Problem-Solving Appraisal and Psychological Adjustment: January 2012
Problem-Solving Appraisal and Psychological Adjustment: January 2012
Problem-Solving Appraisal and Psychological Adjustment: January 2012
net/publication/285976054
CITATIONS READS
56 845
2 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Puncky Paul Heppner on 30 October 2016.
Problem-Solving Appraisal and Human Adjustment: A Review of 20 Years of Research Using the
Problem Solving Inventory
P. Paul Heppner, Thomas E. Witty and Wayne A. Dixon
The Counseling Psychologist 2004; 32; 344
DOI: 10.1177/0011000003262793
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Additional services and information for The Counseling Psychologist can be found at:
Subscriptions: http://tcp.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Problem-Solving Appraisal
and Human Adjustment:
A Review of 20 Years of Research
Using the Problem Solving Inventory
P. Paul Heppner
University of Missouri–Columbia
Thomas E. Witty
North Mississippi Medical Center
Wayne A. Dixon
Southeastern Oklahoma State University
This article reviews and synthesizes more than 120 studies from 20 years (1982-2002) of
research that has examined problem-solving appraisal as measured by the Problem Solv-
ing Inventory (PSI). The goals of the article are fourfold: (a) introduce the construct of
problem-solving appraisal and the PSI within the applied problem-solving literature, (b)
summarize and integrate existing findings using the PSI within and across four major
content areas (psychological adjustment, physical health, coping, and educational and
vocational issues), (c) identify promising research directions that might stimulate future
research and promote theory development, and (d) identify implications for practitioners
in the helping professions. We conclude that problem-solving appraisal as measured by
the PSI is a useful psychological construct with implications for theory and many appli-
cations to build on peoples’ strengths to enhance their life satisfaction and well-being.
OUTLINE
Introduction
Overview of Problem-Solving Appraisal and the PSI
Brief History of Applied Problem Solving, Stress, and Health
Problem-Solving Appraisal
Problem Solving Inventory
Instrument Structure
Illustrations
Reliability Estimates
Construct Validity Estimates
Discriminant Validity and Potential Confounds
Normative Information
THE COUNSELING PSYCHOLOGIST, Vol. 32 No. 3, May 2004 344-428
DOI: 10.1177/0011000003262793
© 2004 by the Society of Counseling Psychology.
344
Introduction
Third, the review will identify promising research directions that might
stimulate future research and promote theory development within each of the
four major content areas. Although much more is obviously known about
problem-solving appraisal now than 20 years ago, the review also reveals
unanswered questions, topics that have received less attention, and more
complex questions about the theoretical conceptualizations of problem-
solving appraisal. Fourth, the review will identify implications for practitio-
ners in the helping professions. Research has indicated that all three PSI fac-
tors (Problem Solving Confidence, Approach-Avoidance Style, and Per-
sonal Control) play a role in predicting a range of physical and psychological
outcomes. This suggests that a number of counseling and psychoeducational
interventions might profitably focus on the various components of problem-
solving appraisal.
The first section of the article introduces the construct of problem-solving
appraisal as measured by the PSI and places it in the larger context of applied
problem solving. The next four sections summarize the research findings
across the content areas of psychological adjustment, physical adjustment,
coping, and educational and vocational issues. We examined all studies with
regard to gender, race, age, and nationality. The sixth section integrates con-
clusions across the research literature and integrates them into the broader
context of applied problem solving. The last two sections identify future
research directions and discuss implications for practitioners, respectively.
D’Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; Fretz, 1981; Krumboltz, 1965; Urban & Ford,
1971; Weitz, 1964). Moreover, problem solving has been applied to the ther-
apy process in terms of (a) an integrative theoretical model (e.g., D. N. Dixon
& Glover, 1984; D’Zurilla, 1986; D’Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; Nezu, Nezu,
& Perri, 1989), (b) a central construct in assessing counseling outcomes
(Heppner, Cooper, et al., 2001), (c) the assessment of how clients process
information and cope with a multitude of personal problems (e.g., Heppner
& Krauskopf, 1987; Long, Kahn, & Schutz, 1992), and (d) a broad range of
counseling interventions, such as problem-solving training for both remedial
and preventive interventions (e.g., D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1982; Heppner &
Hillerbrand, 1991) or strategies for decision-making interventions (e.g.,
Horan, 1979). In addition, research indicated that effective counseling seems
to result not only in resolving clients’ problems but also in improving clients’
perceived problem-solving abilities (Heppner, Cooper, et al., 2001; M. J.
Heppner et al., in press). Thus, problem solving has been seen as a useful and
central construct for psychologists and other helping professionals not only
to conceptualize the counseling process but also to take counseling out of a
pathological rubric and place it squarely into positive psychology and
building on people’s skills.
Problem-Solving Appraisal
In the cognitive revolution of the 1970s and 1980s, investigators were
becoming more interested in how individuals appraise their abilities (e.g.
Antonovsky, 1979; Bandura, 1982). This research was influenced by the
notion of higher order or metacognitive variables (e.g., Brown, 1977; Flavell &
Wellman, 1977; Meichenbaum & Asarnow, 1979). Metacognition refers to
an individual’s awareness of the processes that affect the efficient use of cog-
nitive skills. A few years later, Bandura (1986) published his influential book
on self-efficacy and provided substantial evidence to support the notion that a
situation-specific appraisal (self-efficacy) affects motivation, behavior,
thoughts, and emotional reactions to stressful situations. Most important, in
1981, Butler and Meichenbaum integrated metacognitive variables into the
problem-solving literature. They suggested that problem-solving research
should be concerned not only with “the specific knowledge or processes that
individuals may apply directly to the solution of problems but with higher
order variables that affect how (and whether) they will solve problems” (p.
219). Butler and Meichenbaum went on to hypothesize that an individual’s
appraisal of his or her problem-solving skills will not only affect problem
solving performance but might be an important predictor variable of the
problem-solving process. In short, in the late 1970s and early 1980s, scholars
health (D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1990). A fourth strategy has assessed verbal
reports of one’s problem-solving activities that facilitate or inhibit progress
toward resolving problems, such as the Problem Focused Style of Coping
(PF-SOC) (Heppner, Cook, et al., 1995). The PF-SOC assesses perceived
effectiveness of applied problem-solving activities; research suggests that
the PF-SOC adds meaningful variance in predicting psychological distress
even after major problem-solving and coping instruments are removed
(Heppner, Cook, et al., 1995). The multiple assessment strategies reflect the
multifaceted nature of applied problem solving. Only the PSI, however, is
conceptualized as a measure of problem-solving appraisal and has been rec-
ognized as one of the most widely used self-report inventories in applied
problem solving (Nezu et al., 1989).
In the past, researchers have distinguished between verbal self-reports of
problem solving versus observational assessments of problem-solving
behaviors (e.g., Butler & Meichenbaum, 1981; D’Zurilla, 1986). Observa-
tional approaches assess overt problem-solving performance in natural or
simulated problem-solving situations. Observational methods are useful for
assessing overt problem-solving performances or the product of problem-
solving processes. Despite the appeal of observational approaches, this
assessment strategy encounters complex measurement issues; see D’Zurilla
(1986) for a more thorough discussion of the advantages and limitations of
this assessment approach. The self-report method has been the most common
method of assessment and has been more fully developed through empirical
research in the literature. For the purpose of this article, it is important to note
that the PSI is a verbal self-report measure of one’s problem-solving capaci-
ties. Our review concludes that the pattern of research findings suggests that
the PSI seems to be substantially associated with problem-solving perfor-
mance but should not be considered synonymous with problem-solving
effectiveness.
The initial exploratory factor analysis (Heppner & Petersen, 1982) sug-
gested three factors within the PSI: (a) Problem Solving Confidence (PSC,
11 items), (b) Approach-Avoidance Style (AAS, 16 items), and (c) Personal
Control (PC, 5 items); the PSI total score is the sum of these three subscales.
Each factor provides an appraisal of a specific dimension of one’s problem-
solving style, and the total score reflects an individual’s overall appraisal of
his or her problem-solving style. Scores for all three factors and the total PSI
are continuous rather than categorical scores. In some of the initial studies
using the PSI, high (perceived effective problem solvers) and low (perceived
ineffective problem solvers) were identified by using cutoff scores as one
standard deviation above or below the mean PSI total score (e.g., Heppner,
Hibel, Neal, Weinstein, & Rabinowitz, 1982). For the most part, however,
perceived effective and ineffective problem solvers are operationalized as
those who score toward one end versus the other end of the continuous range
of PSI scores. PSC is defined as an individual’s self-assurance, beliefs, and
trust in his or her ability to effectively cope with a wide range of problems
(e.g.,”When faced with a novel situation, I have confidence that I can handle
problems that may arise”). Lower scores on the PSC reflect higher levels of
problem-solving confidence. AAS refers to a general tendency to approach
or avoid different problem-solving activities (e.g., ”When making a decision,
I weigh the consequences of each alternative and compare them against each
other”). Lower scores are associated with a style of approaching rather than
avoiding problems. PC is defined as believing one is in control of one’s emo-
tions and behaviors while solving problems (e.g., “Even though I work on a
problem, sometimes I feel like I am groping or wandering and am not getting
down to the real issue”). Lower scores on PC indicate a more positive individ-
ual perception of personal control in handling problems. Lower scores on
each factor (and thus on the total PSI score) are generally considered more
functional (important exceptions are discussed later under Problem-Solving
Effectiveness). The intercorrelation among these three factors ranged from
.39 to .69 across a range of studies on PSI (see Heppner, 1988). Results sug-
gest that the factors are not only interrelated but also independent enough to
be considered as separate factors (see Heppner, 1988; Heppner & Wang,
2003). Subsequent studies using either exploratory or confirmatory factor
analyses indicate that the PSI factors tend to replicate well across different
age groups from various backgrounds, such as midwestern White college
students (e.g., Heppner, Baumgardner, Larson, & Petty, 1988), French Cana-
dian adults (LaPorte, Sabourin, & Wright, 1988), Turkish college students
(Sahin, Sahin, & Heppner, 1993), South African college students (Heppner,
Pretorius, Wei, Lee, & Wang, 2002), and midwestern high school students
(using an adolescent version of the PSI with language adjusted to a fourth-
grade reading level; Heppner, Manley, Perez, & Dixon, 1994).
ing. He decided to “sit out” the next semester to “get some perspective,” but
he never returned to college.
These stories highlight the role of one’s appraisal of one’s problem-
solving style in responding to stressful situations and how people with differ-
ent problem-solving styles (e.g., approaching vs. avoiding problems, prob-
lem-solving confidence) respond to similar situations in very different ways.
The examples indicate how self-assessment of one’s problem-solving skills,
such as problem-solving confidence, can affect how one proceeds in grap-
pling with a problem, which often results in very different psychological out-
comes. In fact, research from a number of areas (consistent with the basic ten-
ets of cognitive therapy) has indicated that when people are confronted with
stressful events, it is not the stress per se but rather how one responds to phys-
ical or psychological stress that can either help or hinder its resolution and
determine whether one reaches physical and psychological well-being (see
D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1982; Friedman, 1992; Snyder & Ford, 1987; Zeidner &
Endler, 1996).
Reliability estimates. The PSI has been found to have acceptable internal
consistency estimates across a number of populations and cultures (e.g.,
Heppner, 1988; Heppner et al., 1994, 2002). Summing across studies, the PSI
total obtains average alpha coefficients in the high .80s, whereas two of the
factors (PSC and AAS) obtain average alpha coefficients in the low to mid-
.80s, and the third factor (PC) obtains average alpha coefficients in the low
.70s. These results suggest that the PSI is internally consistent across differ-
ent forms of the PSI used across different cultural groups (Heppner & Wang,
2003).
Several studies provided estimates of stability of PSI scores over various
time intervals, from 2 weeks to 2 years across different samples. Total PSI
scores are correlated .80 over 2 weeks, .81 over 3 weeks as well as 4 months,
and .60 over 2 years across samples of White college students, Black college
students, and French Canadian adults (e.g., Heppner, 1988). The results sug-
gest that the PSI scores are stable over time across different populations and
cultures (Heppner & Wang, 2003).
and low scores on the PSI (Heppner et al., 1982; Larson, 1984). The PSI also
outperforms standard neuropsychological problem-solving measures to dif-
ferentiate adults with traumatic brain injuries from uninjured adults (e.g.,
Rath, Langenbahn, et al., in press; Rath, Simon, Langenbahn, Sherr, & Diller,
2000) and predicts external observers’ judgments of the ability of traumatic
brain-injured patients to function independently in the community (Rath,
Hennessy, & Diller, 2003) and their performance on standard neuropsy-
chological timed attention tasks (Rath, Langenbahn, et al., in press). The PSI
has also been found to prospectively predict objective behavioral health out-
comes, such as decubitus ulcers and urinary tract infections, in theoretically
predicted manners (e.g., Godshall & Elliott, 1997; Herrick, Elliott, & Crow,
1994). The PSI has also predicted a wide range of psychological adjustment
indices in theoretically consistent manners—notably, global measures of
adjustment such as self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and hopelessness but
also those associated with specific types of psychological adjustment prob-
lems such as eating disorders (see Heppner & Baker, 1997, for a partial list-
ing). Moreover, mothers’ PSI scores predict independent observations of
children’s social and emotional development (Walker & Johnson, 1986). The
PSI has been found to relate to a wide range of cognitive responses, including
expectations, attributions, and negative self-statements, and affective
responses such as increased emotional arousal and emotionally focused self-
statements when coping with difficult situations (e.g., Larson, Potenza,
Wennstedt, & Sailors, 1995; Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996).
Richeson, Pickelman, & Franklin, 1996; Heppner, Lee, Wei, Anderson, &
Wang, 2001). A large amount of data suggests that the PSI does not have sim-
ilar relations with NA-related variables as depicted by Clark and Watson
(1991): (a) When examined with other coping variables, NA failed to be a
significant predictor of unique variance of the PSI (Larson, Piersel, Imao, &
Allen, 1990); (b) NA is fundamentally a mood-based dimension with little
relationship to behavioral outcomes (Clark & Watson, 1991) and is more
strongly related to emotion-focused coping (e.g., Scheier & Carver, 1985);
conversely, the PSI is fundamentally a cognitive-based inventory, consis-
tently related to problem-focused coping activities (e.g., Heppner, Reeder, &
Larson, 1983; Larson et al., 1990), has predicted behavioral outcomes in sev-
eral studies (e.g., Elliott, 1992), and has even linked mothers’ PSI scores and
their children’s social and behavioral adjustment (e.g., Reis & Heppner,
1993; Walker & Johnson, 1986); (c) the PSI predicts psychological distress
(Heppner, Lee, et al., 2001) and behavioral outcomes (Elliott, Shewchuk,
Richeson, et al., 1996) even after variance as a result of NA is removed; (d)
contrary to NA-related variables, longitudinal research has found the PSI to
be a significant predictor of depression over time after initial estimates of
depression were removed (W. A. Dixon, Heppner, Burnett, Anderson, &
Wood, 1993); (e) NA-related variables like hardiness or optimism have not
consistently moderated relations between stress and illness (Allred & Smith,
1989; Funk & Houston, 1987), whereas in several studies, the PSI has consis-
tently moderated stress and depression (e.g., Cheng & Lam, 1997; Rich &
Bonner, 1987); and (f) the PSI relates to affective, cognitive, and behavioral
variables such as problem-focused coping and attributional patterns that sug-
gest responsibility in ways that are inconsistent with a mood-based dimen-
sion of NA. In short, the PSI does not seem to be another measure of intelli-
gence, social desirability, or negative affectivity.
and the career maturity theory (Crites, 1978). In short, the PSI has been used
to test and confirm the role of problem solving within a broad range of theo-
retical frameworks of human behavior, thereby demonstrating the general
utility of the PSI in understanding human behavior.
PSYCHOLOGICAL ADJUSTMENT
experiences (Reid & Dixon, 2000); and (f) racial identity statuses in African
American students (Neville, Heppner, & Wang, 1997). In addition, research
indicates that the PSI is related to different self-report indices of social skills.
Perceived ineffective (as opposed to effective) problem solvers reported hav-
ing (a) fewer social skills (Elliott, Godshall, Herrick, Witty, & Spruell, 1991;
Larson, 1984) and (b) more social uneasiness/distrust/distress (DeClue,
1983; Heppner et al., 1982; Larson, Allen, Imao, & Piersel, 1993; Sabourin
et al., 1990). Two studies also provided behavioral ratings from external
observers that confirmed the self-report findings (Heppner et al., 1982;
Larson, 1984).
Most of the research in this area has examined zero-order correlations
between problem-solving appraisal and psychological adjustment. However,
several studies indicate that there are more complex relations between
problem-solving appraisal and social and psychological adjustment. For
example, Hanson and Mintz (1997) used canonical correlations with a group
of older adults (mean age 77) and found that the majority of variance was
accounted for by two roots indicating that increases in psychological health
were related to more positive problem-solving appraisal. A third root, how-
ever, revealed that a tendency to both avoid problems and report a lack of per-
sonal control was related to increased interpersonal ties and relationships.
These results suggest that in some cases, there may be a dominant positive
relationship between problem-solving appraisal and psychological adjust-
ment as well as another independent relationship between a negative problem-
solving appraisal and the secondary interpersonal gains that might result
from interpersonal relationships.
Although there has been a strong association between problem-solving
appraisal and psychological distress, there is some evidence that the PSI fac-
tors may constitute more complex relationships among problem-solving
constructs in predicting psychological adjustment. Two studies found evi-
dence for a problem-solving mediational (PSM) model of psychological dis-
tress (Witty et al., 2001; Heppner et al., 2002). The PSM model posits that
one PSI factor, Problem-Solving Confidence, will mediate the relationship
between a second factor, Approach-Avoidance Style, in predicting psycho-
logical distress. The results suggested that although a person’s tendency to
avoid problems was directly related to psychological distress, that tendency
was associated with less problem-solving confidence, which, in turn, was
associated with more psychological distress. The PSM model was supported
across two very different samples (chronic low-back pain in adult patients in
the United States and Black South African college students). The PSM model
underscores not only the role of approaching or avoiding problems in pre-
dicting psychological distress but, more important, how an individual’s prob-
Depression
Depression is a common and serious problem of enormous personal and
social significance. Millions of people around the world have been diagnosed
with depression. Depression is generally conceptualized as a complex, multi-
dimensional process hypothesized to result from an interplay between envi-
ronmental stress and psychosocial vulnerability or moderator factors (e.g.,
Hammen, 1985). In the late 1970s and early 1980s, researchers began identi-
fying psychosocial factors to explain why some individuals experience life
changes without becoming depressed but others become depressed in the
face of stress. Because problem solving was theorized to affect psychological
adjustment in general (e.g., Spivack & Shure, 1974), researchers began to
examine relations between problem solving and depression. The basic
hypothesis was that ineffective problem solving would likely result in
increased levels of depression.
Research from at least 35 studies suggests strong empirical support for a
relationship between problem-solving appraisal and depression across a
wide range of populations and research designs. This research strongly sup-
ports the hypothesis that the more a person perceives himself or herself to be
an ineffective problem solver, the more likely he or she will experience
increased levels of depression. More specifically, eight studies that used ex
post facto (between-groups) designs found that self-appraised effective (as
opposed to ineffective) problem solvers (which was the nonmanipulated sta-
tus variable) reported themselves to be significantly less depressed on a vari-
ety of depression inventories measuring short- and long-term depression,
such as the MMPI, Cornell Medical Index, Beck Depression Inventory, Zung
Depression Scale, and Feelings and Concerns Survey (Heppner & Anderson,
1985; Heppner et al., 1985, 1987; Nezu, 1985, 1986a; Nezu & Carnevale,
1987; Pretorius, 1996; Rich & Bonner, 1987).
studies that examine the universal nature of this relationship in very different
contexts. Beyond the main effect of the PSI in predicting depression, the evi-
dence also suggests that consistent with Nezu and Ronan’s (1985) theory,
problem-solving appraisal has more complex relations with depression, such
as moderating and reciprocal roles. Such complex relationships as well as
other more complex structural models merit additional attention to test their
applicability across situations, populations, and cultures. Given there are at
least 7 million women and 3.5 million men in the United States with diag-
nosed depression (McGrath, Keita, Strickland, & Russo, 1990), and many
more around the world, this area of research seems particularly important
from an intervention perspective.
scores are associated with increases in hopelessness (Bonner & Rich, 1987,
1988, 1992; Cannon et al., 1999; W. A. Dixon et al., 1991, 1994; Priester &
Clum, 1993; Rudd et al., 1996; Witty et al., 2001) and suicide ideation
(Bonner & Rich, 1987; Clum & Febbraro, 1994; W. A. Dixon et al., 1991,
1994; Rudd et al., 1996; Wright & Heppner, 1991). Because hopelessness is
one of the best predictors of suicidality, Cannon et al. (1999) examined a
range of potential predictors of hopelessness. Although a number of predic-
tors were associated with hopelessness, only dysfunctional cognitions and
problem-solving appraisal offered nonredundant prediction of hopelessness
scores and accounted for 20% of the variance. Moreover, consistent with
Schotte and Clum’s theory, across all of the studies that measured both
hopelessness and suicide ideation, there was a stronger association between
problem-solving appraisal and hopelessness (rs = .47 to .62) than between
problem-solving appraisal and suicide ideation (rs = .11 to .43). This indi-
rectly indicated that problem-solving appraisal is associated with suicide
ideation primarily through its impact on hopelessness.
In addition, as predicted by Schotte and Clum’s model, self-appraised
negative problem solvers under high levels of stress were especially likely to
report high levels of hopelessness in three of six studies (Bonner & Rich,
1992; Clum & Febbraro, 1994; Priester & Clum, 1993). One explanation for
the absence of the interaction in the other three studies is that general mea-
sures of stress (e.g., scores on the Life Events Survey, which measures stress
in the year preceding its assessment) were not proximal to the time when
problem-solving appraisal was measured to be differentially affected by the
stress. Priester and Clum (1993) hypothesized that only stresses that are rela-
tively recent would be moderated by problem-solving appraisal in regard to
its effect on adjustment, such as hopelessness and suicidality. Two studies
support this interpretation (Bonner & Rich, 1992; Priester & Clum, 1993).
When researchers examined the predictive utility of the three PSI factors,
Problem-Solving Confidence was found to have the strongest association
with hopelessness and suicidal ideation (W. A. Dixon et al., 1991, Dixon
et al., 1994; Priester & Clum, 1993). However, Clum and Febbraro (1994)
found more complex patterns across the PSI factors; they found that both
individuals who lacked Problem-Solving Confidence and individuals who
approached problems were more likely to report high levels of suicidal
ideation under high levels of stress. Although this seemingly contradictory
finding could be an artifact of the sample, it might also suggest more complex
dynamics across the PSI factors.
W. A. Dixon et al. (1994) directly tested and found strong support for the
mediational role of hopelessness between problem solving and suicidal
ideation. Consistent with Schotte and Clum’s theory, the W. A. Dixon et al.
(1994) study found that problem-solving appraisal was related to suicide
hopeless and suicidal when they experience high levels of stress. Moreover, it
is likely that the path to suicidal ideation is multidimensional and may vary
across levels of ideation severity (Bonner & Rich, 1987). It may be that the
relationship between suicide and problem-solving appraisal is also recipro-
cal. A negative problem-solving appraisal may lead to increases in hopeless-
ness and suicide ideation, and increases in hopelessness and suicide ideation
may lead to a temporary decrease in appraised problem-solving
effectiveness.
Alcohol Use/Abuse
Cognitive-social learning approaches to drug and alcohol abuse theorize
that abuse is largely the result of having few coping strategies for dealing
with situations that are appraised as stressful and having expectations that a
drug will help in coping (e.g., Bandura, 1977, 1986; Lazarus & Folkman,
1984). It has been hypothesized that individuals who lack a sense of self-
efficacy for coping with stressful situations are more likely to resort to alco-
hol and drugs as an emotion-focused coping strategy to alter personal feel-
ings of inadequacy (Annis & Davis, 1989; Marlatt & Gordon, 1985). More-
over, Wills and Hirky’s (1996) coping model of substance abuse posits, in
part, that avoidance, as opposed to approach coping, plays a central role in
substance abuse. Consistent with these models and hypotheses, it would be
expected that problem-solving appraisal would have an association with
alcohol use and abuse.
Five studies have assessed the role of problem-solving appraisal with
alcohol/drug use among college students, and a sixth has examined this rela-
tionship with a group of inpatient alcoholics. All studies have been descrip-
tive and correlational and thus provide only an indirect test of applications of
Bandura’s (1986) theory for alcohol use. Consistent with the theory, three of
the six studies (Godshall & Elliott, 1997; Heppner et al., 1982; Wright &
Heppner, 1991) suggest a significant linear relationship between problem-
solving appraisal and alcohol use/abuse. Another study (Larson & Heppner,
1989) suggests that as a group, inpatient alcoholics appraised their problem
solving considerably more negatively than nonclinical adults.
However, there is some evidence that the relationship between problem-
solving appraisal and the frequency of alcohol use may be more complex
than depicted by zero-order relationships. For example, Williams and
Kleinfelter (1989) did not find a significant association between problem-
solving appraisal and frequency of alcohol use in undergraduate students but
rather found more complex relations through canonical analysis. One canon-
ical variate indicated that a lack of problem-solving confidence and a ten-
dency to avoid problems were related to the use of alcohol to control negative
emotions and escape problems. The second variate indicated students who
lacked confidence but approached their problems reported that they drank to
control negative emotions and enhance their mood. Larson and Heppner
(1989) examined a clinical sample of inpatient alcoholics and found that the
PSI did not correlate with severity of alcohol abuse, with the exception of pre-
vious number of alcohol treatments. (Given the relatively high frequency of
alcohol abuse, ceiling effects may have mitigated against finding significant
correlations with the PSI.) Moreover, despite some individuals’ scoring high
(i.e., a negative problem-solving appraisal) on the PSI as a group, Larson and
Heppner (1989) noted that their PSI scores in the alcoholic sample indicated
a very positive problem-solving appraisal; interviews with these individuals
revealed that they were overapproaching and perseverating their problems.
Thus, at least some individuals who perceived their problem-solving
appraisal to be very positive also had significant problems with alcohol
abuse; this finding is consistent with Williams and Kleinfelter (1989), who
found that both high and low PSI scores were related to drinking patterns. A
final study (Slavkin, Heimberg, Winning, & McCaffrey, 1992) also did not
find a linear relationship between the PSI and alcohol intake/abuse, but rather
an interaction between alcohol abuse and parental drinking (i.e., students
who reported more alcohol abuse and whose parents had problems with
drinking had more negative problem-solving appraisal).
In sum, consistent with theories by Wills and Hirky (1996) and Bandura
(1986), three of the six studies suggest a significant linear relationship
between problem-solving appraisal and alcohol use/abuse. Another study
suggests that as a group, inpatient alcoholics score significantly more nega-
tively on the PSI than nonclinical adults. Moreover, three of the studies sug-
gest more complex relationships between problem-solving appraisal and
alcohol use/abuse and that there may be different drinking patterns associ-
ated with the PSI factors. Although some of the research supports a signifi-
cant linear relationship between problem-solving appraisal and alcohol use/
abuse, there is also evidence to suggest more complex relationships among
these variables. The relationships between problem-solving appraisal and
alcohol use/abuse also may be clouded by common psychological defenses
of alcoholics, such as denial, rationalization, excuse making, and minimizing
(e.g., Wilson, 1987). Future research might address this area by avoiding reli-
ance on self-report measures and focusing on legal documents, medical
records, work history, and collaborative data from spouses or family
members.
Eating Disorders
Although the etiology of eating disorders is conceptualized as being
multifactorial (Striegel-Moore & Cachelin, 2001), personal vulnerability
factors have long been regarded as important contributing factors (Striegel-
Moore & Cachelin, 1999). In fact, it has been proposed that individuals at risk
for eating disorders are vulnerable not only because of stress but also because
of poor problem solving (Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, & Rodin, 1986). Sub-
sequently, research has examined whether problem-solving appraisal might
be one factor that differentiates women with and without eating disorders.
Specifically, two studies examined U.S. women who were clinically diag-
nosed according to DSM-III and DSM-III-R diagnostic criteria (American
Psychiatric Association, 1987) as having an eating disorder. Etringer,
Altmaier, and Bowers (1989) found bulimic women to have statistically sig-
nificant higher PSI scores (PSI Total and all three factors) than a nonbulimic
control group. Soukup, Beiler, and Terrell (1990) found both bulimic and
anorexic women (as opposed to those women without an eating disorder) to
have statistically higher PSI scores on the PSI Total and specifically on the
Problem-Solving Confidence and Approach-Avoidance Style factors. These
studies suggest that women with eating disorders scored higher on the PSI
than normal-eating comparison groups. An unpublished study conducted in
Italy (Campanelli, Bove, & D’Annunzio, 1993) reported a group of women
experiencing bulimia for an average of 5 years had a total PSI score of 147, a
very negative problem-solving appraisal and the highest mean PSI score of
all the samples found in the literature.
In sum, three studies link a negative problem-solving appraisal in women
with eating disorders in both U.S. and Italian samples. Thus, problem-
solving appraisal is associated with a specific type of psychological disorder
and may play a role in how individuals cope with various risk factors related
to eating disorders in at least two cultures.
Childhood Adjustment
ilarly, another study based on Black South African samples (Pretorius, 1996)
found that positive problem-solving appraisal was associated with family
environments characterized by helpful and supportive relationships, encour-
agement for open displays of emotions and behavior, and a low level of
interpersonal conflict.
Two studies provide evidence that parents’ problem-solving appraisal is
related to children’s behavior. Walker and Johnson (1986) provide particu-
larly important evidence that parents’ problem-solving appraisal is related to
independent behavioral ratings of children’s social and emotional develop-
ment. Two other studies with a culturally diverse U.S. sample and a Black
South African sample indicate parental problem-solving appraisal was
related to the family environment and corporal punishment. These four stud-
ies suggest that parents’ problem-solving appraisal may be related to family
environments, parental punishment, and children’s social and behavioral
development. If research is able to continue to find associations between
parental problem-solving appraisal, family environments, and child rearing
styles, researchers might examine the utility of problem-solving training as a
preventive intervention for a range of childhood psychological problems.
Research might also explore the potential relationship between problem-
solving appraisal and parental management of children’s medical problems
because parental problem solving has been identified as an important compo-
nent of children’s adjustment to a medical problem (e.g., Peterson, 1989).
The role of problem-solving appraisal in parenting is relatively unexplored
but may be useful in developing a range of remedial and preventive training
interventions.
Childhood Trauma
Research has indicated that life events and traumas affect how people
cope with subsequent traumatic events. Moreover, it has been hypothesized
that stressful events and traumas may be risk factors in developing mal-
adaptive coping styles and even psychopathology (Resick, 2001). Although
previously discussed theories (e.g., the diathesis-stress-hopelessness theory
of suicidality) have implicated ineffective problem solving as a risk factor
predicting psychological maladjustment, it also may be possible that trau-
matic events are so emotionally powerful that they affect one’s subsequent
problem-solving style.
Two areas of research have examined the relationships between childhood
traumas and problem-solving appraisal: (a) childhood sexual abuse (incest)
and (b) parental alcoholism. Reis and Heppner (1993) found that adolescent
(mean age = 15 years) incest victims reported more negative problem-
PHYSICAL HEALTH
adults from noninjured adults but also the best predictor of community inte-
gration (even over standard neuropsychological measures of problem solv-
ing). Thus, these results suggest that the “evaluation of functioning problem-
solving deficits can be enhanced by using social problem solving
approaches” such as the PSI (Rath et al., 2000, p. 730).
COPING
How people cope with their personal problems and process information—
how they think, feel, and behave when grappling with a real-life problem—is
a complex and dynamic process (Heppner & Hillerbrand, 1991). Is there any
evidence that problem-solving appraisal is linked to specific cognitive, affec-
tive, or behavioral activities associated with how people cope with stressful
life events?
One way researchers have attempted to examine how people cope or solve
problems is to present participants with hypothetical or laboratory problems
and study how participants respond to them. However, some researchers
(e.g., Wickelgren, 1974) have maintained that hypothetical or laboratory cre-
ations differ from real-life personal problems along several dimensions. For
example, stressful life problems usually contain more emotional elements,
particularly anxiety. Thus, it is questionable whether generalization from
laboratory problems is warranted.
Nine studies examined the relations between problem-solving appraisal
and hypothetical or laboratory problem-solving tasks (Baumgardner,
Heppner & Arkin, 1986; Davey, 1994; Heppner & Petersen, 1982; Larson
et al., 1995; Larson & Sailors, 1997; Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996; Moss,
1983; Nezu & Ronan, 1988; Sarmány & Sládeková, 1990). The findings
were inconsistent. Two studies did not find differences across levels of
problem-solving appraisal (Heppner & Petersen, 1982; Moss, 1983), while
three others did (Davey, 1994; Nezu & Ronan, 1988; Sarmány & Sládeková,
1990), and four other studies provided more complex findings
(Baumgardner et al., 1986; Larson et al., 1995; Larson & Sailors, 1997;
Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996).
For example, Heppner and Petersen (1982) initially investigated whether
problem-solving appraisal was related to generating steps for solving hypo-
thetical story problems, as measured by the first three stories of the Means-
End Problem Solving Procedure (MEPS) (Platt & Spivack, 1975); the results
revealed that the PSI was unrelated to MEPS scores. Conversely, two studies
that used six of the stories of the MEPS found that the Approach-Avoidance
Style scale was related to the MEPS (Davey, 1994) and that there was a sub-
stantial relationship between the total scores of the two measures (r = –.69;
Nezu & Ronan, 1988). The results of these two studies suggest that self-
appraised effective problem solvers generated more means or steps in solv-
ing the story problems. Nezu and Ronan (1988) suggested the restricted
range of scores from three stories in the Heppner and Petersen (1982) study
might explain the differential results. Another study (Sarmány & Sládeková,
1990) examined performance on computer tasks under two conditions (feed-
back and no feedback), as well as by levels of problem-solving appraisal;
their results revealed no differences in the feedback condition but significant
differences between high and low PSI scorers in the no-feedback condition.
Sarmány and Sládeková (1990) concluded that the problem-solving situation
“without feedback is too complex and complicated to be able to elaborate an
efficient strategy of its solution” for the self-appraised ineffective problem
solvers (p. 102). They also reported a significant correlation (r = .46)
between the number of errors and higher avoidance scores on the Approach-
Avoidance factor.
Other studies guided by cognitive theories (e.g., attributional style, infor-
mation processing) provided more complex findings involving important
differences in behaviors, cognitions, and affective reactions. Baumgardner
et al. (1986) conducted two studies; Study 2 examined attributions to six
hypothetical problems and manipulated bogus success/failure feedback. The
results revealed that the perceived effective problem solvers attributed their
“successful” performance more to their ability and effort and their “failed”
performance less to ability and effort. Conversely, the perceived ineffective
problem solvers did not differ in attributing their success or failure to ability
and effort. Larson et al. (1995) and Larson and Sailors (1997) examined
teacher trainees’performance in handling classroom disruptions while deliv-
ering a 15-minute classroom presentation. The results revealed that the per-
ceived effective problem solvers did not respond more effectively to the dis-
ruptions than the perceived ineffective problem solvers. However, the latter
group reported more negative self-assessments, emotional arousal, intense
affect, self-focused as opposed to problem-focused statements, and debilitat-
ing cognitions than the perceived effective problem solvers. Larson and Sail-
ors (1997) also reported that the participants were randomly given either suc-
cess or failure bogus feedback and asked to rate their attributions for their
performance. The results confirmed the earlier findings of Baumgardner
et al. (1986), indicating that perceived ineffective problem solvers did not dif-
fer in their attributions when given success or failure feedback, whereas the
perceived effective problem solvers varied their attributions for the two con-
ditions. Moreover, the perceived ineffective problem solvers also attributed
more to task difficulty. A final study (Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996) exam-
ined self-statements while participants responded to an interpersonal prob-
lem depicted on videotape. Although level of problem-solving appraisal was
unrelated to the total number of solutions generated or solution effectiveness
scores, perceived ineffective problem solvers again used more task-inhibiting
self-statements, more emotion-focused self-statements, and fewer problem-
focused self-statements. The perceived ineffective problem solvers also
tended to view the interpersonal problems as not within their power to
change.
The complexity of the type of laboratory task might explain some of these
discrepant results. On laboratory tasks involving simple procedures, like ana-
gram tasks, significant relationships were not found with problem-solving
appraisal (e.g., Heppner & Petersen, 1982). Conversely, with more complex
procedures and repeated trials, relationships with problem-solving appraisal
were found (Larson et al., 1995; Sarmány & Sládeková, 1990). Although this
area of research is inconclusive, future researchers might use laboratory tasks
that more closely approximate real-life personal problems with uncertainty
and sufficient complexity for repeated problem-solving trials (e.g., see
Dorner, 1983). The Larson et al. (1995) and Larson and Sailor (1997) studies
are noteworthy as they involved a simulated problem situation for teacher
trainees, with class disruptions three times during a 15-minute presentation.
These simulations and the exercise in the Mayo and Tanaka-Matsumi (1996)
study are closer than anagram tasks are to real-life problem solving. More
important, these studies allowed a more thorough assessment of outcomes
(which revealed no differences on behavioral effectiveness but important dif-
ferences on emotional arousal, self-focused statements, debilitating
cognitions, and negative feelings) and provided evidence for a complex
interplay between behavioral performance, cognitions, and affective
responses.
In sum, the research findings suggest a relationship between problem-
solving appraisal and responses to hypothetical problems across seven of
nine studies, especially when the problem situations more closely approxi-
mate the nature of real-life personal problems and involve repeated trials,
uncertainty, and emotional arousal. The Larson et al. (1995), Larson and
Sailors (1997), and Mayo and Tanaka-Matsumi (1996) studies underscore
the necessity to examine a range of outcomes—not only behavioral effective-
ness but also emotional arousal, cognitions, and feelings.
ple report coping with stressful life problems (see Lazarus, 2000; Zeidner &
Endler, 1996). Although this line of research has focused on reports about
how participants believe they coped with stressful events, there is a consider-
able amount of research documenting that coping self-reports are related to
coping outcomes as well as several indices of psychological distress (see
Friedman, 1992; Snyder & Ford, 1987; Zeidner & Endler, 1996). A common
strategy in studying coping has been to assess a broad range of cognitive and
affective coping activities that a person might use in responding to stressful
life problems. Based on a cognitive transactional theory (Lazarus, 1966; Laz-
arus & Folkman, 1984), two major functions of coping are widely agreed on
(Endler & Parker, 1990): (a) Problem-focused coping includes activities to
alter the cause of the distress or problem and (b) emotion-focused coping
includes activities to regulate stressful emotions associated with the distress
(Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
Consistent with a cognitive transactional theory of coping, 10 studies
using 13 separate samples examined associations between problem-solving
appraisal and problem- and emotional-focused coping. It was consistently
indicated that a positive problem-solving appraisal was associated with
reports of problem-focused coping or approaching and attempting to alter the
cause of the stressful problem (Corcoran, 1991; Heppner, Cook, et al., 1995;
Heppner, Cooper, et al., 2001; Heppner et al., 1983; Larson et al., 1990;
MacNair & Elliott, 1992; Neal, 1983; Reeder & Heppner, 1985; Ritchey,
Carscaddon, & Morgan, 1984; Sabourin et al., 1990). The findings support-
ing this conclusion are based on both dispositional and situation-specific
measures of coping, based on primarily White college student samples but
also U.S. racial/ethnic minorities (Reeder & Heppner, 1985) and Canadian
adults (Sabourin et al., 1990). MacNair and Elliott’s (1992) study used a pro-
spective design, which provides particularly strong empirical evidence.
Moreover, the Larson et al. (1990) study provides evidence of a strong rela-
tionship, based on simultaneous regressions, that problem-solving appraisal
was related to positive coping as well as a problem-solving self-efficacy fac-
tor across two samples. Conversely, three studies found no association
between problem-solving appraisal and emotion-focused coping (Corcoran,
1991; Heppner et al., 1983; Reeder & Heppner, 1985), weak associations
(Neal, 1983), or associations only with the Personal Control component
(e.g., Heppner, Cook, et al., 1995; MacNair & Elliott, 1992; Ritchey et al.,
1984; Sabourin et al., 1990). Overall, these findings suggest that the PSI is
more associated with problem-focused rather than emotion-focused coping.
Other studies provide additional information about relations between
problem-solving appraisal and expectations as well as attributions. In one of
the first attempts to explore relations between problem-solving appraisal and
coping, Heppner et al. (1982) conducted a structured interview with self-
appraised effective and ineffective problem solvers about how they cope with
stressful life problems. Their results suggested that the self-appraised effec-
tive problem solvers (a) are more motivated to solve their problems, (b)
expect themselves to be more successful, (c) attribute their successful efforts
to their ability and effort as opposed to luck, (d) persist longer in solving
problems, (e) delay gratification longer in struggling with problems, and (f)
infrequently hope a problem would just disappear. Baumgardner et al. (1986)
found much stronger evidence regarding attributional styles. In Study 1, the
participants were asked to assess their attributions for common psychologi-
cal and social problems. The study found that a distinguishing feature
between the self-appraised effective and ineffective problem solvers was the
causal role of effort. Self-appraised effective problem solvers saw effort (or
lack of it) as a major cause of their personal problems as well as their failure
to solve a laboratory problem. This suggests that perceived effective problem
solvers tend to assume personal responsibility for personal problems. Their
attributing failed coping attempts to inadequate effort underscores that they
believe that effort is the means by which problems can be overcome. Such
patterns in attributional style are consistent with a problem-solving style that
approaches rather than avoids personal problems.
Four studies suggest that two components of problem-solving appraisal,
Problem-Solving Confidence and Approach-Avoidance Style, may be the
strongest contributors to problem-focused coping reports, while a third com-
ponent, a lack of Personal Control, is associated with reports of disengaging,
denial, and emotion-focused coping (Heppner, Cook, et al., 1995; MacNair
& Elliott, 1992; Ritchey et al., 1984; Sabourin et al., 1990). For example,
higher levels of problem-solving confidence and a tendency to approach
problems were associated with reports of positive or engagement coping;
conversely, a lack of Personal Control and problem-solving confidence was
associated with more reports of disengagement coping activities (Ritchey
et al., 1984). Likewise, less personal control was associated with more resig-
nation for both men and women, and more emotional discharge and higher
levels of suppression and reactivity were associated with women in dis-
tressed marital relationships (Heppner, Cook, et al., 1995; Sabourin et al.,
1990).
In sum, consistent with Folkman and Lazarus’s cognitive transactional
theory of coping, there is a well-established association across 10 studies
between a positive problem-solving appraisal and problem-focused coping,
or approaching and attempting to alter the cause of a stressful problem. This
finding appears to be robust across different measures of coping, concurrent
and prospective designs, linear and cluster analytic procedures, and different
U. S. populations, and in one international sample. Two additional studies
suggest underlying mechanisms influencing the coping process; the
help from physicians and psychologists (Neal & Heppner, 1986; Tracey
et al., 1986), which suggests that there may be different patterns of help seek-
ing across different resources. Parenthetically, Heppner (1987) found that
students who made use of counseling services formed a bimodal distribution
of problem-solving appraisal scores, which suggests complex and perhaps
nonlinear relations in seeking help from counseling services.
In sum, three studies primarily using self-efficacy and career theories con-
sistently found that a more positive problem-solving appraisal was associ-
ated with help seeking in terms of being more aware of campus resources,
using them more often, and reporting greater satisfaction with such
resources. Conversely, problem-solving appraisal was not related to students
seeking help from physicians or psychologists, which may suggest different
patterns of help seeking across different resources. These studies focused on
predominately White college students in the Midwest, and the findings may
not generalize to minority populations.
gest that the relation between problem-solving appraisal and the utilization
of medical resources results in important differences in behavioral outcomes,
such as behavioral medical outcomes. The Heppner et al. (1988) study pro-
vides additional evidence that there may be important differences in the cog-
nitive processes of perceived effective and ineffective problem solvers,
which suggests that cognitive processes may be very useful target interven-
tions for perceived ineffective problem solvers. Although some of the 14
studies discussed in this section were guided by specific theories, for the
most part, research was guided by past research.
control over academic problems (Blankstein, Flett, & Watson, 1992). More-
over, when both the PSI and the Ways of Coping Scale (Folkman, Lazarus,
Dunkel-Schetter, Delongis, & Gruen, 1986) were used to predict trait and
state test anxiety, higher PSI avoidant scores added approximately 20% and
8% of unique variance in trait and state test anxiety, respectively. A third
study (Elliott, Godshall, Shrout, & Witty, 1990) of an academically at-risk
group found that problem-solving appraisal seems to be related to study
skills and habits that are important to functioning effectively in an academic
environment and, most important, to an important academic outcome, such
as course grades. Thus, self-appraised effective problem solving (and partic-
ularly Problem-Solving Confidence) was significantly associated with adap-
tive study habits and effective attitudes toward studying, as well as higher
semester grade point averages (the latter of which might also be considered a
behavioral outcome measure of coping). Likewise, Blankstein et al. (1992)
found that more positive appraisals of both Problem-Solving Confidence and
Personal Control were related to better academic grades.
The PSI has been found to have some associations with educational level
(e.g., Felton, Parsons, Bartoces, 1997; Neal, 1983); those with higher educa-
tional levels indicated a more positive problem-solving appraisal (Neal,
1983). Moreover, when levels of education were controlled across age
groups, older adults (M = 70 years) tended to score more positively on the PSI
total as well as the Problem-Solving Confidence factor (Haught, Hill, Nardi,
& Walls, 2000). When only groups with higher levels of education were
examined (college students and faculty), older adults tended to score more
positively on the PSI (r = –.34; Wright, Carscaddon, & Lambert, 2000).
In sum, nine separate investigations indicate that problem-solving
appraisal was not found to be significantly related to measures of intelligence
and academic aptitude but is related to test anxiety, education level, and age.
In essence, four studies suggest that problem-solving appraisal is not another
measure of intelligence. Three studies found problem-solving appraisal to be
related to test anxiety as well as study skills for at-risk students, which sug-
gests that problem-solving appraisal may be an important construct for aca-
demic interventions with some students. Moreover, two studies suggest that
problem-solving appraisal is related to educational level, and two other stud-
ies also found relations with age of the participants.
We will now synthesize the major findings and themes from this review
and draw the conclusion that in general, problem-solving appraisal is related
to problem-solving effectiveness, with some exceptions. Evidence will be
compiled regarding relationships between problem-solving appraisal and (a)
psychological, educational, and vocational adjustment and physical health;
(b) coping; (c) behavioral observations and outcomes; and (d) cognitive and
affective processes.
Psychological/Educational/Vocational
Adjustment and Physical Health
Coping
When cognitive processes were examined, data from a wide range of stud-
ies indicated that the self-perceived effective (as opposed to ineffective)
problem solvers reported cognitions theoretically consistent with effective
problem solving. For example, self-perceived effective problem solvers (a)
reported fewer irrational beliefs and dysfunctional thoughts (Heppner et al.,
1983); (b) attributed successful problem-solving performances to ability and
effort (Baumgardner et al., 1986; Larson & Sailors, 1997), attributed the
cause of their personal problems to a lack of effort (Baumgardner et al.,
1986), and viewed the solution as being more in their power to change (Mayo
& Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996), all of which increases the likelihood of persis-
tence in problem solving (Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987); (c) reported fewer
negative self-statements and debilitating cognitions (Larson et al., 1995) and
fewer task-inhibiting self-statements during difficult interpersonal encoun-
ters (Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996); (d) reported that they are more moti-
vated to solve problems, persist longer, and delay gratification longer in
struggling with problems (Heppner et al., 1982); (e) expected themselves to
be successful in problem solving (Heppner et al., 1982) and were more likely
to try difficult anagram tasks after poor performances (Moss, 1983); (f) gen-
erated more positive thoughts in general during a problem-solving training
course and specifically more positive thoughts about the class (Heppner
et al., 1988); (g) reported less test-irrelevant thinking in academic exams
(Blankstein et al., 1992); and (h) reported using more rational career
decision-making strategies (Phillips et al., 1984). The constellation of cogni-
tive processes reported by the perceived effective problem solvers suggests a
functional or adaptive problem-solving style. These motivational and
attributional patterns are associated with an increased likelihood of persis-
tence in problem solving (Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987) as well as problem-
focused coping associated with resolving problems (see Lazarus & Folkman,
1984), along with fewer negative and debilitating cognitions that typically
detract from task-relevant behavior.
Likewise, when affective processes are examined, data indicated the fol-
lowing composite. Self-perceived ineffective problem solvers reported (a)
more frequently hoping a problem would just disappear (Heppner et al.,
1982), (b) more emotional discharge for women in distressed marital rela-
tionships (Sabourin et al., 1990), (c) more emotion-focused coping attempts
(notably when participants lacked Personal Control; e.g., MacNair & Elliott,
1992), (d) more emotional arousal and intense affect (Larson et al., 1995) and
more emotion-focused self-statements (Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996) in
difficult interpersonal encounters, and (e) more emotional problems that they
could not handle without help (Heppner & Anderson, 1985). In sum, after
more than 20 years of research and more than 120 empirical studies, there is
now very strong support for the explanatory and predictive utility of the con-
struct of problem-solving appraisal. As a whole, the constellation of evi-
dence from the accumulated research can be interpreted as suggesting that
perceived effective problem solvers have better psychological and physical
health, better coping effectiveness, and better vocational adjustment in gen-
eral. This pattern of findings is found across a very wide range of adjustment
indices, cognitive and affective processes, behavioral observations or mea-
sures, research designs (e.g., between groups, cross-sectional, prospective,
longitudinal, qualitatively oriented process analyses), and at least nine differ-
ent cultures. In addition, problem-solving appraisal has been a useful con-
struct to explain a wide range of theoretical predictions and hypotheses of
human behavior. Moreover, these findings cannot be easily explained as
being the result of self-report biases, such as social desirability or negative
affectivity. In short, the picture that emerges is that the perceived effective
problem solvers appear to be more effective problem solvers in ways that
have been predicted by a wide range of theories.
Problem-Solving Effectiveness
A study by Larson et al. (1995) was one of the first to focus extensively on
the relations between problem-solving appraisal and problem-solving per-
formance. Larson et al. found that problem-solving appraisal did not relate to
problem-solving performance in responding to a disruptive student during a
15-minute lecture. Note, however, through interpersonal process recall
(Kagan, 1975) and thought listing (Cacioppo & Petty, 1981), findings indi-
cated that people with a negative problem-solving appraisal emitted more
negative statements, were more emotionally aroused, reported more intense
affect, had more self-focused statements, and reported more debilitating
cognitions and feelings during the task. A later study by Mayo and Tanaka-
Matsumi (1996) reported a similar pattern of results related to responding to
a difficult interpersonal problem.
Thus, the evidence suggests that problem-solving appraisal reflects
problem-solving performance or skill. However, there is evidence that a per-
son’s PSI score does not always reflect effective problem solving.
This article shows that research has created a broad knowledge base about
the PSI and identified a number of consistent patterns in the findings.
Although much more is now known from the PSI literature compared with 20
years ago, the existing findings also raise new questions and hypotheses. The
purpose of this section is to identify promising areas of inquiry that build on
and are likely to extend the PSI literature in significant ways. This section
focuses on identifying (a) topics that seem to be particularly worth examining
at this time and (b) general research strategies (rather than specific studies),
and it uses specific topics to illustrate the various strategies. The recommen-
dations for future research directions will be grouped into the following
seven subsections: Evidence of Problem-Solving Effectiveness, Examine
and Develop More Complex Models of Human Adjustment, Examine the
Utility of Problem-Solving Appraisal in Other Subgroups/Populations/
Cultures, Explore a Broader Array of Associations in Physical Health,
Examine the Utility of Enhancing Problem-Solving Appraisal in Remedial
and Preventive Interventions, Identify Determinants of Problem-Solving
Appraisal, and Further Explicate the Role of Problem-Solving Appraisal in
Coping. We hope that this section will (a) stimulate researchers to increase
the knowledge base of the PSI, (b) facilitate refinements in problem-solving
theory as well as other theories of human behavior, and (c) consequently,
enhance service delivery to clients by promoting the development of reme-
dial and preventive psychological interventions.
The evidence from this review suggests that PSI scores reflect problem-
solving skill in many but not all instances. Additional research is needed to
further explicate the link between the PSI and problem-solving skill.
One strategy may be to conceptualize problem-solving effectiveness
explicitly as an interplay of behavioral, cognitive, and affective operations
aimed at resolving the problematic situation. Although behavioral perfor-
mance is an essential component of problem-solving effectiveness, it may
only reflect one dimension of that effectiveness. For example, the studies by
Mayo and Tanaka-Matsumi (1996) and Larson et al. (1995) revealed that
overt behavioral performances were not often consistent with covert cog-
nitions and affective reactions; the measurement of overt behavioral perfor-
mance did not tell the whole story. Thus, examining the interplay between
problem-solving performance and concurrent cognitive and affective opera-
tions may provide a more complete understanding of problem-solving
effectiveness. Future researchers might broaden the conceptualization of
problem-solving effectiveness beyond behavioral performance and use a
variety of techniques such as thought listing and Interpersonal Process Recall
to assess a broader range of problem-solving variables related to problem-
solving ness.
A second strategy may be to continue to use contrived laboratory settings
to intensively study applied problem-solving behavioral performance (and
associated cognitive/affective operations). It may be most useful to create
laboratory studies that closely simulate real-life applied problems (see
Heppner, Kivlighan, & Wampold, 1999, for a discussion of dimensions of
analogue designs), such as those developed by Larson et al. (1995) and Mayo
and Tanaka-Matsumi (1996). These studies revealed a lack of theoretically
expected differences in problem-solving behavioral performance but theo-
retically consistent negative and debilitating cognitions and emotional
arousal. It is unclear whether the analogue nature of both studies (e.g., 15
minutes of a class presentation) allowed an adequate sampling of behavioral
problem solving; little is known about behavioral problem solving and the
dynamic nature of problem solving over time (see Heppner & Krauskopf,
1987). It might be necessary to examine behavioral problem-solving perfor-
mances (and associated cognitive/affective operations) on multiple occa-
sions to obtain an accurate assessment of applied problem solving. Research
could examine repeated problem-solving performances over time and across
different personality styles because some personality characteristics may
moderate different types of problem-solving performances (see Heppner &
Krauskopf, 1987).
lems (e.g., marital distress) may be particularly useful in clarifying the differ-
ential role of problem-solving appraisal across various indices of
psychological adjustment.
In areas where linear relationships are well established between the PSI
and other variables with particular populations, we recommend that
researchers examine more multifaceted models that include moderators,
mediators, and structural paths between problem-solving appraisal and indi-
ces of psychological and physical health as well as career adjustment. Such
lines of inquiry have a great deal of potential to identify more complex rela-
tions within applied problem solving, which will help researchers more
closely approximate the complexity people experience when grappling with
stressful life problems. We also encourage researchers to use longitudinal
designs that would allow stronger conclusions about casual relationships as a
result of problem-solving appraisal. In addition, we also encourage research-
ers to use more complex findings from one topic area to form hypotheses in
another topic. For example, given that problem-solving appraisal has served
to consistently moderate the relationship between stress and depression, this
paradigm might be useful to examine other areas, such as predicting physical
health outcomes like disease progression. Research that examines more mul-
tifaceted relations between problem-solving appraisal and various physical
and psychological health indices across different populations holds a great
deal of promise for identifying the multiple roles that problem-solving
appraisal plays in human adjustment, thereby extending theory and
enhancing practice interventions.
It may also be useful to examine the differential roles of the three PSI fac-
tors in more complex models of human adjustment. For example, this review
suggests that Personal Control and Approach-Avoidance Style may have dif-
ferent roles within physical health. In the career area, the study by Chartrand
et al. (1993) also found evidence for complex relations with the PSI factors,
namely, that Problem-Solving Confidence and Personal Control tended to
serve as mediators between some personality variables (especially
neuroticism) and the antecedents of career indecision. The results suggest
more complex mediating roles between personality variables and career
decision making. In addition, studies that examine interrelationships among
the problem-solving appraisal components, such as the Problem-Solving
Confidence Mediational Model of Psychological Adjustment (e.g., see
Heppner et al., 2002), suggest additional complexity among the PSI factors
and psychological adjustment. Examining more complex relations among
the PSI factors and indices of psychological well-being, physical health, and
vocational adjustment may help researchers understand more complex rela-
tions within problem-solving appraisal, which would be useful for both
theory and practice.
play a role in disease progression of cancer patients (Meyer & Mark, 1995),
researchers might develop interventions that target cancer patients’ coping
processes as well as their problem-solving appraisal. Researchers might also
examine the utility of problem-solving appraisal interventions in outreach
programs for at-risk populations. Because problem-solving appraisal was
related to study skills for at-risk students (as well as career planning and
decision-making variables), research might examine the utility of interven-
tions that target problem-solving appraisal along with other interventions
aimed at enhancing academic performance. The research literature indicated
that self-appraised ineffective problem solvers tend to experience more psy-
chological distress, to be unaware of helping resources and use them less fre-
quently, and to be less satisfied with resources or training opportunities.
These findings suggest a need to develop outreach interventions to facilitate
access to and use of helping resources for ineffective problem solvers.
Because training studies have revealed dysfunctional cognitions and affec-
tive reactions of perceived ineffective problem solvers during training (see
Heppner et al., 1988; Larson et al., 1990), research might also study the cog-
nitive, affective, and behavioral operations of participants across levels of
problem-solving appraisal during outreach and preventive interventions. In
sum, it seems promising to conduct investigations aimed at enhancing partic-
ipants’ problem-solving appraisal as an integral part of remedial and preven-
tive interventions, particularly for at-risk populations. The results of such
applied research could provide not only useful new information about devel-
oping remedial and preventive interventions but also new information about
the role of problem-solving appraisal in human adjustment and promote the-
ory development.
A slightly different research strategy may be to examine whether
problem-solving appraisal as an individual difference, or personal attribute
variable, interacts with interventions to affect an array of counseling out-
comes. This recommendation is in line with Fretz’s (1981) call for attrib-
ute-treatment-interaction (ATI) research in career counseling in which he
argued that career interventions could be enhanced if researchers considered
clients’ individual differences that might interact with treatment interven-
tions. One career study (M. J. Heppner, et al., in press) found that pre-
counseling PSI scores moderated counseling outcomes so that a more nega-
tive problem-solving appraisal was associated with less positive outcomes.
The results suggest that counselors may need to use different counseling
interventions with clients who have a negative problem-solving appraisal.
There is also some preliminary data that clients’problem-solving appraisal is
positively related to their perceptions of the counseling working alliance (M. J.
Heppner et al., in press), which suggests that clients’ PSI scores are also
related to counseling process variables. Examining the role of problem-solv-
plex and probably multidetermined coping process would facilitate both the-
ory development and practice implications.
One research strategy to increase knowledge about the coping process
would be to construct and test models that depict the role of problem-solving
appraisal and other constructs within the coping process through more
complex statistical methods. For example, studies might examine whether
problem-solving appraisal serves a moderating or mediating role between
variables such as level of stress and problem-focused coping (see section on
Depression for such designs). Other research might employ structural equa-
tion modeling to examine potential complex relationships among variables
such as problem-solving appraisal, problem- and emotion-focused coping,
help seeking, and outcomes of coping, such as degree of problem resolution.
It is possible that there are multiple paths to problem resolution across differ-
ent problem types, with complex interactions among coping, help seeking,
and problem-solving appraisal. Both theory and practice could be enhanced
by testing more complex and comprehensive models of the coping process in
hopes of identifying more specific relations between problem-solving
appraisal, various coping activities, and problem resolution.
Another research strategy would be to use analog designs to identify
important differences between individuals who perceive themselves as effec-
tive versus ineffective problem solvers. Granted there are many challenges in
using laboratory or hypothetical problem-solving studies, particularly
related to external validity (see Heppner & Wang, 2003), but such studies
lend themselves to greater experimental control. These types of methodolo-
gies have been underutilized in the PSI literature but may facilitate greater
understanding of cognitive, behavioral, and affective operations across indi-
viduals with different levels of problem-solving appraisal. Three studies
(Heppner et al., 1988; Larson et al., 1990; Mayo & Tanaka-Matsumi, 1996)
provide methodologies that enhanced understanding of cognitive and affec-
tive operations across different levels of problem-solving appraisal and that
might be useful in future studies. In addition, although a number of investiga-
tors have suggested that important problem-solving differences may be
found across different problem types (e.g., Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987;
Janis & Mann, 1977; Wickelgren, 1974), relatively little research has been
conducted in this area. Using hypothetical laboratory problems lends itself to
exercising a great deal of experimental control over different problems or
even different dimensions or characteristics of the problems being examined
(see Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987). Research using hypothetical problems
would be well suited to examining the associations between different types of
problems (or problem characteristics) and problem-solving appraisal with
subsequent cognitive, affective, and behavioral operations in coping.
Implications of Problem-Solving
Appraisal Research for Practitioners
Our review of the literature has delineated the robust role of problem-
solving appraisal in predicting a host of adaptive behaviors. Although it is
important that the findings of this review be applied to practice with caution,
problem-solving appraisal is a construct that has a great deal of relevance to a
broad range of practitioners including licensed professional counselors,
social workers, managed health care professionals, and student services per-
sonnel. The implications of problem-solving appraisal for practice are many
and varied (see Heppner, 1983; Heppner & Baker, 1997; Heppner & Wang,
2003). This section will briefly discuss implications for practice within three
major areas: conceptualization diagnosis, treatment interventions, and
evaluation of service delivery. Being aware of clients’PSI score can give the
counselor a significant amount of information about their problem-solving
appraisal and raise hypotheses about their psychological and vocational
adjustment and the conceptualization and diagnoses of clients’psychological
dynamics. Clients’problem-solving appraisal also can be helpful in planning
and conducting treatment interventions and can help provide an evaluation of
service delivery to clients.
Treatment Interventions
Treatment decisions can also be informed and enhanced by the PSI (see
Heppner & Baker, 1997). For example, a client with a highly avoidant
problem-solving style might prompt the practitioner to explore the reasons
underlying the avoidant tendencies, particularly when the avoidance relates
to the client’s presenting problem. In addition, the counselor can use this
knowledge of the client’s style to modify interventions, such as providing
more prompting and reinforcement of new behaviors for avoidant clients. If
clients lack confidence in their problem solving, they may need more emo-
tional support to complete homework assignments or other interventions
designed by the counselor and to develop a sense of self-efficacy to respond
appropriately to issues pertaining to their presenting problem. Helping pro-
fessionals might assist such clients by identifying specific goals and tasks
and how to take small steps toward their completion. The client could be
helped to feel success and reinforcement, could receive verbal reinforcement
from the counselor and others, and could participate in group interventions to
see others like themselves succeeding in the change process.
PSI scores also can suggest which clients will benefit most from interven-
tions and which may encounter the most obstacles (see Heppner, Cooper,
et al., 2001; Heppner et al., 1988), thus aiding practitioners in planning treat-
ment interventions. For example, the self-appraised effective problem solv-
ers are likely to think about the interventions more (Heppner et al., 1988) and
report higher levels of satisfaction with interventions (e.g., Heppner &
Krieshok, 1983), more resolution of their presenting problems in counseling
(e.g., Heppner, Cooper, et al., 2001), and more gains from training interven-
tions (e.g., Heppner et al., 1988). If a client has a positive problem-solving
appraisal, research would suggest that on average, this client has a greater
possibility of successfully resolving his or her problem. Conversely, if the
person has a negative problem-solving appraisal, research would suggest that
he or she may be less likely to feel satisfied with the intervention (e.g.,
Heppner & Krieshok, 1983) and less likely to resolve his or her presenting
problems (Heppner, Cooper, et al., 2001). If clients come to career counsel-
ing with a positive problem-solving appraisal, the likelihood that they will
have more effective outcomes in both career and psychological adjustment is
considerably greater (M. J. Heppner et al., in press). Thus, knowledge of cli-
ents’ PSI scores can be helpful in planning and evaluating case assignments
and in foreseeing the type of obstacles that clients may encounter and how
satisfied clients will be.
Problem-solving training has received considerable attention (see reviews
by D’Zurilla & Nezu, 1982; Heppner & Hillerbrand, 1991). For example,
training has focused on enhancing specific problem-solving skills, such as
decision making (e.g., Nezu & D’Zurilla, 1979), altering self-measurement
principles (e.g., Heppner et al., 1988), and increasing problem-solving confi-
dence (e.g., Heppner et al., 1988). Training has varied from 45 minutes (e.g.,
Nezu & D’Zurilla, 1979) to 2 months (e.g., Heppner et al., 1988). Research
has indicated that problem-solving training is a successful intervention strat-
egy for specific presenting problems such as depression (e.g., Nezu et al.,
1989).
Thus, problem-solving training may be a helpful intervention for a range
of client problems that have been predicted by problem-solving appraisal.
Practitioners might design interventions for a variety of presenting problems
and disorders that include problem-specific interventions (e.g., Stein et al.,
2001) along with general problem-solving training components addressing
problem-solving confidence and avoidance tendencies (see Etringer et al.,
1989; Soukup et al., 1990). Moreover, because problem-solving appraisal is
associated with a broad range of psychological, vocational-adjustment, and
physical health indices, problem-solving training might profitably be incor-
porated into preventive interventions (see Heppner, Neal, & Larson, 1984).
For example, because problem-solving appraisal is associated with a broad
range of adaptive outcomes, it may be helpful to promote the development of
effective problem-solving skills (and consequently a positive problem-
solving appraisal) in settings such as schools (K-12) and through a variety of
programs, such as marriage enrichment programs and parent effectiveness
training. Because research indicates that individuals with a negative
problem-solving appraisal are at risk for depression under high levels of
stress (e.g., Cheng & Lam, 1997; Nezu & Ronan, 1988), preventive interven-
tions might profitably be developed in schools and universities for at-risk
individuals. In short, the research suggests that practitioners might develop
remedial and preventive interventions aimed at a variety of societal problems
that incorporate problem-solving training components.
FINAL COMMENT
resolving difficult problems. More than 120 studies have established consis-
tent patterns in the knowledge base. However, as knowledge accumulates,
more complex questions should be examined that stimulate additional
research and theory development. We hope that research will continue to
expand knowledge about problem-solving appraisal and stimulate additional
theory development in problem solving, depression, suicide, and other areas
of concern to the helping professions. But most important, because problem-
solving appraisal is learned, it implies it is amenable to change, thus provid-
ing hope for millions to bring positive change to their lives. There appear to
be many exciting possibilities for applied interventions to build on people’s
strengths in problem-solving appraisal to enhance life satisfaction and well-
being.
REFERENCES
Achenbach, T., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and revised
Child Behavior Profile. Unpublished manuscript, Departments of Psychiatry and Psychol-
ogy, University of Vermont. (Available from the first author)
Allred, K. D., & Smith, T. W. (1989). The hardy personality: Cognitive and physiological
responses to evaluative threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 257-266.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Andersen, B. L., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1994). A biobehavioral model of cancer
stress and disease course. American Psychologist, 49, 389-404.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Annis, H. M., & Davis, C. S. (1989). Relapse prevention training: A cognitive-behavioral
approach based on self-efficacy theory. Journal of Chemical Dependency Treatment, 2, 81-
103.
Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New York: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37,
122-147.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective
of emotion theory. American Psychologist, 55, 1247-1263.
Baumgardner, A. H., Heppner, P. P., & Arkin, R. M. (1986). Role of causal attribution in personal
problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 636-643.
Ben-Yishay, Y., & Prigatano, G. P. (1990). Cognitive remediation. In M. Rosenthal, E. R.
Griffith, M. R. Bond, & J. D Miller (Eds.), Rehabilitation of the adult and child with trau-
matic brain injury (2nd ed., pp. 393-409). Philadelphia: Davis.
Black, C. (1982). It will never happen to me. Denver, CO: Mac Publishing.
Blankstein, K. R., Flett, G. L., & Batten, I. (1989). Test anxiety and problem-solving self-
appraisals of college students. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 531-540.
Blankstein, K. R., Flett, G. L., & Watson, M. S. (1992). Coping and academic problem-solving
ability in test anxiety. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 37-46.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). General affective dispositions in physical and psychological
health. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology:
The health perspective (pp. 221-245). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
Clum, G. A., & Febbraro, G. A. R. (1994). Stress, social support, and problem-solving appraisal
skills: Prediction of suicide severity within a college sample. Journal of Psychotherapy and
Behavioral Assessment, 16, 69-83.
Cook, E. P. (1990). Gender and psychological distress. Journal of Counseling and Development,
68, 371-375.
Cook, S. W., & Heppner, P. P. (1997). A psychometric study of three coping measures. Educa-
tional and Psychological Measurement, 57, 906-923.
Corcoran, C. M. (1991). Self-appraised personal problem solving in an inmate population.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri, Columbia.
Courtois, C. A. (1988). Healing the incest wound: Adult survivors in therapy. New York: Norton.
Coyne, D. C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). Cognitive style, stress perception, and coping. In D. C.
Kutash & A. L. B. Schlesinger (Eds.), Handbook on stress and anxiety (pp. 144-158). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Crites, J. C. (1978). Career maturity inventory: Theory and research handbook (2nd ed.).
Monterey, CA: McGraw-Hill.
Crowne, D., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approved motive. New York: Wiley.
Davey, G. C. (1994). Worrying, social problem-solving abilities, and social problem-solving
confidence. Behavior Research & Therapy, 32, 327-330.
Davey, G. C., Hampton, J., Farrell, J., & Davidson, S. (1992). Some characteristics of worrying:
Evidence for worrying and anxiety as separate constructs. Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 13, 133-147.
Davey, G. C., Tallis, F., & Capuzzo, N. (1996). Beliefs about the consequences of worrying. Cog-
nitive Therapy and Research, 20, 499-520.
DeClue, G. S. (1983). Patterns of intellectual functioning, ability, personality, and problem solv-
ing style. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri, Columbia.
Dewey, J. (1933). How we think. New York: Heath.
Dixon, D. N., & Glover, J. A. (1984). Counseling: A problem solving approach. New York:
Wiley.
Dixon, W. A. (2000). Problem-solving appraisal and depression: Evidence for a recovery model.
Journal of Counseling and Development, 78, 87-91.
Dixon, W. A., Heppner, P. P., & Anderson, W. P. (1991). Problem-solving appraisal, stress, hope-
lessness, and suicide ideation in a college population. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38,
51-56.
Dixon, W. A., Heppner, P. P., Burnett, J. W., Anderson, W. P., & Wood, P. K. (1993). Distinguish-
ing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences of problem-solving appraisal and
depressive symptoms. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 357-364.
Dixon, W. A., Heppner, P. P., & Rudd, M. (1994). Problem-solving appraisal, hopelessness, and
suicide ideation: Evidence for a mediational model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41,
91-98.
Dohrenwend, B. S., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (1978). Some issues in research on stressful life events.
Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 166, 7-15.
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy; an analysis in terms of learn-
ing, thinking, and culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Dorner, D. (1983). Heuristics and cognition in complex systems. In M. G. R. Groner, & W. F.
Bischof (Eds.), Method in heuristics (pp. 89-107). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dugas, M. J., Letarte, H., Rheaume, J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995). Worry and
problem solving: Evidence of a specific relationship. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19,
109-120.
Durlak, J. A. (1983). Social problem solving as a preliminary prevention strategy. In R. D. Flener,
L. A. Jason, J. N. Moritsugu, & S. S. Faber (Eds.), Preventive psychology: Theory, research
and practice (pp. 31-48). New York: Pergamon.
D’Zurilla, T. J. (1986). Problem-solving therapy: A social competence approach to clinical
intervention. New York: Springer.
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem solving and behavior modification. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 107-126.
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. (1982). Social problem solving adults. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.),
Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (pp. 202-275). Orlando, FL: Aca-
demic Press.
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1990). Development and preliminary evaluation of the Social
Problem-Solving Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 2, 156-163.
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1999). Problem solving therapy: A social competence approach
to clinical intervention (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
Educational Testing Service (1979). School and college ability tests (SCAT): Series IIII. Menlo
Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.
Elliott, T. R. (1992). Problem-solving appraisal, oral contraceptive use, and menstrual pain.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 286-297.
Elliott, T. R., Godshall, F. J., Herrick, S. M., Witty, T. E., & Spruell, M. (1991). Problem-solving
appraisal and psychological adjustment following spinal cord injury. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 15, 387-398.
Elliott, T. R., Godshall, F., Shrout, J. R., & Witty, T. E. (1990). Problem-solving appraisal, self-
reported study habits, and performance of academically at-risk college students. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 37, 203-207.
Elliott, T. R., Herrick, S. M., MacNair, R. R., & Harkins, S. W. (1994). Personality correlates of
self-appraised problem solving ability: Problem orientation and trait affectivity. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 63, 489-505.
Elliott, T. R., Herrick, S. M., & Witty, T. E. (1992). Problem-solving appraisal and the effects of
social support among college students and persons with physical disabilities. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 39, 219-226.
Elliott, T. R., & Marmarosh, C. L. (1994). Problem-solving appraisal, health complaints, and
health-related expectancies. Journal of Counseling and Development, 72, 531-537.
Elliott, T. R., Sherwin, E., Harkins, S. W., & Marmarosh, C. (1995). Self-appraised problem-
solving ability, affective states, and psychological distress. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
ogy, 42, 105-115.
Elliot, T. R., Shewchuk, R., Hagglund, K., Rybarczyk, B., & Harkins, S. (1996). Occupational
burnout, tolerance for stress, and coping among nurses in rehabilitation units. Rehabilitation
Psychology, 41, 267-284.
Elliot, T. R., Shewchuk, R., Richeson, C., Pickelman, H., & Franklin, K. W. (1996). Problem-
solving appraisal and the prediction of depression during pregnancy and in the postpartum
period. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74, 645-651.
Ellis, T. E., Rudd, M., Rajab, M., & Wehrly, T. E. (1996). Cluster analysis of MCMI scores of sui-
cidal psychiatric patients: Four personality profiles. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52, 411-
422.
Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. (1990). Multidimensional assessment of coping: A critical evalua-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 844-854.
Etringer, B. D., Altmaier, E. M., & Bowers, W. (1989). An investigation into the cognitive func-
tioning of bulimic women. Journal of Counseling and Development, 68, 216-219.
Felton, G. M., Liu, Q., Parsons, M. A., & Geslani, G. P. (1998). Health-promoting behaviors of
rural adolescent women. Women & Health, 27, 67-80.
Felton, G. M., Parsons, M. A., & Bartoces, M. G. (1997). Demographic factors: Interaction
effects on health-promoting behavior and health related factors. Public Health Nursing, 14,
361-367.
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually victimized children. New York: Free Press.
Fisher, S. (1986). Stress and strategy. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Per-
spectives on the development of memory and cognition (pp. 3-34). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and coping processes: A theoretical analysis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 839-852.
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Delongis, A., & Gruen, R. (1986). Dynamics of
a stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 50, 992-1003.
Fraser, K. P., & Tucker, C. M. (1997). Individuation, stress, and problem-solving abilities of col-
lege students. Journal of College Student Development, 38, 461-467.
Fretz, B. R. (1981). Evaluating the effectiveness of career interventions. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 28, 77-90.
Friedman, H. S. (Ed.). (1992). Hostility, coping, & health. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
logical Association.
Funk, S. C., & Houston, B. (1987). A critical analysis of the Hardiness Scale’s validity and utility.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 572-578.
Geiser, R. (1979). Hidden victims: The sexual abuse of children. Boston: Beacon.
Gelman, D. (1994, April). The mystery of suicide. Newsweek, 44-49.
Godshall, F. J., & Elliott, T. R. (1997). Behavioral correlates of self-appraised problem-solving
ability: Problem-solving skills and health-compromising behaviors. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 27, 929-944.
Goisman, R. M., Allsworth, J., Rogers, M. P., Warshaw, M. G., Goldenberg, I., Vasile, R. G., et al.
(1998). Simple phobia as a comorbid anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 7, 105-112.
Goldapple, G. C., & Montgomery, D. (1993). Evaluating a behaviorally based intervention to
improve client retention in therapeutic community treatment for drug dependency. Research
on Social Work Practice, 3, 21-39.
Grant, J. S. (1999). Social problem-solving partnerships with family caregivers. Rehabilitation
Nursing, 24, 245-260.
Grant, J. S., Elliott, T. R., Giger, J. N., & Bartolucci, A. A. (2001). Social problem-solving abili-
ties, social support, and adjustment among family caregivers of individuals with a stroke.
Rehabilitation Psychology, 46, 44-57.
Hammen, C. L. (1985). Predicting depression: A cognitive-behavioral perspective. In P. C.
Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (pp. 29-71). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Hanson, K. M., & Mintz, L. B. (1997). Psychological health and problem-solving self-appraisal
in older adults. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 433-441.
Harrison, H. A. (1994). The relationship among Jungian psychological type preferences, prob-
lems experienced, and perceived problem-solving capabilities for African Americans hold-
ing graduate degrees. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, St. Louis University.
Haught, P. A., Hill, L. A., Nardi, A. H., & Walls, R. T. (2000). Perceived ability and level of edu-
cation as predictors of traditional and practical adult problem solving. Experimental Aging
Research, 26, 89-101.
Henry, P. (1996). Perceived problem-solving and vocational identity: Implication for nontradi-
tional premedical students. Psychological Reports, 78, 843-847.
Henry, P. (1999). Relationship between perceived problem solving and career maturity: Implica-
tions for minority and disadvantaged premedical students. Psychological Reports, 84, 1040-
1046.
Heppner, M. J., Lee, D., Heppner, P. P., McKinnon, L. C., Multon, K. D., & Gysbers, N. C. (in
press). Examining the role of problem-solving appraisal in the process and outcome of career
counseling. Journal of Vocational Behavior.
Heppner, P. P. (1983). Strategies for facilitating client’s personal problem solving. In P. A. Keller
& L. G. Ritt (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A source book (Vol. 2, pp. 73-85).
Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange.
Heppner, P. P. (1987). Introduction: Broadening the conceptualization of problem solving in
career planning. Journal of Career Development, 14, 2-7.
Heppner, P. P. (1988). The Problem Solving Inventory (PSI): Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists.
Heppner, P. P., & Anderson, W. P. (1985). On the perceived non-utility of research in counseling.
Journal of Counseling and Development, 63, 545-547.
Heppner, P. P., & Baker, C. E. (1997). Applications of the Problem Solving Inventory. Measure-
ment and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 29, 229-241.
Heppner, P. P., Baumgardner, A., & Jackson, J. (1985). Problem solving self-appraisal, depres-
sion, and attributional styles: Are they related? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 105-113.
Heppner, P. P., Baumgardner, A. H., Larson, L. M., & Petty, R. E. (1988). The utility of problem
solving training that emphasizes self-management principles. Counseling Psychology Quar-
terly, 1, 129-143.
Heppner, P. P., Cook, S. W., Strozier, A. L., & Heppner, M. J. (1991). An investigation of coping
styles and gender differences with farmers in career transition. Journal of Counseling Psy-
chology, 38, 167-174.
Heppner, P. P., Cook, S. W., Wright, D. M., & Johnson, W. C., Jr. (1995). Progress in resolving
problems: A problem-focused style of coping. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 279-
293.
Heppner, P. P., Cooper, C. C., Mulholland, A. M., & Wei, M. F. (2001). A brief, multidimen-
sional, problem solving based psychotherapy outcome measure. Journal of Counseling Psy-
chology, 48, 330-343.
Heppner, P. P., Hibel, J. H., Neal, G. W., Weinstein, C. L., & Rabinowitz, F. E. (1982). Personal
problem solving: A descriptive study of individual differences. Journal of Counseling Psy-
chology, 24, 580-590.
Heppner, P. P., & Hillerbrand, E. T. (1991). Problem-solving training: Implications for remedial
and preventive training. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social and clini-
cal psychology: The healthy perspective (pp. 681-698). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
Heppner, P. P., Kampa, M., & Brunning, L. (1987). The relationship between problem solving
self-appraisal and indices of physical and psychological health. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 11, 155-168.
Heppner, P. P., Kivlighan, D., Jr., & Wampold, B. E. (1999). Research design in counseling (2nd
ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Heppner, P. P., & Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). An information-processing approach to personal prob-
lem solving. The Counseling Psychologist, 15, 371-447.
Kagan, N. (1975). Interpersonal process recall: A method for influencing human interaction.
East Lansing: Michigan State University.
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al.
(1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III—R psychiatric disorders in the
United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Study. Archives of General Psychia-
try, 51, 8-19.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1995). Psychoneuroimmunology and health consequences:
Data and shared mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 269-274.
Kim, B. S., Atkinson, D. R., & Umemoto, D. (2001). Asian cultural values and the counseling
process: Current knowledge and directions for future research. The Counseling Psychologist,
29, 570-603.
Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and health: An inquiry into hardiness.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1-11.
Kobasa, S. C. (1982). Commitment and coping in stress resistance among lawyers. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 707-717.
Krumboltz, J. D. (1965). Behavioral counseling: Rationale and research. Personnel and Guid-
ance Journal, 44, 383-387.
Laporte, L., Sabourin, S., & Wright, J. (1988). L’inventaire de resolution de problemes
personels: Une perspective metocognitive. International Journal of Psychology, 23, 569-
581.
Larson, L. M. (1984). Training self-appraised effective and ineffective problem solvers in asser-
tion. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Missouri, Columbia.
Larson, L. M., Allen, S. J., Imao, R. A., & Piersel, W. C. (1993). Self-perceived effective and
ineffective problem solvers’ differential views of their partners’ problem-solving styles.
Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 528-532.
Larson, L. M., & Heppner, P. P. (1985). The relationship of problem-solving appraisal to career
decision and indecision. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 26, 55-65.
Larson, L. M., & Heppner, P. P. (1989). Problem-solving appraisal in an alcoholic population.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 73-78.
Larson, L. M., Heppner, P. P., Ham, T., & Dugan, K. (1988). Investigating multiple subtypes of
career indecision through cluster analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 439-446.
Larson, L. M., Piersel, W. C., Imao, R. A., & Allen, S. J. (1990). Significant predictors of prob-
lem-solving appraisal. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 482-490.
Larson, L. M., Potenza, M. T., Wennstedt, L. W., & Sailors, P. J. (1995). Personal problem solv-
ing in a simulated setting: Do perceptions accurately reflect behavior? Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 19, 241-257.
Larson, L. M., & Sailors, P. J. (1997). Perceived effective problem solvers’ attributions concern-
ing success and failure of a simulated real-life problem. Behavior Modification, 21, 341-354.
Larson, L. M., Toulouse, A. L., Ngumba, W. E., Fitzpatrick, L. A., & Heppner, P. P. (1994). The
development and validation of Coping with Career Indecision. Journal of Career Assess-
ment, 2, 91-110.
Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Lazarus, R. S. (2000). Toward better research on stress and coping. American Psychologist, 55,
665-673.
Lazarus, R. S., DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Gruen, R. (1985). Stress and adaptational out-
comes: The problem of confounded measures. American Psychologist, 40, 770-785.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1966). Internal versus external control of reinforcement: A review. Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 65, 206-220.
Long, B. C., Kahn, S. E., & Schutz, R. W. (1992). Causal model of stress and coping: Women in
management. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 227-239.
MacNair, R. R., & Elliott, T. R. (1992). Self-perceived problem-solving ability, stress appraisal,
and coping over time. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 150-164.
Marcotte, D., Alain, M., & Gosselin, M.-J. (1999). Gender differences in adolescent depression:
Gender-typed characteristics or problem-solving skills deficits? Sex Roles, 41, 31-48.
Mayo, V. D., & Tanaka-Matsumi, J. (1996). Think aloud statements and solutions of dysphoric
persons on a social problem-solving task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 97-113.
McCracken, R. S., & Weitzman, L. M. (1997). Relationship of personal agency, problem-solving
appraisal, and traditionality of career choice to women’s attitudes toward multiple role plan-
ning. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 149-159.
McCubbin, H. I., & Patterson, J. M. (1982). Family adaptation to crisis. In H. I. McCubbin, A. E.
Cauble, & J. M. Patterson (Eds.), Family stress, coping, and social support (pp. 26-47).
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
McCullough, L., & Farrell, A. D. (1983). The computerized assessment for psychotherapy evalu-
ation and research. New York: Department of Psychiatry, Beth Israel Medical Center.
McGrath, E., Keita, G. P., Strickland, B. R., & Russo, N. F. (1990). Women and depression: Risk
factors and treatment issues (Final report of the American Psychological Association’s
National Task Force on Women and Depression). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Mechanic, D. (1968). The study of social stress and its relationship to disease. In D. Mechanic
(Ed.), Medical sociology (pp. 294-322). New York: Free Press.
Mechanic, D. (1970). Discussion of research programs on relations between stressful life events
and episodes of physical illness. In B. S. Dohrenwend & B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds.), Stressful
life events (pp. 87-97). New York: Wiley.
Meichenbaum, D., & Asarnow, J. (1979). Cognitive-behavioral modification and metacognitive
development: Implications for the classroom. In P. C. Kendall & S. D. Hollon (Eds.),
Cognitive-behavioral interventions: Theory, research, and procedure (pp. 11-35). New
York: Academic Press.
Meiselman, K. C. (1978). Incest: A psychological study of causes and effects with treatment rec-
ommendations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Meyer, T. J., & Mark, M. M. (1995). Effects of psychosocial interventions with adult cancer
patients: A meta-analysis of randomized experiments. Health Psychology, 14, 101-108.
Miller, M. J., Karriker, C. S., & Pilgreen, J. C. (1984). Personality consistency and perceived
problem-solving behavior and attitudes: An extension of Holland’s typology. Journal of
Employment Counseling, 21, 162-167.
Miller, S. I. (1986). Adult children of alcoholics. Hospital & Community Psychiatry, 37, 235-
236.
Mooney, R. L., & Gordon, L. V. (1950). Manual: The Mooney Problem Checklists. New York:
Psychological Corporation.
Moss, S. (1983). Formal and informal problem solving and age related differences. Unpublished
master’s thesis, University of Missouri, Columbia.
Neal, G. W. (1983). Personal problem solving: Awareness and utilization of campus helping
resources. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri, Columbia.
Neal, G. W., & Heppner, P. P. (1986). Problem-solving self-appraisal, awareness, and utilization
of campus helping resources. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 39-44.
Neville, H. A., Heppner, P. P., & Wang, L. (1997). Relations among racial identity attitudes, per-
ceived stressors, and coping styles in African American college students. Journal of Coun-
seling and Development, 75, 303-311.
Platt, J. J., Spivack, G., Altman, N., Altman, D., & Peizer, S. B. (1974). Adolescent problem-
solving thinking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 787-793.
Pretorius, T. (1992). Problem-solving appraisal in the association of life stress and depression: A
South African study. Psychological Reports, 71, 855-862.
Pretorius, T. (1996). The family environment of students self-appraised as effective and ineffec-
tive problem solvers. Psychological Reports, 79, 915-921.
Pretorius, T. B., & Diedricks, M. (1994). Problem-solving appraisal, social support and stress-
depression relationship. South African Journal of Psychology, 24, 86-90.
Pretorius, T., Heyns, P., & Broekmann, N. (1991). The effect of a computer-assisted career guid-
ance system on the career development of South African students. South African Journal of
Psychology, 21, 119-123.
Priester, M. J., & Clum, G. A. (1993). Perceived problem-solving ability as a predictor of depres-
sion, hopelessness, and suicide ideation in a college population. Journal of Counseling Psy-
chology, 40, 79-85.
Ptacek, J., Smith, R. E., & Zanas, J. (1992). Gender, appraisal, and coping: A longitudinal analy-
sis. Journal of Personality, 60, 747-770.
Rabin, L.A. (2001). Test usage patterns and perceived ecological utility of neuropsychological
assessment techniques: A survey of North American clinical neuropsychologists. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Fordham University.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general
population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401.
Rath, J. F., Hennessy, J. J., & Diller, L. (2003). Social problem solving and community integra-
tion in postacute rehabilitation outpatients with traumatic brain injury. Rehabilitation Psy-
chology, 48, 137-144.
Rath, J. F., Simon, D., Langenbahn, D. M., Sherr, R. L., & Diller, L. (2000). Measurement of
problem-solving deficits in adults with acquired brain damage. Journal of Head Trauma
Rehabilitation, 15, 724-733.
Rath, J. F., Simon, D., Langenbahn, D. M., Sherr, R. L., & Diller, L. (in press). Group treatment of
problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: A randomized outcome
study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation.
Rath, J. F., Langenbahn, D. M., Simon, D., Sherr, R. L., Fletcher, J., & Diller, L. (in press). The
construct of problem solving in higher-level neuropsychological assessment and
rehabilation. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology.
Reeder, B. L., & Heppner, P. P. (1985). Personal problem-solving activities of Black university
students. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 13, 154-163.
Reid, J. K., & Dixon, W. A. (2000). The relationships among grief experience, problem-solving
appraisal, and depression: An exploratory study. Journal of Personal and Interpersonal Loss,
5, 77-93.
Reis, S. D., & Heppner, P. P. (1993). Examination of coping resources and family adaptation in
mothers and daughters of incestuous versus nonclinical families. Journal of Counseling Psy-
chology, 40, 100-108.
Resick, P. A. (2001). Stress and trauma. Philadelphia: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
Rich, A. R., & Bonner, R. L. (1987). Interpersonal moderators of depression among college stu-
dents. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28, 337-342.
Rich, A. R., & Bonner, R. L. (1989). Support for a pluralistic approach to the treatment of depres-
sion. Journal of College Student Development, 30, 426-431.
Ritchey, K. M., Carscaddon, D. M., & Morgan, C. H. (1984). Problem-solving appraisal versus
hypothetical problem solving. Psychological Reports, 55, 815-818.
Roos, P. E., & Cohen, L. H. (1987). Sex roles and social support as moderators of life stress
adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 576-585.
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforce-
ment. Psychological Monographs, 80, 1-28.
Rowe, H. A. H. (1985). Problem solving and intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rudd, M., Rajab, M., & Dahm, P. (1994). Problem-solving appraisal in suicide ideators and
attempters. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 64, 136-149.
Rudd, M., Rajab, M., Orman, D. T., Stulman, D. A., Joiner, T., & Dixon, W. (1996). Effectiveness
of an outpatient intervention targeting suicidal young adults: Preliminary results. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 179-190.
Sabourin, S., Laporte, L., & Wright, J. (1990). Problem-solving self-appraisal and coping efforts
in distressed and nondistressed couples. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 16, 89-97.
Sahin, N., Sahin, N. H., & Heppner, P. P. (1993). Psychometric properties of the Problem Solving
Inventory in a group of Turkish university students. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17,
379-396.
Sarmány, I., & Sládeková, L. (1990). Personality aspects of strategy production in solving the
anticipatory task. Studia Psychologica, 32, 97-106.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implica-
tions of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247.
Schlossberg, N. K. (1981). A model for analyzing human adaptation to transition. The Counsel-
ing Psychologist, 9, 2-18.
Schotte, D. E., & Clum, G. A. (1982). Suicide ideation in a college population: A test of a model.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 690-696.
Schotte, D. E., & Clum, G. A. (1987). Problem-solving skills in suicidal psychiatric patients.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 49-54.
Selye, H. (1984). The stress of life (Rev. ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Sher, K. J. (1991). Children of alcoholics: A critical appraisal of theory and research. Chicago:
The University of Chicago Press.
Shorkey, C. T., McRoy, R. G., & Armendariz, J. (1985). Intensity of parental punishments and
problem-solving attitudes and behaviors. Psychological Reports, 56, 283-286.
Shure, M. B. (1982). Interpersonal problem solving: A cog in the wheel of social cognition. In
F. C. Serafica (Ed.), Social-cognitive development in context (pp. 133-166). New York:
Guilford.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1972). Means-ends thinking, adjustment, and social class among
elementary-school-aged children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 38, 348-
353.
Slavkin, S. L., Heimberg, R. G., Winning, C. D., & McCaffrey, R. J. (1992). Personal and paren-
tal problem drinking: Effects on problem-solving performance and self-appraisal. Addictive
Behaviors, 17, 191-199.
Smith, C. A. (1991). The self, appraisal, and coping. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth (Eds.),
Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The healthy perspective (pp. 116-137).
Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
Snyder, C. R., & Ford, C. E. (1987). Coping with negative life events: Clinical and social psycho-
logical perspectives. New York: Plenum.
Soukup, V. M., Beiler, M. E., & Terrell, F. (1990). Stress, coping style, and problem solving abil-
ity among eating-disordered inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 592-599.
Spence, J. T. (1991). Do the BSRI and PAQ measure the same or different concepts? Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 15, 141-165.
Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. (1974). Social adjustment of young children: A cognitive approach
to solving real-life problems. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Stein, R. I., Saelens, B. E., Dounchis, J. Z., Lewczyk, C. M., Swenson, A. K., & Wilfley, D. E.
(2001). Treatment of eating disorders in women. The Counseling Psychologist, 29, 695-732.
Wortman, C. B., Sheedy, C., Gluhoski, V., & Kessler, R. C. (1992). Stress, coping, and health:
Conceptual issues and directions for future research. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Hostility, cop-
ing, & health (pp. 227-256). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wright, D. M., & Heppner, P. P. (1991). Coping among nonclinical college-age children of alco-
holics. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 565-572.
Wright, D. M., & Heppner, P. P. (1993). Examining the well-being of non-clinical college stu-
dents: Is knowledge of the presence of parental alcoholism useful? Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 40, 324-334.
Wright, M. B., Carscaddon, D.M., & Lambert, S. D. (2000). Sex, education, age, and cautious-
ness: Implications for counselors. Adultspan Journal, 2, 113-122.
Zeidner, M., & Endler, N. S. (Eds.). (1996). Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications.
New York: Wiley.