10 1371@journal Pone 0228404 PDF

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Emotion-body connection dispositions modify


the insulae-midcingulate effective
connectivity during anger processing
Viridiana Mazzola ID1,2*, Giampiero Arciero3,4, Leonardo Fazio5, Tiziana Lanciano6,
Barbara Gelao5, Alessandro Bertolino5, Guido Bondolfi1,2
1 Department of Psychiatry, Liaison Psychiatry, University Hospitals of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland,
2 Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, 3 Department
of Psychiatry, University Hospitals of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, 4 Institute of Post-Rationalist
a1111111111 Psychology IPRA, Rome, Italy, 5 Department of Basic Medical Sciences, Neuroscience and Sense Organs,
a1111111111 University of Bari, Bari, Italy, 6 Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari,
a1111111111 Bari, Italy
a1111111111
a1111111111 * [email protected]

Abstract
OPEN ACCESS The link between anger and bodily states is readily apparent based on the autonomic and
Citation: Mazzola V, Arciero G, Fazio L, Lanciano T, behavioral responses elicited. In everyday life angry people react in different ways, from
Gelao B, Bertolino A, et al. (2020) Emotion-body being agitated with an increased heart rate to remaining silent or detached. Neuroimaging
connection dispositions modify the insulae-
evidence supports the role of mid-posterior insula and midcingulate cortex/MCC as key
midcingulate effective connectivity during anger
processing. PLoS ONE 15(2): e0228404. https:// nodes of a sensorimotor network that predominantly responds to salient stimuli, integration
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 of interoceptive and autonomic information, as well as to awareness of bodily movements
Editor: Mukesh Dhamala, Georgia State University, for coordinated motion. However, there is still a lack of clarity concerning how interindividual
UNITED STATES variability in bodily states reactions drives the connectivity within these key nodes in the sen-
Received: July 17, 2019 sorimotor network during anger processing. Therefore, we investigated whether individual
differences in body-centered emotional experience, that is an active (inward prone) or inac-
Accepted: January 14, 2020
tive (outward prone) emotion-body connection disposition, would differently affect the infor-
Published: February 13, 2020
mation flow within these brain regions. Two groups of participants underwent fMRI scanning
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the session watching video clips of actors performing simple actions with angry and joyful facial
benefits of transparency in the peer review
expressions. The whole-brain group-by-session interaction analysis showed that the bilat-
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author eral insula and the right MCC were selectively activated by inward group during the angry
responses alongside final, published articles. The session, whereas the outward group activated more the precuneus during the joyful session.
editorial history of this article is available here: Accordingly, dynamic causal modeling analyses (DCM) revealed an excitatory modulatory
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404
effect exerted by anger all over the insulae-MCC connectivity in the inward group, whereas
Copyright: © 2020 Mazzola et al. This is an open in the outward group the modulatory effect exerted was inhibitory. Modeling the variability
access article distributed under the terms of the
related to individual differences in body-centered emotional experience allowed to better
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and explain to what extent subjective dispositions contributed to the insular activity and its con-
reproduction in any medium, provided the original nectivity. In addition, from the perspective of a hierarchical model of neurovisceral integra-
author and source are credited.
tion, these findings add knowledge to the multiple ways which the insula and MCC
Data Availability Statement: The University of Bari dynamically integrate affective and bodily aspects of the human experience.
Aldo Moro is not allowed to share data beyond the
terms accepted by participants in the informed
consent form at the time of recruitment. The

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 1 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

responsible ethics committee at the University of Introduction


Bari Aldo Moro is: Comitato etico indipendente |
Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Consorziale Our bodily states dynamically change along with our emotional experiences. According to the
Policlinico | Piazza Giulio Cesare 11, 70124 Bari | e- kind of the emotions in question, our bodies are differentially affected in terms of autonomic
mail: [email protected] | phone: +39 reactivity and readiness to sets of possible actions [1,2]. This becomes evident also when we
080 559 3399. Data access request can be directed consider anger, where the link between emotions and bodily states is readily apparent based
to Prof. Alessandro Bertolino (alessandro.
on the autonomic and behavioral responses elicited [3,4]. Indeed, in everyday life, angry peo-
[email protected]) and Dr. Giulio Pergola (giulio.
[email protected]). ple may begin to yell and experience the urge to throw or break things, they may feel hot and
have an increased and rapid heart rate. Others react in a different way. When angry they
Funding: The authors received no specific funding
become silent, even sarcastic, they withhold without perceiving neither heat nor an increase in
for this work.
heart rate, as if this anger-bodily states connection were almost not active. Accordingly, it is
Competing interests: The authors have declared not surprising that anger plays a role as a risk factor and trigger for acute cardiovascular events
that no competing interests exist.
in some but not all persons [5–14]. The psychology of emotions describes anger experience
variability in terms of type and relevance of triggers (who is involved and the situation in ques-
tion), autonomic involvement, duration and regulation, as well as behavioral outcome [15].
However, such an approach does not lead to a fully understanding of individual differences in
bodily states reactions in response to anger experience. At the brain level, the insula cortex
may constitute a link between cognitive, sensorimotor, and social-emotional systems in
human behavior [16]. Neuroimaging research has provided robust evidence supporting the
hub function of insula and its connections in dynamically integrating affective and bodily
aspects of the inner and outer, as well as our sense of limb ownership and limb movement
awareness [17–19]. Several results about insula connectivity showed how the mid-posterior
insula in particular is linked to motor and somatosensory cortices [20,21]. Precisely, the mid-
posterior insula together with the midcingulate cortex/MCC monitor the ongoing sensory
context in order to select the behavioral responses via skeletomotor control and orient our
body according to the salient stimuli [22–25]. Further, our previous fMRI study showed that
the right mid-posterior insula and its connectivity play a key role while facing others’ anger
[26]. Thus, the mid-posterior insulae and MCC are together key nodes of a sensorimotor net-
work that predominantly respond to salient stimuli, integrate interoceptive and autonomic
information, and increase our awareness of bodily movements for coordinated motion
[17,20,21,27–29]. Nevertheless, there is still a lack of clarity about how individual differences
in emotion-body connection would differently drive the insula-MCC causal relation during
emotional processing.
Given the main role played by the bodily states in response to anger, individual differences
in emotion-body connection during emotional experiences might be considered as an impor-
tant source of heterogeneity that challenges our understanding of the anger-bodily states con-
nection and its neural signatures. Therefore, the question arises as to whether an active or
inactive attitude in emotion-body connection would differently engage the insulae-MCC con-
nectivity when facing anger situations. To address this question, we selected two groups of par-
ticipants according to their attitude to focus primary on their bodily activations (inward
prone) or on contexts or people (outward prone) to make sense of the ongoing situation when
emotionally involved [30–33]. Our concept of dispositional affective styles emphasizes the
need to account for the way in which each person, in dealing with others and the different cir-
cumstances of everyday life, feels situated in the environment. Within this perspective, two
general dispositional affective styles can be defined: primarily based on basic emotions (inward
disposition) or primarily based on emotions which are co-perceived through others (non-
basic emotions) (outward disposition) [2,34,35]. On one hand, inward subjects tend to be
more body-centered, i.e. viscerally aware, more sensitive in the detection of changes in bodily
states occurring during emotional experience [32,33]. On the other hand, during emotional

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 2 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

experience outward subjects tend to be more context-centered, i.e. externally aware, focusing
primarily towards contexts, people or rules and norms [32,33].
In the present study, all the participants faced angry and joyful situations during the fMRI
scanning session. They watched two runs of video clips of an actor and an actress performing a
daily act like grasping an object with different emotional expressions (neutral, joyful, or angry)
[26,36]. One run included only neutral and joyful expressions, while the other featured only
neutral and angry expressions. We compared the contextual modulation of connection
strengths within the insulae-MCC connectivity elicited by the emotional situations in the two
groups using dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analyses. For this purpose, we created an archi-
tecture that served as our base model to test how the two emotional acts were more likely to
exert an excitatory or an inhibitory effect within our target connection. Then, we obtained sta-
tistical estimates of which model offered the optimum balance between simplicity and fit to
the data using Fixed Effects Bayesian Model Selection family inference analysis (FFX BMS)
[37], followed by the model parameter estimates analysis Bayesian Parameters Averaging
(BPA) (see Methods). The resting state data were acquired before the task in order to check for
any non task-related difference between groups in the mid-posterior insulae-MCC connectiv-
ity. Spectral DCM analyses of resting state data were performed according to the winning
model structure [38].
Based on the evidence mentioned, we hypothesized that the mid-posterior insulae-MCC
connectivity would be differently engaged by the priors for one’s emotion-body connection
disposition during emotional processing. In particular, we expected that anger vs joy would
have a stronger excitatory modulatory effect on connectivity within the mid-posterior insulae-
MCC in the inward disposition than in the outward one.

Methods
Participants
The sample was drawn from two larger cohorts of people who participated in a previous psy-
chometric study according to the In-Out dispositional affective style questionnaire/IN-OUT
DASQ and the MRI scanning criteria [33]. Indeed, the current participants were classified as
higher inward or higher outward prone depending on their highest scores given to the
IN-OUT DASQ subscales, the Self-centric engagement for the inwardness and Other-centric
engagement for the outwardness [33]. In addition, a semi-structured interview was adminis-
tered independently by two trained investigators (GA, TL) who were blind to each other’s
results. Two groups of fifteen participants each were enrolled. Due to technical problems, a
participant of the outward group was excluded. In order to better balance the two groups we
excluded one participant of the inward group matched for gender and age. The final two
groups were composed by fourteen participants each (Inward group: 7 females; mean age
27.36; standard deviation [SD] 2.92, range = 22–33; Outward group: 8 females; mean age
26.50; standard deviation [SD] 3.5, range = 22–34). Exclusion criteria included a history of
drug or alcohol abuse, previous head trauma with loss of consciousness, pregnancy, and any
significant medical or psychiatric conditions as evaluated with the Structured Clinical Inter-
view (SCID). All the participants had more than 16 years of schooling. After the semi-struc-
tured interview, participants were asked to describe the way they usually get angry, namely,
what is the trigger (situations), the level of involvement of bodily states in terms of higher-
medium-low intensity, how long their anger lasts and how it ends (duration and regulation).
All the participants completed the State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) [39] in
order to control for their general tendency to experience anger. Before the scanning session,
each participant also completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [40] to evaluate their

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 3 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

current state of anxiety in addition to the other questionnaires. The present study was
approved by the Comitato Etico Indipendente Locale of the Azienda Ospedaliera “Ospedale
Policlinico Consorziale” in Bari, Italy. Informed written consent was obtained from all partici-
pants before participation. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guide-
lines and regulations.

Self-report questionnaires analyses


Questionnaire data were analyzed using SPSS 23 (Inc., 2009, Chicago, USA). Mann-Whitney
U tests between groups were performed to test for the group differences effect on DASQ,
STAXI, and between males and females on the STAXI measures to test the gender effect. We
reported the exact significance level (two-tailed). In order to test for any significant correlation
between DASQ, STAXI, we performed Spearman’s rho correlation analyses. We reported the
exact significance level (two-tailed).

fMRI task
The fMRI task employed was described in detail in other previous studies [26,36]. Briefly, the
functional MRI session consisted of two successive scanning runs with an event-related design.
Each run included one emotion (joy or anger), plus the neutral facial expression, and consisted
of four experimental view conditions. All visual stimuli consisted of video clips of 1.3 second.
There were 4 different video conditions in each run, showing the following actions: the trunk
with an arm grasping an object on a table (acting alone), a person with a neutral facial expres-
sion grasping an object on a table (neutral acting), a person with a joyful or angry facial expres-
sion grasping an object on a table (joyful acting in the joyful run or angry acting in the angry
run), and a joyful or angry dynamic facial expression without any acting action (joyful face in
the joyful run or angry face or in the angry run). The recording and editing of videos were
made using the Blue Screen technique in order to superimpose on the same trunk different
emotional facial expressions. Thereby, we investigated the brain activity elicited by the obser-
vation of someone acting in angry and joyful situations, while keeping action kinematics con-
stant. Two professional actors, a female and a male, were enrolled as models for the videos.
The everyday objects to be grasped were put on a table, e.g. phone, pen, keys, bottle, cup, and
glass. To circumvent any motor interference, we used a passive viewing task and participants
were instructed to remain still without performing any movement, and to avoid any imitation
or mental imagery of the actions shown, and to carefully look at the video clips in order to get
involved. The presentation order of the two runs was counterbalanced across subjects. In each
scanning run, there were 160 visual stimuli presented in random order, with an average 1810
ms interstimulus interval (ISI). 48 additional null events, each lasting 2700 ms, contributed to
randomly jitter the stimulus onsets. Total scanning run time was about 19 minutes.

fMRI data acquisition and analyses


Three-dimensional images were acquired using a T1-weighted SPGR sequence (TR/TE/
NEX = 25 s/3 ms/1; flip angle 6˚; matrix size 256×256; FOV 25×25 cm) with 124 sagittal slices
(1.3 mm thick, in-plane resolution of 0.94×0.94). Resting state and task-related fMRI data
were acquired on a 3T GE (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI) MRI scanner with a gradient-
echo echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence and covered 26 interleaved axial slices (5 mm thick,
1mm gap), encompassing the entire cerebrum and the cerebellum (TR 2 s; FOV 24 cm; matrix,
64 x 64, a voxel size of 3.75x3.75x5 mm). A total of 150 EPI volume images were acquired for
the resting state, whereas for each scan of the task, a total of 285 EPI volume images.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 4 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Preprocessing
Data were preprocessed and analyzed using statistical parametric mapping SPM12 (Wellcome
Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK), implemented in MatLab R2014b
(MathWorksTM). A fixed-effect model at a single-subject level was performed to create images
of parameter estimates, which were then entered into a second-level random-effects analysis.
For each subject, functional images were first slice-timing corrected, using the middle slice
acquired in time as a reference, and then spatially corrected for head movement, using a least-
squares approach and six-parameter rigid body spatial transformations. High-resolution ana-
tomical T1 images were coregistered with the realigned functional images to enable anatomical
localization of the activations. The two runs were then entered as multiple sessions as imple-
mented in SPM12. Structural and functional images were spatially normalized into a standard-
ized anatomical framework using the default EPI template provided in SPM12, based on the
averaged-brain of the Montreal Neurological Institute and approximating the normalized
probabilistic spatial reference frame of Talairach and Tournoux [http://brainmap.org/
icbm2tal/]. Functional images were spatially smoothed with a three-dimensional Gaussian fil-
ter (10mm full-width at half-maximum). The time series was temporally filtered to eliminate
contamination from slow drift of signals (high-pass filter, 128 s) and corrected for autocorrela-
tions using the AR(1) model in SPM12.

General linear model


We performed two parallel but identical statistical analyses on the functional data for the
whole-brain and cerebellar normalized images. Four event-types were defined per subject per
scanning run, corresponding to each condition of interest. In the joyful run, the conditions of
interest were: acting alone, neutral acting, joyful acting, joyful face. In the angry run, the condi-
tions were: acting alone, neutral acting, angry acting, angry face. Eight contrast images corre-
sponding to these conditions from individual participants were entered at the second level
into a repeated-measures 2 (groups) x 2 (sessions) x 4 (conditions) ANOVA (flexible factorial
design implemented in SPM12). We reported regions that survived a threshold of P < 0.05
cluster-level FWE corrected, cluster size Ke = 8, as implemented in SPM12. Cerebral MNI
coordinates were converted to the Talairach coordinate system by icbm2tal [http://brainmap.
org/icbm2tal/]. Anatomic and Brodmann areas labeling of cerebral activated clusters was per-
formed with the Talairach Daemon database [http://www.talairach.org/] and SPM Anatomy
Toolbox [41].

Cerebellar normalization
We used a separate normalization process for data from the cerebellum. The registration
between individuals and MNI space is suboptimal in the cerebellum when using a standard
whole-brain normalization process [42]. Because cerebellum vary relatively little between indi-
viduals compared with the cortical landmarks used for whole-brain normalization, it is possi-
ble to achieve a much better registration by normalizing the cerebella separately. Moreover,
precise spatial registration is important because cerebellar structures are small compared to
cortical structures. To this aim, we used the SUIT toolbox [42] for SPM12 allowing us to nor-
malize each individual’s structural scan to an infratentorial template, and then used the result-
ing deformation maps to normalize the cerebellar sections of each person’s functional images.
The SUIT toolbox has the additional advantage that coordinates can be adjusted from MNI
space to the corresponding coordinates on the unnormalized Colin-27 brain, which is
described anatomically in a cerebellar atlas. We used this feature to identify anatomical regions
within the cerebellum.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 5 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Dynamic causal modeling


VOI extraction. Volumes of interest (VOIs) were defined bilaterally in the insula and in
the MCC based on the sensorimotor network [22,25]. The centers of the VOIs were identified
as the peaks of sensorimotor independent component map. To identify these coordinates, a
spatial independent component analysis (ICA) was performed in the resting state on a joint
group with all the participants using the Group ICA of fMRI Toolbox (GIFT) (http://icatb.
sourceforge.net/). GICA3 was used for back-reconstruction type and 20 components were
extracted. The resulting independent component maps were verified by visual inspection to
identify the sensorimotor network. The regions included the right insula (centered at MNI
space 42, -18, 4), the left insula (centered at MNI space -42, -18, 2), the right MCC (centered at
MNI space 4, 4, 28), and the left MCC (centered at 0, −2, 34). For each subject, the VOIs were
defined as spheres centered at those coordinates mentioned above with a 6 mm radius. This
procedure ensures that cross-spectral density and of task-related DCM analyses were per-
formed on the same VOIs coordinates identified as a temporally coherent network.
Specification of model architecture. All the DCM analyses were performed with the
DCM12 routine implemented in SPM12. We used dynamic causal modeling (DCM) to exam-
ine between-groups differences in insula-MCC effective connectivity, i.e. the impact that activ-
ity in one region exerts over another [43]. More specifically, the DCM explains changes in
neuronal population dynamics as a function of the network’s connectivity (endogenous con-
nectivity) and regional effects in terms of the changing patterns of connectivity among regions
according to the experimentally induced contextual modulation of connection strengths.
Accordingly, we examined the mutual influences within this brain regions involved both in
the angry vs joyful session using DCM. Our DCM design matrices comprised a regressor
modeling of the sensory input (all conditions in each session), and a regressor modeling of the
effect of the emotional acting condition. We grouped into three families all the models accord-
ing the following connections: RINS$MCC$LINS, RINS$LINS$MCC as a right top-
down family, LINS$MCC$RINS, LINS$RINS$MCC as a left top-down family, and
MCC$RINS$LINS, MCC$LINS$RINS as a bottom-up family. For each architecture we
constructed 17 different models with bidirectional connections allowing experimental modu-
lation on all paths and regions, resulting 102 possible DCMs per emotional session per side of
MCC (Fig 1). In all models, the first region received all the conditions of the session as a driv-
ing input. In order to answer our research questions, we varied the regions and connections
that were affected by the emotional (angry and joyful) acting across the models (Fig 1).
Model comparison. Inference on family structure was performed using Fixed Effects fam-
ily inference analysis (FFX BMS) [37]. The model’s Free Energy, F, a lower bound of the mod-
el’s log-evidence, accounting for model complexity as well as data fit, was used to compare the
likelihood of the different models to explain the data. Relative log-evidences, or differences in
F, were converted into model posterior probabilities, p, indicating that the respective family
has a probability p of being the best family/model explaining the data amongst all considered.
Evidence was “strong” if p>0.95, which stands for a difference in F greater than 3, and “posi-
tive” if 0.75<p<0.95, which stands for a difference of F between 1 and 3 [37,44]. Secondly,
inference on the optimal model parameters was performed. The structure of the connectivity
model was assumed to be the same for both sequences and a FFX analysis of the model param-
eter estimates was performed using Bayesian Parameters Averaging (BPA) [45–47].
Since spectral DCM has been found to be more accurate and more sensitive to group differ-
ences compared to stochastic DMC [38], DCM of cross spectral density analyses of the win-
ning model were performed to control for any significant differences in connectivity strength
between groups at resting state [38]. A spectral DCM with all endogenous connectivity

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 6 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Fig 1. The models tested with Bayesian model selection. Numbers 1, 2, 3 refer to the regions of interest. The dashed lines indicate the modulatory influences of
emotional acting on connectivity among right insula (rINS), left insula (lINS), and right/left MCC (MCC).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404.g001

specified (full model) was built for each participant. The BPA approach was adopted to obtain
model parameters for each group, separately. Then, a two sample T-test was performed on
group parameters to check for any significant difference.

Results
Self-report questionnaires
The two groups significantly differed in the Self-centric engagement/SCE scale scores. Indeed,
the inward group scored significantly higher (W 141, p<0.05, mean difference 6, SE 2.76, d’
0.646) (Table 1). This result was consistent with the semi-structured interview classification.
According to the STAXI assessment of the experience, expression and control of the anger, the
two groups showed only a different tendency to control the anger expression. Indeed, the
inward group scored higher on the Anger control scale (W 139.5, p<0.05, mean difference 3,

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 7 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Table 1. Self-report questionnaires.


INWARD OUTWARD
Scale Mean SD Mean SD
Self-centric engagement 27.21 5.01 22.50 9.019
Other-centric engagement 29.64 6.08 27.50 7.623
State-anger 15.43 6.11 12.57 3.5
Trait-anger 20.50 4.64 22.28 5.45
Trait-anger-temperament 6.71 1.86 6.86 2.85
Trait-anger-response 9.85 2.62 11.36 2.37
Anger-in 19.36 2.73 18.50 5.27
Anger-out 14.57 2.85 14.92 4.99
Anger control 26.64 3.71 22.43 6.32
Anger expression 23.28 6.14 27.00 9.56
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404.t001

SE 1.95, d’ 0.813). Consistently, the inwards’ SCE scores correlated negatively with the inward
Anger control (r = -0.68 p<0.001), and positively with the Anger expression (r = 0.71
p<0.001), and the Trait-anger-temperament (r = 0.76 p<0.001). Taken together, these results
suggest that the inward dispositional affective style revealed a different profile in anger expres-
sion. Indeed, the inward participants showed a tendency to be more angered and, accordingly,
to control their anger expression. These results were consistent with the usual way of getting
angry described by all the inward participants. Actually, at the apex of their own anger, they
reported to perceive a general involvement of their body (in terms of the urge to act also
against the person/situation, increasing transpiration and heart beating, feeling hot) that lasted
for a time interval of between 30 minutes and 2 hours according to the relevance of the epi-
sode. On the other hand, the outward participants referred that they usually did not get angry
but rather irritated, at the peak they did not report to perceive any significant change in bodily
states, and the peak lasted from 2 to 15 minutes in the most relevant episodes.

Neuroimaging Results
Whole-brain fMRI results. Across sessions, the inward group activated the right mPFC,
the bilateral ACC, the left MCC, and left insula more, as well as the right VII lobule crus I and
II (P<0.05 FWE corrected) (Table 2, Fig 2A). On the other hand, the outward participants
showed a greater involvement of visual areas, right precuneus, right precentral, right parahip-
pocampus and the right anterior cerebellum (P<0.05 FWE corrected) (Table 2, Fig 2B). Across
groups, the angry session elicited higher activity in bilateral superior frontal gyrus and the
right posterior insula (P<0.05 FWE corrected) (Table 2). In contrast, no activation reached
the statistical significance in the reverse contrasts.
To address whether the processing of anger impacts on the activity of the network of
salience and action link system, we carried out a group x session interaction. According to our
predictions, we found greater activation in the salience-action link system during the angry
session than the joyful one. Indeed, the bilateral mid-posterior insula and the right MCC were
engaged by the angry session along with the left caudate, right hippocampus, left parahippo-
campus, and left precuneus (P<0.05 FWE cluster-level corrected) (Table 3). Namely, the activ-
ity in the bilateral mid-posterior insula and the right MCC was mainly driven by the inward
group. On the other hand, the right mid-posterior insula and the left precuneus activations
were enhanced by the joyful session in the outward group.
Group x emotional acting interaction also revealed a differential activity. Indeed, the angry
session elicited activity in the left claustrum, the right putamen and caudate, right STG, right

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 8 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Table 2. Whole-brain general linear model main effects analyses, P<0.05 FEW. Inward group > Outward group across sessions.
MNI coordinates
Region x y z Ke Z Scores
Right Medial Prefrontal Cortex BA10 24 44 10 276 5.9
Right ACC BA32 14 34 8 5.89
Right IPL BA40 32 -42 46 61 5.56
Left ACC BA32 -20 40 10 201 5.43
Left MCC BA24 -10 -16 36
Left Insula BA13 -34 16 18 20 5.38
Left Claustrum -24 22 0 20 5.23
Right Superior Temporal Gyrus BA22 54 -32 2 26 5.22
L Pallidum -16 0 -2 17 5.1
Left Cingulate Cortex BA32 -14 16 36 61 5.04
Right VIIa crusII 42 -54 -51 699 5.36
Right VIIa crusI 48 -48 -35 363 5.81
Left VIIIa lobule -40 -48 -61 352 4.86
Outward group > Inward group across sessions
Right Lingual Gyrus BA18 16 -72 -2 Inf 332
Left Lingual Gyrus BA18 -10 -80 2 222 6.24
Right Middle Occipital gyrus BA37 38 -64 6 128 6.09
Right Fusiform FG2 40 -70 -20 66 5.89
Right Amygdala 18 2 -16 48 5.7
Right Calcarine Cortex BA18 4 -76 16 49 5.48
Right Precentral Gyrus BA4 24 -22 66 9 4.89
Right Middle Frontal Gyrus BA46 34 -2 58 11 4.82
Right Parahippocampal Gyrus BA28 16 -12 -14 8 4.78
Right IV lobule 30 -38 -23 236 4.33
Angry > Joyful sessions across groups
Left Superior Frontal Gyrus BA10 -20 50 -2 178 4.82
Right Superior Frontal Gyrus BA10 18 50 -6 133 4.68
Right Insula Ig2 30 -24 12 86 4.56
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404.t002

paracentral lobule, and right thalamus (P<0.05 FWE cluster-level corrected) (Table 3). Specifi-
cally, the activity in the right mid-insula was mainly driven by the inward group as well as the
right MCC (Fig 2C and 2D).
Consistent with our a priori hypothesis, these results support the view that facing others’ anger
enhances activity within the mid-posterior insulae-MCC network in the inward participants.

Effective connectivity
Next, we applied DCM analyses to investigate the causal architecture of insulae-MCC effective
connectivity that may account for group differences in processing of others’ emotions. Among
the models constructed with the right MCC, the right top-down family was the winning in the
inward group during the angry session with a posterior probability (Pp) of.99. The optimal
model showed a positive modulation by angry acting on the forward connection from the
right mid-posterior insula both in the left mid-posterior insula and the right MCC, and on the
backward connection from the right MCC to the left mid-posterior insula, as well as a direct
slight negative effect on the right MCC (Fig 3A) (Pp = .99). This winning model reveals note-
worthy findings on the information flow when inward participants processed angry people.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 9 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Fig 2. (A) Inward group > Outward group across sessions and (B) Outward group > Inward group across sessions, whole-brain analysis between groups P<0.05 FWE;
(C-D) The peak signal changes that occurred in the right posterior insula and right MCC in groups x emotional acting interaction results (P<0.05 FWE cluster-level
corrected). (E-F) Contrast estimates and 90% confidence intervals in the right posterior insula and in the right MCC presented for visualization purpose.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404.g002

First, it revealed the central role played by the right insular as input when facing an anger situa-
tion. Indeed, angry acting had an excitatory modulatory effect from the right mid-posterior
insula both to the left mid-posterior insula and the right MCC. Secondly, the right MCC
played the role of the output node in the winning model. Moreover, the right MCC backward
connection to the left insula was positively modulated by angry acting.
On the other hand, the winning family in the outward group was the left top-down one (Pp
= .99) in the angry session, showing that the input role was played by the left insula (Fig 3D).
The optimal model showed a negative modulation by angry acting on the backward connec-
tion from the right MCC to the left insula and from the right insula to the left insula, as well as
a direct negative effect on the right MCC (Pp = .99). This winning model revealed a different
information flow in the outward group. Indeed, the main modulatory effect of the angry acting
was to inhibit the input region, i.e. the left insula, and the right MCC.
Among the models constructed with the left MCC, the left top-down family won in the
inward group when facing anger (Pp = 1) (Fig 3B). The optimal model revealed that the angry

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 10 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Table 3. Whole-brain general linear model interaction analyses, P<0.05 FWE. Groups x sessions.
MNI coordinates
Region x y z Ke Z Scores
Right Hippocampus CA1 36 -28 -16 4491 5.76
Right ACC BA32 12 30 -10 5.21
Right Insula BA13 34 -22 12 436 4.69
Left Insula BA13 -32 -24 8 114 4.34
Left Caudate -20 26 0 899 5.48
Left Parahippocampal Gyrus BA37 -36 -36 -6 1816 5.45
Right MCC BA24 14 2 38 947 462
Right Calcarine Cortex BA31 16 -64 16 166 4.5
Right Precuneus BA7 14 -62 42 323 4.12
Groups x emotional acting interaction
Left Claustrum -22 26 0 448 5.07
Right Superior Temporal Gyrus BA41 36 -32 6 865 5.01
Right Insula Ig1 34 -24 10
Right Parahippocampal Gyrus CA1 34 -26 -16
Left Parahippocampal Gyrus BA37 -36 -36 -4 686 4.31
Right Putamen 28 -18 2
Right MCC BA24 14 0 38 333 5
Right Paracentral lobule BA6 12 -30 54 460 4.61
Right Precuneus BA5 10 -44 58
Right Lingual Gyrus BA30 16 -40 -2 278 4.57
Right Thalamus 10 -28 -2 3.57
Right Caudate 2 10 0 271 4.47
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404.t003

acting positively modulated the backward connection from the right insula to the left one, and
negatively from the left MCC to the left insula, with a direct slight negative effect on the right
insula (Pp = .99). This model showed that there was not any reciprocal influence between the
right insula and left MCC in the inward group. In the outward group, the winning family was
the right top-down, but no one model reached the probability threshold above chance.
The joyful session elicited an opposite information flow compared to the angry one in the
inward group. Indeed, among the models with the right MCC, the winning family was the bot-
tom-up (Pp = 1). The optimal model showed an inhibitory modulatory effect exerted by joyful
acting from the RMCC to the right insula and a positive one to the left insula, as well from the
left insula to the right one, and a direct inhibitory effect on the left insula (Pp = .81) (Fig 3C).
In the outward group the winning family was the right top-down (Pp = 1), whereas no one
model related to the joyful session reached the probability threshold above chance in the out-
ward group. Among the left MCC models, in both the groups the winning family was the left
top-down (Pp = 1) and no one model reached the probability threshold above chance.
DCM cross-spectral analyses revealed that the two groups did not differ significantly con-
cerning the strength of connections in the winning models during the resting-state.

Discussion
Given the interindividual variability in bodily states reactions when facing anger, our research
question concerned whether the angry vs joyful context differently affected the mid-posterior
insulae-MCC connectivity according to individual differences in emotion-body connection.
To this aim, we enrolled participants who had two distinct priors for one’s emotion-body

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 11 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

Fig 3. The winning models. Black dotted lines indicate input into the system by all conditions; grey lines indicate fixed connectivity; black bold lines and circles indicate
the modulatory effect of emotional acting. Ep.B: connection strength index; Pp: posterior probability.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404.g003

connection disposition. Indeed, the inward disposition focuses more on bodily state changes
when they are affectively engaged, while the outward disposition focuses on the external world
and people. During the fMRI scanning session, the participants watched video clips of actors
doing an ordinary action like grasping objects from a table in joyful and angry contexts.
The DCM analyses here employed allowed us to provide a more systematic and nuanced
directional depiction of the connectivity between the mid-posterior insulae and the MCC in
our two groups. The present findings let us point out two major aspects. First, the winning
model in the inward group revealed that angry acting spread an excitatory modulatory effect
all over the forward connections from the right mid-posterior insula and on the right MCC.
On the other hand, the DCM model that better explained the modulatory effect of the angry
acting in the outward group depicted an opposite information flow. It spread an inhibitory
modulatory effect all over the backward connections to the input, i.e. the left mid-posterior
insula, and on the right MCC. These findings confirmed our expectation about the different

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 12 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

involvement of these key nodes within the sensorimotor network in our groups when process-
ing anger situation.
Second, in the inward group the angry acting effect modulated more selectively the right-
sided ipsilateral MCC-insula connectivity, whereas in the outward group the contralateral one,
that is right MCC-left insula. This reveals that the right-sided ipsilateral insula-MCC causal
relation had been prioritized by the inward disposition when processing anger-related situa-
tion, unraveling the neural mechanism underlying the inward emotion-body connection.
Consistently, under joyful acting the modulatory effect was reversed in the inward group.
Indeed, the joyful acting spread from the right MCC an inhibitory effect on the right insula.
Indeed, this depiction of connectivity showed a shift in the emotion-body connection activa-
tion in the inward group.
The whole-brain analyses also supported our hypothesis about a different affective engage-
ment between the two groups. Indeed, the inward group showed across sessions a general
involvement of more limbic areas, whereas the outward group more visual areas. Accordingly,
the group-by-session interaction analysis showed that the bilateral insula and the right MCC
were selectively activated by the inward group during the angry session. On the other hand,
the activity in the precuneus was mainly driven by the outward group. This result is consistent
with the inward-outward affective engagement as revealed by our previous study on pain
empathy [32].
Taken together, these findings reveal that an active or inactive emotion-body connection
disposition modulates differently the causal relation within the insulae-MCC in response to
angry and joyful situations, highlighting a different neural implementation of emotional pro-
cessing. Clearly, in people prone to be more body-centered, when exposed to others’ anger, the
information flow started from a hierarchically higher cortical level, the right mid-posterior
insula, and ended in a lower subordinate cortical level, the right MCC. This right-sided insula-
MCC downward connectivity may be referred to the anger-brain-body connection described
by these participants at a peak of their angry behavioral level as an ‘urge to act’. Indeed, both
these regions are directly involved in skeletomotor body orientation and response selection
[17,21,27]. In addition, as a visceromotor limbic cortex, the right MCC is directly implied as
the output in the ‘urge to act’ feeling elicited by anger, after having received excitatory input
from the right mid-posterior insula [48–50]. Furthermore, both the mid-posterior insula and
the MCC are key regions within the central autonomic network (CAN) [51], playing a direct
role in sympathetic regulation [52]. Assuming the lateralized hypothesis of insular autonomic
control, according to which the right insula elicits more sympathetic cardiovascular reactions,
e.g. tachycardia and hypertension, whereas the left side elicits more the parasympathetic reac-
tions, e.g. bradycardia and hypotension [53–57], these results could be consistent with a gen-
eral increased sympathetic response elicited by anger in inward participants. Unfortunately,
the lack of physiological measures concerning autonomic activity acquired during the scan-
ning session is a limitation of the present study that cannot let us further address this
interpretation.
Consistent with our previous fMRI study, the present study confirmed a primary role of the
right insula when processing angry situation [26]. In addition, modeling the variability related
to individual differences in body-centered emotional experience allowed us to better explain
the subjective affective dispositions contribution to the insular activity and connectivity. This
is an important contribution if we consider to what extent psycholgical factors may adversely
affect other medical conditions by, for instance, influencing the underlying pathophysiology to
precipitate or exacerbate symptoms, as in the case of chronic pain. As a matter of fact, the
insula-MCC connectivity plays the primary role as a gate for nociceptive hypersensitivity, as
well as the intrinsic connectivity between insula-MCC at the resting state is also altered in

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 13 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

patients affected by chronic pain [58,59]. Moreover, in line with the concept of precision medi-
cine, according to which the prevention and treatment strategies has to take individual vari-
ability into account, the present findings may be useful in personalizing anger management in
several clinical domains where anger effects are known to be detrimental [9,12,13,60].
To sum up, the depiction of the causal relation between the insulae-MCC drawn out by
angry vs joyful situations in the inward group was consistent with their behavioral attitude as
revealed by both the STAXI scores and the description of their own anger experience. Indeed,
what emerged as a group effect was their tendency to get angry and consequently to control
their anger expression more, intertwined with a lasting bodily involvement that clearly resem-
bles a sympathetic-like response. It is worth noting that there was no significant difference
between the two groups in high-low trait anger scores. Thus, these differences in attitude
toward anger between groups, outlined by the semi-structured interview and supported by the
questionnaires, were observed also at a neural level within the sensorimotor network connec-
tivity. It implies that the sensorimotor network, or at least part of it, is dynamically involved
according to the ongoing emotional situation and the kind of affective disposition. At the same
time, we acknowledge that due the sample size further investigations are required.
In conclusion, the results show a different neural implementation of anger vs joyful process-
ing within the insulae-MCC effective connectivity in accordance with individual differences in
emotion-body connection dispositions. Indeed, a prior disposition to be more external context-
or body-centered during emotional experiences differently affects the information flow within
these key nodes of the sensorimotor network. This leads us to remark that the dispositional
affective styles here considered open to a much more fine-grained understanding of the vari-
ability of the anger experience, unveiling other facets of the anger-brain-body connection phe-
nomenon. Finally, in the perspective of a hierarchical model of neurovisceral integration
[51,61,62], the dimensions of inward and outward emotion-body connection dispositions as a
priors add knowledge to help understand the multiple ways of the insula and its connections to
dynamically integrate affective and bodily states of the human experience.

Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Viridiana Mazzola, Giampiero Arciero.
Data curation: Viridiana Mazzola, Giampiero Arciero, Leonardo Fazio, Tiziana Lanciano,
Barbara Gelao.
Formal analysis: Viridiana Mazzola.
Investigation: Leonardo Fazio, Tiziana Lanciano, Barbara Gelao.
Methodology: Viridiana Mazzola.
Resources: Alessandro Bertolino.
Supervision: Alessandro Bertolino, Guido Bondolfi.
Writing – original draft: Viridiana Mazzola, Giampiero Arciero.
Writing – review & editing: Viridiana Mazzola, Giampiero Arciero.

References
1. Frijda NH, Kuipers P, Schure E. Relations Among Emotion, Appraisal, and Emotional Action Readiness.
1989; 57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212
2. Arciero G, Bondolfi G. Selfhood, identity and personality styles. Wiley-Blackwell; 2009.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 14 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

3. Stemmler G, Wacker J. Personality, emotion, and individual differences in physiological responses. Biol
Psychol. 2010; 84: 541–551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.012 PMID: 19800934
4. Garfinkel SN, Zorab E, Navaratnam N, Engels M, Mallorquı́-Bagué N, Minati L, et al. Anger in brain and
body: The neural and physiological perturbation of decision-making by emotion. Soc Cogn Affect Neu-
rosci. 2015; 11: 150–158. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv099 PMID: 26253525
5. Lampert R, Joska T, Burg MM, Batsford WP, McPherson CA, Jain D. Emotional and physical precipi-
tants of ventricular arrhythmia. Circulation. 2002; 106: 1800–1805. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.cir.
0000031733.51374.c1 PMID: 12356633
6. Craig AD. Forebrain emotional asymmetry: A neuroanatomical basis? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005; 9: 566–
571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.005 PMID: 16275155
7. Suls J, Bunde J. Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: the problems
and implications of overlapping affective dispositions. Psychol Bull. 2005; 131: 260–300. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.260 PMID: 15740422
8. Suls J. Anger and the Heart: Perspectives on cardiac risk, mechanisms and interventions. Prog Cardio-
vasc Dis. 2013; 55: 538–547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcad.2013.03.002 PMID: 23621963
9. Taggart P, Boyett MR, Logantha SJRJ, Lambiase PD. Anger, emotion, and arrhythmias: From brain to
heart. Front Physiol. 2011; 2 OCT: 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2011.00001
10. Steptoe A, Kivimaki M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2012; 9: 360–370. https://
doi.org/10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45 PMID: 22473079
11. Edmondson D, Newman JD, Whang W, Davidson KW. Emotional triggers in myocardial infarction: Do
they matter? Eur Heart J. 2013; 34: 300–306. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehs398 PMID:
23178642
12. Mostofsky E, Penner EA, Mittleman MA. Outbursts of anger as a trigger of acute cardiovascular events:
A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Heart J. 2014; 35: 1404–1410. https://doi.org/10.1093/
eurheartj/ehu033 PMID: 24591550
13. Buckley T, Hoo SYS, Fethney J, Shaw E, Hanson PS, Tofler GH. Triggering of acute coronary occlusion
by episodes of anger. Eur Hear J Acute Cardiovasc Care. 2015; 2048872615568969. https://doi.org/
10.1177/2048872615568969 PMID: 25713468
14. Lampert R. Behavioral influences on cardiac arrhythmias. Trends Cardiovasc Med. 2016; 26: 68–77.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tcm.2015.04.008 PMID: 25983071
15. Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber LB. Emotional inertia and psychological maladjustment. Psychol Sci.
2010; 21: 984–91. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610372634 PMID: 20501521
16. Kurth F, Zilles K, Fox PT, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB. A link between the systems: functional differentiation
and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct. 2010; 1–16.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0255-z PMID: 20512376
17. Farrer C, Franck N, Georgieff N, Frith CD, Decety J, Jeannerod M. Modulating the experience of
agency: A positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage. 2003; 18: 324–333. https://doi.org/10.
1016/s1053-8119(02)00041-1 PMID: 12595186
18. (Bud) Craig AD. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci.
2009; 10: 59–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555 PMID: 19096369
19. Baier B, Karnath HO. Tight link between our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions.
Stroke. 2008; 39: 486–488. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.495606 PMID: 18162622
20. Cauda F, D’Agata F, Sacco K, Duca S, Geminiani G, Vercelli A. Functional connectivity of the insula in
the resting brain. Neuroimage. 2011; 55: 8–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.049
PMID: 21111053
21. Cauda F, Costa T, Torta DME, Sacco K, D’Agata F, Duca S, et al. Meta-analytic clustering of the insular
cortex. Characterizing the meta-analytic connectivity of the insula when involved in active tasks. Neuro-
image. 2012; 62: 343–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.012 PMID: 22521480
22. Taylor KS, Seminowicz DA, Davis KD. Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula
and cingulate cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009; 30: 2731–2745. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20705
PMID: 19072897
23. Karnath HO, Baier B. Right insula for our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Brain
Struct Funct. 2010; 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0250-4 PMID: 20512380
24. Deen B, Pitskel NB, Pelphrey KA. Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster
analysis. Cereb Cortex. 2011; 21: 1498–1506. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq186 PMID: 21097516
25. Dosenbach NU et al. Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science (80-). 2010; 329: 1358–
1361. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1194144 PMID: 20829489

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 15 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

26. Mazzola V, Arciero G, Fazio L, Lanciano T, Gelao B, Popolizio T, et al. What impact does an angry con-
text have upon us? The effect of anger on functional connectivity of the right insula and superior tempo-
ral gyri. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016; 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00109 PMID: 27375449
27. Christopher L, Koshimori Y, Lang AE, Criaud M, Strafella AP. Uncovering the role of the insula in non-
motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2014; 137: 2143–2154. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/
awu084 PMID: 24736308
28. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic con-
nectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007; 27: 2349–2356.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007 PMID: 17329432
29. Franciotti R, Ciancetta L, Della Penna S, Belardinelli P, Pizzella V, Romani GL. Modulation of alpha
oscillations in insular cortex reflects the threat of painful stimuli. Neuroimage. 2009; 46: 1082–1090.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.034 PMID: 19327401
30. Bertolino A, Arciero G, Rubino V, Latorre V, De Candia M, Mazzola V, et al. Variation of human amyg-
dala response during threatening stimuli as a function of 50 HTTLPR genotype and personality style. Biol
Psychiatry. 2005; 57: 1517–1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.031 PMID: 15953488
31. Rubino V, Blasi G, Latorre V, Fazio L, d’Errico I, Mazzola V, et al. Activity in medial prefrontal cortex dur-
ing cognitive evaluation of threatening stimuli as a function of personality style. Brain Res Bull. 2007;74.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.06.019 PMID: 17720547
32. Mazzola V, Latorre V, Petito A, Gentili N, Fazio L, Popolizio T, et al. Affective response to a loved one’s
pain: Insula activity as a function of individual differences. PLoS One. 2010; 5. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0015268 PMID: 21179564
33. Mazzola V, Marano G, Biganzoli EM, Boracchi P, Lanciano T, Arciero G, et al. The In-Out dispositional
affective style questionnaire (IN-OUT DASQ): An exploratory factorial analysis. Front Psychol. 2014; 5.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01005 PMID: 25309478
34. Arciero G. Evolutionary Epistemology and Scientific psychology. American Association of the Advance-
ment of Science San Francisco. 1989.
35. Arciero G, Bondolfi G, Mazzola V. The foundations of phenomenological psychotherapy. The Founda-
tions of Phenomenological Psychotherapy. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-78087-0
36. Mazzola V, Vuilleumier P, Latorre V, Petito A, Gallese V, Popolizio T, et al. Effects of Emotional Con-
texts on Cerebello-Thalamo-Cortical Activity during Action Observation. PLoS One. 2013; 8. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075912 PMID: 24086664
37. Penny WD, Stephan KE, Daunizeau J, Rosa MJ, Friston KJ, Schofield TM, et al. Comparing families of
dynamic causal models. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010; 6: e1000709. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.
1000709 PMID: 20300649
38. Razi A, Kahan J, Rees G, Friston KJ. Construct validation of a DCM for resting state fMRI. Neuroimage.
2015; 106: 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.027 PMID: 25463471
39. Spielberger CD. Manual for the state-trait anger expression scale (STAXI). Odessa, FL Psychol Assess
Resour. 1988.
40. Spielberger CD, Sydeman SJ, Owen AE, Marsh BJ. Measuring anxiety and anger with the State-Trait
Anxiety Inventory (STAI) and the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI). 1999.
41. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, et al. A new SPM toolbox for
combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage. 2005; 25:
1325–1335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034 PMID: 15850749
42. Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, Ramnani N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cer-
ebellum. Neuroimage. 2009; 46: 39–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045 PMID:
19457380
43. Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage. 2003; 19: 1273–1302.
https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00202-7 PMID: 12948688
44. Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ. Comparing dynamic causal models. Neuroimage.
2004; 22: 1157–1172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.026 PMID: 15219588
45. Acs F, Greenlee MW. Connectivity modulation of early visual processing areas during covert and overt
tracking tasks. Neuroimage. 2008; 41: 380–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.007
PMID: 18387824
46. Garrido MI, Kilner JM, Kiebel SJ, Stephan KE, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modelling of evoked poten-
tials: a reproducibility study. Neuroimage. 2007; 36: 571–580. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2007.03.014 PMID: 17478106
47. Neumann J, Lohmann G. Bayesian second-level analysis of functional magnetic resonance images.
Neuroimage. 2003; 20: 1346–1355. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00443-9 PMID: 14568503

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 16 / 17


Insulae-MCC connectivity and emotion-body connection dispositions

48. Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain. 1995;
118: 279–306. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/118.1.279 PMID: 7895011
49. Vogt BA, Laureys S. Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: Cytology and compo-
nents of the neural network correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res. 2005; 150: 205–217. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50015-3 PMID: 16186025
50. Vogt BA. Midcingulate cortex: Structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases. J Chem
Neuroanat. 2016; 74: 28–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchemneu.2016.01.010 PMID: 26993424
51. Smith R, Thayer JF, Khalsa SS, Lane RD. The hierarchical basis of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev. 2017; 75: 274–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.003 PMID: 28188890
52. Beissner F, Meissner K, Bar K-J, Napadow V. The Autonomic Brain: An Activation Likelihood Estima-
tion Meta-Analysis for Central Processing of Autonomic Function. J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 10503–10511.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1103-13.2013 PMID: 23785162
53. Critchley HD, Corfield DR, Chandler MP, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Cerebral correlates of autonomic car-
diovascular arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in humans. J Physiol. 2000; 523: 259–270.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00259.x PMID: 10673560
54. Oppenheimer SM. Cardiovascular effects of human insular cortex stimulation. Cardiology. 1992; 42:
1727–1732.
55. Oppenheimer S. Cerebrogenic cardiac arrhythmias: Cortical lateralization and clinical significance Ste-
phen Oppenheimer. Clin Auton Res. 2006; 16: 6–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10286-006-0276-0
PMID: 16477489
56. Oppenheimer S, Cechetto D. The Insular Cortex and the Regulation of Cardiac Function. Compr Phy-
siol. 2016; 6: 1081–133. https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c140076 PMID: 27065176
57. Ruiz Vargas E, Sörös P, Shoemaker JK, Hachinski V. Human cerebral circuitry related to cardiac con-
trol: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Ann Neurol. 2016; 79: 709–716. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24642
PMID: 30240034
58. Malinen S, Vartiainen N, Hlushchuk Y, Koskinen M, Ramkumar P, Forss N, et al. Aberrant temporal and
spatial brain activity during rest in patients with chronic pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010; 107: 6493–
6497. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001504107 PMID: 20308545
59. Tan LL, Pelzer P, Heinl C, Tang W, Gangadharan V, Flor H, et al. A pathway from midcingulate cortex
to posterior insula gates nociceptive hypersensitivity. Nat Neurosci. 2017; 20: 1591. https://doi.org/10.
1038/nn.4645 PMID: 28920932
60. Fernandez E, Johnson SL. Anger in psychological disorders: Prevalence, presentation, etiology and
prognostic implications. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016; 46: 124–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.
012 PMID: 27188635
61. Seth AK, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence.
Front Psychol. 2012; 2: 395. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395 PMID: 22291673
62. Barrett LF, Simmons WK. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015; 16: 419–429.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3950 PMID: 26016744

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228404 February 13, 2020 17 / 17

You might also like