 73% to 68% to 66%. Similarly the rate of reporting theft of
and from vehicles has consistently reduced from 56% to 53% to 51%. In
Table 6.2 below we show the effect that changes in the public reporting rate
and police recording rate between the years 1991 and 1995 would have on
the trends in recorded crime.
In the context of CCTV, imagine a system being introduced to a mixed
residential and shopping area in early 1992, where the evaluation took as
its base-mark the recorded crime rate in 1991. These figures were then
compared with the figures for 1993 and 1995. If the trends revealed in the
British Crime Survey were replicated at the local level, regardless of the
impact of the CCTV system, the official police recorded crime statistics
would have shown a reduction in crime of 20%, merely because of changes
in reporting and recording practices. For the offences of burglary, vehicle
theft and bicycle theft reductions would have been 20%, 22% and 34%
respectively. Unless the impact of these changes had been taken into
account by the researchers, which they almost never are, the observed
reductions would have been attributed to the CCTV system. In other
words, the real rate of crime could have remained the same, CCTV having
no impact, and yet the evaluators would have certainly claimed that CCTV
was an unqualified success in reducing crime.
All offences 44 54 24
Theft of vehicle 97 87 84
Robbery 57 30 17
Theft from vehicle 43 59 25
Theft from person 35 29 10
154
Table 6.2: Percentage change in British Crime Survey offences reported to and recorded by the police
1991, 1993, 1995
% reported % reported % of total % reported % reported % of total % reported % reported % of total % reduction
recorded recorded recorded recorded recorded recorded 1991 to 1995
Burglary 73 62 45 68 60 41 66 55 36 20
All vehicle theft 56 65 36 53 60 32 51 55 28 22
Bicycle theft 69 59 41 72 48 35 63 44 27 34
All BCS offences 49 60 29 47 55 26 46 50 23 20
155
Introducing Criminology
Displacement
One of the first problems in assessing any crime prevention initiative is
that while it may appear that a number of crimes have been prevented, in
156
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
fact they may have merely been displaced to another area or committed at
other times or in different ways. Criminologists have identified six types
of displacement associated with crime prevention initiatives (Barr and
Pease 1990; Gabor 1978).
157
Introducing Criminology
overall reduction in crime was only 6%. (Skinns 1998: 185). Similarly in
Birmingham, the evaluation by Brown (1995) produced strong evidence
for geographical displacement for two different offence types, ‘street
robbery and theft from the person’ and ‘theft from a motor vehicle’. In the
case of the former, Brown concluded that:
Over the time span of data collection many events occur in addition
to the study’s independent variable. The history factor refers to the
possibility that any one of the events rather than the hypothesised
independent variable might have caused the observed changes in the
dependent variable.
(1981: 334)
In the real world then, it is likely that while the evaluation of CCTV is
158
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
being undertaken, other changes are occurring to the town centre. For
example, in Birmingham pedestrianisation was introduced to key areas of
the city centre at the same time as the cameras were installed (Brown 1995:
37), perhaps attracting more people, but fewer cars, into the town centre,
and this in turn may have had a completely independent impact on the
crime rate. As Skinns notes in relation to Doncaster:
What impact these changes had is of course difficult to assess, but one
might speculate that if more people are going out of town to shop and to
be entertained, there might be fewer offences in the town centre.
Reactivity
A problem for all evaluators is the extent to which the very act of
evaluation itself influences the results of the study. This is generally
referred to as reactivity and can be defined as ‘any time participants
suspect or know they are being observed, experimented with, or tested
there is a chance that their behaviour may be modified by the measuring
instrument’ (Smith 1981: 335). In the case of the newly installed scheme in
Windsor and Eton it was revealed that officers would be redeployed from
other areas to cope with the expected increase in workload that would
arise from the introduction of CCTV in the town centres. The presence of
the extra officers patrolling the town centre may well have had a signifi-
cant deterrent effect on crime in its own right, thus making any claims
about the success of the CCTV system impossible to disentangle from the
effect of the increased police presence (Norris and Armstrong 1999).
159
Introducing Criminology
the previous year. For instance, beat 1 incurred more than a 50% reduction
in recorded crime while beat 2 had an increase of over 70% and similarly
beat 14 enjoyed a 60% reduction while beat 16 showed an increase of over
80%. There can also be fluctuations for total recorded crime within the
same beat. For instance in one police beat in the West Midlands, Ekblom
showed that recorded crime fluctuated over a three-year period from over
90 crimes per month to less than 20 (Ekblom 1992: 39).
These variations mean that if one had been measuring the impact of a
new crime prevention measure introduced in June 1988 and compared the
recorded crime figures for one year before and one year after implementa-
tion there would have been an impressive reduction by about one-third,
regardless of the measure. On the other hand if the measure had been
introduced in June 1989 there would have been a disappointing 25%
increase in recorded crime (derived from Ekblom 1992: Figure 2). As Tilley
has argued, ‘these wide natural fluctuations in local rates, especially in a
small area over a limited period ... make interpreting real effects as against
pseudo-random short-term changes very difficult’ (Tilley 1998: 142).
Given such fluctuations, if in the years preceeding the introduction of
CCTV crime had risen exceptionally in an area, one might naturally expect
a fall. Interestingly, given that the rush to CCTV was funded by a
competitive bidding process, it may well be that local partnerships only
put forward areas with ‘abnormally’ high and rapidly increasing crime
rates, ones which were likely eventually to show a decline, regardless of
any crime prevention initiative (see Ditton and Short 1999: 216–7).
It is not just small-scale local random variations that evaluators have to
contend with. There are also wider background changes to crime rates
against which any changes have to be judged. For instance, if nationally
crime trends are on a downward path then it is important isolate the
impact of CCTV from the general trend. It is perhaps arguable that one
reason for the widespread belief in the efficacy of CCTV was that the rapid
growth of the number of CCTV systems (between 1993 and 1997) occurred
at precisely the same time as the only sustained fall in recorded crime since
the 1950s.
At the more local level, such changes are also important to take into
account when judging the efficacy of CCTV: if crime falls faster in the areas
not under the watchful eye of the cameras then surely one would need to
be cautious about attributing the fall in the area under surveillance to the
impact of CCTV. This point is almost entirely overlooked by politicians,
the media, and local advocates of CCTV. For them a reduction is a
reduction full-stop, and must be attributable to the effect of CCTV. If we
take the example of Sutton, the evaluation conducted by researchers from
the University of the South Bank showed a reduction in total recorded
crime in the area covered by the cameras of 13%. However, in the police
division as a whole it fell by 17% and across the borough it reduced by 30%
160
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
(Sarno 1996: 22). On the face of it, it would appear that rather than
facilitating a reduction in crime, the introduction of CCTV hindered its
reduction. However as Norris and Armstrong have argued, in the face of a
reduction, few are prepared to accept that it may not be attributable to
CCTV:
What the above discussion has highlighted is that for the criminological
evaluator, the simple statement that ‘after CCTV was introduced crime
reduced by 20%’ cannot be treated at face value and needs to be inter-
preted in the light of the various threats to validity we have outlined above.
The question of validity is absolutely central to the evaluator’s effort, since
validity refers to the ‘degree to which the researcher has measured what he
or she set out to measure’ (Smith 1981: 333) and the extent to which the
researchers are able to assert that the changes in the dependent variable
(the crime rate) are caused by the introduction of the independent variable
(CCTV) and not by other factors. This means that researchers must active-
ly seek, through the design of their evaluations, to rule out alternative
explanations.
Experimental design
The strongest form of research design to rule out alternative explanations
is by evaluators mimicking the methodology of the natural scientist and
utilising the experimental method. Classically this entails comparing
two groups: an experimental group and a control group. Ideally, the
units which comprise both groups should be chosen at random, since
this eliminates systematic bias or error. Once the two groups are allocated,
both groups are observed and measured before the experimental variable
is introduced to the experimental group, while the control group is
left untouched. Finally both groups are measured again and the results
161
Introducing Criminology
compared. Any changes in the two groups are then compared and if
the results are significantly different then it is legitimate to infer that it is
the introduction of the experimental variable which caused the changes.
The importance of the control group is that is allows us to see if any
changes that did take place occurred ‘naturally’, that is not as a result of
the introduction of the experimental variable. As Campbell and Stanley
(1963: 13–16) have shown, if the procedures have been carried out
correctly, the threats to internal validity discussed above may all be ruled
out.
Of course, this strict control can only really be achieved under
laboratory conditions; however, in the social sciences, there are many who
advocate that researchers should at least strive towards the ideal of the
classic experiment by approximating, as far as possible, experimental
conditions in the field. These are generally referred to as quasi-
experimental methods which, as Jupp notes, have a long pedigree in
criminology and are often referred to as ‘field experiments’ or ‘reforms as
experiments’ and have been used to evaluate a variety of new initiatives
such as the most effective regimes for the control and treatment offenders
(see Jupp 1989), neighbourhood watch (Bennett 1990) and police patrol
strategies (Kelling 1974).
In the case of CCTV, one of the more imaginative quasi-experiments
was conducted by Beck and Willis (1999), who wanted to examine the
effect of introducing CCTV systems in the retail sector to prevent stock
loss, mainly through shop-lifting. The study aimed to assess the impact of
different types of CCTV systems on levels of stock loss in fifteen stores of a
large national UK fashion retailer. The research used a before-and-after
experimental design involving the following:
• a stock-take being carried out in each store before CCTV was installed.
• CCTV systems being introduced into each store. Three types of system
were introduced to look at the differential impact (if any) of systems
with varying levels of technical sophistication.
• a stock-take being carried out in each store after CCTV was installed to
calculate the number of units stolen and their value. The stock-takes
162
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
were carried out twice: 13 weeks after installation and then 28 weeks
after. This enabled the researchers to assess the impact in both the short
and the medium term.
What then did the study find? In the short term, based on the figures
after 13 weeks, so far as the theft prevention possibilities of CCTV are
concerned, the findings were impressive. Taking all the stores together, the
average number of units lost per week fell from 72 to 52, a 28% reduction.
In total, for the 15 stores in the study, if this trend continued there would
have been savings of nearly £90,000 per year. Given that the total cost of
introducing CCTV to these stores was £180,000, it would take two years
for the savings made to pay for the capital costs of installation. In business
terms, this can be seen as an adequate but not spectacular return on the
investment but one that is probably worthwhile financially given that the
savings might be expected to continue in the third and fourth years of
operation.
But this projection is on the basis of figures collected after just three
months of operation and criminologists are well aware that the effect of
crime prevention measures often degrades over time. This frequently
seems to be due to the fact that when new schemes are introduced, there is
often an initial blaze of publicity which enhances any deterrent effects.
Over time, as potential offenders either forget or become blasé about the
new measures, they will no longer be deterred. Moreover, the professional
or persistent offenders may gradually adapt their techniques to continue
to avoid detection. For these reasons it is important that assessment about
impacts be taken over longer rather than shorter time periods.
In this study, while the second stock-take only occurred after six
months, it still allows some assessment of the medium-term effect and
whether the impact has degraded. In the twelve stores that had a six-
month stock-take, the average number of units lost per week before
the introduction of the CCTV systems was 64; six months after installation
the number had reduced, but only by one to 63. In financial terms,
this represented an average reduction in weekly loss of merely £4 and on
this basis it would take an average of 58 years to recoup the capital out-
lay.
Overall, and in the medium term, it would appear that CCTV is not a
cost-effective measure of reducing stock loss and does little to prevent the
crime of shop-lifting.
However, there is a final twist to this tale, arising from the fact that this
rather negative finding is based on the aggregated results from different
systems. The original experimental design, it will be recalled, included the
introduction of three different systems of varying degrees of technical
sophistication. At the lowest level this included a CCTV display monitor at
the entrances to the store to alert people to CCTV, twelve dummy cameras,
and no recording or monitoring facilities; the medium-level system had
entrance display monitors, between six and twelve static colour cameras,
the facility both to record and monitor the images from the cameras, but no
provision for permanent monitoring; the high-level system had entrance
164
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
display monitors, between two and four pan, tilt and zoom cameras,
between eight and twelve static colour cameras, the facility to both record
and monitor the images from the cameras, and the provision of permanent
staff to continuously monitor the screens.
The aggregate results mask considerable variation in the impact of
different systems since the high-level stores showed ‘an impressive
reduction of 26%, whilst there was an increase of 32% in medium-level
stores and an increase of 9% in low-level stores’ (Beck and Willis 1999: 260).
In financial terms the results were as follows:
The strength of this study is that it allows us to give clear advice to the
financially motivated retailer, concerned with the bottom line of profit-
ability: ‘If you are going to install a CCTV system, it needs to be a
sophisticated one that is permanently monitored, otherwise your expendi-
ture will be greater than your savings’. Even then, CCTV does not offer a
magic bullet to the problems of stock loss and it will still take over two-
and-a-half years to recover the investment. Moreover this does not include
the costs of monitoring and running the high-level system, which on a
conservative estimate would be in the region of £12,000 a year. If this is
included, it would take nearly five years for the system to recover its costs
and the hard-headed business person might decide that with such a slow
rate of return, it might be more rational to just accept the losses!
*These studies have been conducted in Airdrie (Ditton and Short 1998; Short and Ditton
1996), Birmingham (Brown 1995), Brighton (Squires and Measor 1996, 1997), Burgess Hill
(Squires 1998c), Burnley (Armitage, Smyth and Pease 1999), Crawley (Squires 1998b),
Doncaster (Skinns 1998), East Grinstead (Squires 1998a) Glasgow (Ditton and Short 1999),
Ilford (Squires 1998d), Kings Lynn (Brown 1995), Newcastle (Brown 1995), and Sutton (Sarno
1996).
165
Introducing Criminology
166
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
The findings
In Burnley, Armitage et al (1999) found a reduction of 25% sustained over
two years. A decrease was found for every offence type measured by the
study; importantly, the fact that the reductions were sustained over a
period of two years indicated that they were not merely the result of quasi-
random fluctuations in the crime rate. There was no evidence of displace-
ment and some evidence of a diffusion of benefits. In crime prevention
terms this is undoubtedy a success; however, as we shall see, no other
study has found such consistent and positive results.
In Airdrie (Short and Ditton 1996) there was an overall reduction of
crime of 21%, which the research demonstrated was greater than one
would expect on the basis of the downward trends in the surrounding
area. The reductions were sustained over a two-year period. However,
unlike in Burnley, there were significant differences in changes to recorded
crime levels for different offences. A number of offence categories showed
increases, such as drug offences, low-level public order offences and
minor traffic violations, while crimes of dishonesty, such as housebreaking
and theft of and from motor vehicles showed a dramatic reduction of 48%.
Since these crimes of dishonesty account for about 40% of overall recorded
crime, reducing them by nearly half more than offsets the rise in other
offence types. Moreover, as the authors state, increases in these offence
categories ‘are not necessarily indicative of the failure of CCTV... and
increases in drug offences may reflect the surveillance ability of CCTV to
detect crimes that might otherwise have gone unnoticed. The same could
be said of “breach of the peace” offences, and minor traffic violations’
(Ditton and Short 1999: 206). The researchers found no evidence of
functional or geographical displacement and no evidence of a diffusion of
benefits.
These success stories are paralleled by the findings from Newcastle and
Kings Lynn (Brown 1995). In Newcastle, for example, although Brown
does not provide figures for the decrease in recorded crime as a whole, he
showed reductions in the major offence categories of burglary (–57%),
criminal damage (–34%), theft of and from motor vehicles (–49%). The
reductions were greater in the CCTV area than in the control areas. There
was no evidence of displacement but some of diffusion of benefits, and
there was some evidence that for motor vehicle crime the effects were
fading over time.
However, these rather unequivocal success stories have to be measured
against rather more mixed findings. Skinns’ evaluation of Doncaster
showed a 16% reduction which initially would seem to make the scheme
an unequivocal success. However, this reduction was offset by a
statistically significant increase for nearly all major offence categories in
the outlying townships. There was also evidence, particularly for burglary,
of a diffusion of benefits to the areas immediately surrounding the
167
Introducing Criminology
168
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
wanting to know whether CCTV will reduce crime in their area, this is
hardly a useful reply. But it does invite us to ask rather different questions.
If CCTV did reduce crime in Burnley, why did it not reduce crime in
Glasgow? If we can answer this question, then we will be in better position
to predict whether CCTV will work in a given locale and not in others.
However, this requires a radically different methodological approach to
the one typically adopted by evaluators of crime prevention measures; this
approach, developed by Pawson and Tilley, is termed realistic evaluation
and it is to a consideration of this that we now turn.
Realistic evaluation
The sort of mixed findings that we have described for CCTV evaluations
should come as no surprise to crime prevention specialists. As Tilley (1998:
143) has noted, whenever the results of the evaluation studies are com-
pared, the results are almost always inconsistent and contradictory. For
Pawson and Tilley (1997), this considerably undermines the utility of
evaluation studies for practitioners. This is because the focus of evalua-
tions has been almost solely on outcomes (does the crime rate change?)
rather than considering the processes, or ‘mechanisms’ which might
account for the changes. The point has been forcefully reiterated by Gill
and Turbin in the context of crime prevention in the business sector, which
wishes to use the research as the basis for its security policy:
There is no guarantee that the results of one study will have any rele-
vance for a different location or context. The common sense observa-
tion that what has an impact in site A may not necessarily have an
impact in site B has, to a large extent, been ignored by previous
research that focuses largely on collecting figures to show whether
the measure has worked at all. The main issue is not so much
whether the measure worked but rather how it did so, conversely,
why it failed to work when logic indicated that it should, or as
Pawson and Tilley state, ‘what works, for whom and in what
circumstance’.
(Gill and Turbin 1999: 181)
Ironically, it is the emphasis on the quasi-experimental design by social
science evaluators which is partially to blame for this. The focus of these
designs has generally been to determine whether a specific measure
reduced crime in a specific context, and by utilising a properly constructed
experimental design this question can be answered. But what it does not
tell us is why the measure had the effect that it did. Realistic evaluation
refocuses attention on the processes that produce the particular outcomes
rather than just on the outcomes themselves. For Pawson and Tilley this
169
Introducing Criminology
Context
Context refers to the specific features of the the particular scheme being
evaluated; this requires detailed description by evaluators and an analysis
as to how these features may impact on the outcome of the study. For
instance, consideration needs to be given to: the level of technological
sophistication of the system; the organisation of the system and how it is
integrated with deployment practice; the physical and social make-up of
the area; the patterns of use of the area and the pattern and incidence of
crime in an area.
By explicitly detailing the contexts, not only will this enable more
systematic comparison between different evaluations but it will also
facilitate an understanding of the particular mechanisms which are
triggered by different contexts.
Mechanisms
In the context of CCTV, Pawson and Tilley (1994 and 1997) and Tilley
(1998), have identified a range of potential mechanisms which may be
triggered. These include:
Outcomes
We can see that while standard evaluations have been concerned with a
single outcome, the change to the recorded crime rate, realistic evaluation
would be concerned with multiple outcomes. For example: what is the
level of public awareness of the system? Has CCTV changed the pattern of
usage of the city centre? Have deployments to incidents increased? Have
more people been arrested? and so on. All of these are, in a sense,
intermediate outcomes which will have an effect on the impact of CCTV.
And it is only by measuring these that we can can start to unpack why
CCTV has the effects that it does. For Pawson and Tilley, realistic evalua-
tion requires that researchers explicitly hypothesize which mechanisms
will be triggered in any given crime prevention context. These hypotheses
should then ‘frame the requisite data and research strategies, and thus call
upon a range of evidence entirely different from the standard compari-
sons’ (1997: 80).
This has some major criminological implications, since if one social group
is being targeted disproportionately, then this will have the effect of
172
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
173
Introducing Criminology
The first thing to understand is that public peace – the sidewalk and
street peace – of cities ... is kept primarily by an intricate, almost
unconscious, network of voluntary controls and standards among
people themselves, and enforced by the people themselves.
(Jacobs, 1961, quoted in Bannister et al 1998: 23)
There are two things to note about this. First, order is maintained not
primarily through the intervention of police or security staff (indeed their
intervention is often a sign of disorder) but by the ordinary mass of
citizenry. Secondly, that order is localised and, as such, is capable of being
sensitive to a diversity of community norms.
For Bannister et al (1998), cities are sites of difference: sites where those
of different classes, races, cultures and backgrounds intermingle. And the
encountering of difference brings with it not only excitement and stimula-
tion but also fear. Fear is not necessarily something to be concerned about,
indeed it is part of the the adventure of the city. However, while some
‘visions’ of the city celebrate and promote this aspect of metropolitan life,
other, more powerful voices envisage the need to manage this fear and
create homogeneous spaces through the exclusion of difference.
The exclusionary impulse is intimately bound up with the commerciali-
sation of city centre space and in particular the threat posed to the
commercial profitability of the town centre by out-of-town retail parks
(such as the Metro Centre in Gateshead) where private finance has created
sterile, orderly, but above all private areas for consumption. This leads to
a fortress mentality where ‘difference is not so much to be celebrated as
segregated’ (Bannister et al 1998–27). CCTV becomes part of the process
whereby difference is excluded not by targeting the criminal, but by
targeting the ‘other’: the homeless, the poor, the political activist, who do
not contribute to the commercial success of the city. This exclusionary
impulse is almost entirely overlooked by those who focus on the crime
reduction potential of CCTV.
However, for a number of radical commentators, CCTV is part of the
process whereby city centres are being transformed from democratic
spaces to which all citizens have access and in which they can freely
interact, whatever their race, class or status, to mere sites of consumerism.
As the public realm becomes increasingly privatised, as housing, parks,
leisure facilities, and shopping spaces are sold off to commercial de-
velopers, they become subject to private standards of justice and control.
As France and Wiles warn:
174
CCTV and crime prevention: questions for criminology
These fears are well grounded: as McCahill (2000) has shown in his study
of CCTV in shopping malls, the exclusion of ‘flawed’ consumers is actively
promoted by the CCTV system, as it is used to systematically exclude
youth and others deemed undesirable. Moreover, it leads to a form of
private justice, where people are barred from certain spaces, on the
authority of the security guards, who then use the cameras to collectively
enforce such bans. This is achieved without any recourse to the formal
criminal justice system and therefore bypasses legal safeguards embodied
in the public justice of the courts.
This exclusionary impulse is starting to assert itself in what remains of
the public sphere, as Reeve’s study of town centre managers revealed. The
managers wanted to discourage certain people and activities within the
city centre which were seen as non-conducive to their consumer-led vision
of the desirable. A quarter wanted to discourage political gatherings, a half
to discourage youths from ‘hanging out’, and a half to prohibit begging in
the streets (Reeve 1998: 78).
As Young has argued, this exclusionary impulse is not the outcome of
over-zealous security guards inappropriately using the the system. It is
embedded in structural changes in the nature of crime control in the
transition from modernity to late modernity. While the modernist project
had faith in the ability to intervene both structurally and individually,
through secondary and tertiary crime prevention techniques, it was
predominantly inclusionary; it sought to keep the deviant and potential
deviant within the fold. In late modernity such hopes have largely been
abandoned and instead society ‘responds to deviance by exclusion and
separation’ (Young 1999: 26).
175
Introducing Criminology
Chapter 7
This book began by inviting you to a party. Those who were seduced
by this invitation and are still with us might be feeling rather disappointed
by now. Perhaps anticipating party hats, games and laughter, readers have
been taken aside by two rather serious academics who proceeded
to ask a lot of rather difficult questions. Thankfully, parties don’t oftenturn
out like that. We hope that those who are still reading will agree that those
questions, although difficult, are often important and interesting ones.
One major role for criminology is to subject ‘obvious’, ‘common sense’
knowledge – the sort of thing that often enters the conversation at parties –
to critical scrutiny. As we have seen, although it may be thought obvious
that more police patrolling the streets or the presence of CCTV cameras
will inevitably reduce crime, things are more complicated than that.
Criminology subjects such ideas to rigorous enquiry, which often includes
systematic empirical research by a variety of methods. Whether or not this
qualifies criminology as a science – another difficult question that has
taken up a lot of energy over the years, often to little effect – is not an issue
that we have discussed in this book. But the striving for knowledge that
will somehow go beyond common sense, speculation, belief or assertion is
certainly something that has characterised much work in criminology, a
child of what has been called a modernist project.
In the first chapter, we began with some basic questions about crime
and the criminal. We found that there are different views about the
definition of these terms, which had some important consequences,
including for the very scope and nature of the discipline. While many
criminologists in the past did not spend too much time considering such
definitional questions, we suggested that the terms should be regarded as
problematic. According to this view, criminologists should consider the
use of these terms when they appear. Even where the definitions of crime
176
Criminology: some concluding thoughts
and the criminal are clear, there still remains the problem for criminol-
ogists of identifying crime and the criminal for research purposes. If a
legalistic position is adopted, for example, can we use official data from
the criminal justice system in order to assess the nature, extent and
distribution of crime and offenders?
We then outlined in more detail what seems to be the subject matter of
criminology, and its multi-disciplinary nature. Our position is that
criminology is a broad-ranging subject, which benefits from the insights of
a variety of disciplines and perspectives. There are, however, those
working in these areas who do not consider their work to be criminology,
and do not regard themselves as criminologists. Although the argument is
still to be heard that criminology should be abolished and the study of
crime and justice undertaken by other disciplines and perspectives, the
future of criminology looks assured. It has become institutionalised, with
apparently firm foundations in both academia and government, although
the complexion it assumes often varies with the context and the sponsor.
Finally, we tried to trace the history of criminology. Again, there were no
easy answers here, with the discussion seeming to hinge on what is meant
by ‘criminology’. Nevertheless, such histories tell us a little more about the
subject and where it (if, in view of the current fragmentation and
diversification, we can speak of ‘it’) stands today.
No introduction to criminology would be complete without consider-
ing the attempt to develop theories of crime. We began our discussion of
this in chapter 2 in looking at biological and psychological contributions to
this enterprise. While early efforts involved crude attempts to spot
biological or psychological differences between offenders and others, later
research became increasingly sophisticated in this quest. First, it stressed
the notion of a complex interaction between a range of different sorts of
factors, whether biological, psychological, social or of some other sort.
Second, it appeared to soften the deterministic tenor of some earlier
pronouncements, by stressing that such factors operated in a probabilistic
fashion – they were risk factors. Finally, it was often declared that attempt-
ing to develop a general theory of crime was a fruitless or unrewarding
task. While some tried to refashion the subject matter as anti-social
conduct or something similar, others suggested that we needed to be
much more specific in what we are trying to explain – such as by focusing
on aggression, or life-course persistent anti-social behaviour – in view of
the heterogeneity of the offender population. We saw how what we called
the new biology of crime was still engaged in the search for differences,
often on this modified basis. By contrast, rational choice, routine activities
and situational perspectives played down the idea of difference, partly
because they were more interested in those aspects (usually in the
immediate situation, rather than in the person) that could be more easily
changed in order to reduce crime.
177
Introducing Criminology
178
Criminology: some concluding thoughts
179
Introducing Criminology
Bibliography
181
Introducing Criminology
182
Bibliography
Cain, M. (1973) Society and the Policeman’s Role. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Cain, M. (1986) ‘Realism, feminism, methodology and law’, International Journal of
the Sociology of Law, 14: 255–67.
Cameron, D. and Frazer, E. (1987) The Lust to Kill. Cambridge: Polity.
Campbell, A. (1984) The Girls in the Gang. Oxford: Blackwell.
Campbell, D. and Stanley, J. (1963), Experimental and Quasi-experimental Designs for
Research. Chicago: Rand MacNally.
Canter, D. (1994) Criminal Shadows. London: Harper Collins.
Caputi, J. (1987) The Age of Sex Crime. London: The Women’s Press.
Carlen, P. (1992) ‘Criminal women and criminal justice’, in R. Matthews and J.
Young (eds) Issues in Realist Criminology. London: Sage.
Carlisle, A. (1998) ‘The divided self: toward an understanding of the dark side of
the serial killer’, in R. M. Holmes and S. T. Holmes (eds) Contemporary
Perspectives on Serial Murder. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Carrington, K. (1998) ‘Postmodernism and feminist criminologies’, in P. Walton
and J. Young (eds) The New Criminology Revisited. Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Chatterton, M. (1976) ‘Police in social control’, in J. King (ed) Control Without
Custody? Cambridge: Cambridge Institute of Criminology.
Chatterton, M. (1983) ‘Police work and assault charges’, in M. Punch (ed) Control in
the Police Organisation. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Clarke, R. and Hough, M. (1984) Crime and Police Effectiveness. London: Home
Office Research Study 79.
Clarke, R. (1992), Situational Crime Prevention: Successful Case Studies. New York:
Harrow and Heston.
Cleckley, H. M. (1976) The Mask of Sanity, 5th edn. St Louis: C. V. Mosby.
Cloward, R. and Ohlin, L. (1960) Delinquency and Opportunity. New York: Free
Press.
Cohen, A. K. (1955) Delinquent Boys. Glencoe: Free Press.
Cohen, A. K. (1966) Deviance and Control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Cohen, L. and Felson, M. (1979) ‘Social change and crime rate trends: a routine
activities approach’, American Sociological Review, 44: 588–608.
Cohen, S. (ed) (1971) Images of Deviance. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Cohen, S. (1973a) Folk Devils and Moral Panics. London: Paladin.
Cohen, S. (1973b) ‘The failures of criminology’, The Listener, 8 November.
Cohen, S. (1985), Visions of Social Control. Polity Press: Cambridge.
Cohen, S. (1993) ‘Human rights and crimes of the state’, Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Criminology, 26: 97–115.
Cohen, S. (1998) ‘Intellectual scepticism and political commitment’, in P. Walton
and J. Young (eds) The New Criminology Revisited. Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Coleman, C. and Moynihan, J. (1996) Understanding Crime Data: Haunted by the Dark
Figure. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Comrie, M. and Kings, E. (1974) Study of Urban Workloads: Final Report. Home
Office Police Research Services Unit, Unpublished.
Connell, R. (1987) Gender and Power. Cambridge: Polity.
183
Introducing Criminology
184
Bibliography
Eaton, M. (1986) Justice for Women. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.
Edwards, S. (1984) Women on Trial. Manchester: Manchester University Press.
Edwards, S. (1989) Policing ‘Domestic’ Violence. London: Sage.
Egger, S. (ed) (1990) Serial Murder: An Elusive Phenomenon. New York: Praeger.
Ekblom, P. (1992), ‘The Safer Cities Programe Impact Evaluation: Problem and
Progress’, Studies on Crime and Crime Prevention, Vol. 1. No 1.
Ekblom, P. and Heal, K. (1982) The Police Response to Calls from the Public. Research
and Planning Unit Paper 9. London: Home Office.
Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D. and Ageton, S. S. (1985) Explaining Delinquency and Drug
Use. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Ericson, R. (1982) Reproducing Order: A Study of Police Patrol Work. Toronto:
University of Toronto Press.
Eysenck, H. (1977) Crime and Personality, 3rd edn. London Routledge and Kegan
Paul.
Eysenck, H. ‘Personality theory and the problems of criminality’, in B. J. McGurk,
D. M. Thornton and M. Williams (eds) Applying Psychology to Imprisonment.
London: HMSO.
Gabor, T. (1978), ‘Crime Displacement: The Literature and Strategies For Its
Investigation,’ Crime and Justice, Vol 6 No 2: 100–107.
Garland, D. (1985) ‘The criminal and his science’, British Journal of Criminology, 25:
109–37.
185
Introducing Criminology
Garland, D. (1996) ‘The limits of the sovereign state’, British Journal of Criminology,
36: 445–71.
Garland, D. (1997) ‘Of crimes and criminals: the development of criminology in
Britain’, in M. Maguire, R. Morgan, and R. Reiner (eds) The Oxford Handbook of
Criminology, 2nd edn. Oxford: Clarendon.
Gelsthorpe, L. (1997) ‘Feminism and criminology’, in M. Maguire, R. Morgan, and
R. Reiner (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Criminology, 2nd edn. Oxford: Clarendon.
Gelsthorpe, L. and Morris. A. (eds) (1990) Feminist Perspectives in Criminology.
Buckingham: Open University Press.
Giddens, A. (1990) The Consequences of Modernity. Cambridge: Polity.
Giddens, A. (1991) Modernity and Self-identity. Cambridge: Polity.
Gill, M. and Turbin, V. (1999) ‘Evaluating ‘Realistic Evaluation’: Evidence from a
study of CCTV’, in Painter, K. and Tilley, N. (eds) Crime Prevention Studies, Vol
10: 225–251. Special edition entitled ‘Surveillance of Public Space: CCTV, Street
Lighting and Crime Prevention’.
Gilroy, P. (1982) ‘The myth of black criminality’, Socialist Register. London: Merlin.
Glover, E. (1960) The Roots of Crime. London: Imago.
Goldstein, J. (1960) ‘Police discretion not to invoke the criminal process’, Yale Law
Journal, 69: 543–94.
Goodwin, M., Johnstone, C. and Williams, K. (1998), ‘New Spaces of Law
Enforcement: Closed Circuit Television, Public Behaviour and the Policing of
Public Space’, unpublished paper, Aberystwyth: Institute of Geography and
Earth Sciences, University of Wales.
Gouldner, A. (1975) For Sociology. Harmondsworth: Pelican.
Goring C. (1913) The English Convict. London: HMSO.
Gottfredson, M. and Hirschi, T. (1990) A General Theory of Crime. Stanford CA:
Stanford University Press.
Graham, J. and Bowling, B. (1995) Young People and Crime, Home Office Research
Study No. 145. London: HMSO.
Graham, S. (1998), ‘Towards the Fifth Utility? On the Extension and Normalisation
of Public CCTV’ in C. Norris, J. Moran and G. Armstrong (eds), Surveillance,
Closed Circuit Television and Social Control, Aldershot: Ashgate.
Gresswell, D. and Hollin, C. (1994) ‘Multiple murder: a review’, British Journal of
Criminology, 34: 1–14.
186
Bibliography
Hancox, P. and Morgan, J. (1975), ‘The Use of CCTV for Police Control at Football
Matches’, Police Research Bulletin, vol. 25, pp. 41–4.
Harding, S. (ed) (1986) The Science Question in Feminism. Milton Keynes: Open
University Press.
Hare, R. D. (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health
Systems.
Healy, W. and Bronner, A. F. (1936) New Light on Delinquency and its Treatment. New
Haven, Conn: Yale University Press.
Heidensohn, F. (1985) Women and Crime. Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Heidensohn, F. (1992) Women in Control? The Role of Women in Law Enforcement.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Heidensohn, F. (1996) Women and Crime, 2nd edn. Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Henderson. S. K. (1939) Psychopathic States. New York: Norton.
Henry, S. and Milovanovic, D. (1996) Constitutive Criminology. London: Sage.
Hester, S. and Eglin, P. (1992) A Sociology of Crime. London: Routledge.
Hickey, E. W. (1986) ‘The female serial murderer’, Journal of Police and Criminal
Psychology, 2: 72–81.
Hickey, E. W. (1990) ‘The etiology of victimization in serial murder’, in Egger, S.
(ed) Serial Murder: An Elusive Phenomenon. New York: Praeger.
Hickey, E. W. (1991) Serial Murderers and Their Victims. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/
Cole.
Hickey, E. W. (1997) Serial Murderers and Their Victims, 2nd edn. Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Hirschi, T. (1969) Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Hirschi, T. (1979) ‘Separate and unequal is better’, Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency, 16: 34–8.
Hirschi, T. (1986) ‘On the compatibility of rational choice and social control
theories of crime’, in Cornish, D. and Clarke, R. (eds) The Reasoning Criminal.
New York: Springer-Verlag.
Hirst, P. (1975) ‘Marx and Engels on law, crime and morality’, in I. Taylor, P. Walton
and J. Young (eds) Critical Criminology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Holdaway, S. (ed) (1979) The British Police. London: Edward Arnold.
Holdaway, S. (1983) Inside the British Police. Oxford: Blackwell.
Hollin, C. (1989) Psychology and Crime. London: Routledge.
Holmes, R. M. and DeBurger, J. (1988) Serial Murder. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Holmes, R. M. and Holmes, S. T. (1998) Serial Murder, 2nd edn. London : Sage.
Holmes, R. M. and Holmes, S. T. (eds) (1998) Contemporary Perspectives on Serial
Murder. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Home Office (1967) Three Working Parties on Police Manpower, Equipment and
Efficiency. London: HMSO.
Home Office (1993) Police Reform: A Police Service for the Twenty-first Century.
London: HMSO.
Home Office (1994) CCTV: Looking Out For You, London: Home Office.
Home Office (1995) Review of Police Core and Ancillary Tasks. London: HMSO.
Home Office (1998) Criminal Statistics England and Wales 1997. London: The
Stationery Office.
Honess, T. and Charman, E. (1992) Closed Circuit Television in Public Places, Its
187
Introducing Criminology
Jacobs, J. (1962) The Death and Life of Great American Cities. London: Jonathan
Cape.
James, D. (1979) ‘Police-black relations: the professional solution’, in S. Holdaway
(ed) The British Police. London: Edward Arnold.
Jamieson, R. (1998) ‘Towards a criminology of war in Europe’, in V. Ruggiero, N.
South and I. Taylor (eds) The New European Criminology. London: Routledge.
Jayewardene, C. (1963) ‘The English precursors of Lombroso’, British Journal of
Criminology, 4: 164–70.
Jefferson, T. (1997) ‘Masculinities and crime’, in M. Maguire, R. Morgan, and R.
Reiner (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Criminology, 2nd edn. Oxford: Clarendon.
Jefferson T. and Walker, M. (1992) ‘Ethnic minorities in the criminal justice system’,
Criminal Law Review, 83–95.
Jefferson, T. and Walker, M. (1993) ‘Attitudes to the police of ethnic minorities in a
provincial city’, British Journal of Criminology, 33: 251–66.
Jeffery, C. R. (1965) ‘Criminal behavior and learning theory’, Journal of Criminal
Law, Criminology and Police Science, 56: 294–300.
Jenkins, P. (1988) ‘Serial murder in England 1940–1985’, Journal of Criminal Justice,
16: 1–15.
Jenkins, P. (1994) Using Murder: the Social Construction of Serial Homicide. New York:
Aldine de Gruyter.
Johnston, L. (1992) The Rebirth of Private Policing. London: Routledge.
Johnston, L. (2000) Policing Britain. Harlow: Longman.
Johnstone, G. (1996) Medical Concepts and Penal Policy. London: Cavendish.
Jones, T., MacLean, B. and Young, J. (1986) The Islington Crime Survey. Aldershot:
Gower.
Jupp, V. (1989), Methods of Criminological Research, London: Unwin Hyman.
188
Bibliography
189
Introducing Criminology
McBarnet, D. (1979) ‘Arrest: the legal context of policing’, in S. Holdaway (ed) The
British Police. London: Edward Arnold.
McBarnet, D. (1981) Conviction. London: Macmillan.
McCabe, S. and Sutcliffe, F. (1978) Defining Crime. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
McCahill, M. (1998) ‘Beyond Foucault: Towards a Contemporary Theory of
Surveillance’, in C. Norris, J. Moran and G. Armstrong (eds), Surveillance, Closed
Circuit Television and Social Control. Aldershot: Ashgate.
McCahill, M. (2000) ‘The Surveillance Web: The rise and extent of visual
surveillance in a Northern City’. Unpublished PhD, Hull: University of Hull.
Maguire, M. (1998) ‘Restraining Big Brother? The Regulation of Surveillance in
England and Wales’, in Norris, C., Moran, J. and Armstrong, G. (eds), CCTV,
Surveillance and Social Control, Aldershot: Ashgate.
Maguire, M. and Norris, C. (1992) The Conduct and Supervision of Criminal
Investigations, Royal Commission on Criminal Justice Research Report No. 5.
London: HMSO.
Mahalingham, V. (1996) ‘Sutton Town Centre Public Perception Survey’, in M.
Bulos and D. Grant (eds), Towards a Safer Sutton? CCTV: One Year On. London:
London Borough of Sutton.
Mannheim, H. (1965) Comparative Criminology, vols 1 & 2. London: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Manning, P. (1977) Police Work. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Manning P. (1982) ‘Organisational work: structuration of the environment’, British
Journal of Sociology, 33: 118–139.
Mark, R. (1977) Policing a Perplexed Society. London: Allen and Unwin.
Martinson, R. (1974), ‘What Works? Questions and Answers about Prison Reform’,
The Public Interest, 35: 22–54.
Masters, B. (1985) Killing for Company: The Case of Dennis Nilsen. London: Jonathan
Cape.
Matza, D. (1964) Delinquency and Drift. New York: Wiley.
Mawby, R. (1979) Policing the City. Farnborough: Gower.
Mednick, S., Gabrielli, W., and Hutchings, B. (1987) ‘Genetic factors in the
etiology of criminal behaviour’, in S. Mednick, T. Moffit and S. Stack (eds) The
Causes of Crime: New Biological Approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Mednick, S. and Volavka, J. (1980) ‘Biology and crime’, in N. Morris and M. Tonry
(eds) Crime and Justice, vol 2. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Merton, R. (1938) ‘Social structure and anomie’, American Sociological Review, 3:
672–82.
Merton, R. (1957) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: Free Press.
Messerschmidt, J. (1993) Masculinities and Crime. Lanham, Md: Rowman and
Littlefield.
Messner, S. and Rosenfeld, R. (1994) Crime and the American Dream. Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Michael, J. and Adler, M. (1933) Crime, Law and Social Science. New York: Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich.
Mirrlees-Black C., Budd, T., Partridge, S., and Mayhew, P. (1998) The 1998 British
Crime Survey, Home Office Statistical Bulletin, Issue 21/98, London:
Government Statistical Service.
190
Bibliography
Packer, H. (1968) The Limits of the Criminal Sanction. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Pahl, J. (1982) ‘Police response to battered women’, Journal of Social Welfare Law, 5:
337–43.
191
Introducing Criminology
Painter, K. and Tilley, N. (1999), Editors’ Introduction: Seeing and Being Seen to
Prevent Crime, in K. Painter, and N. Tilley, (eds) Crime Prevention Studies, Vol 10
pp. 1–15. Special edition entitled ‘Surveillance of Public Space: CCTV, Street
Lighting and Crime Prevention’.
Parker, H. (1974) View From the Boys. Newton Abbott: David and Charles.
Passas, N. (1990) ‘Anomie and corporate deviance’, Contemporary Crises, 14: 157–
78.
Pawson, R. and Tilley, N. (1994), ‘What Works in Evaluation Research?’, British
Journal of Criminology, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 291–306.
Pawson, R. and Tilley, N. (1997), Realistic Evaluation, London: Sage.
Pearce, F. and Tombs, S. (1992) ‘Realism and corporate crime’, in R. Matthews and
J. Young (eds) Issues in Realist Criminology. London: Sage.
Pease, K. (1997), ‘Crime Prevention’ in M. Maguire, R. Morgan and R. Reiner
(eds) The Oxford Handbook Of Criminology, second edition, Oxford: Clarendon
Press.
Pease, K. (1999) ‘A Review of Street Lighting Evaluations: Crime Reduction
Effects’, in Painter, K. and Tilley, N. (eds) Crime Prevention Studies, Vol 10 pp 47–
76. Special edition entitled ‘Surveillance of Public Space: CCTV, Street Lighting
and Crime Prevention’.
Phillips, C. and Brown, D. (1998) Entry into the Criminal Justice System: a Survey of
Police Arrests and their Outcomes, Home Office Research Study No. 185. London:
Home Office.
Phillips, C. (1999) ‘A review of CCTV Evaluations: Crime Reduction Effects and
Attitudes Towards its Use’ in Painter, K. and Tilley, N. (eds) Crime Prevention
Studies, Vol 10 pp. 123–156. Special edition entitled ‘Surveillance of Public
Space: CCTV, Street Lighting and Crime Prevention’.
Plummer, K. (1979) ‘Misunderstanding labelling perspectives’, in D. Downes and
P. Rock (eds) Deviant Interpretations. Oxford: Martin Robertson.
Pick, D. (1989) Faces of Degeneration: A European Disorder. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Police Foundation/Policy Studies Institute (1996) The Role and Responsibilities of the
Police: Report of an Independent Inquiry. London: Police Foundation/Policy
Studies Institute.
Policy Studies Institute (PSI) (1983) Police and People in London, vol IV, D. J. Smith
and J. Gray, The Police in Action. London: PSI.
Pollak, O. (1961) The Criminality of Women. New York: A. S. Barnes.
Poyner, B. (1988) ‘Video Cameras and Bus Vandalism’, Journal of Security and
Administration, no. 11, pp. 44–51.
Poyner, B. (1992) ‘Situational Crime Prevention in Two Parking Facilities’, in R.
Clarke, (ed.), Situational Crime Prevention: Successful Case Studies, New York:
Harrow and Heston.
Prentky, R., Burgess, A., Rokous, F., Lee, A., Hartman, C., Ressler, R. and Douglas, J.
(1989) ‘The presumptive role of fantasy in serial sexual homicide’, American
Journal of Psychiatry, 146: 887–91.
Punch, M. and Naylor, T. (1973) ‘The police: a secret social service’, New Society, 24:
358–61.
Raine, A. (1993) The Psychopathology of Crime. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
192
Bibliography
Reckless, W. C. (1961) The Crime Problem. New York: Appleton, Century Crofts.
Reeve, A. (1998) ‘The Panopticisation of Shopping: CCTV and Leisure
Consumption’, in C. Norris, J. Moran and G. Armstrong (eds), Surveillance,
Closed Circuit Television and Social Control, Aldershot: Ashgate.
Reiner, R. (1992) The Politics of the Police, 2nd edn. London: Wheatsheaf.
Reiss, A. (1980) ‘Police brutality’, in R. Lundman (ed) Police Behaviour. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Ressler, R., Burgess, A. and Douglas, J. (1988) Sexual Homicide: Patterns and Motives.
Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Rice, M. (1990) ‘Challenging orthodoxies in feminist theory: a black feminist
critique’, in Gelsthorpe, L. and Morris. A. (eds) Feminist Perspectives in
Criminology. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Rock, P. (1973) Deviant Behaviour. London: Hutchinson.
Rock, P. (1997) ‘Sociological theories of crime’, in M. Maguire, R. Morgan, and R.
Reiner (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Criminology, 2nd edn. Oxford: Clarendon.
Rose, D. (1996) In the Name of the Law. London: Vintage.
Roshier, B. (1989), Controlling Crime: The Classical Perspective in Criminology,
Buckingham, Open University Press.
Rotter, J. B. (1975) ‘Some problems and misconceptions related to the construct of
internal versus external control of reinforcement’, Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 43: 56–67.
Royal Commission on Criminal Justice (1993) Report. Cm 2263. London: HMSO.
Rozenberg, J. (1992) ’Miscarriages of Justice’ in E. Stockdale and S. Casale (eds)
Criminal Justice Under Stress, London: Blackstone.
Rubinstein, J. (1973) City Police. New York: Ballantine.
Rutter, M. (1972) Maternal Deprivation Reassessed. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Rutter, M. (1981) Maternal Deprivation Reassessed, 2nd edn. Harmondsworth:
Penguin.
Rutter, M. and Giller, H. (1983) Juvenile Delinquency: Trends and Perspectives.
Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Rutter, M. Giller, H. and Hagell, A. (1998) Antisocial Behavior by Young People.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sanders, A., Bridges, L., Mulvaney, A. and Crozier, G. (1989) Advice and Assistance
at Police Stations and the 24 Hour Duty Solicitor Scheme. London: Lord Chancellors
Dept.
Sanders, A. and Young, R. (1994) Criminal Justice. London: Butterworths.
Sarno, C. (1996), ‘The Impact of Closed Circuit Television on Crime in Sutton Town
Centre’, in M. Bulos and D. Grant (eds), Towards a Safer Sutton? CCTV One Year
On, London: London Borough of Sutton.
Schulsinger, F. (1977) ‘Psychopathy: heredity and environment’, in S. Mednick and
K. Christiansen (eds) Biosocial Bases of Criminal Behaviour. New York: Gardner
Press.
Schur, E. (1971) Labeling Deviant Behavior. New York: Harper and Row.
Schwendinger, H. and Schwendinger J. (1970) ‘Defenders of order or guardians of
human rights?’ Issues in Criminology, 5: 123–57.
Scraton, P. and Chadwick, K. (1991) ‘The theoretical and political priorities of
critical criminology’, in K. Stenson and D. Cowell (eds) The Politics of Crime
193
Introducing Criminology
194
Bibliography
Tame, C. (1991) ‘Freedom, responsibility and justice: the criminology of the “New
Right”’, in K. Stenson and D. Cowell (eds) The Politics of Crime Control. London:
Sage.
Tappan, P. (1947) ‘Who is the criminal?’ American Sociological Review, 12: 96–102.
Taylor, I. (1999) Crime in Context: A Critical Criminology of Market Societies. Oxford:
Polity.
Taylor, I., Walton, P. and Young, J. (1973) The New Criminology. London: Routledge
and Kegan Paul.
Taylor, I., Walton, P. and Young, J. (eds) (1975) Critical Criminology. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Thornton, D. M. and Reid, R. L. (1982) ‘Moral reasoning and the type of criminal
offence’, British Journal of Social Psychology, 21: 231–8.
195
Introducing Criminology
Van Maanen, J. (1978) ‘The asshole’, in P. Manning and J. Van Maanen (eds)
Policing: A View From the Street. Santa Monica, CA: Goodyear.
196
Bibliography
Yochelson, S. and Samenow, S. (1976) The Criminal Personality. New York: Jason
Aronson.
Young, J. (1971) The Drugtakers. London: Paladin.
Young, J. (1981) ‘Thinking seriously about crime’, in M. Fitzgerald, G. McLennan
and J. Pawson (eds) Crime and Society: Readings in History and Theory. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Young, J. (1986) ‘The failure of criminology’, in R. Matthews and J. Young (eds)
Confronting Crime. London: Sage.
Young, J. (1994) ‘Incessant chatter: current paradigms in criminology’, in M.
Maguire, R. Morgan, and R. Reiner (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Criminology.
Oxford: Clarendon.
Young, J. (1997) ‘Left realist criminology’, in M. Maguire, R. Morgan, and R. Reiner
(eds) The Oxford Handbook of Criminology, 2nd edn. Oxford: Clarendon.
Young, J. (1999) The Exclusive Society. London: Sage.
Young, M. (1991) An Inside Job. Oxford: Clarendon.
197
Introducing Criminology
Index
198
Index
199
Introducing Criminology
200
Index
201
Introducing Criminology
202