The Rhythms of English Poetry-Routledge (1982)
The Rhythms of English Poetry-Routledge (1982)
The Rhythms of English Poetry-Routledge (1982)
TI TLE NO. 14
AN INTRODUCTION TO 7
MODERN ENGLISH WORD-FORMATION
Valerie Adams
COHESION IN ENGLISH 9
M. A. K. Halliday and Ruquaiya Hasan
AN INTRODUCTION TO ENGLISH 10
TRANSFORMATIONAL SYNTAX
Rodney Huddleston
DESIGNS IN PROSE 12
Walter Nash
STYLE IN FICTION 13
Geoffrey N. Leech and Michael H. Short
THE RHYTHMS OF ENGLISH POETRY 14
Derek Attridge
THE LANGUAGE OF HUMOUR 16
Walter Nash
GOOD ENGLISH AND THE GRAMMARIAN 17
Sidney Greenbaum
DEREK ATTRIDGE
University of Strathclyde
H Routledge
Taylor S. Francis Group
LONDON AND NEW YORK
First published 1982 by Pearson Education Limited
Fifth impression 1999
,
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group an informa business
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in
any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter in
vented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or re
trieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
Notices
Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and
experience broaden our understanding, changes in research methods, professional
practices, or medical treatment may become necessary.
Practitioners and researchers must always rely on their own experience and knowledge
in evaluating and using any information, methods, compounds, or experiments de
scribed herein. In using such information or methods they should be mindful of their
own safety and the safety of others, including parties for whom they have a professional
responsibility.
To the fullest extent of the law, neither the Publisher nor the authors, contributors, or
editors, assume any liability for any injury and/or damage to persons or property as a
matter of products liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or operation of
any methods, products, instructions, or ideas contained in the material herein.
Attridge, Derek
The rhythms of English poetry. -(English language
series; 14)
1. English language - Versification
I. Title II. Series
921’.009 PEI 509 80-42114
Foreword
In one o f the first books in this series, Ian Gordon was concerned with
the claim that the style and rhythm, The M ovem ent o f English P ro se ,
displayed a throb o f continuity over a millennium and more. D r
Attridge in the present volum e is concerned with no less a claim, no
less grand a them e: the unity o f tradition, extending over six hundred
years, m anifested by the main stream o f English ‘accentual-syllabic’
verse. His exposition o f this them e dem ands an initial exam ination o f
the partly distinct, partly intertwined theories that have informed
critical approaches to poetics: and then - the bulk o f this volum e - a
detailed analysis in turn o f ‘rhythm’ and ‘m etre’, them selves also partly
distinct and partly intertwined, and dem anding from the reader both a
sensitive ear and an appreciation o f technical, logical argument.
D r Attridge brings to this daunting enterprise a well-practised
expertise in the field. H e w on high acclaim - to give one outstanding
exam ple - for his book on Elizabethan classical verse, Well-weighed
Syllables, which was published in 1974. But in that work, as in this, one
is struck not only by the keen historical know ledge o f poetic form but
by the deep personal involvem ent in (and love o f) poetry itself. Even
these, though essential, are not sufficient. D erek Attridge has made
him self expert in linguistics - historical, traditional, structural, and
transformational. A nd all these aspects o f his scholarship he is able to
com m unicate withl enthusiasm and conviction. A s with som e other
successful books in this series, w e have here an author w ho is a true
‘philologist’, effortlessly straddling literary values and linguistic
technicalities, convincingly showing the relevance o f each to the other,
excitingly indicating analogies with music at one m om ent and basic
relationships with ordinary speech at another.
Indeed, for all its artfulness and (som etim es strenuous) com plexity,
poetry is not disjunct from but intimately bedded in the m ost
com m onplace fundam entals o f our everyday speech rhythm and
grammar: even in the m ost everyday strategies o f conversational
vi
RANDOLPH QUIRK
University C ollege London
January, 1982
Preface
in m ost cases left identification to the end o f the book. Exam ples are
numbered throughout each chapter, using the following conventions:
(8), quotation identified in the list o f sources; (8 ), repeated quotation;
[8], invented construct; [8a], rewritten quotation or construct, relating
directly to (8) or [8]. I have had to make frequent reference to a
hypothetical (though never ideal) poet or reader; if these individuals
are consistently m asculine, it is only because I have found no
practicable way o f evading the established convention.
My work on this subject ow es a great deal, both directly and
indirectly, to my teachers and fellow -teachers, students and
fellow-students (to use four categories that overlap considerably)
during the course o f many years. I can single out for individual thanks
only a few o f those who have responded willingly and helpfully to
questions and drafts: Sidney A llen, John Birtwhistle, John Hollander,
Samuel Jay Keyser, Frank Prince, Frank Stack, John Swannell, and
Edward W eismiller. N or can I list all the qualities Randolph Quirk has
shown as an editor; despatch, m eticulousness, and humour will have to
suffice. I was fortunate to have in H eather King a typist who did not
always assume that the author must be right. Various stages o f this
work were made possible by grants from the Fulbright-Hays
Programme, the British A cadem y, and the Southam pton University
Advanced Studies Fund, and by the hospitality o f Clare C ollege,
Cambridge, and the English D epartm ent o f the University o f Illinois at
Urbana. I am grateful to the editors o f Essays in Criticism for allowing
me to use, in Chapter 9, material from an article published in that
journal. Thanks, too, to Robert, Richard, Randy, and Penny for seeing
me down the final straight.
Foreword v
Preface vii
1 TRADITIONAL APPROACHES 3
1.1 The classical approach 4
1.2 The temporal approach 18
2 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES 28
2.1 The phonemic approach 29
2.2 The generative approach 34
4 TH E FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM 76
4.1 The perception of rhythm 76
4.2 Underlying rhythm 80
4.3 Metrical patterns and unrealised beats 84
4.4 Offbeats; duple and triple rhythms 96
4.5 Line-openings, line-ends, and line-junctures 102
4.6 Rising and falling rhythms 108
4.7 Dipodic rhythms 114
Bibliography 363
Index 388
Acknowledgements
Traditional approaches
One kind o f insight into the history o f metrical study in English can be
gained simply from a glance at the collections on the subject held by
most large, long-established libraries. The shelves are dom inated by
fading volum es from the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries:
unwieldy surveys thick with scanned quotations; elegant essays
dabbling in this or that prosodic sidestream; scientific investigations of
syllabic duration or vocalic quality; handbooks for the schoolroom
parading lists o f G reek terms and recherché m etres culled from
Swinburne and Bridges. M ost o f these works evince a deep passion for
the subject: absolute truths are proclaimed in heavy capitals,
opponents despatched in savagely civil footnotes, snippets o f verse
triumphantly displayed like newly-discovered zoological specim ens.
Very few fail to offer som e illumination of a corner or two, or to
provide som e problem atic exam ple which demands an explanation;
but by and large their undisturbed repose on the library shelves is not
unmerited.
W ithin this vast dem onstration o f scholarly and critical ardour
ranging from the com ically idiosyncratic to the laboriously obvious it is
possible to trace two main approaches to English metre, and these
form the subject o f this chapter. To categorise in this way is, of course,
to over-simplify and misrepresent a com plex web o f arguments; but
the survey that follow s is intended not as a history o f prosodic study,
but as an exam ination o f those ways of dealing with metre which have
proved m ost tenacious in their hold on the English literary
consciousness, and which are m ost likely to affect - whether we realise
it or not - our present reading, teaching, and criticism o f verse. This
exam ination will have the double aim o f providing an outline o f the
metrical assumptions which underlie m ost critical discussions of
English poetry, and o f assessing what is valuable and what misleading
about these traditional accounts. W hile prosodic approaches of more
recent origin, to be discussed in the next chapter, have begun to make
4 TRADITIONAL APPROACHES
them selves felt in literary criticism, it is still true to say that m ost
com m ents on the rhythms o f English poetry ow e their existence to
theories which bear the dust - or the patina —o f centuries upon them.
1.1 T H E C L A SSIC A L A P P R O A C H
W hen, in the sixteenth century, English poets, scholars, and educators
joined the general European revaluation o f classical literature, and set
the verse o f G reece and R om e on the highest of pedestals, it was
inevitable that the literary endeavours o f Englishm en in their own
language should com e under fresh scrutiny, and equally inevitable
(until the last quarter of the century) that in the ensuing judgem ent the
home-grown product should be found wanting. One obvious respect in
which English verse failed to match the high art o f the ancients was in
its apparent lack o f metrical organisation and subtlety, since the only
tools o f analysis possessed by the early humanists were those o f
classical prosody, and these afforded no purchase on the lines o f
English poets. The syllables o f the English language, unlike those o f
G reek and Latin, had not been definitively classified by means o f
minutely detailed rules and the hallowed exam ple o f great poets, and
any attempt to scan English verse by the familiar procedures o f
classical prosody revealed only chaos. T o be sure, the sound o f English
verse had a kind o f crude regularity, but very few educated readers
expected to find the principles o f metrical patterning so obviously in
what they heard: they read Latin verse with a m ode of pronunciation
which gave no aural em bodim ent to its metrical structure, and their
sense o f its fine artistic precision came from an intellectual perception
o f the ordered ranks o f abstractly categorised syllables.1
One natural result o f this dissatisfaction was the protracted
endeavour by English poets to create in their own language an
equivalent o f classical metrical forms: the efforts o f som e thirty writers
survive from betw een 1540 and 1603, including exam ples by Ascham ,
Sidney, Spenser, G reene, and Campion. A lthough som e o f this
‘quantitative’ verse achieved critical acclaim and a degree of
popularity, by the end o f the century it was evident even to the m ost
diehard humanist that poetry in the native accentual tradition had
com e closer to equalling the achievem ents o f G reece and R om e than
any imitations o f classical m etres, and that a more valuable enterprise
might be the study o f the indigenous verse forms created by English
THE CLASSICAL APPROACH 5
staple educational diet from an early age, and we should therefore find
it easier than our forebears to question the appropriateness for English
verse o f terms and concepts borrowed from an ancient language, which
were them selves a borrowing from an even earlier one. N evertheless,
because o f its importance in the writing o f both poets and critics, the
outlines o f the classical approach, at least, have to be mastered by
anyone with an interest in English poetry. What follows is a mere
sketch of its com m onest form: upon this simple foundation much more
elaborate theoretical edifices have been erected to take account of the
huge variety in English metrical practice, but these should be consulted
in their original presentations.3
(a) Types o f fo o t
x / iambic foot or iamb
/ x trochaic foot or trochee
x x pyrrhic foot or pyrrhic
/ / spondaic foot or spondee
x x / anapaestic foot or anapaest
/ x x dactylic foot or dactyl
Iambic pentameter
| x / I x / I x 7 I x / lx / l
(1) I Enforced! to seek! som e co Ivert nigh I at hand I
Trochaic trimeter
| I X I I X I / x I
(2) IHigher |still and I higher I
Anapaestic tetrameter
I X X / I X X / | X X / IX X / I
(3) IW hen the voil ces o f chill dren are heard I on the green I
Dactylic dimeter
I /X X I / X X I
(4) I Happy and Iglorious I
In order to relate these simple schem es to the much more varied
lines which poets actually write, the classical approach has recourse to
the notion o f substitution, according to which the feet o f the basic
metre can be replaced by other feet. Thus a trochee can be substituted
for an iamb, and vice versa; and a spondee or a pyrrhic for either. The
following lines will serve as an illustration, the lower set o f symbols
indicating the basic metre, and the upper set the actual stresses and
nonstresses o f a possible reading:
x / x / X X x /
x / x / x / x /
(5) B ehold her, sing le in the field,
x / X X x / x /
x / x / x / X /
Y on so lita ry High land lass!
/ x x / X X x /
x / x / x / x /
Reaping and sing ing by herself;
8 TRADITIONAL APPROACHES
/ / x / X / 1
X / x / X /
Stop here, or gent ly pass!'
In the basic metre, three lines of iambic tetram eter are follow ed by an
iambic trimeter. H owever, each o f the three tetrameters has one
pyrrhic substitution; the third line begins with a trochaic substitution
(often called an inversion, and in this position an initial inversion ); and
the final line begins with a spondaic substitution. It is also possible to
replace a duple foot by a triple foot; this is known as trisyllabic
substitution, and its most com m on form involves the doubling of the
unstressed syllable to replace an iamb by an anapaest, or a trochee by a
dactyl. Thus in the following exam ple the iambic tetram eters are
varied by means o f an occasional anapaestic substitution (I show only
the substituted feet on the upper level):
XX /
X / x / x / x /
(6) The Ba bylo nian star light brought
X x /
x / X / x / x /
A fa bulous, form less dark ness in
Triple m etres are dealt with on the same principle, though less
elegantly, since the many possible substitute feet demand further raids
on the stock o f Greek prosodic terminology.
A nother classical term inherited by English prosody with a changed
signification is caesura. In the analysis o f English verse it is used to
refer to a pause within the line created by the syntax; thus one can say
that in (5) the first and the fourth lines have a prominent caesura, the
former after the third and the latter after the second syllable. The term
does not refer to anything in the structure o f most English verse,
however, and there is no reason to prefer it to ‘pause’ or ‘syntactic
break’ in describing a line. Two other terms, o f more value in metrical
discussion, can be introduced here: a line which ends with a syntactic
break is end-stopped, and one which does not is run-on (or enjambed).
A ll the lines in (5) are end-stopped; the first line o f (6) is run-on. It is
also worth m entioning a metrical phenom enon which the classical
approach does not easily accommodate: lines o f iambic verse may have
an extra unstressed syllable, or occasionally two, after the final stress;
these are, respectively, feminine and triple endings (as opposed to the
masculine ending which terminates the line on a stress), and have to be
regarded as ‘extrametrical’. W e shall see in due course that the endings
THE CLASSICAL APPROACH 9
o f much trochaic verse also create problems for the classical approach.
What I have described is merely a m ode o f scansion, but it implies a
particular conception o f poetic rhythm: a simple underlying metre on
which is superim posed a more com plex pattern representing with
greater fidelity the actual pronunciation o f the words. M ost modern
defenders o f the classical approach would argue that this picture of two
levels, partly coinciding, partly conflicting, reflects what in fact
happens as we read metrical verse, and reveals one source of its special
character. Many attempts have been made to capture the level of
actual pronunciation by m eans o f an analysis more delicate than that
provided by classical prosody, and we shall consider som e o f these in
1.2 and 2.1 below . But how ever the two levels are represented, the
notion o f the interplay, or counterpoint, or tension, betw een a simple
metrical pattern and a more varied arrangement o f stresses
corresponding to the pronunciation o f the line in one o f the m ost
suggestive features o f the classical approach, and one to which we shall
return.
Let us now subject the concept o f the foot itself to closer scrutiny,
without allowing ourselves to be awed by its classical pedigree. A t the
heart o f the analysis o f English verse in terms o f feet is the
understanding that the line is constituted by a series o f repeated
events, and that its character is determ ined in part by the number of
those repetitions; thus a tetram eter has a distinctively different quality
from that o f a pentam eter. English verse is shown by this means to be
different from, say, French or Italian verse, in which there are no such
repeated syllabic groups. So far so good; but the rub is that in offering
this insight into the structure o f the lines, the classical approach
imports further assumptions which may not be justifiable. What is the
difference betw een the following graphic representations of the stress
pattern o f an iambic pentameter?
X / X I XI XI XI
| x / | x / | x / | x / | x / |
x
(7) IThe chal pel, where| no o r ig a n ’s peal|
IX / | X / | X / I X / I
I Invests I the stern I and nal ked prayer I
Only a reader for whom foot-divisions had becom e solid walls could
fail to respond to the rhythmic progression from two roughly equal
nonstresses to two roughly equal stresses in the four italicised syllables.
That the foot should take on a substantial existence in the minds o f its
users need not surprise us, however; the bar-line in music frequently
suffers the same fate, although that at least has the merit o f occurring
THE CLASSICAL APPROACH 11
consistently in the same place relative to the main beats o f the rhythm.
This reification o f the foot is most evident in discussions o f
alternative scansions o f a single metrical form. Consider the following
lines, in a metre which occurs widely throughout the English tradition
(the sym bols here indicate the basic metrical schem e, not the actual
stress pattern):
/ X / X / X /
(9) H ere the anthem doth com m ence:
/ X / X / X /
Love and constancy is dead;
/ X / X / X /
Phoenix and the Turtle fled
/ X / X I x /
In a mutual flame from hence.
These lines cannot be divided into four identical units, yet one does not
experience them as in any way metrically irregular or anomalous. For
the classical prosodist, however, they pose a problem: are they to be
divided into feet like this:
I / XI / X I / X I / I
I Here the I anthem | doth com | mencel
that is, as trochaic tetram eters with a missing final syllable, or like this:
| / I x / I x / | x / I
IHere I the an I them doth] com m ence I
O ne could choose almost any foot for the basic metre and explain the
actual pattern o f stresses in terms of substitutions; indeed, one o f the
w eaknesses of the classical approach is that any succession o f syllables
can be divided into recognised feet. But the choice of a basic foot here
would be an arbitrary one, not reflecting anything in the reader’s
experience, and the impression which such an analysis would give o f a
highly com plex and deviant metre would be quite false. The rhythm is
bold and strong, with the firm four-beat structure o f the ballad or
nursery rhyme. Som e theorists would argue that such verse is not in the
accentual-syllabic tradition in which the bulk o f English poetry is
written, and which im poses restrictions on both the placing o f stresses
and the number o f syllables, but in a quite distinct form, closer to the
strong-stress metre o f m edieval alliterative verse in its indifference to
the number o f syllables in the line; but to argue this is to drive a wedge
betw een metrical types which shade into one another, and, by denying
English literary verse its intimate links with the popular tradition, to
ignore one o f its great sources o f vitality. We need a way o f talking
about poetic rhythm which will be useful for all varieties o f English
THE CLASSICAL APPROACH 13
I 1 x x x
x I x / x /
x 1
( 11) «When in disgrace with For tune and men s eyes,
x / x / x 7 x 7 x 7
I all! alone I bew eepl my out I cast state,
/ x
x I X / X / X / x /
A nd troub le deaf heav’n with my boot less cries,
| X / 1X / I X / IX / x 7
I A nd look! upon I m yself land curse I my fate
carrying the next beat, which slows down the m ovem ent over both
words and creates a point o f rhythmic emphasis; and this in turn is
follow ed by two lightly pronounced nonstresses. The rhythm therefore
undergoes a temporary deform ation - a slowing down o f the syllabic
m ovem ent, follow ed by a com pensatory speeding up - which starts on
the word ‘d e a f and ends with a return to the regular pace on ‘bootless’;
and the five beats o f the line, instead o f being evenly distributed among
the ten syllables, are irregularly dispersed.
The first line o f the exam ple, however, appears in a classical scansion
to be even less regular than the third: three of its five feet involve
substitutions. This hardly accords with o n e’s experience o f the verse,
and again the culprit is the principle o f the foot. What is classically
called an ‘initial trochaic substitution’ or ‘inverted first foot’ is simply a
line-opening with a stress, follow ed by two nonstresses instead o f one;
a relatively minor departure from regular alternation, involving no
dislocating successive stresses. There is another extra nonstress in
‘Fortune and’, follow ed by a rhythmic effect similar to that o f ‘deaf
heav’n’: two adjacent stresses which attract two beats and
momentarily retard the m ovem ent. Once again, a change in tem po in
one direction receives com pensation by a change in the other, but this
time in the reverse order. These two patterns, / / x x and x x / / , make
frequent appearances in strict verse, since they allow metrical variety
and expressive rhythmic effects without any consequences for the
syllable count; we shall discuss them fully in 7.6 and 8 .6 -7 . In classical
scansion, however, they are presented as com pletely distinct
phenom ena, the first involving trochaic substitution, or inversion, and
the second involving two substitute feet which have no necessary
connection. This would imply that the former is the simpler and more
com m on variation in English poetry, but in fact the reverse is true;
prosodists using the classical approach, however, often devote greater
attention to the formation which is more easily accounted for by their
theory. Som e foot-prosodists have recourse to a four-syllable foot, the
ionic, to take care o f the xx / / pattern, but this creates further
com plexities in scansion by reducing the number o f feet in the line. The
clear rhythmic structure o f lines like those in (1 1 ) can be fully brought
out in a reading that remains faithful to the normal pronunciation o f
English, and a metrical analysis should show this; foot-scansion
obscures this fact by seem ing to invite som e audible m anifestation o f
the ghostly divisions on which it is based, and by implying phonetic
equivalences which are no more than theoretical. The classical
THE CLASSICAL APPROACH 15
/ /
Xx / x /
/ X / x /
(1 3 ) W hen I have seen by T im e’s fell hand defaced
/ / i /
x I x I x / x i x I
The rich proud cost o f out worn bu ried age
X / X / X I I I
(1 4 ) To walk, and pass our long love’s day
X / X / X X / /
[14a] To walk, and cherish our love’s day
to which a line is irregular: we saw in exam ple (11) that a line with
three substitutions can be more regular to the ear than a line with one,
and it is not difficult to invent lines which, in terms of feet, show only
limited departures from the basic pattern, but which the ear registers as
highly deviant. The follow ing line, for exam ple, has only two
substitutions, but sounds clearly out o f place in the context of regular
iambic pentam eter verse:
/ x / /
x / x / X 1 x / x /
W hen men see by T im e’s hand the world destroyed
If we rem ove the foot-divisions, however, we can see why it strikes the
ear so uncomfortably: it has six main stresses, all vying for the five main
beats o f the line: x / / x / / x / x / . A t best, then, the division o f lines
into feet adds nothing, at worst it hinders accurate analysis o f the
metrical variations which all readers perceive.
marked regularity and at tim es departs from it, constantly arousing and
thwarting rhythmic expectations. It is in this sense that we can apply
the term ‘tension’ to poetic rhythm, without implying foot-scansion
and substitution, or the perception o f two discrete patterns at different
levels and a relationship betw een them , and we shall find it an
invaluable concept in the chapters that follow. Tension arises out o f the
twin tendencies o f language, towards variety and towards regularity:
the voice, or rather the speech faculty o f the human brain, enjoys its
freedom to range over a finely gradated scale of intensities, timbres,
pitches, and durations, but also feels the pull towards simple patterns
and repetitions. This is a feature o f all speech, perhaps o f all human
activity; but metre marks off the language of poetry from the language
o f daily existence by formalising and controlling this natural tension,
and the classical approach to prosody has always shown an awareness
o f this central fact.
, J* d JJ-TJ.T Jj
(1 6 ) But hail thou goddess, sage and holy
By this m eans, som ething o f the rhythmic variety o f English verse - the
way the voice hurries over certain syllables and lingers on others - can
be shown in a manner im possible within the strict classical approach;
and one could refine this notation further by adding such details as
accent-marks above the stressed syllables, rests, pauses, and tem po
indications. H ow ever, even with these additions, scansion o f this kind
offers very little insight into the metrical structure o f the line, giving
merely an impressionistic record o f one possible reading o f it. M ost
writers in this tradition m ake use o f a further principle, fundamental to
W estern music from the seventeenth century to the tw entieth century,
by dividing the line into bars, or measures, o f equal duration, and
apportioning note-values accordingly. A possible notation for our
exam ple might be:
■ rid 7 j I j. j. I d j I j« t \
(16) But mail thou Igoddess,! sage and Iholy
A s in music, the bar-lines always fall before the main beats, and
therefore, unlike foot-divisions, make no distinction betw een iambic
and trochaic, or anapaestic and dactylic, metres. In our exam ple,
classical scansion would hesitate betw een iambic or trochaic feet;
musical scansion avoids this unreal dilemma by automatically inserting
bar-lines in the appropriate positions.
What this notation im plies, o f course, is that the stresses function in
the same way as musical beats, and that the line, when read, is divided
into temporally equal units, each beginning with the onset o f a stressed
syllable. This constitutes a much stronger claim about the structure o f
THE TEMPORAL APPROACH 21
the line than the previous exam ple, and offers a clear alternative to the
classical m ode o f analysing m etre, though its connection with the
durational theory o f Latin prosody is obvious. Instead o f two levels,
involving ‘substitution’ and ‘counterpoint’, we have only one level,
w hose units are determ ined by the positions o f the main stresses; thus
the com m on sequence / / x x , for exam ple, treated in the classical
approach as an ‘inversion’, is regarded as a measure o f one syllable
follow ed by a measure o f three, with the lengths o f the syllables
adjusted to make the two m easures equal. .Compare the following
hypothetical musical analysis with the foot-scansion o f the same line
earlier in this chapter:
•r I j. j j d. I d / j* | j. J. | d *i
(11) A nd | trouble | deaf | heav’n with my I bootless Icries I
The use o f standard musical notation implies a further principle: that
the individual syllables, although not classifiable simply into ‘long’ and
‘short’, do bear simple durational ratios to one another, capable o f
being represented within the lim ited arithmetical system o f
note-values. Thus in (11) the claim is made that ‘d e a f takes exactly
one-and-a-half tim es as long to pronounce as ‘heav’n’. In addition,
accounts o f English verse using this approach frequently introduce the
idea that measurable intervals o f silence function as an integral part o f
the rhythmic structure, as rests do in music; in both our exam ples, for
instance, a rest at the end o f the line extends the final bar to the same
duration as the others. (The first syllable might be regarded as an
anacrusis, or upbeat, or as the final part o f a bar beginning in the
previous line.) The underlying assumption o f the w hole approach is
that the reader or hearer o f verse perceives the durations o f individual
syllables, and that his sense o f a rhythmic structure derives from the
simple patterns in time that they create.
m ovem ent o f spoken English and as the basis o f English verse; but he
also acknow ledges that isochrony is not an exact and absolute
phenom enon, but an approximate equality towards which speech
tends, and that its rhythmic beats may occur mentally rather than
materially.
Som e phoneticians and prosodists, however, take very literally the
notion o f equal intervals in speech, and com bine it with the musical
analogy which we have already discussed. The view that stresses in
English tend to be equally spaced in time does not, o f course, imply
that the syllables occurring within those intervals are o f any particular
duration; if there are two such syllables, for instance, they may divide
the time in any proportion whatever and still fulfil the dem ands o f
isochrony. But a number o f writers, influenced by the exact notations
o f music, have argued that these durations are in simple arithmetical
proportion to one another.8 Thus one com m on theory divides
utterances into equally tim ed units consisting o f a stress and all the
follow ing nonstresses, very like bars o f music, and these units are held
to be subdivided into tim e-lengths in simple ratios. A m easure of two
syllables, for instance, is said to exhibit only three kinds o f proportion:
1:2, 2:1, or Exam ples given by David Abercrom bie (1964b ) of
each o f these are, respectively, \shilling\, I tea fo r | tw o, I lim pid I.
Regular duple verse is then seen as a sequence o f equal disyllabic
m easures, each o f which is divided into one or other o f these simple
proportions. A further implication o f this particular analysis is that the
com m on ‘iam bic’ or ‘trochaic’ m etres o f English are regarded as
having a triple rhythm, since each measure is made up o f three units. In
order to preserve the equality o f the m easures in such an analysis, rests
are freely used, and som e writers in this tradition (for exam ple Steele,
1775; D avid A bercrom bie, 1964a, 1971) even allow for the
occurrence o f ‘silent stresses’, perceived in the gaps betw een
pronounced sounds.
I I I I x x I 1 1 I
(11) IWhen in I disgrace Iwith Fori tune and I m en’s eyes I
1 i i 1 x x i 1 r i
IWhen in dis| grace with I Fortune and I m en’sle y e sl
The latter is probably close to the experience o f the unanalytical reader
using normal pronunciation, since it shows how the final words both
attract a rhythmic beat, but it fails to register the resulting disturbance
to the regular alternations. What is m ore, it implies that there is
com plete freedom to add or drop unstressed syllables without metrical
consequences. This is obviously not so, as the subtraction o f one
nonstress will show:
I 1 \ 1 I 1 I ' I'
[11a] | W hen in dis | grace with I Love and I m en’s! eyes
i 1 i 7 i 1 r i
IW hen in dis I grace with! Love and m en’sle y e sl
The cardinal difference betw een the rhythms of song and those of
verse is that in the former it is the note-values which constitute the
THE TEMPORAL APPROACH 25
rhythmic forms and to specify the constraints which bring them into
being - som ething which cannot be done if the differences betw een
verse and speech, or betw een verse and music, are blurred from the
start.
only in this century that we have been made fully aware of the degree
to which that vital m ovem ent is the product o f the reader’s own acts of
perception. In responding to the metrical organisation o f a poem , the
reader is exercising a skill developed over a lifetim e, through the daily
experience o f rhythmic m ovem ent in the actions o f the body, in the use
o f language, and in the enjoym ent o f every level of sophistication of
the arts o f dance, music, and verse itself. That skill is intimately bound
up with the perception o f tim e, and whatever blind alleys the temporal
tradition has wandered into, it has been true at least to that
fundamental insight.
N o te s
1. See Attridge (1974) for an account of the Elizabethan understanding of Latin
metre, and its consequences in English literary practice.
2. See the studies o f eighteenth-century prosody by Culler (1948) and Fussell (1954).
3. Among the more influential works published this century to have stated and built on
the classical theory have been: Saintsbury (1 9 0 6 -1 0 ,1 9 1 0 ), Lascelles Abercrombie
(1923), Hamer (1930), Brooks & Warren (1938), Thompson (1961), Gross (1964),
Nabokov (1964), Shapiro & Beum (1965), M alof (1970), and Fussell (1979). Two
frequently-cited articles in defence of the classical approach are those by Wimsatt &
Beardsley (1959) and C. S. Lewis (1960). Countless studies of English poetry and
poets make use of or imply the classical approach.
4. See, for instance, Roger Fowler (1 966a, 1966b, 1968); and the further discussion in
4.6 below.
5. See, in particular, the discussions of Latin metre by W. S. Allen (1 9 6 4 ,1 9 6 5 ,1 9 6 9 ,
1973) and Zirin (1970); a summary of current views is given in Attridge (1974,
Ch. 1).
6. The other 1595 edition of this work, The Defence o fP o esie , gives ‘words and time’,
as does the standard modern text, but ‘tune’ makes the point clearer.
7. Among the more noteworthy contributions to the tradition of temporal analysis
since Steele have been: Patmore (1857), Lanier (1880), Ruskin (1880), Omond
(1921), Egerton Smith (1923), Thomson (1923), Croll (1929), Stewart (1930),
Hendren (1 9 3 6 ,1 9 5 9 ), and Classe (1939). A justification of the temporal approach
within the framework of aesthetics is given by Prall (1929, Ch. 9; 1936, Ch. 4) and
Perry (1965), and useful essays employing this approach have been written by Sapir
(1921), Croll (1923), Baker (I9 6 0 ), Schwarz (1962), Calvin Brown (1965), Leech
(1969, Ch. 7), and Stevenson (1970). An instructive account of the whole tradition
is given by Hollander (1956); see also the summaries in Barkas (1934), Lightfoot
(1970), and Sumera (1970).
8. See Jones (1960, Ch. 28), David Abercrombie (1964b), Halliday (1967), and
Albrow (1968).
9. See, for example, Warner Brown (1908), Verrier (1909), Snell (1918-19),
Scripture (1921), Schramm (1934), Classe (1939), Chatman (1956a), Shen &
Peterson (1962), Bolinger (1965), Uldall (1971), Dillon (1976), and Funkhouser
(1979); and the summaries by Jacob (1918, Ch. 10), Chatman (1965, Ch. 4),
Lehiste (1977), and Adams (1979, Part I).
Chapter 2
Linguistic approaches
takes us back to a linguistic approach which had its heyday prior to the
Chomskyan revolution, though its main influence on metrical studies
was delayed, and has outlived its pre-em inence within its own domain;
it is as if the heightened status o f linguistics as an intellectual discipline
has opened the eyes o f literary prosodists, but all they have seen, or
been willing to see, are the relatively circumscribed linguistic
approaches which prevailed before that status was achieved.
M eanwhile, however, linguists them selves have not been slow to
incorporate metrical study as part o f their own intellectual enterprise,
both as a m eans o f investigating certain characteristics o f language and
as a subject in its own right, and the second section o f this chapter will
focus on som e exam ples o f metrical analysis made within the
framework o f contemporary linguistic theory. One other approach
should be m entioned here, as a product o f the application of
quantitative m ethods to metrical study: the statistical analysis o f
bodies o f verse. This tradition, largely Russian in origin, has produced
som e valuable information about those properties o f English verse
amenable to numerical description, which we shall make use o f in Part
Three (see the studies by B ailey, 1975b, and Tarlinskaja, 1976; and
the collection of essays translated by G. S. Smith, 1980). The drawback
o f this approach, however, is that it is only as strong as the metrical
theory from which it derives its categories, and this has tended to be o f
a strongly traditional cast.
2.1 T H E P H O N E M IC A P P R O A C H
A t the heart o f modern linguistics lies the observation that what
matters in any given language is not the multitude o f physical
distinctions betw een the actual sounds produced by individual
speakers, but the very lim ited set o f distinctions that constitute that
language’s phonological system, and the discrepancy we have already
noted betw een instrumentally measurable sounds and perceived
speech is one aspect o f this wider insight. It finds its m ost telling
expression in the theory o f the pho nem e, which has been one o f the
most powerfully suggestive linguistic concepts to have em erged this
century, even if its status as an elem ent in the language system has been
questioned in recent years. A lthough a spoken utterance is physically a
com plex sonic continuum, which even the m ost finely detailed
phonetic transcription cannot fully represent, it is interpreted by
30 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES
The concept o f the phonem e, with its em phasis on the way the
speaker o f a language uses his acquired knowledge to make sense o f a
barrage o f information, is a valuable one for the student o f metre; for
exam ple, it helps one to distinguish betw een what Rom an Jakobson
(1 9 6 0 , pp. 3 6 4 -6 ) has called delivery instances, the actual readings o f a
line on particular occasions, and verse instance, the metrical structure
which underlies all readings o f the line; and betw een verse instances
and verse design, the metrical schem e that underlies all the lines in a
particular metre. But at the level o f specific linguistic detail, it was the
extension o f the phonem ic principle to other features o f the language
which proved particularly attractive to metrists; and by far the most
influential work in this field was A n Outline o f English Structure by
George L. Trager and Henry Lee Smith (1 9 5 1 ). In their attempt at a
com prehensive and detailed description o f spoken English, Trager and
Smith propose several suprasegmental p h o n e m e s ; that is, phonem es
which are not them selves segm ents in the chain o f sounds that make up
the utterance, but features o f pronunciation which affect these
segm ents in a system atic way. They include pitch, juncture (the
transitions betw een segm ents), and, m ost important for metrical study,
stress. In dealing with stress, which they regard as a matter o f loudness,
the question that most concerns them is, ‘H ow many significantly
different degrees o f stress are there in the system o f English sounds?’,
and they conclude that it is necessary to postulate four distinct degrees
to account for the stress contours which English speakers use. To these
they allot four symbols: prim ary ' , secondary A, tertiary' , and weak w.
Every syllable in any English utterance, they claim, has one o f these
degrees o f stress, determ ined by the phonological structure o f the
language. O ne o f their exam ples, representing a normal pronuncia
tion, will indicate the kind o f stress contour which results from this
theory:
They then argue, in effect, that ‘to thee’ and ‘blithe Spirit’ are
perceived as iambic feet because they contain contrasts betw een weak
and tertiary stress in the first case, and secondary and primary in the
second, and that the reader ignores the marked increase in stress
betw een ‘th ee’ and ‘blithe’ because it occurs over a foot-boundary. The
prosodic cart seem s to have got in front o f the rhythmic horse here;
metrical markings should reflect our perception o f the m ovem ent o f
language and not determ ine it. Even a traditional classical scansion,
innocent of phonem es and degrees of stress, is capable of reflecting the
main stresses o f this three-beat line more clearly, by m eans o f an
‘inverted fo o t’ betw een two trochaic feet:
THE PHONEMIC APPROACH 33
/ X| X / 1 IX I
(1 ) Hail tol thee, blithe I Spirit!!
In most stanzas o f the poem , at least two o f the five lines begin with a
stress and end with a nonstress, a pattern which lends itself, pace
Wimsatt and Beardsley, to trochaic scansion.
This is by no m eans the most extrem e instance o f phonological
theory imposing on aural reality, however. That honour probably goes
to metrical analyses arising from another prosodic principle derived
from Trager and Smith, stated as follow s by the latter: ‘W hen two
instances o f the same stress phonem e occur on syllables im m ediately
follow ing each other, the occurrence o f the second in the sequence will
be phonetically more “ prom inent” than the first’ (introductory
chapter to Epstein and Hawkes, 1959, p. 7). N ot only is this ‘fact of
phonetics’ unsupported by any evidence; it undermines what is
valuable in the phonem ic principle by implying that we respond to the
rhythms o f speech not at the level o f language structure but at the
surface, ‘allophonic’, level o f mere sound. N evertheless, it allows
Smith to ‘prove’ in another article (1 9 5 9 ) that all English verse is
predominantly iambic, and gives Epstein and Hawkes (1 9 5 9 ) the
opportunity o f demonstrating, in all seriousness, that there are 6,236
types o f iambic foot in English. The old dilemm a posed within the
classical approach by ‘ambiguous lines’ is given a new sophistication by
this theory, and a laborious analysis o f Shakespeare’s ‘Full fathom five’
enables the authors to pronounce that it is indisputably iambic. Since
they have in effect devised their own definition o f ‘iambic verse’, their
conclusion has little relevance to anything outside their study; it
certainly has no bearing on the fact that o f the song’s seven lines and
implied eighth line o f refrain, all but the first are exam ples o f what the
classical tradition normally regards as trochaic, that is, they begin on
the metrical beat. O nce again, the experience o f the reader has been
brushed aside in the im position o f a grid o f theory on the syllables of
the language.
But the failings o f this approach are more fundamental (and more
instructive) than the fact that it permits o f such excesses; they lie in the
nature o f the linguistic traditions from which it derives. To conceive of
the task o f metrical theory as the exhaustive description o f the lines of
English verse, in the same way that the Bloom fieldian school saw the
task o f linguistics as the description of a corpus o f utterances, is tp offer
little in the way o f an explanation o f metre and its poetic effects, and
such an analysis can becom e m eaningless when the descriptions only
34 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES
2.2 T H E G E N E R A T IV E A P P R O A C H
The rules should reflect all the consistent features of such judgem ents,
and if two or more features can be accounted for by one rule, that rule
has captured a significant generalisation, and may be a pointer to the
basic principles on which metrical form rests. Thus the formulated
rules for, say, iambic pentam eter should be capable of generating all
the lines which readers would accept as iambic pentam eters without
generating any lines which would not be accepted; and in so doing they
should exhibit the metrical structure o f the acceptable lines, as w ell as
the ways in which the unacceptable lines deviate from metricality. Or,
if we limit the task to a single author, we might attempt to formulate
the rules of, say, Chaucer’s iambic pentam eter, which, let it be noted,
would generate not only all Chaucer’s iambic pentam eters, but also all
those pentam eters which a reader thoroughly familiar with Chaucer
would accept as being ‘Chaucerian’. Traditional approaches to metre,
which are confined largely to the description of metrical forms, cannot
satisfy these demands: any line, however unacceptable to the ear, can
be scanned as a succession o f classical feet or musical measures.
H alle and Keyser approach this formidable task by proposing two
com ponents in the set o f rules for any metrical form: an abstract
m etrical p a tte rn , and correspondence rules (also called m apping rules
or realisation rules) which relate that pattern to the stress contours o f
particular stretches o f language. (This is, of course, a restatem ent of
the familiar notion of a simple metrical base and a more com plex
pattern o f stresses in the line o f verse itself.) The metrical form most
exhaustively analysed in these terms has been the iambic pentameter,
and the following are the rules for that metre proposed by Halle and
Keyser in the 1971 version o f their theory (1971a, p. 169):
(a) ABSTRACT M ETRICAL PATTERN
(W )*S WS WS WS WS (X )(X )
where elem ents enclosed in parentheses may be om itted and
where each X position may be occupied only by an unstressed
syllable
(b) CORRESPONDENCE RULES
(i) A position (S, W, or X ) corresponds to a single syllable
OR
to a sonorant sequence incorporating at most two vow els
(im m ediately adjoining or separated by a sonorant
consonant)
DEFINITION: W hen a fully stressed syllable occurs betw een
THE GENERATIVE APPROACH 39
/ i l l i
(2) Batter my heart, three-personed G od, for you
w s w s w s w s w s
The stressed syllables which fall in the first and third weak positions are
not stress maxima, being neutralised in one case by the adjacent
line-opening and in the other by the adjacent stresses and syntactic
boundary. H alle and Keyser give the following exam ple o f a sequence
o f words which, though it has an appropriate number of syllables,
cannot be accounted for by the rules, and hence is unmetrical (p. 171):
/ / / i / /
[3] Ode to the W est Wind by Percy Bysshe Shelley
w s w s w s w s w s w
weak positions, and we have rem oved all the stress maxima. We could,
therefore, exchange strong and weak positions and the line would still
pass muster as a realisation, albeit a com plex one, o f the opposite
metrical pattern. What is m ore, the rules make it very easy to avoid
stress maxima: one merely has to keep stresses in pairs or at the
extrem ities of lines:
/ i i i i / /
(4) John is dead drunk and w eeps tears from red eyes
w s w s w s w s w s
H alle and Keyser would accept this as an iambic (or for that matter
trochaic) pentam eter, whereas the only rhythmic context in which the
ordinary reader of verse might find it tolerable is that o f four-beat
triple metre. Furthermore, the exclusion o f stress patterns created by
minor category words such as prepositions means that a line like the
following, which to most readers would be a paradigmatic iambic
pentam eter, is regarded as having no metrical structure (or, what
amounts to the same thing, any metrical structure):
(5) Before, behind, betw een, above, below
The neutralisation o f stress maxima next to syntactic boundaries
creates further anom alies, which I shall illustrate later in this chapter.
But the rules are not only too tolerant in what they accept; they are
also too strict in som e o f the lines they exclude. One which Halle and
Keyser quote is the following opening o f a sonnet by Keats:
/ i t / /
(6) H ow many bards gild the lapses o f time!
w s w s w s w s w s
therefore, when put to the type o f test which the generative approach
invites; that the principle has been uncritically accepted in many recent
studies o f metre in spite o f its inadequacy is perhaps a testim ony to the
widely-felt desire for a simple key to unlock the secret chambers o f
prosody.4 W hile the failure o f this particular set o f rules suggests
certain dangers inherent in the generative approach to metre, it does
not discredit the w hole endeavour. The proposals put forward by Halle
and Keyser, how ever unsatisfactory in them selves, highlight som e of
the problems to be overcom e in a satisfactory theory o f m etre, and
clear the way for further attempts at an adequate form ulation o f the
rules. B efore we attempt to assess the generative approach as a w hole,
we shall look briefly at two such alternative proposals.
The first o f these was put forward by Karl M agnuson and Frank G.
Ryder (1 9 7 0 ,1 9 7 1 ) as a counter-theory to that o f H alle and Keyser. It
also regards the metrical pattern as a sequence of weak and strong
positions, and attem pts to formulate rules which define the types o f
syllable possible in these positions. (I have slightly altered the
term inology o f the later theories I discuss in order to facilitate
com parison.) But M agnuson and Ryder expose som e of the
w eaknesses o f the H alle-K eyser rules, and propose a different set of
linguistic properties on which to base the rules; instead o f a series o f
syllabic classifications progressing from unstressed syllables to stressed
syllables to stress maxima, they make use o f a set o f binary features
which, they claim, determ ine the degree o f acceptability o f a syllable in
a weak or strong position. A n exam ple o f such a feature is, o f course,
stress, which they indicate as [+ S T ] for a stressed syllable and [ - S T ]
for an unstressed syllable. A syllable classed as [+ S T ] is said to be
affirming when it occurs in strong position, because it reinforces the
metre, and nonaffirming in weak position, because it contradicts it;
and the converse is true for [ —ST]. A s in all generative accounts o f
m etre, the aim is to deduce rules which will capture as many
generalisations about the positioning o f syllables as possible; in effect,
this entails a claim that the shorter and simpler the set o f rules, the
more likely they are to constitute a correct account o f the principles
underlying the metrical diversity o f English verse.
In order to illustrate the kinds o f rule used in this approach, I shall
sketch the account given by Chisholm (1 9 7 7 ) as a m odification o f the
som ew hat more com plicated rules proposed by the original authors.
44 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES
3. Positional Rules:
a. [ F ] - > [ + S T ] / 3,9 [+ S T ] —
b. 11 [F] -► [ - S T ] (p. 150)
Rule 1 states that any syllable which is word-initial and not
phrase-terminal, and which occurs before a syllable which is stressed
and also word-initial, is regarded, for the purposes o f the metre, as
unstressed. Or, more simply, a stressed m onosyllabic word before
another stressed syllable within the same phrase loses its stress. The
rule depends, o f course, on the prior analysis o f the language into
stressed and unstressed syllables, and into syntactic phrases, a far from
autom atic process, but one which need not be discussed here. Som e
exam ples will be more useful: in the phrase the green tree, ‘green’ is
changed from [+ S T ] to [ —ST]; in the man s p o k e , however, both
stressed syllables retain their stress, since they occur, according to
standard generative syntactic theory, in different phrases, a N oun
Phrase and a Verb Phrase. In swaying tree and renowned tree, o f
course, the rule does not apply since in the first o f these the stresses are
not adjacent, and in neither is the first word a m onosyllable.
The base rule states that for a line to be metrical any syllable
(represented by [F]) must be unstressed if it occurs after a syllable in
strong position (shown by x ) that is not the final syllable o f a phrase.
Or, looking at it the other way round, a stressed syllable can occur in a
weak position only if it constitutes the beginning o f a new phrase. Thus
the following sequences are regarded as acceptable iambic
pentameters:
/ i i i i
[7] He struck at the tall cook with heavy blows
w s w s w s w s w s
I I I I I
[8] The weeping man fell to his knees in pain
w s w s w s w s w s
In [7], ‘tall’ loses its stress by the prosodic transformation rule, and so
does not contravene the base rule; and in [8], the stressed syllable
which occurs in weak position, ‘fell’, is the start of a new phrase, and is
therefore permissible. But the base rule classes the following
sequences as unmetrical:
/ i i i i
[9] He struck the am azed cook with heavy blows
w s w s w s w s w s
46 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES
I I I I I
[10] They sent the huge man to his knees in pain
w s w s w s w s w s
In the first o f these, ‘am azed’ does not lose its stress as it is not
monosyllabic; and in neither is the stressed syllable in a weak position
the start o f a new phrase. In making these distinctions, Chisholm is
aiming at a subtler em bodim ent o f readers’ judgem ents than H alle and
Keyser, for whom all these lines would be metrically similar. There is
no doubt that the differences are perceptible, though one might wish to
question a theory which draws such a sharp line betw een metrical and
unmetrical lines; both [9] and [10] would be acceptable in som e
metrical styles.
Finally, there are the two positional rules, the first of which reflects a
tendency often noted in classical treatments o f the iambic pentameter:
the avoidance o f inversions in the second and fifth foot. It requires that
a stress in the third position or the ninth position o f the line be follow ed
by another stress, thus creating a ‘spondee’ instead o f the forbidden
‘trochee’. The second positional rule prohibits stressed syllables in the
eleventh position; that is, fem inine endings can only be unstressed.
Chisholm claims that his set o f rules ‘will generate over 97 per cent of
the actually occurring iambic pentam eter lines in the English tradition
[from Shakespeare to the onset o f the Rom antic period] without
generating any unmetrical lines’ (p. 150). W e shall test this claim in
due course.
one or two exam ples, will have to suffice. Like Chisholm, he m akes a
distinction betw een rules which determ ine what features o f the
language are relevant to the metre, which he calls p ro so d ic rules, and
rules which determ ine the ways in which those features may be
arranged, which he terms m etrical ru les. A lthough the prosodic rules
occupy the same place in the theory as Chisholm ’s prosodic
transformation rule, their content is different: the subordination o f a
m onosyllabic stress to a following stress which is the substance o f
Chisholm ’s rule is for the m ost part regarded as a phonological feature
o f the language itself, and the only exam ples o f prosodic rules which
Kiparsky discusses are those which allow certain syllables o f English to
be discounted for metrical purposes, making it possible, for instance,
for words like p o e tr y , v ic to ry, or envious to be treated as either
trisyllables or disyllables.
Kiparsky’s m ethod o f analysing a line o f verse involves matching its
phonological structure, as determ ined by the procedures suggested by
Liberman and Prince, with its metrical structure, the familiar
succession o f W and S positions with the addition o f brackets
corresponding to the feet o f the classical approach. In m ost cases there
will not be a perfect match, but Kiparsky draws a distinction betw een
permissible and impermissible m ismatches, the former rendering the
line more com plex, but only the latter rendering it unmetrical. Various
com binations o f mismatches are exam ined in an attem pt to state
precisely what departures o f the phonological structure from the
metrical pattern render a line unmetrical for a particular poet. Like
Chisholm, Kiparsky focuses on the conditions under which a stressed
syllable may or may not occur in a weak position. To simplify his
argument drastically, the m ost fundamental type o f unmetricality in
iambic verse is created when the stressed syllable in a weak position is
too closely connected with a preceding weak syllable in strong
position, either as parts o f the same word or, for many poets, as parts o f
a single phrase which contains only one lexical item (for exam ple, a
preposition with a noun or an auxiliary with a verb). Exam ple [9]
above is classed as unmetrical for m ost poets because the stressed
syllable in a weak position and the preceding w eak syllable are part o f
the same word, ‘am azed’; [10], on the other hand, is more acceptable,
because ‘huge’, though classified as weak, is lexically separate.
Kiparsky quotes som e exam ples from Shakespeare which suggest that in
this case his rules are more accurate than Chisholm ’s: the following
line from the Sonnets, for instance, has a similar structure to [10] and
48 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES
/ i i 1 . 1 1
[18] Harold - look! Enem ies! Beat it! Run home!
ws w s w s w s w s
trochaic lines which are accepted as iambic (or anything else) by these
theories (again I underline weak positions with neutralised stresses):
i i i I i
[19] Harold, Charlie, Carol, Sidney, Horace
ws w s ws w s ws
Chisholm ’s second positional rule does, it is true, rule out the last
inversion, but none o f these theories indicates that the rhythm o f the
line runs precisely counter to that im plied by the metrical pattern.
Dactylics which these theories would accept as iambic are equally easy
to invent:
i i i i
[20] Jittery Caroline, skittery Lil
w s w s w s w s w s
There are no stress maxima here by the H alle-K eyser rules, and the
three adjectives are regarded as weak, and therefore metrically
unoffending, by both Chisholm and Kiparsky, the former by his
prosodic transformation rule and the latter as a result o f the
phonological theory he is using.
The fact that such counter-exam ples can readily be invented points
to a curious feature o f the generative approach to metrical analysis: it
has not made full use o f one o f the m ost distinctive and powerful
procedures o f the linguistic m ethod from which it is derived. A linguist
attempting to formulate the grammar of a language will constantly test
the output o f his rules against the com petence o f a native speaker; if he
is working on his own language, this will usually, at least in the first
52 LINGUISTIC APPROACHES
instance, be himself. But there has been only limited evidence o f this
procedure in generative studies of metre, which have tended instead to
concentrate on a corpus - Chaucer’s iambic pentameters, Shake
speare’s Sonnets, Paradise L o s t - and to aim at rules which will generate
all the lines in this corpus. What is missing is close scrutiny o f the other
types o f line which will be generated by the proposed rules. The result
has been, as we have noted, theories which are far too accom m odating
in what they accept as metrical. The kind o f appeal to the sensitive
reader that I am suggesting is, o f course, subject to the accusations
which are som etim es levelled at the equivalent procedure in
linguistics, such as its lack o f objectivity and scientific rigour, and I
shall discuss som e o f the problems involved in transferring the notion
o f ‘com petence’ from linguistics to metrics in Chapter 6, but it remains
true that generative metrics has so far failed partly because it has not
taken full advantage o f one o f the major insights o f the Chomskyan
theory from which it originates.
The process o f testing, recasting, and refining the rules o f generative
metrics will undoubtedly continue, and may eventually produce a set
o f rules which will m eet the objective o f generating all the acceptable
lines of, say, iambic pentam eter within a given metrical style, without
generating any unacceptable lines. The qualification, ‘within a given
metrical style’, is important, because rules which would generate all
the lines acceptable within D o n n e ’s metrical practice would, as
Kiparsky’s work makes clear, fail to distinguish betw een acceptable
and unacceptable lines in the metrical tradition exem plified by Pope.
A nother way o f saying this is that an experienced reader does not
simply make judgem ents o f the type, ‘this is an acceptable iambic
pentam eter’, but ‘this line would be out o f place in W ordsworth’s
poetry but not in Y eats’s’. The rules should also reflect the reader’s
awareness o f varying degrees o f com plexity or tension in the line,
which might be the same as the scale along which poets can be ranged,
from the freedom o f D onne to the strictness o f Pope, but might show
interesting differences. The process o f arriving at such rules is likely to
be a prolonged one, if only because their correct formulation depends
on the correct formulation o f the rules o f English phonology. N or will
they be simple rules; there is no reason to suppose that the capacity
possessed by readers o f English verse is explicable in terms o f one or
two general principles, any more than one can suppose that the
apparently natural and effortless use o f language is the result o f a few
elem entary grammatical rules. O ne o f the achievem ents o f generative
THE GENERATIVE APPROACH 53
For Halle and Keyser, this is an iambic pentam eter, albeit one which is
too com plex ever to be used, because it is accepted by their rules; an
alternative interpretation might be that a metrical theory which can
generate such a monstrosity needs som e reconsideration.
In Chapter 6 we shall consider in more detail the value and the
dangers o f using generative linguistics as a m odel for metrical studies;
the point I wish to em phasise here about the proposals we have been
discussing is that while they have rightly increased the demands made
upon any metrical theory, they have failed them selves to satisfy those
demands, through their over-reliance on certain aspects o f their
m odel. To write metrical verse is not just to select arbitrarily an
abstract pattern and give this a material em bodim ent in a sequence of
sounds, as if it might be equally well represented by beads on a string,
or by an arrangement of words with odd and even numbers o f letters; it
is the ordering o f those sounds them selves in ways which are
determ ined by the nature o f the language and by the general aesthetic
and psychological properties o f rhythm. We shall return in Part Three
to the problem o f formulating adequate rules o f English metre, but
before we consider how they can best be expressed, it is essential that
we make a study o f the medium in which they function and the general
principles according to which they operate. This means asking som e
fundamental questions about the rhythmic structure o f the English
THE GENERATIVE APPROACH 55
Notes
1. Many of this century’s developments in metrical study were foreshadowed in a short
paper by the philologist Otto Jespersen, first published in Danish in 1900. Though
his reliance on absolute degrees o f stress leads to some implausible explanations of
metrical phenomena, his approach is a model o f intelligent enquiry.
2. A Kenyon Review symposium on ‘English Verse and What It Sounds Like’ in 1956
included fanfares for phonemic analysis as a metrical tool from Harold Whitehall
and Seymour Chatman, and it has been employed in many subsequent studies,
including Sutherland (1958), H. L. Smith (1959), Epstein & Hawkes (1959),
Chatman ( 1960), Wells ( 1960), Thompson (1961), Hawkes ( 1962), Fowler ( 1966a,
1966b), Fraser (1970), Hewitt (1972), and Dougherty (1973). The fullest and most
useful work in this tradition is Chatman’s Theory o f Meter (1965). A s late as 1971
one finds the then twenty-year-old Trager-Smith analysis of phonology described
by a metrical commentator as ‘the most exhaustive available’ (Hawkes, p. 887).
3. See Halle & Keyser (1971a, 1971b), Keyser (1969a, 1969b), and Halle (1970). The
underlying assumptions o f this approach have been made most fully explicit by
Beaver (1968a); see also his restatements of the theory, with some modifications
(1968b, 1969, 1971a, 1971b, 1973, 1974, 1976). Among the many other
contributions to this approach have been those of Freeman (1968, 1969, 1972),
Hascall (1969, 1971), Levin (1973), Newton (1975), and Wilson (1979). The
Halle-Keyser theory has been applied to Romance languages by Roubaud (1971)
and Lusson & Roubaud (1974).
4. See, however, the criticisms of the H alle-Keyser approach by Wimsatt (1970),
Cable (1972, 1 9 7 3 ,1 9 7 6 ), Youmans (1974), Standop (1975), Devine & Stephens
(1975), Barnes & Esau (1978, 1979), and Groves (1979).
This page intentionally left blank
Part Two : Rhythm
This page intentionally left blank
Chapter 3
3.1 T H E SY L L A B L E
A language can be analysed into syntactic units o f varying scope, these
into words and m orphem es, and these in turn into phonem es and
distinctive features, to provide an account o f the abstract structures by
means o f which meaning is conveyed. The syllable, however, is the
sm allest rhythm ic unit of the language; like the step in walking, it is the
repeatable event which keeps the utterance going, the carrier o f all the
elem ents in the linguistic system. Its rhythmic character is clearly
revealed by what we do if we wish to count the syllables in a word or
phrase: we pronounce them in a strong, regular rhythm, perhaps
accompanying each with a muscular m ovem ent. D oing this com es
quite naturally, whereas counting individual phonem es, or whole
words, by m eans o f the same procedure creates the feeling o f an
unnatural rhythm being foisted on the language. The syllable has been
the subject of much linguistic debate; its status in relation to the
language-system is not settled, and there is disagreem ent about its
precise phonetic constitution.1 M ost o f this discussion is o f little
importance for the study o f rhythm, but it will be worth giving som e
attention to the articulatory basis o f the syllable, and to the perception
o f syllables in speech, as these are directly relevant to our purpose.
A ll speech is created by the forcing o f air under muscular pressure
through the orifices o f the vocal tract, the differing configurations o f
which produce different qualities of sound. It is primarily the deflation
o f the lungs which produces the airstream on which speech-sounds are
im posed by the higher organs, and it is possible to record the action o f
the muscles which effect this deflation by m eans o f an experim ental
THE SYLLABLE 61
3.2 STRESS
its primary, determinant. I shall use the term ‘nonstress’ for syllables
which do not possess this salience in the sequence. A s is the case with
the syllable, explorations o f the physical nature o f stress can proceed in
two directions: one, the province o f articulatory phonetics, is the
exam ination o f the way in which it is produced by the speech
apparatus; the other, the province o f acoustic and auditory phonetics,
is the study of the sounds produced and their perception as stress by the
hearer. To the com posite picture which they provide must be added
the insights o f phonology, the study o f the linguistic system which is
em bodied in the physical sounds.
Experim ental evidence is fullest in the field o f acoustic and auditory
phonetics, and several studies have been made o f the perceptual cues
which signal stress in English (see the summaries in Lehiste, 1970a;
and Adam s, 1979, Ch. 3). Our perception o f a syllable as stressed is
based on one or more o f three major features: its pitch, its duration,
and its amplitude. O f these, pitch has long been considered the m ost
effective cue: experim ents have shown that the stress pattern of
individual words is largely determ ined by pitch-changes, and it is easy
to test the role o f pitch by reading a sentence like the following aloud,
first with the final word stressed on its initial syllable, then on its second
syllable:
Stress is the subject o f much controversy.
The intonation contour rises sharply on the syllable that is stressed,
and then falls for the remaining syllables o f the sentence. A number of
studies have suggested that the next m ost effective cue is duration,
though som e recent work on the subject points to its being, at least in
connected speech, even more important than pitch (see Liberman and
Prince, 1977, p. 250; Gay, 1978; A dam s, 1979). A syllable that is
longer than its neighbours will tend to be perceived as stressed, if other
parameters are held constant (which is what happens, for instance,
when a prayer is intoned on a single note and at a constant volum e).
The organisation o f a chain o f syllables into sequences o f varying
length (or speed in utterance) is therefore crucial to the perception o f
stress; and this m eans that durational patterns and stress are intimately
connected, a point to which we shall return in section 3.4 below. The
third perceptual cue is the one that uninstructed speakers often
assume to be o f paramount importance: loudness. Experim ents have
repeatedly shown that the degree o f intensity with which a syllable is
uttered, though in isolation it will signal stress, can be overridden by
64 THE RHYTHMS OF ENGLISH SPEECH
those sounds. It seem s reasonable to assume that the same is true o f the
other cues for stress, and that what we perceive as equal pitches or
durations in an utterance are felt to be equal because they require
similar activity in the speech musculature, not necessarily because they
are objectively so.
The kind o f prom inence that is m anifested by stress, then, is one
which engages physical as well as mental responses in the reader; it is
not merely an abstract phenom enon induced by contrast, like the red
bead in the black necklace, but an em pathetic response based on
a shared way o f using the speech apparatus. This view o f stress
is particularly associated with the name o f R. H. Stetson, who
elaborated a theory of language production and perception based on
recordings o f the speech musculature in action (see Stetson, 1905,
1 9 4 5 ,1 9 5 1 ). Stetson’s work has been criticised by later experim enters
using more sophisticated equipm ent, but som e aspects o f his theory
have considerable explanatory power in the analysis of stress contours,
as has been shown by W. S. A llen (1 9 7 3 , pp. 4 0 -4 5 , 6 2 -8 2 , 1 9 1 -9 ).
For our purposes, it is sufficient to note that Stetson’s theory, as
developed by A llen, suggests a classification o f English stress into two
types, depending on whether the muscular m ovem ent which produces
the stress is arrested within the stressed syllable, usually by means o f a
long vowel (or diphthong) or a final consonant, or whether a following
unstressed syllable is used in the arrest o f the stress, usually when there
is a single consonant betw een the syllables. The first type o f stress,
which occurs in words like keeper, la z y , cam ping, can be called
self-arrested stress; the second, in words like k ip p e r, L iz z ie , com in g,
can be called disyllabic stress. Such an analysis would suggest that the
relationship betw een adjacent syllables is often determ ined by the
muscular activity which produces them, creating different kinds of
rhythmic sequence. It is interesting to note that L adefoged et al.
(1958, p. 6 ) cite p ity as an exam ple o f a word they found to be
accompanied by a single burst o f muscular activity, and that Bridges
(1 9 0 9 , p. 100) gives as exam ples o f quantitative pyrrhics (feet o f two
short syllables) h a b it, v e ry , silly, s o lid , and scurry - all cases of
disyllabic arrest. The distinction made by D avid Abercrom bie (1964b )
betw een disyllables with durational ratios o f 14:1 i and 1 :2 (see above,
p. 2 3 ) is based on the same difference in syllabic structure, though
we have already noted the dangers o f specifying arithmetical ratios for
these perceived rhythmic relationships. W hen we m ove beyond the
word, the picture becom es even more complex; there is, for instance, a
STRESS HIERARCHIES 67
s w s
blackbird pie
This indicates that at the low est level o f the hierarchy, black is the
strong mem ber and bird the weak, while at the next level, blackbird is
the weak mem ber and p ie the strong. This procedure can be extended
to larger structures, producing a com plex tree-diagram which gives
every syllable a place in the hierarchical order.3
There is no need to go into further detail: what is important is the
general view o f the stress contour as a set o f relationships extending
over, and reflecting the linguistic structure of, an entire syntactic unit,
in contrast to the rather myopic view prevalent in metrical studies
(fathered perhaps by Jespersen in his 1900 essay) which considers only
contrasts betw een adjacent syllables. H ow ever, it is worth noting two
STRESS HIERARCHIES 69
[ 1 ] Eng-lish-is-m arked-by-its-strong-use-of-stress
[2] Its-use-of-strong-stress-is-what-marks-Eng-lish
A lthough the stress contour is given no physical m anifestation, an
English speaker cannot but feel its presence in the places dictated by
the linguistic structure, and will register the first sequence as
rhythmically more regular than the second. Listening to them read in
this way, he may even perceive stresses which are not objectively
present. On the other hand, som eone unfamilar with English would
hear a similar sequence o f ten equal sounds in each case.
What we hear in an English sentence, then, is not simply a series of
pitches, volum es, and durations, but a group o f syllables held together
in a linguistic structure which determ ines their patterns o f
subordination to one another. It seem s likely that the perception o f a
hierarchical structure is in part a reflection o f the way syllables are
70 THE RHYTHMS OF ENGLISH SPEECH
3 .4 A L T E R N A T IO N A N D STR ESS-TIM IN G
In our discussion o f the hierarchical analysis o f English stress contours,
we noted that a fundamental principle o f most studies using this
approach is the existence o f binary structures, and at the level of
syllables, alternation betw een strong and weak is an easily observable
preference.4 For instance, it produces the characteristic stress pattern
/ i i / . 1 1 .
of English polysyllables: rh ythm icality, independence, interpretation.
Furthermore, there are a number o f English words in which the
position o f the stress is not fixed, but is influenced by the rhythmic
context in which the word occurs: many speakers will say outdoor
/ i i l l [ I
activities but ou tdoor sp o rts, thirteen bananas but thirteen elephants,
thus avoiding successive stressed syllables. A n alternating pattern may
even be perceived in stresses which the normal phonological rules
specify as equal: Chomsky and Halle (1 968, p. I l l ) cite tired old man
ALTERNATION AND STRESS-TIMING 71
French utterances show that English has by far the greatest durational
ratio betw een its longest and shortest syllables, and Adam s (1 9 7 9 ) has
dem onstrated that native speakers o f syllable-timed languages have
difficulty in achieving the correct timing o f English speech, however
good their command o f other aspects o f the language.
But if stress-timing were the only rhythmic principle in English, or
one that overrode all others, there would be no way o f explaining the
preference for an alternation betw een stressed and unstressed
syllables: the number o f nonstresses betw een stresses could vary freely
without affecting rhythmic regularity. W e have already noted, how
ever, that the syllable is itself a rhythmic unit, and although it is clearly
secondary to stress, its part in English speech rhythm should not be
overlooked. It has been found that whatever objective tendency to
stress-timing exists in the language decreases as the variation in
number o f nonstresses increases; U ldall (1 9 7 1 ), for instance, found
evidence for isochrony in one subject’s speech as long as the number of
nonstresses betw een stresses remained below three. (O ne reason for
this is the preference for rhythmic alternation itself: as we noted
earlier, when the number o f nonstresses reaches three, the middle one
will tend to take on som e o f the characteristics o f a stressed syllable,
thus lengthening the interval.) English speech rhythm is therefore
characterised by a certain degree o f tension betw een two rhythmic
principles, and the preference for an alternation betw een stressed and
unstressed syllables that the language exhibits can be understood as a
way o f minimising that tension, since only if the number of nonstresses
betw een stresses is held constant can the two principles be brought into
harmony. A lternation betw een single stresses and single nonstresses is
clearly the simplest form o f rhythm that can achieve such a marriage.
Other factors prevent this from being any more than a general
tendency in the language at large, but it lies at the heart o f English
metrical form, which capitalises on both the satisfying sense of
regularity produced by bringing the two rhythmic tendencies into
accord, and the expressive possibilities inherent in the conflict betw een
them.
It would be more accurate, therefore, to describe English as a
language in which stress-timing dom inates syllable-timing, rather than
one which is wholly stress-timed. The dominance of stress-timing can
som etim es em erge in the daily use o f language: for instance, we may
impose a regular rhythm on an utterance for special purposes (see
Crystal, 1969, p. 163; Q uirke ta l., 1972, p. 1043). The pressure o f an
74 THE RHYTHMS OF ENGLISH SPEECH
i i i i i
For G od’s sake, all I want is som e attention!
In doing this, the frustrated speaker is merely heightening the natural
stress-timing o f the English language ; it would be unnatural to give an
evenly-spaced emphasis to every syllable. A nother way in which
stress-timing can be illustrated is by means o f ‘choral reading’: if a
group o f English speakers is asked to read a passage together they will
tend to exaggerate the natural rhythmic tendencies of the language to
make unison pronunciation possible. Boom sliter, Creel, and Hastings
(1 9 7 3 ) found that the intervals betw een stresses in the recitation o f
verse com e much nearer to being equal when such choral reading is
performed. The more closely an utterance approaches to isochrony,
the more fully the stressed syllables are experienced as b ea ts; and
although this term is frequently used in phonetic studies to refer to the
function o f stressed syllables in all utterances, it is most appropriate
when the regularity o f the stress pattern is such that a clear rhythmic
structure o f alternations is perceived. W hen this occurs, the rhythm
becom es self-reinforcing, since we instinctively give the beats more
emphasis, and control the duration o f the intervals betw een them to
heighten the regularity.
The m ovem ent o f English speech, then, is determ ined by two
rhythmic phenom ena, the syllable and stress, and most regular verse in
English acknowledges this in its control of the number and disposition
o f both these in the metrical line. The poet writing in English is not
handling an infinitely m alleable substance; the language has its own
highly distinctive rhythmic character, which the skilful artist will
exploit to the full, as a sculptor brings out the natural forms and
textures o f his material. But regular verse involves the shaping of those
linguistic features into structures that obey more general principles o f
rhythmic form, and it is to these that we turn next.
Notes
1. For a valuable discussion of the debate over the syllable, see W. S. Allen (1973,
pp. 27-73).
2. See the hypothesis advanced by Ohman (1967) that stress should be understood as
NOTES 75
4.1 T H E P E R C E P T IO N O F R H Y T H M
To perceive a regular rhythm is to com prehend a sequence o f events as
a pattern in tim e, with two mutually reinforcing features, repetition and
THE PERCEPTION OF RHYTHM 77
/ X / X / X /
(1) G o, and catch a falling star
/ x x / x x / X X I
(2) Sweet be thy matin o ’er moorland and lea
pp. 1 5 0 -5 1 ; Harding, 1976, Ch. 3 and 4), and one which we shall find
very useful in discussing the rules o f metre in Part Three. In general
terms, it m akes possible a clearer account o f the notion o f metrical
tension, which we touched upon in 1.1. In the perception o f rhythm,
tension may be regarded as the psychological experience produced by
a local failure to satisfy com pletely an established regular rhythmic set.
It is felt only if the stimuli com e close enough to their expected form to
be interpreted as at least a partial fulfilment o f the set, and it has the
effect o f heightening the perceiver’s attention to the rhythmic
substance (whereas an absolutely regular rhythm often works to exactly
the opposite effect), and, by creating a demand for a return to the
momentarily thwarted regularity, o f increasing the sense o f forward
propulsion. There are therefore two principles o f onward m ovem ent
involved in rhythm: underlying patterns o f expectation and
satisfaction, and sequences o f tension and relaxation produced by
variations in the degree to which that satisfaction occurs. In many cases
the two cannot be separated, o f course: the relaxation attendant on a
return to strict rhythmic regularity, for instance, acts to heighten the
sense o f fulfilled expectation. Tension can also inhere in the relations
betw een rhythmic and other levels o f the verse, and com plete
relaxation occurs only if the patterns o f expectation and satisfaction at
every level coincide. W e shall return to the question o f tension
frequently in the pages that follow; see in particular 7.1 0 and 9.5.
It should be clear from this discussion that in whatever medium a
rhythm occurs, it always takes place in the dim ension o f time, albeit
psychological rather than objective tim e. ‘R hythm ’ is, o f course, often
used metaphorically - the rhythms o f a painting, or chimney pots
against the sky - but one must not lose sight o f the fact that such uses
are metaphorical; and that we use the word unmetaphorically when
discussing speech and poetry, which occur in tim e, and in which
sequential and dynamic relations are o f the utmost importance. It is
difficult to escape from the tyranny o f the sense o f sight, and many o f
the terms one falls back on in describing rhythm - ‘groups’,
‘structures’, ‘positions’, even ‘lines’ - have spatial origins. I can only
hope that in what follow s they will not be construed as having spatial
implications, but simply as a reflection o f the poverty o f the lexical
store on which one draws to refer to the richly various qualities o f
m ovem ent through time.
80 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
4.2 U N D E R L Y IN G R H Y T H M
One might say that the tendency o f a rhythm to continue once
established is a consequence o f its escape from the normal limitations
o f tim e, since it converts a succession o f different events into a
repetition o f the same event, and part o f its fascination may lie in this
illusory triumph over mutability. But an art-form requires a shape, not
a series extending into infinity; middles should feel like middles, and
ends like ends (and surprises are only possible if these norms have been
established). H ence in verse, as in music, rhythm is always organised,
and it is with the elem entary forms which provide this organisation that
we are now concerned. A s we are investigating the basic elem ents from
which sophisticated literary works are built, we shall give much of our
attention to anonym ous, popular verse in which a prevalent rhythmic
phenom enon is more likely to be a reflection o f a fundamental
property o f rhythm than a literary convention. N ot that separation of
the two is easy to achieve; even in looking at conventional aspects of
form it is pertinent to ask why certain poetic choices have becom e
established conventions and others have never been made a second
tim e, and the answer may be that only the former coincide with
som ething in the nature o f the medium itself. A nd if we find
elem entary patterns repeated again and again in popular verse from
m edieval tim es to the present, and reflected in a large body of more
literary verse, we can assume that there are reasons for this recurrence
which lie deeper than convention. Just as myth and folk-tale reveal in
stark form the plots which may be disguised in more self-conscious
novels, so nursery rhymes and ballads, which are not the product o f a
single conscious artistic act (or if they are, have been taken up by
audiences because they conform to the popular tradition), present the
simplest rhythmic forms in clear outline. O ne must not, o f course,
conclude that the reader’s or critic’s task is done when he has released
the popular form from its sophisticated envelope; as we shall see, the
artist’s problem is in part the avoidance o f the ever-tem pting
elem entary forms, which his readers, consciously or unconsciously,
will be only too ready to find. But without grasping the nature o f those
elem entary forms, we cannot hope to understand the achievem ent of
the com plex work of art which builds on them or finds ways round
them.
Rhythmic pulses in verse (and in music) tend to fall into groups, each
o f which the mind perceives as a w hole, with a beginning and an end;
UNDERLYING RHYTHM 81
we can call such a group an underlying rhythm. (O ne could use the term
‘G estalt’, though its usefulness in this context is limited by its
connotations o f visual configuration.) The m ost com m on underlying
rhythm in English popular verse is the group of four beats, and
exam ples com e readily to hand from all periods:
form. Burling’s appeal to our ‘com m on humanity’ does not get us very
far, and neither does Ker’s assertion that ‘this type o f verse is natural
because it runs in periods o f 4, 8, 16, which one may call the natural
rhythm for the human race’ (p. 206). His hint at the importance of the
four-beat rhythm’s capacity to enter into larger structures is a valuable
one, however, and is taken further by Tovey (1 9 1 0 -1 1 ), who states
that we have a ‘natural tendency to group rhythmic units in pairs, with
a stress on the first o f each pair; and hence, if our attention is drawn to
larger groups, we put more stress on the first of the first pair than on the
first of the second; and so with still greater groups’ (p. 279). A s we
shall see later, it is som ewhat artificial to isolate the four-beat line as
the m ost fundamental pattern; what seem s to be at the heart o f simple
rhythmic structuring is, as Tovey suggests, the existence o f rhythmic
pairs, arranged in hierarchies, each pair joining another pair to form a
four-unit whole. W e can leave aside as unproven the question o f
whether this is a truly universal characteristic o f rhythm. Studies o f
‘primitive’ music and verse have found a wide variety o f rhythmic
patterning (see, for instance, N ettl, 1956, Ch. 5; and Finnegan, 1977,
pp. 9 0 -1 0 2 ), but there is no reason to assume that such art is any less
com plicated in its elaboration o f simple underlying forms than our
own. Burling’s use o f children’s verse in his cross-linguistic study is a
more useful pointer to what might be considered ‘rhythmic universals’.
O ne can safely say, at least, that for reasons which go beyond the
separate domains o f music and poetry the four-beat rhythm has been a
recurrent feature in the rhythmic arts o f W estern Europe; and it seem s
likely that this is a reflection o f som ething fundamental in the faculty of
rhythmic production and perception itself.
It will be evident that each pair o f lines quoted above forms a single
unit; the sense o f com pletion after the second line is appreciably
stronger than after the first. It is reinforced by the different kinds o f
syntactic break at these points, though it is clearly not caused by these
breaks. A nd if we exam ine a typical nursery rhyme, we find a second
pair o f lines com plem enting the first pair, and producing another
fourfold structure (I indicate the main beats):
4.3 M E T R IC A L P A T T E R N S A N D U N R E A L IS E D B E A T S
So far there has been no need to differentiate betw een the underlying
rhythmic structure o f a stanza and the way it is actually manifested as a
set o f lines. H ow ever, the 4 x 4 structure is not always realised as a
four-line stanza, and it is important to keep the specific arrangement of
METRICAL PATTERNS AND UNREALISED BEATS 85
with such a feeling o f resolution: it is, in fact, the crucial closing rhyme
o f the familiar abcb schem e.
There are many other metrical patterns which realise this basic
structure, and the structure can be extended to create an effect o f
suspension or prolongation. O ne exam ple is the ‘Burns stanza’, in
which two tw o-beat lines are inserted in the pattern, and the whole
form bound together by an aaabab rhyme-scheme:
Again, such variations are typical o f the literary use o f a popular form.
The literary tradition has made much use of it, perhaps most
com m only when an effect o f simplicity is desired:
Here one can feel the strong pauses after four beats, corresponding to
the line-ends in the four-line form.
D o all these exam ples represent a rhythmic form distinct from the
4 x 4 structure? Careful introspection as one reads shows that the
answer is no. If one chants (1 2 ) very rhythmically, beating time as one
does so, one finds that it is much more natural to follow the second line
with a beat in silence, giving the line four beats, than to go straight on to
the next line; and the final line obviously follow s the same pattern,
making up the full 4 x 4 structure. W e need not be disturbed at the last
beat’s occurring in the silence after the end o f the stanza, since music
provides clear parallels; Cone (1 9 6 8 , p. 18) com m ents that the first
88 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
m ovem ent o f B eeth oven ’s Fifth Symphony forces the listener ‘to add a
silent measure after the last one notated - a measure that is as
essentially a part o f the com position as those actually written’.
Alternatively, one might choose to extend the last syllable o f the
shorter line over two beats in pronunciation, without affecting the
basic form. Hendren (1 9 3 6 ) dem onstrates that there is only one
fundamental type of ballad m elody - four phrases o f two double
m easures each - whether the verse is in long metre or com m on metre,
and such musical settings show clearly the extra beat after the
three-beat lines. A nyone who has recited nursery rhymes in their
normal social context will be familiar with the chant-like delivery that
gives the underlying four-beat structure full emphasis, but if objective
evidence is desired, the experim ent in ‘choral reading’ m entioned
earlier (Boom sliter, Creel, and Hastings, 1973) provides it. (Choral
reading, it will be recalled, induces each individual to adjust his
pronunciation to a norm shared by others, thus bringing out the
com m on underlying rhythms more clearly.) The group o f subjects was
asked to recite in chorus a stanza by Emily D ickinson in com m on
metre; the recorded syllable durations indicate that in such a reading
unrealised beats at the ends o f lines 2 and 4 occupy the full time o f
realised beats. W e can therefore show the underlying rhythmic
structure o f this type o f stanza as follows (using square brackets to
indicate unrealised beats):
In this pattern, the division o f the four-line unit into two pairs is
em phasised by the absence o f a realised beat at the end o f the second
line, a structure which is reflected graphically when the verse is set out
as a fourteener couplet. H ow ever, the further subdivision o f the
seven-beat group into two will always make itself felt, and one of the
difficulties in writing (and reading) fourteeners is the pause or
intonational cadence dem anded after four beats. If this is reflected in
the syntactic structure o f the line, any advantages of the long line as a
vehicle for m editative or narrative verse are lost, since the units o f
com position remain short, and the 4 x 4 structure, with its associations
METRICAL PATTERNS AND UNREALISED BEATS 89
The syllables on these beats invariably rhyme, and we rarely find the
aabb schem e familiar in long metre, which would produce an awkward
linking o f a weaker beat in the fourth position o f the line with a
stronger one in the third position.
It is perhaps as well to em phasise again at this point that what we are
trying to discover is the fundamental rhythmic organisation that gives
rise to our sense o f a unified whole in which temporal relations are
controlled and ordered, and that we are not attempting a transcription
90 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
Though long metre and com m on metre are the two realisations o f
the 4 x 4 rhythmic structure most frequently to be m et with, the same
structure can be m anifested in other metrical patterns. Occasionally
only the last line has an unrealised beat, creating a 4.4 .4 .3 pattern
which, in children’s verse, is especially com m on in counting-out
rhymes, where the rhythmic climax is delayed until the very last
syllable, at which point the victim is selected - one can imagine the
expression o f his chagrin replacing the final unrealised beat. Iona and
Peter Opie record several variants o f one such rhyme (1 9 5 1 , p. 223),
incidentally illustrating how nonsense form ations which observe the
m orphological and phonological principles o f English are perfectly
capable o f establishing a strong rhythm, thanks to our know ledge o f
stress rules and our receptivity to elem entary rhythmic structures. One
version runs:
The short last line, with its distinctive rhythm, can be used in literary
verse as a haunting refrain:
The doubling o f notes in the third bar o f this pattern, while maintaining
the regularity o f the rhythm, gives that bar a special salience, fills it
more fully with physical material, as it were, and transforms four
separate units into a single rhythmic w hole. It does this by implying an
internal structure: a new phase begins at bar 3 which is and yet is not
the same as the first phase, and as a result the fourth bar com es as a
clinching repetition o f bar 2 after the slight disturbance. O ne has only
to try rearranging the order o f the units to test the crucial function o f
this third bar.
W e find variants o f this rhythmic pattern in many individual lines of
nursery rhymes, where the third beat is follow ed by a larger number of
syllables than the other beats:
A nother version of this pattern is found in the long line o f the accentual
sapphic:
This is a form which has recurred in the W estern literary tradition from
m edieval (and perhaps R om an) times; for a discussion see N eedier
(1 9 4 1 ) and Attridge (1 9 7 4 , pp. 2 1 1 -1 6 ). A nd we shall see at the end
of the follow ing chapter that the rhythms o f the iambic pentam eter are
also related to this pattern. It does not seem unlikely, therefore, that
the same fundam ental rhythmic tendency underlies both the shaping
o f many individual lines and the 3.3.4.3 structure o f the short metre
stanza.
This rhythmic form ation is, in fact, a small-scale exam ple o f one o f
the com m onest ways o f effecting closure in an aesthetic form, usefully
discussed by B. H. Smith (1 9 6 8 , pp. 44, 6 5 -6 ): the return to an
96 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
4 .4 O F F B E A T S; D U P L E A N D T R IPL E R H Y T H M S
So far we have been concerned only with the beats o f the underlying
rhythmic structure and their realisation in metrical patterns, and have
proceeded as if what happens betw een the beats is irrelevant. A s far as
the basic architecture o f verse form is concerned this assumption is
valid, but once we start trying to characterise the different qualities o f
m ovem ent that can be built upon this foundation, it becom es
important to exam ine the effect o f syllables which function in the
sequence o f rhythmic alternations as offbeats. A s we have seen in our
discussion o f stress-timing, our sense o f the m ovem ent o f English
speech, whether we are speaking it or hearing it, depends primarily on
the fully stressed syllables, and there is a tendency to lighten and
quicken the unstressed syllables betw een these rhythmic focal points.
We should not be surprised, then, to find that we perceive stresses as
beats (that is to say, as repetitions o f a rhythmic pulse) not only if the
number of syllables betw een each stress is constant, but also if the
variation in their number is kept within certain limits. Thus Coleridge
can write:
OFFBEATS; DUPLE AND TRIPLE RHYTHMS 97
and our perception o f the four realised beats o f the first line and the
three realised beats o f the second line is in no way hindered by the
presence o f som etim es one, som etim es two, unstressed syllables
betw een them. The stress-timed rhythm o f English will, under certain
conditions, allow even successive stressed syllables to be read as two
beats:
Here we will be inclined to give the first ‘lang’ enough weight and
duration to allow the next beat to fall on the second ‘lang’, and in the
follow ing line ‘into’ can be given its normal stress contour if ‘fans’ is
drawn out a little in pronunciation. (In so reading it, we are not going
against the natural speech rhythms o f English; at most, we are bringing
out one possibility in the language at the expense o f another.) Many o f
the exam ples discussed earlier in this chapter exhibit a similar freedom
as regards the realisation o f offbeats. H ow ever, we noted in the
previous chapter that unstressed syllables also have a rhythmic identity
of their own, and as the variation in the number o f syllables betw een
beats increases, the underlying structure becom es blurred; thus the
assigning o f beats in the following exam ple can only be tentative:
rhythm o f four beats, therefore, always has three offbeats betw een the
beats, which we can show as follows:
B o B o B o B
It may have, in addition, an offbeat before the first beat and/or one
after the last beat. In the line o f verse itself, the offbeats o f the metrical
pattern can be m anifested in various ways: as one syllable {single
offbeat), which we shall indicate in scansion by Q; as two syllables
{double offbeat), indicated by 6; or occasionally as three syllables
{triple offbeat), indicated by $, though we have already noted the
tendency o f three unstressed syllables in English to be interpreted as a
beat with an offbeat on either side. Offbeats can also be im plied in the
rhythm but not realised in the language, and we shall indicate this by 6 .
A fuller scansion o f (33) is therefore as follows:
The chilling finality o f the replies is conveyed in part by the shift in the
last line o f each stanza from a light and rapid m ovem ent created by
double offbeats to a weightier alternation of beats and single offbeats
(a change which in the second stanza begins in the nightmarish
headshakes of the penultim ate line). This can be dem onstrated by
OFFBEATS; DUPLE AND TRIPLE RHYTHMS 99
noting the lightening effect o f rewriting the last two lines with double
offbeats throughout:
[35a] A nd the oaks and the beeches all twisted their heads,
A nd they answered him; ‘N ever to you’.
It is, o f course, the stress-tim ed rhythm of English, in co-operation with
the regular verse form, which speeds up the syllables when there are
two nonstresses betw een beats, and slows them down when there is
one. It might be noted in passing that in the first stanza A uden leaves a
final beat unrealised in the relatively rare position o f the third line, thus
emphasising the tense pause after the question, while in the same
position o f the second stanza the inescapable reply fills the full
four-beat structure; and also that the anguished cry in the second
stanza achieves its effect partly by the unusual use o f a triple offbeat.
The various m ethods o f realising offbeats can be used in this way for
local rhythmic effects; if, however, a poet m akes consistent use of
either single or double offbeats in a poem , another kind o f general
rhythmic principle is introduced: the special character o f duple and
triple rhythms. Two o f B lakes’s Songs o f Innocence will illustrate this
familiar distinction:
(3 6 ) W hen the voices o f children are heard on the green
o B o B o B o B
A nd laughing is heard on the hill,
o B o B o B [o B]
My heart is at rest within my breast
o B o B o B o B
A nd everything else is still.
o B o B o B [o B]
In the first o f these, the occurrence o f single offbeats does not upset the
fundamentally triple rhythm established by the first line, with its
characteristic qualities o f rapidity and lightness; while the second
exam ple has the som ewhat heavier m ovem ent o f duple verse. A line
100 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
like the last one in (3 6 ) could occur in duple verse, but we would
experience its rhythm differently in that context: the double offbeat
would merely be a substitute for the expected single offbeat, instead o f
a reminder o f an established triple rhythm. It is important to note that
the m etrical pattern o f these two exam ples is identical, and that the
distinction betw een duple and triple rhythms occurs not as a structural
principle, but in the surface realisation; it is for this reason that there is
no clear dividing line betw een the two. In com posing a bleak
counterpart to (3 6 ) for the Songs o f E xperience, Blake is able to create
a more ambiguous rhythm after the first line by slightly increasing the
proportion o f single offbeats:
4.5 L IN E -O P E N IN G S, L IN E -E N D S , A N D L IN E -JU N C T U R E S
W e have now considered the main beats in the simple rhythmic
structure, and the effect o f differing kinds o f offbeat betw een those
main beats, but what o f the offbeats that may occur before the first beat
and after the last: to what extent do they influence the m ovem ent of
the line? That they do not have the same role in maintaining duple and
triple rhythms is suggested by the fact that popular verse forms show
even freer variation in these positions than betw een beats.* It is very
com m on for lines to begin with the first realised beat and to end with
the last, whatever the arrangement o f syllables in betw een, with an
effect o f simplicity and directness, as in the openings o f many o f the
nursery rhymes already quoted:
In ballad m elodies, the anacrusis is more often than not at a lower pitch
than the accented syllable that follow s, reflecting its introductory
character; and many ballads begin with an unstressed ‘O ’, serving little
LINE-OPENINGS, LINE-ENDS, AND LINE-JUNCTURES 103
A ll these exam ples have had beats at line-end, but a less resounding
culmination is achieved by ending on an offbeat (traditionally, a
fem inine ending):
must be careful to distinguish betw een the genuine fem inine ending
and.the com m on phenom enon o f a normally unstressed syllable taking
the final beat o f the line, or at least going som e way towards giving
substance to an unrealised beat:
(46) G oosey, goosey, gander,
B B B B
W hither shall I wander?
B B B B
eighth beat, and from the ninth to the sixteenth. But there is no
unstressed syllable to bridge the gap betw een ‘b e’ and ‘A s’, and the
result is a pause, not as great as that created by an unrealised beat at
this point, but marked enough to impart a distinctive rhythmic
character to the stanza. If we introduce a final offbeat here, the change
is very noticeable:
[48a] G olden haired and golden hearted
I would ever have you standing,
A s you were when last we parted
Smiling slow upon the landing.
The rhythm becom es w ooden, and the strong outline o f the whole
stanza is clouded, m erely by adding two unstressed syllables. The
rhythm o f the original is also affected if we om it the final offbeats of the
first and third lines:
[48b] G olden haired and golden souled
I would ever have you be,
A s you were in days o f old,
Smiling slow and sad at me.
The loss o f two unstressed syllables has now made the m ovem ent more
abrupt, the lines more self-contained. W e could return to som ething
like the original rhythm by adding unstressed syllables at the beginning
o f lines 2 and 4:
[48c] G olden haired and golden souled
W ould I for ever have you be,
A s you were in days o f old,
Departing sad and slow from me.
One might say that the exact position o f the line-boundary with respect
to the offbeat is relatively unimportant to the metrical pattern, though
it does affect the rhythmic flavour o f the poetry. Musical settings o f 4 x
4 verse clearly dem onstrate this continuity: an unstressed syllable
occurring at the beginning o f a line other than the first is m ost likely to
form part o f the final bar o f the previous line.
Nursery rhymes, too, exhibit the continuity betw een lines very
plainly. W e have seen that a com m on opening line is one which begins
and ends on a beat; the second line, however, does not usually repeat
this pattern, being more likely to begin with an offbeat to sm ooth the
transition from the final beat o f the previous line, as in exam ples (7),
106 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
(1 2 ), and (2 3 ). But in those cases where the initial line ends with an
offbeat and the rhythm is duple, the next line tends to begin with a
beat:
Lines later than the first and second show similar tendencies, the
transition betw een lines 3 and 4 being more likely to demand
realisation o f offbeats than that betw een 2 and 3.
These characteristics are, o f course, m anifested only when
sm oothness o f rhythm is at a premium; nursery rhymes passed on by
oral delivery to children are likely to develop in the direction o f
sm oothness, though som etim es a more abrupt rhythm becom es part o f
the distinctive character o f a particular rhyme. But even in
sophisticated uses o f the four-beat rhythm, the continuity betw een the
units o f the underlying structure is an important aspect o f the form, to
be used or challenged by the poet. For instance, fem inine endings
before initial offbeats can be used to increase the integrity o f the
individual line and inhibit the onward thrust o f the 4 x 4 structure.
W hen an alternation o f fem inine and masculine endings occurs in an
iambic quatrain, such an effect is produced at the end o f the first and
third lines (where the interlineal m om entum is strongest), and the last
syllable o f the stanza is given a satisfying finality:
One aspect o f the relationship betw een the metrical pattern and the
LINE-OPENINGS, LINE-ENDS, AND LINE-JUNCTURES 107
verse is that rhyme is almost never absent, and this is som ething we
shall consider w hen we com pare it with its five-beat counterpart in the
following chapter.
4 .6 R ISIN G A N D F A L L IN G R H Y T H M S
The following stanzas are both from poem s by Samuel Johnson:
(5 3 ) C ondem ned to H op e’s delusive m ine,
A s on we toil from day to day,
By sudden blasts or slow decline,
Our social com forts drop away.
(5 4 ) W hen the bonny blade carouses,
Pockets full, and spirits high,
What are acres? What are houses?
O nly dirt, or w et or dry.
Although both are fully realised 4 x 4 structures, they have strikingly
different rhythmic qualities. In sensing this, we are not responding
m erely to the presence in one and absence in the other o f an initial
offbeat to sm ooth the way into the line, although this is an important
factor; we seem to be encouraged (and not just by the subject-matter)
to read the second with a jauntier rhythm, the beats and offbeats
occurring in a more sharply em phasised alternation. These two kinds
o f rhythm, traditionally called rising and fallin g, spring from the way in
which we group stressed and unstressed syllables, or beats and
offbeats; if there is a strong tendency to link the offbeats with the
follow ing beats, the rhythm will be perceived as rising, and if the
offbeats are felt as com pleting the m ovem ent started by the beats
which precede them , we will experience a falling rhythm. The m ost
obvious influence at work in shaping the line into rising or falling
groups is the structure o f words and phrases. In (5 4 ), the words
‘bonny’, ‘pockets’, ‘spirits’, ‘acres’, ‘houses’, and ‘only’ are them selves
falling units, and in the third line there is a break after an offbeat,
traditionally called a ‘fem inine caesura’ by analogy with fem inine
line-endings, which em phasises this rhythm. By contrast, m ost o f the
words and phrases o f (5 3 ) consist o f an offbeat follow ed by a beat; line
2, for instance, is rising throughout:
/ ------- \-f--------- \ r \ r
A s on we toil from day to day
RISING AND FALLING RHYTHMS 109
(The symbol used to indicate the rhythmic groups here is not in any
way system atically applied and is not part o f the scansion, whose
purpose is to indicate structural and rule-governed features.)
It is not merely fortuitous, however, that all the lines o f (5 3 ) have a
metrical pattern that begins with an offbeat and ends with a beat, while
those o f (5 4 ) begin with a beat and twice end with an offbeat. T o start
lines consistently with an offbeat leading to a following beat is to
encourage the reader to perceive a rising rhythm in what follow s, and
to end with a rising unit is to reinforce this tendency. The reverse is true
o f lines that begin consistently with a beat linked to a following offbeat,
especially if they have fem inine endings. M oreover, a poet who wishes
to establish a strong rising or falling rhythm is likely to match the
openings and endings o f the line with appropriate words and phrases
within it.9 A nd in short lines, the extrem ities may limit the choices
available: in (4 7 ), for exam ple, there is scarcely any room for the triple
falling rhythm o f the opening and close o f each line to be contradicted.
A general correlation is therefore to be expected betw een, on the one
hand, rising rhythms and metrical patterns that begin with an offbeat
(traditionally classified as iambic and anapaestic, though I shall call
them offbeat-initial) and, on the other, falling rhythms and patterns
that begin with a beat (trochaic and dactylic m etres, or beat-initial).
H ow ever, it is important not to confuse this general tendency with the
claim made by many metrical theorists that the distinction betw een
rising and falling rhythms is part o f the metrical structure o f English
verse. To scan a line as iambic, in terms o f classical foot-prosody, is
merely to show that its metre is duple and that it begins with an offbeat;
it provides no information about the rising or falling nature o f the
rhythm, except in so far as the line is subject to the general tendency
already noted. There is nothing structurally anom alous about an
iambic line with a predominantly falling rhythm, or a trochaic line with
a predominantly rising rhythm - indeed, many lines traditionally
classed as trochaic end with a beat, like the second and fourth lines of
(4 8 ) and (54). The last line o f (5 4 ) m oves into a rising rhythm without
any sense o f dislocation, while (5 3 ) contains many falling groups. To
equate ‘iambic’ and ‘anapaestic’ with ‘rising’, and ‘trochaic’ and
‘dactylic’ with ‘falling’, is to confuse metrical structure with metrical
style, and to diminish greatly the usefulness o f the two descriptive
terms we are discussing.10 Similarly, analysts o f musical rhythm - like
Stetson (1 9 2 3 ), C ooper and M eyer (1 9 6 0 ) - point out that
conventional bar-lines are no guide to the way in which rhythmic units
110 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
whole. This m eans that falling words and phrases help impart to the
rhythm the characteristics we have noted: a steady, march-like
m ovem ent, with beats and offbeats tending towards equal duration,
though the beats, unprepared for by any lead-in, com e with a sharp
emphasis. On the other hand, where stressed syllables tend to follow
unstressed syllables in words and phrases, the beats will take up a
longer tim e, and the offbeats will be shorter, resulting in a more varied
m ovem ent, often perceived as sm oother than a falling rhythm because
the short offbeats lead naturally into the longer beats. Som e writers
have made a distinction betw een a characteristically *duple’ timing for
falling disyllables or disyllabic phrases ( ) and ‘triple’ timing for
rising one («TJ), but while this reflects a genuine difference betw een
the rhythms, it im plies a strict temporal basis for verse, which, as we
have seen, is a misleading interpretation; there is little in com m on
betw een waltz-rhythm and iambic metre.
This rhythmic difference betw een rising and falling groups may, as
we noted in Chapter 3, be an outcom e o f the physiological processes o f
stress-production; however, psychological studies suggest that it may
also be a m anifestation o f a more widely occurring feature o f rhythmic
perception (though this in turn is probably based on the fundamental
properties o f muscular action). For exam ple, W oodrow (1 9 0 9 ), in a
series o f experim ents in which subjects were asked to report on their
rhythmic perception of a sequence o f sound stimuli, found that there is
a general tendency for alternating loud and soft sounds to be perceived
in falling groups, but for alternating long and short sounds to be
perceived in rising groups; a strong falling rhythm is therefore likely to
be characterised by durational equality and contrasts in intensity,
while a strong rising rhythm will be relatively even in intensity but
varied in duration. A similar relationship is observable in music:
Stetson (1 9 2 3 , p. 184) notes that a falling rhythm is usually in the form
«T3, s o m e tim e s / J, and only rarely is the accented note longer, J / ;
while the com m on alternation o f long and short notes is in fact
perceived in rising groupings, whatever the bar-lines might suggest:
J I J J I J / 1 J . M eyer (1956, pp. 1 0 4 -9 ) provides further musical
illustrations o f the tendency for durational differences to result in
‘end-accented’ rhythms, and for intensity differences to result in
‘beginning-accented’ rhythms. It seem s likely, therefore, that the
association betw een, on the one hand, falling groups and a
strongly-marked, evenly-divided rhythm, and, on the other hand,
rising groups and a more flowing, unevenly-divided rhythm, is a
RISING AND FALLING RHYTHMS 113
4 .7 D IP O D IC R H Y T H M S
In considering the 4 .3.4.3 pattern o f com m on metre (section 4.3
above) we noted the tendency for rhythmic beats to alternate betw een
stronger and weaker. This is part o f a general tendency in the
perception o f rhythm; W oodrow (1 9 5 1 , p. 1233) reports experim en
tation showing that ‘in a subjective grouping by four, with the first
m em ber accented, the third mem ber is apt to be given a lesser,
secondary accent’. Such an alternation in verse is called a dipodic
rhythm, and we can refer to the two kinds o f beat as prim ary and
secondary. A s is the case with all underlying rhythms, dipodic
alternation can function without the reader being aware o f it; the
easiest way to becom e conscious o f its role in a 4 x 4 structure is to tap
twice in each line during a normal reading, allowing o n e’s rhythmic
instincts to choose the m ost natural places. This will usually turn out to
be on the first and third beats, suggesting an underlying dipodic rhythm
which we can indicate by upper and lower case as follows:
rhyme together into a single unit, while the second masks the
subsidiary rhythmic patterning that is felt betw een the beats. W e could
indicate a dipodic rhythm in both scansions: the primary beats in (6 1 )
would be the ones that correspond to the beats shown in (61a), and the
primary beats in (61a) would form a giant four-beat couplet. But a full
scansion would have to show the interaction o f these different levels o f
rhythmic organisation, perhaps by m eans o f a diagram o f hierarchies
like those som etim es used in the analysis o f musical rhythms (see
Cooper and M eyer, I 9 6 0 ):12
And one for the little boy who lives down the lane
X / (X)/ X I X / X / (X) / X / (X /)
1
1____________ II
IL-— .1
II____ ______ II
II___ It «1- ..____________
----------- II____________
11___
X / X / X / (X),
L. IL II
/ X / X
li ________ 1
The result may look com plex, but the doubling rule which generates
the structure could hardly be more elem entary, and has a clear
relationship with the binary, hierarchical nature o f English
stress-patterns brought out by Liberman and Prince (1 9 7 7 ) (see 3.3
above).
It is tem pting to enlarge the hierarchical diagram to include an
alternation betw een lines and pairs o f lines (see, for instance, the
discussion by Croll, 1929, pp. 4 2 0 -2 1 ), but whether one should use
the same term for the alternating structures at such different levels is a
debatable point. T ovey considers the same issue in music (1 9 1 0 -1 1 ,
p. 279): ‘A s rhythm is contem plated in larger measures, it becom es
increasingly difficult to say where the sense o f rhythm ends and the
sense o f proportion begins. The same m elody that may be felt as a
square and symmetrical piece o f proportion in four-bar rhythm if it is
taken slowly, will be equally rational as a single bar o f “com m on
tim e” . . . if it is taken very quickly; and betw een these two extrem es
there may be insensible gradations.’ Thus the analogy betw een the
relationship o f the first and second pairs o f lines in a 4 x 4 stanza and
the relationship o f a stressed and unstressed syllable in a falling
rhythmic unit may be a tenuous or merely metaphorical one; but the
different kinds o f relationship shade into one another, and it would be
unwise to assume that any particular level o f the hierarchy is primary.
Patmore (1 8 5 7 ) and Croll (1 9 2 9 ) regard the dipodic alternation
betw een beats as the m ost fundamental o f all English rhythms, even o f
verse rhythm in all languages, but to argue this is to isolate artificially
one elem ent in an interrelated network, as well as to lose sight o f the
DIPODIC RHYTHMS 117
fact that m ost verse m oves a long way from the simple forms o f
elem entary rhythms.
D ipodic rhythms are m ost likely to be perceived in strongly rhythmic
verse occurring in 4 x 4 structures, especially if the content encourages
a chant-like m ode o f delivery. It is less com m on in triple verse than in
duple, no doubt because the triple units discourage a hierarchical
structure based on binary units. Som ething else which brings out the
dipodic structure is the om ission o f realised offbeats and beats in
appropriate positions: the first line o f (6 1 ) has no offbeats, and we are
quite likely to give the four solitary beats an alternating emphasis;
while the unrealised fourth beat in alternate lines increases the
emphasis on the third beat. The varying degrees o f stress in the
language can also be used to create a dipodic rhythm by using stronger
stresses for alternate beats; Stewart ( 1 9 2 4 ,1925a) found that popular
verse in four-beat rhythm tends to foster dipodic rhythms by this
means, while literary verse in the same rhythm, unless it is very overtly
dipodic, does not. There is no need to imagine conscious artistry in
such manipulations o f the accentual contours o f the language; a poet
who has the basic dipodic rhythm running in his head will be likely to
produce this configuration o f stresses without realising it, just as it is
possible to read such verse without becom ing conscious o f the nature
o f the rhythm to which we are responding. A nother practice which
encourages dipodic alternation is the com bining o f four-beat units into
eight-beat lines, since we are induced to read the w hole line as a
four-beat structure: such lines are often found in writers like Browning
and Kipling, who favour dipodic rhythms for special effects. If a
regular and strong dipodic rhythm is set up by these m eans, the result is
usually a rapid, insistent rhythm particularly suited to light verse; W. S.
Gilbert’s patter-song lyrics make much use o f it, for instance. This is a
well-known exam ple by Chesterton:
and blind
B
Apart from the running together of the pairs o f lines, the pattern o f
beats and offbeats here is identical to that o f the com m on 4 x 4
beat-initial stanza (as in (4 8 ) and (5 4 )), but this exam ple is given a
lighter, more lilting, m ovem ent by the secondary-prim ary alternation.
N otice, too, that having begun on the secondary beat, the line ends
satisfyingly on a primary beat without any use o f unrealised beats, and
hence with a sm oother transition to the next line.
Taranovsky (1 9 7 1 ) has formulated two laws which operate in
Russian verse to determ ine the strength o f the beats, and which may
have som e applicability to English, since they aim to encapsulate
general rhythmic principles. Putting them in the terms we are using,
they state that the beat after the first offbeat o f the line tends to be
primary (this would apply to the first beat in (62) and the second beat
o f (6 3 )), and that the last two realised beats o f the line tend to be weak
and strong respectively, creating an alternation which extends
regressively back through the line (again, (62) and (63) illustrate this
principle). The two principles may conflict to w eaken the dipodic
alternations, as in the full 4 x 4 stanza with initial offbeats (see (5 9 )),
or coincide to create a strong dipodic rhythm, as in the 4.3.4.3
equivalent considered as two seven-beat units (see (6 0 )). These
principles would help account for som e of the features o f dipodic
rhythm that we have noted, though the greater tolerance in English
verse o f lines ending with weak beats m eans that the fundamental
preference for a primary-secondary rhythm can override them both, as
often happens in the 4 x 4 beat-initial stanza.
Som e lines o f Byron’s will illustrate the distinction betw een the two
DIPODIC RHYTHMS 119
types o f dipodic rhythm, and the way in which the reader’s response is
coloured by them:
(6 4 ) Though the ocean roar around m e,
Y et it still shall bear me on;
Though a desert should surround m e,
It hath springs that may be won.
H ow are we to read this? Two ways present them selves, depending on
the degree o f stress we give to the first word in each line. If we
em phasise it by m eans o f a strong stress, a heavy rhythm is established,
which m oves the verse on with a regularity and insistence which, it
might be felt, is appropriate to the p oet’s em pty posturing. If, on the
other hand, we give it very little weight, and allow the first strong stress
to fall on the third syllable o f each line, the rhythm loses much o f its
march-like solidity, and m oves with an elasticity which permits a
greater variety o f tone, including a sense o f self-awareness and
self-am usem ent. Why is it that this slight difference in reading has such
a marked effect?
The answer lies in the two varieties o f dipodic rhythm. Stressing the
first syllable creates the conditions for the com m on prim ary-secon
dary rhythm, familiar from nursery rhymes, and if the stanza is chanted
with alternating stronger and w eaker stresses, this rhythm will emerge
in an exaggerated form:
The result is a rhythm which tends to dom inate the language, and give
unnatural weight to words like ‘Though’, ‘Y et’, ‘should’, and ‘may’, as
if the speaker were heavily underlining the connectives and auxiliaries
to make his meaning plain to a dull-witted audience. The other
reading, if we exaggerate it, produces the rarer and less dominating
secondary-prim ary dipodic rhythm:
The switch from the perception of one of these rhythms to the other is
rather like the switch betw een the two interpretations o f a visually
ambiguous figure: it may take som e time before the alternative
interpretation is grasped, but when it does appear, all the perceptual
evidence suddenly coheres in a fresh way, the rabbit entirely displacing
the duck. That Byron was alert to these rhythmic qualities, and that the
second of these readings (in a muted form) is the correct one, is made
clear from the first stanza o f the poem , where the initial weak beat is
om itted altogether from the first line, making the wrong reading
im possible, and establishing a rhythmic configuration which a sensitive
reader will allow to carry him through the poem:
(6 5 ) My boat is on the shore,
[b] B b B
And my bark is on the sea;
b B b B
But, before I go, Tom M oore,
b B b B
H ere’s a double health to thee!
b B b B
(66) The days are sick and cold, and the skies are grey and old,
[b] o B o b o B o b 0 B 0 b o B [ o ]
And the twice-breathed airs blow damp;
b o B o b oB o b o B [o b o B o]
And I’d sell my tired soul for the bucking beam -sea roll
b o B o b o B o b o B o b o B [ o ]
O f a black Bil-ba-o tramp
b o B o b o Bob 6 B [o b o B o]
DIPODIC RHYTHMS 121
Notes
1. In the earlier part of this century, there were a number of experimental
investigations of rhythm, including those of Warner Brown (1908), Woodrow
(1909), Wallin (191 1 -1 2 ), and Isaacs (1920). Prall (1929, 1936) provides an
aesthetic foundation for the study of rhythm, and there are discussions of musical
rhythm which have a bearing on verse in Langer (1953, Ch. 8), Meyer (1956), and
Cooper & Meyer (1960). A useful introduction to the psychological background is
given by Harding (1976, Ch. 1), and surveys of the field are provided by Chatman
(1965, Ch. 2) and W. S. Allen (1973, Ch. 8).
122 THE FOUR-BEAT RHYTHM
(2) Since brass, nor stone, nor earth, nor boundless sea
B B B B B
(3) Some natural tears they dropped, but wiped them soon
B B B B B
The fundamental reason why poets over the centuries have turned to
the five-beat line for their most ambitious verse is an obvious but
frequently overlooked one: it is the only simple metrical form of
m anageable length which escapes the elem entary four-beat rhythm,
with its insistence, its hierarchical structures, and its close relationship
with the world o f ballad and song.
The difference in rhythmic character betw een the four-beat and the
five-beat line is often blurred in metrical studies, but som e writers have
com m ented on it, and perhaps even overstated it: Ker (1928,
pp. 2 0 5 -6 ) claims that the four-beat measure ‘agrees with certain
com m on tendencies or habits in the human mind, and is in a sense
m ore natural, more easily found out and appropriated, than, say, the
ten-syllabled line, which is often difficult to understand and im itate’,
and both Lewis (1 9 3 8 ) and Burling (1966; 1970, Ch. 10) assert that
whereas four-beat structures are appreciated without instruction, the
five-beat rhythm requires training before it can be perceived. M alof
(1 9 6 4 ) argues that the four-beat line is a ‘native’ English pattern to
which the language is ‘naturally attracted’, as opposed to the ‘foreign’
pentam eter tradition (p. 586); but while this may throw som e light on
the history o f English metre, it is misleading as a synchronic
description: as we have seen, it is precisely the ‘naturalness’ o f the
four-beat line as an elem entary rhythmic form that lies at the heart o f
its relative insensitivity to the distinctive rhythms of a particular
language. Literary criticism is perhaps too eager to ask what the formal
(and other) properties o f poetry manage to get into language;
som etim es it is more enlightening to take note o f what they succeed in
keeping out. To understand the special character of the five-beat line,
therefore, it is essential to be aware of the properties of the four-beat
rhythm which it escapes, and to exam ine its strategies o f evasion.
UNDERLYING RHYTHM AND METRICAL PATTERN 125
What is remarkable about the five-beat line is not only the success
with which it has been used over a wide range o f poetic kinds, but the
tight constraints observed by the poets who have used it. It is worth
quoting Ruskin’s com m ent on this point, the classical vocabulary of
which is easily translatable into other terms:
U pon adding the fifth foot to our gradually lengthening line, we
find ourselves fallen suddenly under hitherto unfelt limitation.
The verses we have hitherto exam ined may be constructed at
pleasure o f any kind o f metre - dactyl, trochee, iamb, or anapaest.
But all at once, we now find this liberty o f choice refused. We may
write a pentam eter verse in iambs only.
A most notable phenom enon, significant o f much more than I
can at present understand, - how much less explain; [ . . . ] the
historical fact being quite indubitable and unalterable, that no
poet has ever attem pted to write pentam eter in any foot but the
iamb, and that the addition o f another choreus [trochee] to a
choreic tetrameter - or o f another dactyl to a dactylic one, will
instantly make them helplessly prosaic and unreadable. (1880,
pp. 5 5 -6 )
A t least two o f R uskins’s contem poraries had attem pted - not wholly
without success -th e s e ‘unreadable’ m etres (see below , pp. 1 3 0 ,1 3 1 );
but the essential validity o f the observation remains. In this chapter we
shall exam ine the distinctive characteristics o f the five-beat rhythm by
comparing it with the four-beat form already discussed. In asking to
what extent these characteristics reflect the nature o f the five-beat
rhythm itself, rather than literary convention, we can hope to reach a
fuller understanding o f its special status in the English poetic
tradition.1
5.1 U N D E R L Y IN G R H Y T H M A N D M E T R IC A L P A T T E R N
That a five-beat rhythm is a less simple and less salient perceptual form
than a four-beat rhythm scarcely needs demonstrating; if experim ental
evidence is required, W oodrow (1 9 5 1 , p. 1234) reports that subjects
find rhythmic groups o f five more difficult to im pose on a sequence of
undifferentiated sound stimuli than groups o f two, three, four, or six.
Or if we attend to the elem entary rhythms o f popular music, we find
that a five-beat bar or a five-bar phrase is rarely to be encountered. The
126 THE FIVE-BEAT RHYTHM
every second line; and the break enforced by lineation, syntax, and
rhyme after three beats imparts a clipped, staccato m ovem ent to
the verse. Five-beat lines are, o f course, often divided internally by
syntactic breaks, but it is one o f the advantages o f the pentam eter as a
vehicle for long poem s that the rhythmic structure does not create
pressure for one particular subdivision. The most rhythmically
balanced line results, it is true, from a pause after the second or the
third beat, and poets favouring regularity usually prefer one o f these
positions if the line is to have only one break; this was incorporated
into prosodic theory by som e eighteenth-century poets and critics (see
Dillon, 1977). But even this allows variety, and does not result in the
perception o f the line as an edifice built up from smaller blocks.
The traditional stanza forms in which five-beat verse has been
successfully written all use undivided pentam eters, som etim es with an
occasional six-beat line; and because each five-beat line is rhythmically
independent, rhyme plays an active role in binding lines together.
W hen a poetic style demands tightness o f organisation together with
the freedom to em ploy speech rhythms, the pentam eter couplet offers
the ideal combination: the adjacent rhymes create strong formal units
larger than the line, while the five-beat rhythm remains a flexible
medium for the spoken language. N ot surprisingly, therefore, the
couplet form seem s more appropriate to the controlled wit o f The
D unciad than to the free-ranging fantasy o f E n d ym io n . On the other
hand, because the five-beat line is under no rhythmic pressure to form
four-line groups, it lends itself more fully than the four-beat line to the
creation o f com plex stanza forms. These are capable o f com bining a.
sense o f large-scale freedom almost as great as that o f blank verse with
a formal orderliness unmatched by any other metrical form (and in this
case Keats furnishes som e o f the finest exam ples). Because such forms
do not have an underlying rhythmic structure with a natural end, they
often make use o f som e special device to achieve a feeling o f closure,
such as the couplet with new rhymes in the rhyme royal stanza
(<ababbcc) and the ottava rima stanza (abababcc), or the final six-beat
line in the Spenserian stanza (ababbcbcc). Simple stanzas o f four lines
do, o f course, occur in five-beat verse, the most fam ous exam ple being
Gray’s Elegy (which has provided the name elegiac quatrain for the
stanza), and the usual rhyme-scheme of this stanza, ab ab, can,
especially if the syntax em phasises the divisions betw een lines and
pairs o f lines, induce som ething o f the continuity, and the rising and
falling intonation, o f the 4 x 4 structure:
DUPLE AND TRIPLE, RISING AND FALLING RHYTHMS 129
such endings, a beat-initial pentam eter reads more easily, though the
contrast betw een four-beat and five-beat forms is still marked:
(1 2 ) Q ueen and huntress, chaste and fair,
N ow the sun is laid to sleep,
Seated in thy silver chair,
State in w onted manner keep.
[12a] Q ueen and huntress, ever chaste and fair,
N ow the weary sun is laid to sleep,
Seated smiling in thy silver chair,
State in w onted manner do thou keep.
A gain, the lines becom e m ore independent o f one another, and the
lucid rhythmic flow o f Jonson’s lines gives way to a m ore hesitant
m ovem ent. A t the same tim e, the effect o f the initial beat becom es
more pronounced, because it is perceived each time not as part o f a
continuous rhythmic pattern extending over several lines, but as a new
beginning.
Five-beat lines therefore usually begin with an offbeat, keeping the
powerful tread o f the falling rhythm and its associated four-beat
structures at bay. This is not to say that pentam eter verse is always
strongly rising; the longer line in fact allows great flexibility in the
grouping o f syllables, and a poet w ho uses a marked rising rhythm is
exercising an artistic choice:
/ \ ( \ t \ f 's / \
(1 3 ) A heap o f dust alone remains o f thee;
/ \ / \ f A f \ C A
’Tis all thou art, and all the proud shall be!
5.3 L IN E -JU N C T U R E S A N D B L A N K V E R SE
O ne area where five-beat verse seem s to be under weaker rather than
stronger constraints than four-beat verse is in the relationship betw een
metrical structures and syntactic structures: som e poem s observe the
same tendency as four-beat verse to end lines at syntactic boundaries,
while others make use o f run-on lines, including strong run-ons across
close syntactic links, in a way which is rare in the shorter form. The
LINE-JUNCTURES AND BLANK VERSE 133
character o f the transition from one line to the next is clearly different
in the two forms, and this demands careful exam ination.
In the 4 x 4 structure, the end o f each four-beat group is
perceptually very distinct (even if it does not coincide with the
line-end), but it is not until the end o f the fourth group that we
experience a full sense o f finality. The different degrees o f closure at
the end o f each line are controlled by the overall pattern, resulting in
both continuity and a clear articulation into subdivisions. The
pentam eter line, as we have noted, is a more independent form, and
does not have a strong tendency to becom e part o f a larger whole; even
when it is organised by rhyme into stanza forms, the lines retain then-
separate identity as five-beat groups. This m eans that when we reach
the end o f the line there is no com pelling pressure from the larger
structure to register the com pletion o f a rhythmic unit and to m ove on
to the next one. Instead, the syntax has a m ore powerful voice -
another exam ple o f the five-beat rhythm’s less dominating relationship
with the language - and will determ ine whether we pause or read
straight on to the follow ing line. Pentam eter verse which consistently
encourages us to pause at line-end will, o f course, be marked by
stronger rhythms, since the five-beat units will be highlighted; the
liberal use o f run-on lines, on the other hand, will create a continuous
m ovem ent in which the line-divisions may not be very apparent -
Johnson’s com plaint that M ilton’s verse is verse only to the eye is not
without foundation. The latter form com es as close to natural speech
rhythms as is possible in a regular m etre, and dramatic pentam eter
verse is able to take full advantage o f this fact.
It is som etim es suggested that five-beat lines should be understood
as six-beat units with a final silent beat (see, for exam ple, Bracher,
1947; D avid Abercrom bie, 1964a, p. 23; Leech, 1969, Ch. 7), but the
freedom to pause at the end o f a five-beat line if the syntax demands it
should not be confused with the metrical rest produced by an
unrealised beat in a four-beat structure. Careful introspection should
be enough to dem onstrate the difference; com pare, for instance, the
effect o f the run-on at the end o f the three-beat second line o f the
following stanza with the rewritten five-beat version that follows:
(1 4 ) A nd if I pray, the only prayer
That m oves my lips for me
Is - ‘Leave the heart that now I bear,
A nd give me liberty.’
134 THE FIVE-BEAT RHYTHM
(15) If you can keep your head when all about you
B b B b B [b B]
Are losing theirs and blaming it on you
B b B b B [b B]
F ive-beat:
(17) Oh be thou blest with all that H eav’n can send,
Long health, long youth, long pleasure, and a friend
[17a] Oh be thou blest with all that H eav’n doth treasure,
Long health, long youth, a friend, and lasting pleasure
LINE-JUNCTURES AND BLANK VERSE 135
stronger forms doing without it, the relationship is in fact the reverse:
the more perceptually salient the rhythmic units, the more they seem
to demand a closing rhyme, while a less prominent form like the run-on
pentam eter arouses no such expectation. W e have already noted that
where rhyme is used in pentam eter verse, it is less a reflection o f an
underlying structure than a way o f organising the lines in the absence
of such a structure. The four-beat rhythm, on the other hand, with its
emphatic line-endings and propensity to participate in larger
groupings, strongly invites rhyme; in the 4 x 4 structure there is a
definite sense that the third line is a repetition o f the first, and the
fourth o f the second, and this experience is clinched by an abab rhyme
schem e.
Even outside its com m on stanza forms, the insistence o f the
four-beat rhythm seem s to invite a special marking o f the final beat.
Som e four-beat couplets o f Sidney’s, rewritten without rhymes, will
indicate what is lost when they are absent:
(2 0 ) What tongue can her perfections tell
In w hose each part all pens may dwell?
Her hair fine threads o f finest gold,
In curled knots m an’s thought to hold,
But that her forehead says, ‘In me
A whiter beauty you may se e .’
5.4 SY L L A B IC R H Y T H M
G iven their sensitivity to speech rhythms, one might expect five-beat
lines to allow free variation in the number o f unstressed syllables
betw een beats. This is not so, however, even within the literary
tradition; while the four-beat line has often been used with this kind of
variation (C oleridge’s ‘Christabel’ is perhaps the most self-conscious
exam ple), the pentam eter has usually been distinguished by a more
rigorous control o f the number o f syllables, its preference for duple
rhythms and initial offbeats producing in strict metrical styles a count o f
ten, or eleven if a fem inine ending is used, and only slightly more
variety in freer styles.
To understand this difference, we need to look a little more closely
at the role o f the syllable in the rhythms o f English verse. W hile there
are external reasons for the emphasis in different periods o f literary
history on a fixed syllable count, these cannot explain the success o f
this metrical principle in English; the achievem ents o f Shakespeare,
Spenser, M ilton, and W ordsworth can hardly rest on a metre which is
foreign to the language in which they wrote. It will be recalled that in
Chapter 3 we found evidence for regarding the syllable as an
elem entary rhythmic unit o f language, and it is likely, therefore, that
apart from acting as the carrier o f stress patterns and creating duple or
triple rhythms, it can play a metrical role in its own right. The stress
SYLLABIC RHYTHM 139
rhythm o f English is too dom inating for this role to be a major one, as it
is in French verse, but we have already noted that the tendency
towards isochrony in English is circumscribed by the rhythmic effect o f
the syllables betw een stresses. W e can gauge the relative importance o f
the two rhythmic principles by attending to purely syllabic verse, in
which the number and position o f the stresses is allowed to vary freely
while the syllable count is fixed:
(2 2 ) Bare-throated profile with the tum bled bright young hair,
Full face with shining eyes, and the rose-leaf and gold
Granted by our com plaisance to the monochrome:
W ell, thank the Am erican that with both hands he took
A nd offered us ‘G od’s vulgar lyric R upert B ro o k e ’.
M ost readers find it difficult to distinguish the rhythmic effect of such
verse from that o f free verse, although more than one poet who has
written extensively in syllabics has reported that one can becom e
habituated to the syllable count, and recognise instinctively when, say,
the twelfth syllable has been reached. W e can conclude, then, that the
syllables o f the language exert a subsidiary rhythmic influence, but that
this is most likely to be felt within the framework o f a regular stress
pattern.
In 7.6 we shall consider the effects o f the syllabic principle on the
rules o f m etre, but our im m ediate question is, why does it appear to be
o f more importance in five-beat verse than in four-beat verse? The
distinction is not betw een a metrical form which is based on the syllable
count and one which is based on stress-timing, as is som etim es argued;
five-beat verse clearly reflects the stress-timed rhythms o f the language
while much four-beat verse observes a fixed syllable count. It does
appear, however, that the rhythmic character o f the pentam eter line
dem ands a degree o f syllabic strictness which the shorter form can
dispense with. The four-beat line, especially in the 4 x 4 structure,
rapidly sets up an insistent rhythm: we are very ready to interpret a few
rhythmic signals in terms o f that simple structure if we are at all able to
do so, just as we can understand a frequently-heard sentence-type
from only a few fragments. A nd as long as the verbal material does not
deviate too far from that pattern, we will continue to perceive it. But
the five-beat rhythm is not as deeply em bedded in our mental habits: it
is a relatively less prominent form, a w eaker Gestalt, and if a writer
wants to maintain a sense o f the underlying pattern, he has to exercise
careful control over the disposition o f rhythmic elem ents. Or as
140 THE FIVE-BEAT RHYTHM
Johnson put it in his L ife o f M ilton : ‘The music o f the English heroic
line strikes the ear so faintly that it is easily lost, unless all the syllables
o f every line co-operate together’. So where four-beat verse can
tolerate varying numbers o f syllables betw een the beats without any
disturbance to the underlying rhythm, and indeed thrives on such
variation because the principle o f stress-timing is thereby streng
thened, poetry in five-beat lines is more likely to observe the rule o f
one syllable realising one offbeat, departing from this only when
com pensation earlier or later in the line maintains the syllable count.
The result is verse in which the rhythmic function o f the syllable is
relatively more important, and any marked degree o f variation in the
realisation o f offbeats dissolves the rhythmic structure and produces
what is really a form o f free verse. A s such, it still has a great deal o f
potential, and can draw on many o f the strengths of the pentam eter (as
in som e o f H opkins’s experim ents in ‘sprung rhythm’, for instance),
but it is far less closely related to the regular five-beat line than, say,
‘Christabel’ is to duple four-beat verse. This difference can be
illustrated by taking exam ples of both line-lengths with a strict
syllable-count, and introducing a few variations betw een the beats:
Four-beat:
(2 3 ) Sweet, be not proud o f those two eyes,
B B B B
Which star-like sparkle in their skies;
B B B B
Nor be you proud that you can see
B B B B
A ll hearts your captives, yours yet free.
B B B B
Five-beat:
(2 4 ) In sober mornings do not thou rehearse
B B B B B
The holy incantation o f a verse;
B B B B B
SYLLABIC RHYTHM 141
But when that men have both well drunk and fed,
B B B B B
Let my enchantm ents then be sung or read.
B B B B B
The alterations are not sufficient to destroy either the four-beat or the
five-beat rhythm, but the difference betw een the pairs is more marked
in the second case: the cultivated urbanity o f Herrick’s pentam eters is
more damagingly affected than the graceful lyricism o f his four-beat
lines.
But while the number o f syllables betw een beats is more strictly
controlled in the pentam eter than in the four-beat line, the number of
beats in the line is probably slightly less crucial. A n extra beat
anywhere in the 4 x 4 pattern will upset the entire structure, but in a
succession o f pentam eter lines, a line with one beat more or less will
have only a local effect; it may not even be noticed, provided that the
regular alternation o f beat and offbeat is maintained. The
fundamentally five-beat m ovem ent o f Spenser’s verse in The Faerie
Queene is not upset by the six-beat final lines o f the stanzas, nor do
D ryden’s heroic couplets lose their rhythmic character as a result o f the
occasional Alexandrine; and the four-beat lines that occur sporadically
in Shakespeare’s blank verse are not fatal to the rhythm. This is in part
a consequence o f the isolation o f the five-beat line - it is less likely to
affect what occurs before and after it - and in part a result o f the less
prom inent rhythmic pattern o f five units. In other words, the
distinguishing feature o f five-beat verse is that it observes the
continuous alternation o f beat and offbeat as a result o f the strict
control o f stressed and unstressed syllables, and not because it is
carried along by a strong underlying rhythm. If the m otto o f the
characteristic four-beat rhythm might be said to be ‘Take care o f the
beats and the offbeats will take care o f them selves’, there is perhaps
som e appropriateness in regarding the m otto o f the pentam eter as
being the reverse.
142 THE FIVE-BEAT RHYTHM
5.5 F IV E -B E A T A N D F O U R -B E A T R H Y T H M S
•rlj./lj-rljroU
N ot only is this a four-beat rhythm, but it conforms very closely to the
particularly com m on type o f four-beat rhythm which we discussed in
4.3, where the third measure is given special status by an increase in
FIVE-BEAT AND FOUR-BEAT RHYTHMS 143
(26) Up with the day, the sun thou w elcom ’st then,
B B B B B
Sport’st in the gilt plaits o f his beams,
B B B B
And all these merry days mak’st merry men,
B B B B B
Thyself, and melancholy streams.
B B B B
N ote
1. Not much has been made in metrical studies of the distinctive rhythmic qualities of
the iambic pentameter, and the particular dangers it courts as a metrical form. The
most perceptive comments on this topic are probably those by Fitzroy Pyle, made in
the course of several essays (1 9 3 9 ,1 9 6 8 ,1 9 7 3 ). A valuable survey o f discussions of
the iambic pentameter is given by Weismiller (1975, pp. 2 59-83).
Part Three: Metre
This page intentionally left blank
Chapter 6
The distinction we have drawn betw een the rhythm o f the language
and elem entary rhythmic forms is to som e extent a misleading one: our
experience o f rhythm as a general phenom enon may well be coloured
by our deeply-ingrained habits o f speech production and perception,
and the rhythms we create and hear in language are certainly
conditioned by more general rhythmic principles. N evertheless, the
two do have an existence independent o f each other: the m ovem ent of
normal English speech does not exhibit regular rhythmic forms, and
patterns o f rhythm are perceptible in media other than language.
Regular verse is a wedding betw een the two, and like any good
marriage, involves com prom ises on both sides: the language has to
give up its freedom to arrange syllables in whatever patterns the sense
requires, and to submit to a new set o f principles, while the rhythmic
forms have to give up the perfect regularity and symmetry they possess
in their ideal state, and accept the distortions to be expected from
union with the unruly material o f speech. These compromises are made
easier by the underlying compatibility already m entioned, however,
and by a set o f established conventions giving precise expression to
that compatibility. The formal statem ent o f those conventions - the
marriage contract, one might say - is the set o f metrical rules.1
A poet writing in regular verse organises the syllables of the
language in such a way as to bring them within reach o f one o f the
underlying rhythms that readers are predisposed to perceive. In Part
Two we took for granted this arrangement o f syllables in lines of verse,
but it is time to ask what patterns o f stress the poet may use, how close
the language needs to com e to the form o f an underlying rhythm before
the reader will perceive the connection, and to what degree the natural
rhythms o f speech can be retained, or perhaps even heightened, within
the resulting regularity. To attempt to formulate the rules o f English
metre is to undertake a map o f this region o f interaction, giving
concrete representation to the principles which not only guide the
148 WHAT IS A METRICAL RULE?
poet, but also enable the reader - any reader who speaks the language
and has som e acquaintance with rhythmic form - to perceive in a
sequence o f syllables the orderly processes, and the expressive
possibilities, o f regular verse. W e shall begin in this chapter with som e
general considerations concerning the task ahead.
6.1 R U L E S O F M E T R E A N D R U L E S O F L A N G U A G E
M etrical form is an aspect o f poetry that has always attracted
discussion in terms o f rules, and this reflects an abiding intuition that
the activity o f the writer in constructing verse, and o f the reader in
responding to it, is far from haphazard, and rests on shared principles
which are capable o f being formally stated. There is a clear parallel
here with the activity o f producing and understanding the sentences o f
a language: this activity, too, has always been discussed in terms o f
rules, on the basis o f similar assumptions. The traditionally accepted
equivalence o f the two kinds o f rule is reflected in the Latin grammars
produced for Tudor schools, and their successors in the following three
centuries: after such sections as ‘Orthography’, ‘E tym ology’, and
‘Syntax’, each with its appropriate set o f rules, cam e ‘Prosody’, with its
own aw esom e array o f rules governing the quantity o f Latin syllables
and their arrangement into lines o f verse. But with metre as with
language there is no need to have any conscious knowledge o f the rules
if one has assimilated them in a more direct manner: the Elizabethans
formulated few rules o f English grammar and fewer rules o f English
metre, but their conversation and their poetry in the vernacular were
not thereby impaired. It is usually only when we are faced with a
foreign language or verse form that we have to rely on the mechanical
use o f explicit formulations; and we then find ourselves envying those
who have never experienced the need to learn them as rules.
Renaissance scholars who were able to scan Latin verse only by dint of
long labour marvelled at Cicero’s description o f the uneducated
Rom an populace jeering at an actor who com m itted a metrical
blunder.2
To set out to formulate the rules o f o n e’s own language, therefore, is
to attem pt to make explicit what one already knows, in som e sense of
that slippery word ‘know ’; and to propose rules o f metre for a body o f
verse with which one is deeply familiar is to do som ething very similar.
There is therefore a theoretical justification for the tim e-honoured
RULES OF METRE AND RULES OF LANGUAGE 149
strictly literary the verse, the more important will be the conventions o f
the metrical tradition, and in order to exam ine their role, we shall
devote particular attention to the iambic pentam eter as a firmly
established literary form. W e shall find that, far from being arbitrary
im positions, they usually serve a specific purpose - often the purpose
o f evading the simple and insistent forms that result from the most
elem entary realisations o f rhythm in language.
The rules proposed in the following chapter, therefore, should not
be thought o f as strictly analogous with generative rules o f syntax or
phonology. Though they are presented in terms o f a framework which
might suggest that analogy, and can be said to ‘generate’ lines o f
metrical verse, they are concerned not with underlying com petence
but with perceptual experience. The most econom ical way of
presenting rules o f this kind is in terms o f formal conventions, by
means o f which simplicity o f formulation can be used as an index o f a
rule’s generalising power; but the search for algebraic elegance can all
too easily take precedence over the need for accurate representation,
and achieve only empty abstractions. In the following chapter, I give
the rules in the form o f verbal statem ents, though I have used the
Appendix on ‘R ules and Scansion’ to suggest a possible system of
formal conventions, drawing on, but differing significantly from, the
conventions o f generative linguistics. The rules which I propose are
intended to be more explicit than those o f most metrical theories, but,
unlike linguistic rules, they do not aim to be wholly so: there com es a
point where rhythmic distinctions are too fine to be captured in
m anageable rules, and one has to say with Saintsbury, ‘The ear must
decide’. Critical utility may be more valuable than theoretical rigour or
econom y o f form ulation, and the goal of expository clarity more
worthwhile than the dream o f total explicitness.
6.2 M E T R IC A L SET
The word ‘rules’, in the context in which I have been using it, is
som ewhat misleading; it suggests an instruction which poet and reader
are obliged to follow , inscribed like M osaic tablets som ewhere apart
from the verse they write and read. Perhaps a better word would be
‘regularities’, for the rules are simply statem ents of consistencies in the
behaviour o f poets and readers, those habits o f mind and
METRICAL SET 153
rules which represents it, rather as ‘grammar’ can refer both to the
speaker’s internalised rules and to the linguist’s form ulation o f them.
To propose such a general set is to make the claim that all English verse
that we recognise as rhythmically regular falls within the framework it
provides, and can be described in its terms. A ny invented m etre which
cannot be so described is not likely to be perceived as rhythmic. A
particular metrical style, or single poem , or individual line, will make
use of only som e o f the possibilities provided for by the general set, and
the choice it m akes from the available options is a reflection o f its
metrical character. O ne o f the important tasks o f a metrical theory is to
make it possible to specify the relationship betw een the patterns and
rules o f a particular metrical form and the general rules o f the verse
tradition. W hen w e start to read a poem in an orthodox regular metre,
we very rapidly attune our reading to its metrical form; in other words,
the specific metrical set for that poem is quickly established. What
m akes this possible is the fact that it falls within the general set with
which we are already familiar, and it normally takes only a few lines to
determ ine the particular subset o f rules that is being used. A n analogy
would be the rapidity with which a piece o f music written according to
the conventions o f the W estern tonal system establishes in the mind o f
a listener familiar with those conventions a sense o f its key (which does
not entail the ability to name that key). But an atonal work will not
im m ediately signal a place in the key-system, nor will, say, purely
syllabic verse in English fall into place in the reader’s set o f
expectations.
Any metrically hom ogeneous body o f verse will have associated with
it a metrical set, whether we are talking o f a traditional form like the
iambic pentam eter, an author’s distinctive use o f that form, such as
M ilton’s iambic pentam eter, or the metrical style o f a particular poem ,
such as the iambic pentam eter o f Paradise Regained. The more
hom ogeneous the verse, the more precisely defined the metrical set
will be, excluding a greater number o f the options included in the
general set. The metrical set o f a poem can o f course be momentarily
challenged by an exceptional line, like a deliberate dissonance in
music, or can undergo transformation, like m odulation from one key
to another.
The general set for English verse, and the sets for individual m etres
and poem s, can be given a formal statem ent under four headings: the
underlying rh yth m , the metrical p a ttern , the realisation rules, and, in
som e cases, the con dition s. The first two have already been
METRICAL SET 155
betw een the beats and offbeats o f the metrical pattern on the one hand
and the stresses and nonstresses o f the line o f verse on the other, which
m eans that the rules o f m etre and the rules which govern the stress
contours o f English speech interlock, and other linguistic rules play a
part in this com plex w eb as well. H ow ever, the study o f m etre and the
study o f phonology can be kept separate without grave damage to the
former, because it is possible while discussing a metrical rule to rely on
the reader’s implicit knowledge o f the phonological structure o f his
own language; for instance, the assignment o f stresses and nonstresses
in a line o f verse need not be explicitly justified in terms o f
phonological rules if readers agree on their position in a normal
pronunciation. By making this separation the study o f metre can be
kept safely out o f the continuing debates in the field o f phonology, and
made accessible to those w hose main interest is in literature, not
linguistics. M oreover, we thereby avoid the possibility o f circularity
that creeps into som e accounts o f metre within a generative
framework, where interpretations o f metrical and phonological
phenom ena are used to justify each other. Our rules will therefore
m ake use o f nothing more com plex at the level of phonology than a
classification o f syllables as stressed or unstressed; with the implication
that in any given metrical exam ple, a syllable will function as either one
or the other, even if it occupies an interm ediate place in the stress
hierarchy o f the phrase or sentence. This simplifying assumption (which
has been a feature o f m ost traditional accounts o f m etre) will facilitate
the statem ent o f the metrical rules, and probably corresponds to an
actual process o f simplification which occurs in the perception of
regular rhythm. H ow ever, it leaves unsolved a number o f questions
about the relationship betw een metrical form and the m ovem ent o f
spoken English, som e o f which will be taken up in Chapter 8, where we
shall investigate the final level in the metrical edifice: the relationship
betw een the sim plified stress contour - w e can call this the stress
pattern - em ployed in the rules o f metre and the huge variety in sound
and m ovem ent that characterises the language we use.
N o tes
1. A clear statement of the interaction between linguistic material and abstract
patterns is given by Zirmunskij (1925, Ch. 1), though like many other writers on
verse form, he identifies the latter with metre and calls the result of the interaction
rhythm , thereby overlooking the important role of elementary rhythmic forms in
determining what patterns are possible.
NOTES 157
I11 See Erasmus (1528, pp. 110-11) and Ramus (1564, fol. 53v). These passages are
discussed in Attridge (1974, pp. 80-81).
I11 One possible approach would be the use of elicitation experiments to test readers’
metrical responses, though carefully designed and controlled experimental
procedures would be necessary, as is shown by Quirk & Svartvik (1966) and
Greenbaum & Quirk (1970).
I11 Within linguistic theory, the absolute dichotomy between competence and
performance has often been questioned, and the importance of the latter
emphasised: see, for instance, Fodor & Garrett (1966), Wales & Marshall (1966),
Fromkin (1968), Whitaker (1968), Bever (1970), Campbell & Wales (1970), R.
Fowler (1970), Turner (1970), Derwing (1973, Ch. 8), Lakoff (1973), and Kates
(1976). It has recently been argued that the temporal dimension, usually regarded
as an attribute o f performance only, should enter into phonological descriptions (see
Coates, 1980; and C. A. Fowler, 1980): the importance of this suggestion for
metrical theory is obvious.
Chapter 7
7.1 U N D E R L Y IN G R H Y T H M S A N D M E T R IC A L PA T T E R N S
The metrical set for any poem in regular verse includes an underlying
rhythm, usually four-beat or five-beat, and a metrical pattern which
em bodies that rhythm in lines or groups of lines. W e have already
discussed these features o f metrical form in som e detail, and the main
object o f our attention in this chapter is the way in which they are
related to the stress patterns of the language, but a resume may be
useful at this point.
The metrical pattern indicates the number and position of beats and
offbeats in the line of verse. We can specify a particular metrical
pattern as follows:
B o B o B o B
This represents four-beat verse with no initial or final offbeats (or a
single line in that m etre). Since beats are automatically separated by
offbeats, this metrical pattern can also be fully characterised as 4B.
Four-beat verse which regularly opens and closes with an offbeat can
be shown as:
0 B 0 B 0 B 0 B 0
UNDERLYING RHYTHMS AND METRICAL PATTERNS 159
B o b o B o b
are som etim es strong and som etim es weak. In such cases, we can
em ploy different types o f bracketing: the ordinary parentheses as
above when the expectation is neutral, an angled bracket to show that
the stronger expectation is for the presence of an offbeat, and a double
bracket for the reverse:
(o) B o . . . (o ) B o . . . ((o )) B o . . .
7.2 B A S E R U L E S A N D D O U B L E O FFB E A T S
A n underlying rhythm m anifested in a metrical pattern exerts a
simplifying pressure on the com plex stress contour o f the language, so
that the reader o f regular verse perceives not a multiply-gradated
hierarchy o f stresses, but a pattern made up only of two kinds of
syllable, relatively strong and relatively weak, or stressed and
unstressed (using these terms in a way not exactly consonant with their
use in phonology), which w e shall indicate as + s and - s respectively.
(The experience o f reading m ost verse is in fact som ewhat more
com plicated, but we shall postpone consideration o f these
com plications to Chapter 8.) The general set for English metre
includes all the possible ways in which the beats and offbeats o f a
metrical pattern can be realised by a simplified stress pattern o f this
kind, though a particular poem or metrical style may make use of only
a selection o f these realisation rules.
We have already discussed the naturalness with which the stressed
syllables o f English are perceived as rhythmic beats: they act as peaks
o f energy in the utterance, and have a tendency towards periodic
occurrence. The m ost fundamental metrical rule, therefore, is one
which em bodies this fact: it is the first o f the two base rules of English
metre, and we shall call it the beat ru le:
BASE RULES AND DOUBLE OFFBEATS 161
Beat rule
A stressed syllable m ay realise a beat
It must be rem em bered that a rule o f this kind is intended to reflect the
perceptual realities o f performance rather than the abstract relations
o f com petence: it states that a syllable which is perceived as stressed
may be perceived as the beat o f a metrical pattern, and hence o f an
underlying rhythm. It is important, too, to note that the rule specifies a
possible realisation, not an obligatory one; we shall see later that under
certain conditions stressed syllables can play a different role in the
metre. Realisation rules are therefore to be understood as optional: in
the reader’s attem pt (usually unconscious) to find a rhythmic
organisation in the line o f verse, he may make use o f any o f the rules in
the set for that metre; or, to put it another way, every realisation rule
has attached to it the im plied condition: ‘when by so doing the
appropriate metrical pattern is realised.’ Since the beat rule includes
no special environm ent in which this realisation occurs, no other
condition applies to it; in other words, any stressed syllable may realise
a beat if the metrical pattern requires it.
W e need a second base rule to state that the simplest realisation o f
an offbeat is a single unstressed syllable, reflecting the preference in
the language for alternations o f stress and nonstress, and the rhythmic
simplicity o f a single weak pulse betw een the peaks o f energy.
H ow ever, we have seen that freer metrical forms, especially those o f
popular verse, allow a single nonstress to be replaced by two
nonstresses, creating a double offbeat, and that because o f the
stress-timed character o f English, this does not disturb the underlying
rhythm to any great degree:
-s -s
(1) ‘O have they parishes burnt?’ he said
6
Rather than formulate two separate rules, it seem s truer to rhythmic
reality to include this option within the offbeat rule:
O ffbeat rule
7.3 P R O M O T IO N
The scansion here shows that by the application o f the base rules and
the prom otion rule, the stress pattern o f the line can realise a four-beat
metrical pattern; and that it can do so with only one application o f a
deviation rule (and a simple one at that) indicates that it is not a very
com plex line, rhythmically speaking - which is, o f course, consistent
166 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
with o n e’s response to it. W e should not, however, allow the scansion
to suggest that a beat is clearly perceived as falling on the middle o f the
three nonstresses; all it im plies is that this pattern of three nonstresses
is capable o f functioning as the rhythmic equivalent o f offbeat, beat,
offbeat, thanks to the expectations set up by the duple verse and the
alternating tendency in the language. It may be psychologically more
accurate in many cases to say that the beat is blurred rather than being
precisely located, or that the rhythm is suspended - but not
contradicted - over this stretch o f syllables. (A n equivalent in music
might be the perception o f a beat during a held note.) N otice,
incidentally, that the way a syllable is perceived depends not only on
what has gone before, but also on what is to com e; this will be puzzling
only if one believes that the spoken language is produced and
understood syllable by syllable, rather than, as is amply testified by
experim ental evidence, by m eans o f constant forward scanning.
A n unstressed syllable at the beginning o f a line is also very easily
prom oted to take a beat:
This is, o f course, m ost likely to happen when we expect a beat at the
start of the line; otherwise we will be inclined to treat the two
unstressed syllables as an initial double offbeat. To put it more
formally, the metrical pattern (which is part o f the reader’s metrical
set) will determ ine how two initial nonstresses are perceived. In a
five-beat line, with its relatively weaker metrical set, two such syllables
are most likely to receive a double-offbeat interpretation - which is a
further reason for the avoidance of trochaic metre in the pentam eter
noted in 5.2. Som etim es, prom otion in this position can be thought o f
as an exam ple o f the type already considered, with a line-juncture
intervening in the sequence of three nonstresses:
O nce again, there are instances where a run-on may make the
environm ent very similar to that o f the prom otion rule as already
stated:
(1 5 ) M e only cruel immortality
B o!
Consum es
o B
Here the double offbeat earlier in the line inclines us to treat ‘o f the’ as
a further double offbeat, an inclination strengthened by the verbal
similarity, and a degree o f special attention in reading is required to
bring out the third beat o f the line. The rhythm runs more sm oothly if
we om it the extra nonstress:
To conflate the three environm ents for prom otion within a single
rule as we have done is to imply that the generalisation captured in so
doing is a metrically significant one; that the three processes are in fact
168 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
versions o f the same process. It is not difficult to see the basis for this:
prom otion occurs when the metrical set encourages the perception o f a
nonstress as a beat, and this can only happen if there is no com peting
adjacent stress to attract that beat. In fact, if elegance were the prime
criterion, one could phrase the rule in those terms: an unstressed
syllable may realise a beat when it is not adjacent to a stress in the same
line.
7.4 D E M O T IO N
Som e further exam ples will suggest another elem entary rule in English
m etre, applicable to sequences of three stressed syllables:
(1 7 ) Then out bespoke the brown, brown bride
(1 8 ) Thy rapt soul sitting in thine eyes
(1 9 ) But where the ship’s huge shadow lay
These exam ples read easily as four-beat lines, even though ‘brown’ (on
both occurrences), ‘soul’, and ‘huge’ receive full stresses. To utter the
second ‘brown’ with any less force than the first would be to go against
the normal pronunciation o f English, and the equivalent words in both
the other lines dem and a strong semantic emphasis. Y et there is no
sense o f strain when we encounter lines like these in four-beat verse, as
we frequently do. W e can call the rule which operates here the
dem otion ru le, and a tentative formulation would state that a stressed
syllable may realise an offbeat when it occurs betw een two stressed
syllables. Again, metrical practice is a reflection o f a tendency we have
already noted in English speech rhythms (see pp. 7 0 -1 above): in a
sequence o f three stressed syllables, the middle one is often perceived
as playing a subsidiary rhythmic role. (A n analysis of this pattern in
classical terms, incidentally, could only be m ade in terms o f ‘spondaic
substitution’, which would obscure the part played by the sequence o f
three stresses.)
This rule is the converse o f the previous one, and the two could be
com bined by means o f a simple formal convention. To do this would be
to claim that they have the same rhythmic origin, and one might justify
this by arguing that they are both a product o f the tendency to perceive
an alternating rhythm in the language: where there are three syllables
o f equal weight, the middle one will function as a rhythmic contrast to
its partners. Sequences o f syllables do occur in which it is im possible to
DEMOTION 169
make a choice betw een dem otion o f one or prom otion o f another (see
Ch. 8, exam ple (4 )), and in such cases our conflated rule would be
useful; but for the m ost part the generalisation it captures holds only at
an abstract level, and is not a matter o f similar physical or
psychological processes. This will becom e clearer if we exam ine the
other environm ents in which dem otion can occur. O f the following two
lines, only the first is metrically acceptable as a four-beat line:
(2 0 ) Full fathom five thy father lies
[20a] Fathom s five thy father lies drowned
W hereas prom otion can occur at the beginning o f the line and at the
end, dem otion is possible only at the beginning. It cannot be said that a
stress may realise an offbeat when it is not adjacent to a nonstress, and
the dem otion rule is therefore not a strict mirror image o f the
prom otion rule.
The full statem ent o f the dem otion rule, then, includes the
line-initial environm ent, as exem plified by (20):
D em otion rule
A stressed syllable m ay realise an offbeat when it occurs between
tw o stressed syllables, o r after a line-boundary and before a stressed
syllable
The implication o f the ordering o f options is that line-initial dem otion
is the more com plex realisation, which seem s generally true. In
scansion we can use a dot above the offbeat to indicate dem otion:
-s +s -s +s -s +s +s +s
(1 7) Then out bespoke the brown, brown bride
o B o B o B o B
+s +s -s +s - s +s -s +s
(2 0 ) Full fathom five thy father lies
o B o B o B o B
O f the two phenom ena, dem otion is som ewhat more disruptive o f the
steady m ovem ent o f the rhythm, which is why we have treated it as a
later rule in the general set. H ow ever, the precise effect o f these
deviations on the rhythm depends on the actual words being used, and
we shall take up this question in 8.5.
To a certain extent, these effects o f speed and slowness can be
achieved in free verse, since they spring from the nature o f English
speech rhythm. In a line o f metrical verse, however, som ething else
happens: the underlying rhythm itself m oves more quickly or slowly.
The acceleration we experience when a beat is realised by a prom oted
172 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
nonstress is not just a quickening in the rate at which the syllables are
pronounced; it is a quickening in the underlying rhythm, which m oves
faster when a beat is realised with so slight an expenditure o f energy.
And our sense o f a ritardando when dem otion occurs com es in part
from the mom entary holding up o f the underlying rhythm. When we
talk o f the tension created by such variations, then, it is not so much a
tension betw een the metrical pattern and the actual syllables, as our
scansion rather misleadingly suggests, but the result o f a regular
rhythmic m ovem ent being slow ed down and speeded up, or, to use a
spatial metaphor, stretched and com pressed. The alternative view,
that the underlying regular metre ticks away independently o f the
actual line, and that the tension we experience arises from the distance
betw een the two, is not very different from the inadequate notion o f
‘counterpoint’, and is unconvincing as a psychological m odel. Though
it remains true that the easiest way to talk about rhythmic tension is in
terms o f the relationship betw een an underlying metre and a verbal
realisation, one should be fully aware that this does not imply two
independent psychological levels, but a single com plex experience.
7.5 IM PL IE D O F F B E A T S
W e have seen (3 .4 ) that speakers o f English instinctively avoid
sequences o f two stresses when alternative pronunciations or
phrasings are possible, unless there is a third stress to allow dem otion.
Nevertheless, opportunities for evasion are limited, and pairs o f
stresses remain a com m on feature o f English speech. A ny verse which
relies on the incorporation o f speech rhythms, therefore, needs a way
of accommodating this pattern, even though it challenges the
alternating rhythms both o f the language and o f the m etre. The natural
tendency in a careful reading o f such a sequence is to let each syllable
function as a beat, allowing the stress-timed rhythm o f English to
interpose an offbeat. Where stress-timing is reinforced by an insistent
underlying rhythm, as in nursery rhymes, this happens frequently and
easily, the offbeats being literally realised as pauses (m ost naturally by
prolonging the first stressed syllable o f the pair). W e have already
introduced a deviation sym bol to indicate such im plied offbeats:
(3 0 ) Four-and-twenty blackbirds
B o B o B 6 B
More com m only, however, implied offbeats are used in the literary
tradition to create a minor disruption in the rhythm, an eddy in the
sm ooth flow o f the verse:
A n initial stress before another stress is, as the ordering o f the rules
suggests, usually dem oted.
It is important to note that the operation o f the implied offbeat rule
can overlap with that o f the dem otion rule, producing lines like the
following:
+s +s +s +s
(3 9 ) I have a faint cold fear thrills through my veins
B o B o B o B
7.6 P A IR IN G C O N D IT IO N S A N D SY L L A B IC R H Y T H M
We have now established five realisation rules - two base rules and
three deviation rules - which will account for all the normal metrical
variations in duple m etre. Verse which observes these rules, and no
other metrical restraints, will not be characterised by a fixed syllable
count, since they permit the free occurrence o f im plied or double
offbeats in place o f single offbeats. Such verse, som etim es called strong-
stress verse, is characteristic o f the popular tradition, and springs
im m ediately from the m ost predominant feature of English speech
rhythm, its stress-timing. But a large proportion o f literary verse,
traditionally, and accurately, called accentual-syllabic verse, observes a
strict control over syllables, and allows im plied and double offbeats
only under special circumstances. We can capture these in the metrical
set by the form ulation o f conditions. The resulting framework o f
176 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
but here the musical setting would normally allow the last two syllables
to receive equal weight ( ‘fair la-dee’). Similar endings in Chaucer’s
verse usually provoke speculation about changes in pronunciation;
whether or not these are justified, such a response does indicate how
ingrained the prohibition is in our sense o f the iambic pentam eter. It
may in fact be derived from more general rhythmic principles; Baum
(19 5 2 , pp. 9 6 -7 ) found in a sample o f 3,400 clause and sentence
endings in written English prose that the pattern + s - s + s was more
than four times as com m on as + s + s - s .
N ot all verse which limits the use o f implied offbeats in this way
exerts similar control over double offbeats; Browning’s pentam eter
lines, for exam ple, make frequent use o f double offbeats without
adjacent im plied offbeats, but the latter seldom occur without the
former (see Hatcher, 1928, Ch. 6 and 9). In the strictest form of duple
metre, however, double offbeats also occur only in pairing formations.
178 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
Here the stress on ‘world’ is part o f the environm ent for im plied
offbeats on either side, which is allowable, and the pairing conditions
are fully observed in the two double offbeats. A pairing formation can
also overlap with dem otion or prom otion, though careful handling of
the syntax is required to save the reader from getting lost in the four
successive stresses or nonstresses which this produces; (3 9 ) is an
exam ple o f the former, and the following line shows the latter:
The bulk o f English literary verse maintains control over the number
o f syllables in the line by observing both pairing conditions, though
there is more than one way o f interpreting this fact. Som etim es it is
seen primarily as a conventional feature o f English metre, an
im position on native accentually-based rhythms o f the external
principle o f a fixed syllable count. It is certainly true that historically
the triumphs o f this principle reflect foreign influence: the poets who
have, at various stages in the history o f accentual verse, influentially
tightened the restrictions on the syllable count have usually been well
acquainted with French or Italian m odels, whether we think o f
Chaucer in the fourteenth century, W yatt and Surrey in the sixteenth,
or W aller and D ryden in the seventeenth. In eighteenth-century
England, as Culler (1 9 4 8 ) and Fussell (1 9 5 4 ) have shown, the prestige
o f French verse was such that English poetry with any pretensions had
to imitate its syllabic strictness, and influential handbooks like
B ysshe’s A r t o f English P oetry (1 7 0 2 ) enshrined and propagated the
syllabic principle. (O n the other hand, poets who have taken English
verse in the other direction, like Coleridge and Hopkins, have usually
appealed to native precedent.) Syllable counting as a metrical principle
may som etim es have w on favour for a different kind o f external
reason: it is more am enable to the formulation o f metrical rules than
patterning o f stresses, and therefore has a particular appeal to the
technically self-conscious artist. Thus Elizabethan attempts to describe
English metre frequently ignore aural rhythms and concentrate on the
visually more obvious properties o f syllable count and rhyme (see
Attridge, 1974, Ch. 7). The very nam es ‘fourteener’ and ‘poulter’s
m easure’ testify to a conception o f metre rooted in the counting o f
syllables; and Peter Q uince’s name for the ballad stanza, we may
recall, is ‘eight and six’.
180 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
A nd its use in the literary tradition is not confined to verse which fixes
the number o f syllables in the line: M ilton em ploys it, for exam ple, in
V A lle g ro , which has lines o f seven, eight, and nine syllables:
from this feature o f the language; the opening o f the iambic line, in
particular, encourages such alternatives, since inversion in this
position does not produce the disruption o f an im plied offbeat:
This is not to say that in m ost exam ples o f pairing we experience any
inclination to reverse the stresses in order to produce sm ooth
alternation; but it may be that our familiarity with reversible forms
enters our experience in som e way, perhaps by a slight blurring o f the
rhythm betw een the adjacent nonstress and stress, especially when
there is no syntactic break betw een them. N abokov (1 9 6 4 , p. 20) is
referring to this feature o f pairing when he says that its beauty lies in ‘a
certain teasing quality of rhythm . . . [which] ow es its subtle magic to
the balance it tends to achieve betw een yielding and not yielding -
yielding to the metre and still preserving its accentual voice’. Other
metrists have called it a ‘hovering accent’, and it is presumably what
lies behind the assertion by Jespersen (1 9 0 0 , p. 125), W inters (1 9 5 7 ,
p. 94), and W imsatt (1970, p. 775) that there is a steady increase in the
stresses o f sequences like:
-s -s +s +s
(6 0 ) With a huge em pty flagon by his side
o B oB
+s -s -s+ s
(6 3 ) The rustic youth, brown with meridian toil
B o B o B
five beats are perceptible; but the way the line totters on the edge o f
unmetricality shows the importance o f the im m ediate recovery o f the
rhythm in the normal pairing formations. W e can call this variation
postp o n ed p a irin g , and a metrical style which em ploys it as a regular
feature can be indicated by omitting the qualification ‘im m ediately’ in
the statem ent o f the pairing conditions. A ny metrical theory which
regards the syllables o f the line as filling a series o f abstract positions is
bound to rule postponed pairing out o f court, since it appears to
produce a com plete mismatch o f metrical positions and stress contour.
The line by Keats which H alle and Keyser regard as unmetrical (see
above, p. 42), is a straightforward exam ple o f postponed pairing:
The first tremor in the rhythm occurs with the fem inine ending o f the
first line, creating a double offbeat over the line juncture. The effect is
then repeated much more disturbingly within the third line, where the
syntactic pause in the double offbeat increases its rhythmic
disruptiveness and momentarily suspends the rhythm; this gives the
repeated ‘aweary’ an em otionally charged em phasis, as the m ovem ent
o f the line drags against the underlying rhythm, to fall back into the
heavy, inescapable alternation o f the final statem ent. O ne only has to
substitute m onosyllables for ‘dreary’ and the second ‘aweary’ - say
‘bad’ and ‘sad’ - to realise how much expressive force the refrain would
lose if it maintained the strict syllabic form o f the stanza.
It may be thought that verse which exerts control over the syllable
count by means o f the pairing conditions is more distant from the
m ovem ent o f speech than verse based entirely on stress-rhythms, but
this is not so; in being less dom inated by the stress-tim ed rhythm o f the
underlying beats, syllabically strict verse can be more subtle in its
reflection o f speech, as the vivid imitation o f spoken language in
accentual-syllabic verse by writers like Shakespeare and Browning
testifies. W hereas strong-stress verse observes only the stress principle
o f English rhythm, syllables only the syllabic principle, and free verse,
while using both stress and syllabic rhythms, turns its back on
elem entary rhythmic forms, accentual-syllabic verse exploits to the full
all three o f these rhythmic resources.
7.7 IA M BIC V E R SE
A s we have observed, the beginning and end o f the line are points of
IAMBIC VERSE 187
relative metrical freedom: the metrical pattern may or may not have
offbeats in these positions, or may have optional offbeats which permit
variation from line to line. H ow ever, in verse which observes a strict
control o f syllables, this freedom at line-opening and line-end is
necessarily curtailed. It is by means o f different kinds o f restriction at
these points that the traditional categories o f duple verse, iam bic and
trochaic, are distinguished; and when I use these terms I am referring
specifically to this distinction. It should be rem em bered that the
distinction betw een rising and falling rhythms is a separate one, even
though such rhythms are influenced by the way lines begin and end
(see 4.6).
The following are types o f opening that occur frequently in
traditional iambic verse:
-s +s -s
(7 0 ) With loss o f E den, till one greater man
o B o
-s -s -s
(7 1 ) In the beginning how the heav’ns and earth
o B o
+s +s -s
(7 2 ) Hurled headlong flaming from the ethereal sky
o B o
-s -s +s +s
(7 3 ) Nor the deep tract o f hell, say first what cause
o B o B
+s -s -s +s
(7 4 ) R egions o f sorrow, doleful shades, where peace
B o B
+s +s -s -s
[78] Hurled headlong aflame from skies above
B o B o
+s +s +s
[79] Hurled G od flaming from the ethereal sky
B o B
or o B o B
188 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
pentam eter, lines like [76] are in fact accepted; the following exam ples
are by Chaucer, Surrey, Shakespeare,3 Keats, and Browning
respectively:
(8 0 ) Twenty bookes, clad in blak or reed
(81) Norfolk sprang thee, Lam beth holds thee dead
(8 2 ) Stay, the king hath thrown his warder down
(8 3 ) Thea, Thea, Thea, where is Saturn?
(8 4 ) Steadied him a m om ent, set him straight
To account for stricter forms o f iambic pentam eter, however, which
constitute the bulk o f writing in that metre, we need a condition which
will exclude lines like these, but accept the om ission o f the initial
offbeat in lines like (74):
The same double offbeat cannot be used to satisfy both the initial
inversion condition and the implied offbeat condition.
Pentam eter verse which permits headless lines like (8 0 )-(8 4 ) does
not, o f course, consistently observe the initial inversion condition,
since in such lines an initial beat is not always follow ed by a double
offbeat. H ow ever, verse o f this kind usually m akes use o f lines with
initial inversions as w ell, so the second part o f the double offbeat
condition will remain valid, though we have to understand the phrase
‘in observance o f the initial inversion condition’ as applying to the
individual line, which has, as it were, chosen to abide by this restriction
when som e of its fellows have not. (If, however, double offbeats are
used freely throughout the line, the double offbeat condition does not
apply at all.) The same freedom to omit an optional initial offbeat with
or without a following double offbeat occurs in som e four-beat
metrical styles; the following lines are characteristic of the verse of
M ilton’s L ’A llegro a n d // P en seroso , which, with a metrical pattern of
(o )4 B (o ), observes the full double offbeat condition consistently but
the initial inversion condition only intermittently:
(86) A nd ever against eating cares,
o B o B oB o B
+s -s-s +S +S-S-S +s
A ll these lines except the first om it the optional opening offbeat. But
lines 2 and 4 do so with, and line 3 without, a following double offbeat.
(Line 2 continues with stress-initial pairing, giving the line a highly apt
lilt, w hose symmetry is revealed in the stress pattern.) Since the
experienced reader takes in the lines of this verse form at once, rather
than syllable by syllable, the variability of its openings causes few
difficulties in performance; but it has acquired the reputation o f a
highly problematic variety o f metre because o f its unsusceptibility to
scansion in terms o f classical feet (see the survey and discussion by
W eismiller, 1972, pp. 1 0 2 9 -3 3 ).
IAMBIC VERSE 191
-s+ s +s -s
. . . believe . . . happen
o B B o
+s -s -s -s -s-s +s
. . . happiness . . . to disappear
B o
+ s-s -s-s
.. .m elancholy
Bo Bo
+s -s -s +s
(9 0 ) . . . voice o f the shade
B o B
-s -s +s +s
(9 1 ) . . . in a green shade
o B o B
TROCHAIC VERSE 193
It is surely no accident that the rules and conditions o f the most widely
used and versatile English metre have developed in such a way as to
provide the poet with a firm rhythmic structure which at the same time
allows him the greatest possible freedom to use the verbal resources o f
the English language. A further consequence o f the rules that govern
the opening and closing o f the iambic pentam eter is that they make the
establishm ent o f a strong falling rhythm unlikely: the line cannot begin
with the pattern + s - s + s, except in the rare case o f postponed
com pensation, and it m ost com m only ends on a stress. This hostility
towards the more insistent o f the two types o f duple m ovem ent is o f a
piece with many o f the other features o f the iambic pentam eter, such as
its avoidance o f the dominating four-beat rhythm, its use o f duple
metre rather than the more insistent triple m etre, and its dependence
on a fixed syllable count and pairing formations to keep the powerful
stress-timing principle in check. A ll these features prevent the strong,
sim ple, fundamental rhythmic forms from taking precedence over the
more variable patterns o f living speech, and go a long way towards
explaining the remarkable predom inance, and repeated reinvigora-
tion, o f this metrical form in the literary tradition from Chaucer to the
present.
7.8 T R O C H A IC V E R SE
offbeat (the other being an exam ple of dem otion, which Kiparsky’s
foot-analysis does not keep distinct):
+S +S -S -S
(92) Catch the wild goat by the hair, and hurl their lances
B 6 B o
in the sun
A nd there is only one exam ple o f an ‘iambic foot’, that is, the pattern
- s - s + s +s:
-s -s +s +s
(93) Summer isles o f Eden lying in dark-purple spheres of sea
o B o B
E ven in these exam ples, the strong metrical set encourages the reader
to give extra weight to the m onosyllables ‘by’ and ‘in’ to reduce the
rhythmic dislocation.
The initial inversion condition as we have formulated it is o f course
not relevant to trochaic line-openings, but the equivalent phenom enon
does occasionally occur, producing an im plied offbeat without a
balancing double offbeat:
+s -s +s
(1 0 0 ) Only joy, now here you are
B o B
-s -s +s
(1 0 1 ) In a cow slip’s bell I lie
B o B
+s +s +s
(1 0 2 ) W rote one song - and in my brain I sing it
B o B
O f these, the third is rare, no doubt because the line has not had a
chance to establish its strong alternating rhythm. The constraints on
the poet in terms o f verbal choice are therefore much greater than is the
case in iambic or free duple verse, and the rhythmic experience o f the
reader more likely to approach m onotony. O nce again, the metrical
choices o f English poets are vindicated.
TRIPLE VERSE 197
H ow relevant are the other rules that were proposed to deal with
duple verse? The first base rule, allowing a stress to realise a beat, is o f
course fundamental to all English metre. The prom otion rule is
som etim es used in triple verse; the following lines, for instance, may be
read without any special emphasis on ‘didst’ once the strong triple
rhythm o f the poem has been established:
In both, the triple metre is insistent enough to take the single offbeat in
its stride, but there is a degree o f temporal equivalence betw een a
stress and two nonstresses that m akes the first line run m ore sm oothly.
The syllabic m ovem ent slows down, but the rhythmic pace from beat to
beat remains steady. This principle underlies strict accentual
imitations o f the Latin hexameter:
It is also possible, w hen the triple set is very strong, for both syllables in
the offbeat to be dem oted, though this is rare, and creates considerable
tension - whether one chooses to squeeze the words into the rhythmic
pattern, or to disrupt the regular metre:
Browning uses all but the most deviant o f the possible options, and a
rewriting o f the last line will show that even that option - two
dem otions in a double offbeat - can be accom m odated if the same
emphasis is given to the word ‘ey e’:
it is one o f the least com plex kinds o f dem otion; then we have —s + s
and + s - s , the first o f which is less com plex than the second, partly
because a stress is more often subordinated to a follow ing than a
preceding stress (figures cited by Tarlinskaja, 1976, pp. 1 3 2 -3 ,
support this ordering); and the double dem otion o f two stresses, as the
most disruptive, is placed last. A ll o f these can occur in two
environm ents, betw een two stresses and at the beginning o f a line
before a stress, o f which the former is given first in the rule as less
com plex. Stricter forms o f triple metre, of course, make use of a
dem otion rule which excludes one or more o f the later options given
here. H ow ever, so many other factors are at work, including the exact
nature o f the metrical set and the m orphological and syntactic
structures o f the actual words, that this ordering cannot be regarded as
absolute.
A s in duple verse, the rule does not allow for dem otion at the end of
the line, and rewritings o f triple lines suggest that this is correct:
+s +s
[119a] Touch her with soft hands
+s -s +s
[119b] Touch her with tender hands
+s +s -s
[119c] Touch her with soft fingers
The extra stresses here disrupt the triple m ovem ent entirely, and the
only environm ent in which they might not do so is in strongly run-on
lines, when the line-boundary would, in effect, disappear. The
environm ent for the operation o f the dem otion rule, therefore, is
exactly the same for triple metre as it is for duple.
There remains one deviation rule to consider: the im plied offbeat
rule. N ot surprisingly, this is rarely made use o f in triple verse: we are
less likely to feel that a pause enforced by successive stresses is doing
duty for a double offbeat than for a single offbeat. It can only occur
without distorting the rhythm when a powerfully dominating metrical
set has been established, producing very marked beats, as is the case in
popular rhymes:
TRIPLE VERSE 203
( 121) They, with the gold to give, doled him out silver
B ô B ô* B ô B o
Because o f the natural division o f the four-beat line into two tw o-beat
units, and the syntactic break at this point, it is possible to read [121a]
naturally and maintain the underlying rhythm; it does, however, have
the effect o f encouraging a chanted reading, and this would be the only
way o f preserving the rhythm if it occurred in a less favourable
environm ent. It is probably m ost accurate to regard its occasional use
as part o f the freedom allowed to a certain type o f highly rhythmical
verse, in which offbeats can be realised by two, one, or no syllables, and
to state as a general rule that strict triple verse does not make use o f
implied offbeats. The pairing conditions also have no place in triple
metre, o f course; their w hole raison d'être is the pattern o f alternation
betw een single beat and single offbeat.
Traditional prosody attempts to distinguish betw een ‘dactylic’ and
‘anapaestic’ m etres, the distinction once again hinging on the expected
patterns at line-opening and line-end: fully dactylic verse would have a
metrical pattern that begins with a beat and ends with a double offbeat,
and anapaestic verse the opposite. H ow ever, many other com binations
occur without being felt to be oddities; in fact, Tarlinskaja finds in her
sampling o f triple verse that the majority o f lines begin in neither o f
these ways, but with a single offbeat (1 9 7 6 , p. 272, Table 35). It is as
well to refrain from using terms which suggest that two o f the many
possibilities have som e kind o f canonic status simply because they can
be divided into ‘feet’. The consistent use o f different realisations in
different parts o f the line can easily be reflected in the rules; one
com m on form o f triple verse, for instance, w ould be represented by the
follow ing version o f the offbeat rule:
variant of another (the dem otion rule), and lacks the fifth; and it
observes none o f the duple verse conditions. The distinction is not
absolute, how ever, and the rules reflect this by making it possible to
derive lines in either duple or triple m etre, or from anywhere on the
spectrum betw een them, from exactly the same metrical patterns.
To an even greater degree than is the case with duple metre, the
rules for triple metre fall short of providing a machine for processing
strings o f syllables to produce metrical judgem ents. The freedom
im plied by the num erous options o f the dem otion rule is to som e extent
illusory, because the poet has to handle his linguistic material with
great care to m ake sure that his use o f these deviations is not disruptive
o f the underlying rhythm. The rules for the freest triple metre state, in
effect, that betw een two stresses any one or two syllables can realise an
offbeat, whether - s, + s, - s —s, - s + s, + s - s, or + s + s, but there is an
important implicit qualification: ‘provided that a strong enough set for
triple verse is established and m aintained’. A nd this means, ‘provided
that certain limits are observed on the use o f the later deviations, and
o f linguistic structures which pull away from the metrical forms which
they em body.’ Perhaps a much more elaborate metrical theory,
married to a detailed phonological theory, would be able to specify
those limits explicitly. For the time being we have to fall back on the
ear to tell us when the poet has overstepped the borders o f metricality -
or, rather, has begun to travel in the no-m an’s-land around its edges -
by mismatching the rhythms generated by m orphology and syntax with
the rhythms created by the metre, or by making too free with the
deviations allow ed by the rules. W hen we do sense this, however, our
set o f rules ought to be able to tell us exactly where and how it has
happened; where the strain is being felt, why the joints are coming
apart. This is why it is important to be as clear as possible about the
different rhythmic processes involved, and the differing degrees o f
com plexity they create. It is important, too, to evolve a m ethod of
metrical analysis which will make it possible to exam ine the
relationship betw een metrical and linguistic structures, which is the
subject o f the next chapter. We shall find there that this relationship is
com plicated enough in duple verse, and no attem pt will be made to
grapple with the problems posed by triple verse.
COMPLEXITY AND TENSION 205
7. 10 C O M PL E X IT Y A N D T E N SIO N
The metrical style o f a body o f regular English verse, whether it be that
o f a period, a poet, a section o f a p oet’s oeuvre, or a single poem , can be
characterised by a selection from the general set o f rules and
conditions, and any idiosyncracies can be indicated within the
framework provided by that set. The smaller and more hom ogeneous
the body o f verse, the more detailed the specification o f its metrical
characteristics can be, right down to the single line, in which it is
possible to say exactly what rules are being used and what conditions
being observed at every point. A lthough the characteristics o f metrical
styles vary in several ways, one general feature has been implied in our
discussion so far: the variation from simple to com plex. By a simple
metrical style we m ean one in which the selection o f deviation rules
produces a highly regular rhythmic alternation, and by a com plex style
one in which regular alternation is frequently challenged. A nd we have
ordered our rules so that, by and large, the more often a later rule is
used the more com plex the metrical style. Thus we might say that
D ryden’s metre is relatively simple, since he strictly limits his use o f the
later deviation rules, while M ilton’s is com plex, because he freely takes
advantage o f them . Similarly, an individual line can be classed roughly
on a scale o f com plexity according to the deviation rules it makes use
of. The following lines from Shakespeare’s Sonnets are in an
approximately increasing order o f com plexity, and will also serve to
illustrate som e com binations o f the rules we have been considering:
P rom otion
-s +s - s -s -s +s -s + s —s +s
( 124) A nd often is his gold com plexion dimmed
o B o B o B o B o B
206 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
D em otion (m id-line)
-s +s -s +s -s +s +s +s -s +s
(125) Nor Mars his sword, nor war’s quick fireshall burn
0 B 0 B o B 0 B 0 B
D em otion (initial)
+s +s -s +s -s +s - s +s -s +s
(1 2 6 ) Rough winds do shake the darling buds o f May
o B o B o B o B o B
The final exam ple is probably at the limit o f com plexity tolerated in
this particular metrical style.
The role o f conditions also needs to be taken into account in
considering com plexity. To im pose a condition on a rule in a particular
metrical style is to allow its operation only in circumstances which
minimise the com plexity o f the deviation it permits. Thus none o f the
Shakespeare lines quoted contains an implied offbeat without an
accompanying double offbeat; such a deviation would produce a line
too com plex to be accepted in this style. Conditions other than the ones
formulated in this chapter may operate in a particular poem; for
instance the implied offbeat rule may have an attached condition
prohibiting its occurrence im m ediately after the first beat (see above,
pp. 1 7 4 -5 ). A metrical set which includes conditions on the rules
will therefore usually represent a simpler metre than one which does
not, unless the poet, or literary convention, has im posed an
COMPLEXITY AND TENSION 207
Such effects are particularly strong in dipodic verse: see the exam ple
by Kipling quoted above (4 .7 , (6 6 )). In the pentam eter, however,
deviations almost always increase tension, which is another source o f
the subtlety o f which this metrical form is capable. W hen it com es to
line-junctures, on the other hand, the tension created by run-ons in
four-beat verse is, as we have noted, greater than that in five-beat
verse, because the metrical break being challenged by the syntax is
more marked.
Tension o f a different kind can inhere at the level o f the larger
rhythmic grouping as w ell, notably the line. The placing o f syntactic
breaks, for instance, may not only affect the level o f tension at.
particular points, but determ ine the rhythmic quality o f the line as a
w hole. W e have seen already (5.5) how a pause after the second beat of
the pentam eter tends to create a line with a clear rhythmic balance,
which we can indicate as 2:3; a pause after the third beat, 3:2, though
less conducive to a strong rhythmic pattern, also divides the line into
relatively evenly balanced units. Pope states it as a rule (which he does
not always observe) that the pause in the pentam eter occurs after the
fourth, fifth, or sixth syllable (see A dler, 1964, pp. 5 -6 ), and Johnson
asserts that ‘the noblest and m ost m ajestic pauses which our
versification admits, are upon the fourth and sixth syllables’ {The
R a m b ler, N o. 90). A pause which isolates the first and last beat in a 1:4
or 4:1 division, on the other hand, creates a unit with a less simple
rhythmic organisation and less fluent m ovem ent. If such a line occurs
in verse which by a preponderance o f the other type of line has
established a metrical set incorporating the m ore even division as a
preference, it will produce an increase in tension.
It is interesting to consider the role of the pairing formations, and the
rhythmic patterns they bestow on the line, in this context. That
diabolus in p ro so d ia , the ‘second-foot inversion’, which is the earliest
possible occurrence o f stress-initial pairing in an iambic line, produces
an opening o f - s + s + s —s - s + s . . . , and within this, the sequence + s
- s - s + s tends to form a separate unit, thereby creating a hiatus after
the first beat, and an unbalanced 1:4 line. Stress-final pairing, on the
other hand, is frequently em ployed to open the line with - s - s + s
+ s . . . , since this, as a self-sufficient rhythmic grouping, divides the line
2:3. Turning to the end o f the line, we find a similar situation. W hile
the prohibited ‘final inversion’, s + s + s —s, tends to isolate the
final beat o f the line, a stress-final pairing, . . . —s —s + s + s , produces a
more satisfying 3:2 division. Statistics confirm these preferences:
210 THE RULES OF ENGLISH METRE
A lthough widely accepted in pentam eter verse, fem inine endings are
uncom m on in P ope’s pentam eters, and if the metrical set is well
established, the follow ing will be experienced as an increase in tension:
This line fails to observe the iambic inversion condition to which Pope
strictly adheres; it drops the initial offbeat without a following double
offbeat. Such a line would sound less odd am ong Chaucer’s iambic
pentam eters, because o f his acceptance o f ‘headless’ lines; in P ope’s
verse it strongly increases the tension, though it is no more com plex
than the original.
What about the follow ing line?
The scansion shows the only possible metrical pattern that can be
realised by this line: a four-beat rhythm with one double offbeat. In
som e free metrical styles, a four-beat line among five-beat lines could
be accepted as a deliberate variation from the prevailing metre; in
Pope’s verse it is unthinkable. Nor is it necessary to drop a syllable
from the line to create this degree o f irregularity: the following line has
ten syllables, but also allows only four beats, and, what is worse, begins
to slip into a triple rhythm com pletely at odds with the pentam eter’s
duple alternations:
N otes
1. In deriving these comparative statistics from Tarlinskaja’s tables, one has to ignore
the column relating to ‘inversion within the first foot’, which does not produce a
pairing formation (see section 7 of this chapter). It is partly this treatment of initial
inversion and stress-initial pairing as a single phenomenon (‘trochaic substitution’)
which has led to the widespread underestimation of the relative frequency and
importance of stress-final pairing in English verse.
2. For a discussion of the frequent occurrence of this pattern in English verse, see
Scripture (1928).
3. Groves (1979, Appendix) cites 80 examples o f such ‘headless’ lines from
Shakespeare’s plays.
Chapter 8
-s -s +s +s -s +s -s +s -s +s
And a bold, artful, surly, savage race
0 B 0 B 0 B 0 B 0 B
+s +s +s +s -s +s -s +s +s +s
O ne rose, my rose, that sw eetened all mine air
o B o B o y ^ B o B o B
s +s s +s -s +s -s +s s +s
One rose, my rose, that sw eetened all mine air.
o B o B o B o B o B
s
Which makes it fatal to be loved? A h why
o B 0 B 0 B 0 B o B
s
With cypress branches hast thou wreathed thy bowers,
o B o B o B o B o B o
s
And made thy best interpreter a sigh?
o B o B o B o B o B
In this exam ple, the indefinite stresses can be given som e weight by the
reader, or left as unstressed and allowed to function as beats through
prom otion, all the em phasis then falling on the other stresses. To give a
full scansion of these lines, it would be necessary only to add two more
indefinite stresses in offbeat position on ‘O h’ and ‘A h ’; everything else
in the stress pattern is already im plied by the metrical pattern and
deviation symbols.
INDEFINITE STRESS 219
o B B o
and becom ing, verbs which are subordinate parts of larger verbal units,
and, in general, prepositions, conjunctions, and interjections. But it is
im possible to reduce the occurrence o f indefinite stress to rules, since it
depends so much on the specific linguistic context in which the word
appears. The same is true o f indefinite stress in polysyllables, though
the situation here is a little clearer, since there are two distinct types
o f polysyllable involved. The first is the polysyllabic minor-category
word, in which the main stress can be weakened:
s
(5) A nd few, amid the rural tribe, have time
B
s
(6) Creature so fair his reconcilem ent seeking
B
stress contour o f the sentence coincides with the metrical set, such
variable stresses are given a special character: the two perceptual
schem ata reinforce one another, so that a syllable whose level o f stress
is undeterm ined by either is given a kind o f freedom unknown outside
the structures o f regular verse. Its rhythmic status is doubly
guaranteed, so to speak, and its actual pronunciation in a given
performance can be determ ined entirely by the reader’s sense of
sem antic and aesthetic needs.
In som e contexts, a stress that might be variable in prose is forced to
function in only one way by the metre. Thus if we rewrite the second
line o f (2) as follow s, the metre induces a pronunciation o f ‘my’
without a stress:
+s -s -s +s
[2a] G ive me my rose, that sw eetens all the air
B o B
The syllables on either side o f a prom oted or dem oted syllable also
allow o f no variation; the m ost natural reading o f the following line
gives no stress to ‘m e’:
-s -s -s
(7) Or that your prowess can me yield relief
o B o
If one allows ‘upon’ any stress in its normal place, instead o f throwing it
all forward to ‘that’, one is likely to end up thinking, like B ooth (1 9 7 7 ,
p. 29 3 ), that the line is prose; what is true is that the sim ultaneous
prom otion o f the first syllable o f ‘upon’ and the denial o f stress to the
second creates a significant degree o f tension (which an ingenious critic
might regard as em blem atic o f the self-deprecation being expressed).
W hen a minor category disyllable o f this kind is used in a double
offbeat, however, there is no rhythmic pressure on the first syllable,
and the word functions without difficulty as two nonstresses:
+s +s -s -s
(9) Constant, mature, proof against all assaults
B o B o
222 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
-s -s -s -s
Ease after war, death after life does greatly please
B o B o B o B o B o B
o B o B
-s-s s +s
(13) Pride, M alice, Folly against Dryden rose
o B o B
o B o B
The metrical structure, however, does not dem and a nonstress: if there
were sem antic reasons for giving som e degree o f stress to ‘I’ no
dislocation would follow , only a slowing down o f the m ovem ent. If this
were the case the scansion would indicate an indefinite stress; but since
the variability is only a theoretical possibility and is not likely to enter
into a reading, we show the syllable as unstressed. In the following
exam ple, the pattern is reversed, and the pronoun occurs betw een two
nonstresses, again allowing variability o f stress. But the sense suggests
an unstressed pronunciation, realising a beat through promotion:
-s -s -s
(1 6 ) So old the place was, I rem em bered none
o B o
It would, how ever, be possible to scan the line with an indefinite stress
to permit a deliberate, metrically regular, reading. A nd in the next
224 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
Spite o f m y se lf. . .
Since it occurs in one o f the environm ents o f the prom otion rule,
betw een a nonstress and a line-boundary, the pronoun is metrically
free to take any degree o f stress, and the sense and inverted syntax
invite som e degree o f em phasis, though it is not the task o f the scansion
to prescribe how much. The next exam ple also offers the reader a
choice, this time betw een two scansions, depending on the placing o f
emphatic stress:
s s
(18) That I am favoured for unworthiness
o Bo
B o
This allows the main em phasis in the line to fall where the sense
suggests it should, on ‘unworthiness’. Finally, we can look at an
exam ple where the rhythmic pulse tips the balance; I give it first as it
might be read in a prose context:
-s -s +s +s
(19) Though I breathe death with them it will be life
o B o B
-s +s +s +s
Though I breathe death with them it will be life
o B o B
The pronoun needs just enough weight to bring it level with the
follow ing word, and must not take away from the important
contrastive stress on ‘death’, set as it is against ‘life’ later in the line.
This freedom to give personal pronouns a little extra weight to avoid
rhythmic irregularity is capitalised on by many poets. The most
satisfactory performance o f the following line, for instance, is probably
as shown; in representing emphatic stress, I use an underlined s in the
stress pattern, especially where the em phasis is for purposes o f
contrast:
-s s_. +s s s s -s -s s s
(2 0 ) If I lose thee, my loss is my love’s gain
o B o B o B o B o B
This m eans giving both ‘I’ and ‘th ee’ sufficient stress to make them
equivalent to ‘lo se’, thereby allowing dem otion to occur, and stressing
the first ‘m y’ but not the second, so that the double contrast - ‘my’ with
‘my lo v e’s’ and ‘loss’ with ‘gain’ - is fully brought out. The effect is of
five even stresses, lingering on the painful thought o f personal loss,
follow ed by a double offbeat and two more even stresses, this time
separated by an im plied offbeat, to round o ff the line with a firm (if
spurious) assertiveness.
The three em phatic peaks o f this line are all already in metrically
stressed positions; ‘thou’ (the same force already described), ‘sever’
(as opposed to uniting), and ‘us’ (in contrast to all other creatures).
The result is a mutual reinforcem ent betw een the strongly insistent
rhythm, expressive o f the speaker’s em otional state, and the sem antic
weight on particular points o f complaint. There are also instances
where the sem antic dem ands im pose a metrical interpretation on the
line different from that im plied by the neutral stress contour; in the
follow ing exam ple ‘undo’ requires an initial em phatic stress to contrast
it with ‘d o ’, and the result is a line with pairing instead o f a nondeviant
realisation o f the metrical pattern:
The result, as shown, is a four-beat line with one double offbeat and a
fem inine ending. It occurs, however, in a sonnet by Shakespeare, and
there is a clear emphasis on T and ‘th ee’, the terms which the poet is
claiming to regard as identical. This yields a regular iambic pentam eter
with a sm ooth dem otion:
_s +s _s
A nd what is’t but mine own when I praise thee?
B o B o B o B o B
SENSE AND THE STRESS PATTERN 227
A line in which all the beats are carried by em phatic stresses, and the
only major lexical item is dem oted, is the following:
-S S_ +S _S ~S _S ~S s_
(26) Y et he broke them, ere they could him
o B o B o B o B
There can be no doubt about the contrastive stress here, because the
italics are Swift’s own; yet the second em phasis falls on a syllable which
has to be dem oted to preserve the metricality o f the line. What com es
to the rescue o f the reader here is the variety o f ways in which stress in
English can be m anifested. In discussing this subject in 3.2 we noted
that pitch, volum e, duration, and sound-quality can all function as cues
for stress, and that o f these, pitch and duration are the m ost effective.
This m eans that it is possible to give a word a special sem antic em phasis
by pronouncing it with a different pitch-contour from that used in
unemphatic speech; indeed, this is the natural way to create such an
emphasis. In the case o f ‘they’ above, it is by pronouncing it at a higher
pitch (and possibly with greater volum e) than ‘kiss’ that we give it a
contrastive em phasis, not necessarily by any greater duration. The
importance o f this is that the higher pitch is less likely to attract a beat;
the syllable remains tem porally equivalent to its neighbours, and its
syntactic subordination to the verb that follow s is still perceived,
228 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
o B o B
There are tim es, however, when even this flexibility is not sufficient
to prevent the metrical and rhetorical structures from colliding. Let us
look at two rather similar lines, the first by Shakespeare and the second
by Keats:
1 —
(29) H ow can I then be elder than thou art
S. 1
(30) Bright star! Would I were steadfast as thou art
B oth lines imply a contrast betw een T and ‘thou’, with the appropriate
SENSE AND THE STRESS PATTERN 229
em phatic stress, but the result in both cases is a ‘final inversion’ and an
unmetrical, four-beat line. This is because ‘thou’ is preceded by two
nonstresses which we have no option but to treat as a double offbeat
(unlike K eats’s other final inversions, discussed earlier), and follow ed
by a verb which takes very little stress and so cannot function as a final
beat. In a different semantic context, the stress pattern o f ‘thou art’
could be reversed, producing a perfectly acceptable line:
-s -s +s
[30a] Bright star above, as steadfast as ‘t hou art,
o B o B
The reader can consult the debate betw een Stein (1 9 5 6 ) and Chatman
(1956b ) over the pronunciation and scansion o f these lines, which, if it
does not settle the question, at least proves that it is far from easy to
settle. O ne conclusion, however, may be drawn from our discussion: it
appears that the weighting o f stresses dem anded by the sense does not
entirely displace the neutral stress contour produced by phonology and
syntax, and that the variable which determ ines the role o f that neutral
contour in the total rhythmic effect is the metre. W hen, as in (2 4 )-(2 6 ),
the requirements o f the sense coincide with the requirements o f the
metrical structure, in opposition to the neutral stress contour, the latter
is present only as a shadow in the background, pulling against the
rhythmic alternations and thereby slightly increasing the tension.
W hen there is som e degree o f conflict betw een emphasis and metre,
however, as in (2 7 )-(3 2 ), it is the implicit neutral contour which
preserves rhythmic regularity, and the counter-dem ands o f the sense
which create tension. The result is a rhythmic com plexity greater than
that produced by straightforward deviations, and one which can add a
dim ension to the reader’s experience o f the language unavailable
230 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
8.3 M E T R IC A L S U B O R D IN A T IO N
The anom alous stresses in these exam ples are all clear cases of
syntactic subordination, and o f only one type o f subordination at that:
a m onosyllabic adjective follow ed by a noun with a stress on its first (or
only) syllable. It is obvious that subordination is crucial to our sense
that these lines do not transgress the metrical bounds, since it allows
the first stressed syllable to be perceived as part o f a double offbeat.
W hen a line is rendered metrical by virtue o f syntactic subordination, a
phenom enon which I shall call m etrical su bordin ation , the symbol [s]
can be used in the stress pattern; the metrical pattern and deviation
symbols can then be shown beneath the line as if the syllable were a
nonstress:
+s -s [s] +§
(33 ) Drawn o f fair peacocks, that excel in pride
B o B
[s] i
(39) Read or read not what I am now essaying
B o B
In the first exam ple, the verb is subordinated to its object (and the
conventional nature o f the collocation increases the subordination);
while in the second the negative takes an em phatic stress, and the
repeated verb is subordinated to it. In either case, the syllable in
question might more accurately be shown as a nonstress, though the
degree o f stress which it retains is a choice to be made by the reader.
The inclusion in som e metrical theories o f a rule which deletes a stress
before another stress in the same phrase (see M agnuson and Ryder,
1971; and Chisholm , 1977) is an attempt to deal with metrical
subordination, but by extending the principle to all phrases in all
metrical formations it does as much harm as good: in stress-initial
pairing, for instance, such de-stressing would be fatal to the metre.
E ven as an optional rule it would m isrepresent the very specific
circumstances under which metrical subordination occurs. (The
classical approach, o f course, does not offer any way o f dealing with
this phenom enon, except by labelling it ‘trochee + spondee’.)
It is also noticeable that all these exam ples occur in the same
position in the line, as the second syllable o f a double offbeat in an
initial inversion; and this reflects an overwhelm ing preference in the
verse tradition, ignored in the theories just m entioned. One reason for
this is that the equivalent form ation later in the line produces the
highly deviant sequence o f two stresses, a nonstress, and two further
stresses, the effect o f which we shall consider below . A second reason,
however, is suggested by the som ewhat surprising list o f poets who use
this device frequently. Tarlinskaja’s statistics (1 9 7 6 , p. 287, Table 45,
last 4 colum ns) show not only that it forms a relatively high proportion
o f all the strong deviations used by such rhythmically liberated poets as
D onne, Swinburne, and Browning, but also that it is quite com m on in
the strict pentam eters o f Thom son and Pope. The writers w hose
metrical styles fall som ewhere betw een these extrem es, however, are
more likely to avoid it: Shakespeare, W ordsworth, and Byron use it
very sparingly.3 Why should this be so? W e can assume that in poets
like Pope and Thom son it occurs almost exclusively at the beginning of
the line, and it is to the metrical possibilities at this point that we must
look for an answer. The stricter the verse, the stronger and more
clear-cut the metrical set; and strict iambic pentam eter includes as a
sharply-defined possible opening the initial inversion sequence + s —s
METRICAL SUBORDINATION 233
[s]
(4 0 ) W hile late bare earth, proud of new clothing, springeth
B o B o B
[s]
(41) To expiate which sin, kiss and shake hands
B o B o
A ny sensitive reader will register this line as metrically com plex, the
tension occurring especially on the crucial word ‘death’s’; the metre
demands that its syntactic subordination to the noun phrase that
follow s be as fully m anifested as possible by an unemphatic
pronunciation, while the meaning, and the clustering o f consonants,
require that it be given special weight. Metrical subordination o f this
kind testifies to a relatively free metrical style, and even then can be
used only rarely if the five-beat foundation o f the metre is not to be
disturbed. Pope appears to limit his use o f it to the line-opening,
though one exception is a line which, rather unfairly, he makes
deliberately monstrous with its help:
(43) A needless Alexandrine ends the song
[s]
That, like a wounded snake, drags its slow length along
B o B o B
234 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
[S]
(46) Fresh leaves and flowers deck the dead season’s bier
B o B o B
[s]
(47) All baser things pant with life’s sacred thirst
B o B o B
A nd the generous limits o f M ilton’s metrical style let it in, here nicely
illustrative in the way it overfills the rhythmic space:
[s]
(48) In narrow room nature’s whole wealth, yea more
B o B o
_S [S] J5
(4 9 ) Beaus banish beaus, and coaches coaches drive
B o B
which contains more than one possible environm ent for metrical
subordination, and there is no good reason for the preferential
treatment o f any one of these:
(6 0 ) The deep groves and white tem ples and wet caves
located, and its force spreads into the adjacent syllable, momentarily
blunting the sharpness o f the rhythmic alternation. This effect is only
possible because the deviation does not push the line towards any
other com m on rhythm; in fact, metrical subordination can stiffen a line
which might otherwise fall into a triple rhythm, as the follow ing
rewriting shows:
[S]
(62) Hold like rich garners the full-ripened grain
B o B o B o B o B
8.4 D O U B L E O F F B E A T S A N D EL ISIO N
In this and the follow ing three sections we shall take up the various
ways set out in Chapter 7 o f realising the beats and offbeats o f duple
verse as stresses and nonstresses, and exam ine the com plications
entailed by the em bodim ent o f this stress pattern in actual specim ens
240 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
missing, and you will find that articulatory organs which obey English
speech habits are unhappy with the resulting succession o f sounds.
‘O pposite’ in (6 8 ) also creates slight phonetic discomfort if the vow el is
om itted, because the unvoiced/? does not lead easily into the voiced s
(phonetically [z]); and in (69) the sequence o f v and n which would
result from the dropping o f the second vow el o f ‘given’ is not a normal
conclusion to a syllable (note how much easier it would be if the second
consonant could be shifted to the next syllable, as in given a w ay). On
the other hand, the com bination of consonants that is left when the
middle vowel o f ‘uttering’ (72) is not pronounced is a very natural one
in English, hence the ease with which contraction occurs to make it a
disyllabic word - with the slight duration o f the r to adumbrate the
missing syllable.
The other way in which the distinction betw een one and two
syllables can be diminished, which I am calling ‘coalescence’, is by the
running together o f two adjacent vowels, at least one o f them
unstressed, w hether betw een two words or within a single word.
Coalescence can be divided into two broad categories. O ne o f these is
almost the converse o f contraction, in that the first vow el plays a
rhythmic role similar to that o f a consonant. The / of ‘studious’ (7 0 ), for
exam ple, can be pronounced as a full vow el, producing a trisyllabic
word, or as the consonanty (in strict phonetic terms, the sem ivow el or
glide [j]), yielding a disyllabic word with only a suggestion o f an
additional syllable. In ‘hurrying’ (66), the process is thwarted by the
similarity o f the two vow el sounds, and the join is not as seam less. In
(71), ‘B edouin’ can have three syllables, or two plus a vestigial third
manifested as [w]. This type o f coalescence occurs easily betw een a
word ending in y and one beginning with a vowel: one exam ple which
poets are fond o f is m any a pronounced as two syllables. The second
type o f coalescence involves the flowing o f one vow el into the next to
create a diphthong or a triphthong; exam ples o f this happening
betw een words are ‘the illim itable’ (6 6 ), and ‘To exhilarate’ (67). In
careful speech such words retain m ost o f their individual syllabic force,
but a rapid pronunciation brings them closer to a single syllable.
Within the word, the process is smoother: ‘unwieldly’ (7 3 ) and
‘powers’ (74) can be pronounced with four and two syllables
respectively, or by the coalescence o f the adjacent vow els, with three
and one. In fact, this type of coalescence is often made easier by the use
o f the cham eleon sounds [w] and [j], and the effect is then not clearly
distinguishable from that o f the first type.
DOUBLE OFFBEATS AND ELISION 243
the symbols below the line which show the simplest realisation o f the
metrical pattern. W hen the pronunciation is so natural as to create no
tension, however, there is little point in showing elision; ‘powers’ in
(74) could be shown as a simple m onosyllable.
The choice betw een showing a double offbeat or an elision in the
scansion can be an unreal one, and the important feature is the
inherent elidability o f the phonetic material, not the actual
pronunciation. Tennyson said o f his use o f ‘tired’ in ‘The Lotos-Eaters’
that it was ‘neither m onosyllabic nor disyllabic, but a dreamy child o f
the tw o’ (quoted in P oem s, ed. Ricks, p. 431); while the hint o f a triple
rhythm within the duple metre o f M arvell’s ‘winged chariot hurrying
near’ will be felt whatever pronunciation one gives the middle two
words. There is, however, one general guide to the reader and scanner,
already m entioned in the previous chapter: the observance or
nonobservance o f pairing conditions. In verse which does not observe
the double offbeat condition, the poet is free to introduce additional
unstressed syllables, as long as the duple rhythm is not threatened; for
instance, it is clear from the first few exam ples among the lines from
The Prelude quoted earlier that the double offbeat condition is not
being strictly adhered to, and it seem s appropriate, therefore, to
invoke elision only when the pronunciation naturally vacillates
betw een one and two syllables, as in two or three o f the later exam ples.
Som etim es the choice is betw een a double offbeat and a triple offbeat,
in which case we can assume that elision is im plied, as in this line from
the same passage:
(s)
(8 0 ) Gathering upon us; quickening then the pace
B 6 B
-s
(83) N ight’s hem isphere had veiled th eAhorizon round
B o B
M ilton, for all his metrical variety, does not use this licence in the epic
poem s nor in Sam son A g o n istes, but C om us contains several exam ples
(see W eismiller, 1972, pp. 1 0 4 1 -4 ):
-s -s
(8 6 ) A lone and h elp lessH s this the confidence
246 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
A s this last exam ple suggests, elision, like all metrical and prosodic
options, is governed by one strong constraint; it can occur only when
this m akes possible the realisation o f the appropriate metrical pattern.
W here the metrical pattern demands two syllables, elision cannot
occur, as in (8 2 ). The follow ing line could be a three-beat line with
elision:
-S +s
(92) Still clutching the" inviolable shade
o B o B o B
Lines like this are a further dem onstration that the reader’s response to
rhythm is not a response merely to objective details o f sound: one does
not have to read the two occurrences o f the word in different ways,
since the metrical context allows the same pronunciation -
emphasising neither alternative - to realise in one case an offbeat, in
the other a beat and an offbeat.
To end this discussion, som e characteristic lines from Paradise L o st
will illustrate the rhythmic variety which can be created, even in strictly
syllabic verse, by a poet who takes advantage o f the freedom offered by
the elidability o f many English syllables:
+s -s
(94) Shade above shade, a woody theatre
Bo B
-^s +s -s
O f stateliest view. Y et higher than their tops
B o B B o b
(s)
The verdurous wall of Paradise up sprung;
B o B
(s) +s
Which to our general sire gave prospect large
B o B
248 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
-s (s)
Into his nether empire neighbouring round.
B o B o B
+s -s
A nd higher than that wall a circling row
B o B
-s
O f goodliest trees loaden with fairest fruit
B o B
8.5 P R O M O T IO N A N D D E M O T IO N
The prom otion o f nonstresses to the role of a metrical beat seem s to be
a phenom enon very little affected by the linguistic structures in which
it occurs, which is not surprising in this, the least marked o f metrical
PROMOTION AND DEMOTION 249
W e have also noted exam ples where two o f the three syllables
constitute an entire word ((8 )-(1 0 ) o f this chapter). In all these
exam ples one can sense the natural alternating rhythm o f English
co-operating with the metre in the prom otion o f the middle nonstress;
this is particularly noticeable in the case o f polysyllables, since a
syllable at one rem ove from the main stress - like the first syllable o f
‘generations’ in (9 7 ) - will often carry som e degree o f secondary stress.
If in the case o f m onosyllables the syntax encourages a slightly greater
emphasis on the prom oted syllable, the result will naturally be
sm oother; thus ‘as’ in (9 5 ) is a som ewhat easier prom otion than ‘an’ in
(96 ). Poetic practice reflects even such slight differences: Tarlinskaja
(1 9 7 6 , pp. 2 5 2 -3 , Table 14) found that in her sample, most poets used
articles less frequently in the position of the beat than any other type o f
m onosyllable. Prom otion and elision are often interconnected; in (9 8 ),
for instance, ‘solitary’ has to be read as a four-syllable word, because if
it is reduced to three, prom otion is im possible, the line loses a beat, and
one o f the m ost finely controlled o f all concluding paragraphs in
English literature com es down with a bump.
N or is prom otion a respecter o f syntactic boundaries, as the
following exam ples testify:
+s +s -s -s
(9 5 ) This thought is as a death, which cannot choose •
B 6b 6
There are not two hard-and-fast alternatives here: som e blurring o f the
rhythm is possible in a normal reading, though as soon as one tries to
identify the rhythmic pattern, say by tapping, one is forced to opt one
way or the other. H esitation betw een the two can, however, be
experienced as rhythmic tension:
(1 02) O f darkness visible so much be lent
o B O B
o Bo B
H ere the need to give ‘so’ a certain degree o f em phatic stress prevents
the sm ooth operation o f prom otion, w hile the need to keep it
subordinate to the more strongly em phasised ‘much’ inhibits pairing.
The result is a rhythmic uncertainty which is felt in any reading o f the
line.
(1 0 5 ) Fare ill, lie hard, lack clothes, lack fire, lack food?
o B o B o B o B o B
(1 0 6 ) The long day wanes; the slow m oon climbs; the deep
B 6 B B o B
two words form a subsidiary unit within the phrase, and the
subordination o f the second adjective to the noun is therefore more
strongly felt. The rhythm is even more markedly slowed in (1 0 8 ),
where three verbs o f equal importance occupy the pattern. Read as
prose, the three stresses would certainly all function as rhythmic peaks;
in the context o f duple verse, it may be possible to suppress the beat on
the middle syllable in som e way. H ow ever, as with extrem e cases o f
metrical subordination, it may be more accurate to say that we
som etim es accept six-beat lines in pentam eter verse, though only
under certain very special conditions which we have learned to
recognise. But perhaps an even more accurate report on the
psychological experience o f reading such a line would be that the
rhythm is suspended when we com e to those three stresses; that our ear
accepts what happens as the equivalent of two beats and an offbeat, but
that it does not locate these events precisely. It is, in other words, a
further exam ple o f the blurring o f the rhythm such as w e find in the
opposite phenom enon, three equally unstressed syllables, though it
has a more pronounced effect on the m ovem ent o f the line.
These differences in the tension produced by dem otion as a result of
different linguistic structures are, o f course, often used expressively.
M ilton can convey both sm ooth and rough m ovem ents by means o f
dem otion, using syntactic structures that fit snugly into the metrical
pattern for the first and catalogues that resist subordination for the
second:
(1 0 9 ) N ow came still evening on, and twilight gray
o B o B
Had in her sober livery all things clad
B o B
A lthough the line can be ‘saved’ by the rules, no sensitive ear can hear
it as a normal pentam eter. Nor is there any point in trying to make it
sound like one by emphasising particular words, as is som etim es
PROMOTION AND DEMOTION 253
N one o f these exam ples tempts the reader to give the middle stress
m ore weight than the outer two, and so upset the alternation; when
such lines do occur, they make special demands on the reader, as in the
following:
W e have to give ‘Jew ’s’ and ‘cut’ as much weight as ‘head’, and leave
‘o f f unstressed so that ‘at’ can take the next beat by prom otion. This
reading does not contradict the norms o f the language, and, it may be
added, serves to bring out m ost fully the perverse passion o f
Browning’s Bishop. M etrical needs do not always serve expressive
needs in this way, however; in the following exam ple, the sense
demands a slow, em otionally-charged, enunciation, while the strong
four-beat metre o f the poem insists on a brisker m ovem ent to allow
dem otion:
Only the second rewriting markedly affects the rhythm, because in the
first it is still possible to subordinate ‘sm ooth’ to ‘lawns’ and so
preserve the feeling o f alternation. In [1 17b], however, ‘sm ooth’ is not
subordinated to the next word, and com es closer to attracting a beat to
itself, creating a significant increase in tension. O ne could multiply
exam ples o f the interaction o f metrical and linguistic form indefinitely,
but it is more important to ensure that we have the right terms in which
to discuss such exam ples than it is to catalogue them.
The dem otion o f syllables before the first beat is affected by the
linguistic substance o f the line in similar and self-evident ways, and it
will not be necessary to do more than furnish som e exam ples,
illustrating that the stronger the stress on the syllable in question in
relation to what follow s, the more difficult it is to discount it from the
rhythmic structure, and therefore the greater the tension:
8.6 P A IR IN G A N D S Y N T A X
In the first exam ple, the sense and syntax invite a strong stress on each
o f the syllables taking a beat, and the implied offbeat occurs naturally
in the pause betw een the syntactic units. But in the second exam ple,
the adjective ‘sw eet’ is subordinate to the noun ‘rites’ in normal
pronunciation, and in order to bring out the five-beat structure o f the
line, it is necessary to give the first word an em phasis which is
som ewhat artificial in view of the conventionality o f the epithet. If one
does not do this, the line slips into a jaunty triple rhythm; indeed, it
would be perfectly acceptable in the context o f free four-beat triple
verse:
When V enus’ sw eet rites are performed and done
o B o" B o B o B
W e have seen that this kind o f dem otion is very com m on in triple verse.
The possibility o f a quite different rhythm asserting itself constitutes
a greater threat to the integrity o f the metre than a straightforward
increase o f tension would do, and the practice o f poets shows that there
PAIRING AND SYNTAX 257
+s +s
(1 2 6 ) A s testy sick men when their deaths be near
B o B o
s _s
(1 3 8 ) A h, but a m an’s reach should exceed his grasp
B o B o B o
In these exam ples, there is strong subordination o f the first stress to the
second (m ade stronger in (1 3 8 ) by the contrastive stress on ‘reach’);
but there is no danger o f a triple rhythm asserting itself, because in
each case the stress-initial pairing is preceded by an initial inversion
which produces a legitim ate double offbeat and prevents the
subordinated stress from itself becom ing part o f an unwanted double
offbeat. A t worst, the effect approaches that o f three successive
nonstresses, with som e blurring o f the lin e’s second beat. A rewriting
260 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
The only way to com bat this four-beat reading would be a wholly
unnaturalem phasis on ‘m an’s’, and even then the absence o f any
syntactic or sem antic justification for the em phasis would render it
merely a matter o f sound - and as w e have repeatedly had cause to
observe, our response to rhythm is not merely a response to the sound.
In the first o f these, the im plied offbeat falls betw een an adjective and
its noun while in the second it falls betw een two equivalent adjectives
separated by a slight syntactic break (which could be represented by a
com m a). O nce m ore there is a difference in rhythmic effect, but it is
now in the opposite direction, and the reason for this is quite clear: the
risk o f one o f the stresses losing its weight and combining with an
adjacent nonstress to create a double offbeat and a triple rhythm is
now located in the second o f the stresses. Subordination o f the first to
the second stress, as in (1 3 9 ), is now harmless, since a double offbeat
already exists before the two stresses; but a syntactic break betw een
the stresses, as in (1 4 0 ), creates the possibility that the second stress
will be subordinated to a later stress, and suffer dem otion within an
unwanted double offbeat. A dd to this the fact that the ear has already
accepted one double offbeat in the line and is therefore not averse to
accepting another, and it becom es obvious why in this formation poets
usually avoid syntactic breaks, or, m ore generally, subordination o f the
second stress. The alternative rhythm which lurks around a line like
(1 4 0 ) is again that o f a four-beat triple-metre line with dem otion:
With frantic gesture and short breathless cry
0 B 0 B 0 B o B
Both these run a strong risk o f being misread: Spenser’s adjectives are
relatively conventional, and hence do not seem to deserve the weight
which the metre requires them to carry; and Shakespeare has
implanted such a strong suggestion o f a triple metre by m eans o f two
double offbeats, that it demands a firm thrust against that rhythm on
‘silent’ to save the line. In the first exam ple the result is a slight
awkwardness, in the second a sudden focusing on an apparently
tautologous adjective, which gains from the added attention drawn to
it by the dem ands o f the metre. N or are these the m ost extreme
exam ples o f lines which require deliberate stiffening as w e read: the
following can only be understood metrically as exam ples o f stress-final
pairing, but the triple rhythm is knocking hard at the door:
+s
(1 4 3 ) From hence your memory death cannot take
o B o B
+s
(1 4 4 ) By night he fled, and at midnight returned
o BoB
+s
(1 4 5 ) My eyes to fathom the space every way
o B oB
The explanation for such lines may lie partly in the diminishing
importance o f subsidiary stressing in English pronunciation already
alluded to (w e shall reconsider (1 4 4 ) in this light in section 8 below ), or
in a more deliberate style o f verse delivery than is current at present;
but perhaps the m ost significant fact about them is their
uncom m onness in all periods.
The m ost frequent syntactic units making up the pair o f stresses in
this form ation, therefore, are the ones m ost avoided in its mirror
image: adjective+n oun or verb+n oun. N ou n+ verb is less com m on,
because the direction o f subordination is often from the latter to the
former, creating the unwanted triple effect, as a construct will show
262 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
If a break betw een the stresses does occur, the danger o f a triple
rhythm can be averted by ensuring that the second stress is not a
subordinated part o f the syntactic unit that follows:
(147) Productive in herb, plant, and nobler birth
o B o B
(148) But bare of laurel they live, dream, and die
o B o B
(1 49) With his broad, bright, and dropping orb were gone
o B o B
Som e reasons for the favouring o f the line-end for stress-final pairing
have already been suggested, but a further one is that in this position
the danger o f slipping into another rhythm is least present, since the
second stress is follow ed by a line boundary:
(1 5 3 ) I wished the man a dinner, and sat still
o B o B
-s -s +s +s
(1 6 0 ) R em em bering not, retains an obscure sense
o B o B
H ere are several exam ples, with the authors nam ed, and the linkage
indicated:
(163) Through the pure marble air his oblique way (M ilton)6
o B o B
(168) The com plete fire is death. From partial fires (Em pson)
o B o B
+ s-s
Then came hot July boiling like to fire
o B 0 B 0 B 0 B o B
In such a case, it is legitim ate to argue that the line constitutes evidence
o f an earlier pronunciation otJuly with initial stress; and there is in fact
supporting evidence outside verse for this pronunciation o f the word
(see D obson, 1968, p. 44 7 ). Such lines, how ever, are rare; and
discussions o f ‘recession o f accent’ invariably concentrate on the more
com m on phenom enon o f stress-final linkage.
A metrical set for alternating rhythm will always exert pressure on
the two middle syllables o f a syntactically unified pairing formation to
blur or exchange their stresses; it is, o f course, the language’s
resistance to this interference with its internal structures that creates
the tension associated with the device o f pairing (though the metre is
only heightening a tension in the language itself betw een
lexically-determ ined stress patterns and a rhythmic preference for
alternation). A nalysts who invoke ‘variable stress’ to account for every
occurrence o f linkage are finding (or inventing) historical reasons for
yielding to this pressure; and even when there is independent evidence
for a change in accentuation the modern reader may prefer the tension
o f a pairing form ation to the oddity o f an obsolete pronunciation. Even
if we could be certain that com plete was always given an initial stress in
Elizabethan English, we might still choose to give it its modern
pronunciation in the follow ing line:
-s +s +s
(1 7 0 ) A maid o f grace and com plete majesty
o B o B
with prefix and stem given the same degree o f stress to permit
dem otion instead o f pairing (in the first exam ple, I include the
preceding line to show the neutral stress pattern o f a similar word when
it co-operates with the metrical alternation):
-S +S
(1 71) Thou dost beguile the world, unbless som e mother.
o B
+s +s +s
For where is she so fair whose uneared womb
B o B
+s +s +s
(1 72) The pangs of disprized love, the law ’s delay
B o B
W e noted in the previous section that in exam ples like (1 5 9 ) there may
be som e rhythmic seepage from ‘grim’ (which is subordinated to
‘shape’) to ‘the’, taking us a little way towards the rhythmic pattern in
which ‘grim’ would be w eakened and ‘the’ would becom e the focal
point o f the beat. To go further and allow the beat actually to shift to
the normally unstressed syllable would, o f course, be a highly
unnatural reading, but it would at least do no damage to the lexical
structures involved. In (1 6 0 ), on the other hand, there can be no such
rhythmic blurring, since the lexical identity o f ‘obscure’ depends on the
contrast o f stress betw een the syllables. So this can be nothing other
than a full-blooded instance o f an implied offbeat betw een two
syntactically connected words, with the maximum rhythmic tension o f
which this deviation is capable. O ne can imagine an apprentice poet
stressing every second syllable o f his line to m ake sure he has written a
correct iambic pentam eter, and passing (1 5 9 ) in spite o f the oddly
stressed ‘the’ in such a reading, while rejecting (1 6 0 ) because o f the
way it reverses the stress pattern o f ‘obscure’. This is, o f course, a crude
notion o f the poet at work, but this elem entary procedure may not be
unrelated to the nuances o f rhythmic intuition which have kept such
lines out o f Spenser’s or P ope’s poetry.
The regularising effect of a pause betw een the stresses is evident even
when the syntactic break in this position is relatively slight, as long as
som e integrity is allowed to the rhythmic pattern that follow s it
(though the third exam ple below was still harsh to Johnson’s ears,
judging from his com plaints about it in The R am bler, N o. 86):
i-------- 1
(1 7 9 ) The vain travail hath wearied me so sore
B o B o
272 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
The last two exam ples illustrate the triple-rhythm threat that attends
this formation: ‘m en’ and ‘each’ require a som ewhat self-conscious
stressing to preserve the pentam eter structure.
Once again, the explanation for such lines from earlier periods may
lie in changes o f pronunciation, but exam ples like these do not
constitute proof o f such changes, and it appears to be a reasonable
conclusion that, once the effect o f syntax is taken into account,
stress-initial linkage is like its mirror image in being rare but not
unknown. The slight rhythmic difference that is at stake here can be
illustrated by comparing a stress-initial pairing without linkage (and
without a syntactic break) with a rewriting that introduces linkage:
In [187a] the need to press straight on to the rest o f the word, with its
inbuilt stress contrast, seem s to heighten the challenge to the rhythm,
PAIRING AND WORD-BOUNDARIES 273
rewriting runs less easily as a pentam eter than the original, and is
rhythmically more like exam ples (1 7 9 )-(1 8 6 ):
I---------- 1
[188a] H ow many bards burnish lapses o f time
B o B o B o
I------- 1
[190b] Your business does not invite men with show
o B o B o B
8.8 C O M P O U N D S
The relationship betw een metrical patterns and linguistic structures
becom es particularly problematic in those cases where a close match is
not possible. N ot every linguistic form ation slips easily into an
alternating or triple rhythm, but poets are understandably loath to
exclude any o f the resources o f their language from their verse. With
regard to duple verse, the m ost important body o f individual words
which com es into this category consists o f certain types of com pound,
and these words have consequently been the subject of much metrical
debate. A satisfactory metrical theory should at least facilitate
accurate identification o f the problem s they cause.
In com pounds form ed from two m onosyllabic words, the second
word is usually subordinated to the first but retains som e degree of
stress (see the discussion in 3.3), and the easiest way to use them in
verse is in contexts which permit an indefinite stress on this syllable:
276 METRICAL RULES AND THE STRUCTURES OF LANGUAGE
+s s +s
( 191) But later ages’ pride, like corn-fed steed
B o B
+s s +s
(1 9 2 ) A nd burn the long-lived Phoenix in her blood
B o B
+s s +s
(193) Old knights, and over them the sea-wind sang
B o B
It is probably accurate to say that the two patterns m erge, and som e
blurring o f the rhythm occurs: we have already noted that this seem s to
be a com m on feature o f stress-final pairing, but that it is rare in the
stress-initial variant because o f the problems posed by subordination
when the two stresses are part o f the same phrase. If both stresses
belong to the same com pound, however, these problem s do not arise,
because it is the second elem ent that is subordinated - there is no
threatening triple rhythm in (1 94) as there is in (1 2 8 )—(1 3 1 ).
Som ething similar happens in lines like the following, in which two
scansions are again possible, depending on whether the second item in
the com pound is treated as a stress or a nonstress, and a reading may
include hints o f both:
(196) Thy beauty’s shield, heart-shaped and vermeil dyed
B o B o
B o B o
Unless the line is to lose a beat, the finalsyllable in these exam ples
must have a stress as strong as its predecessor; and we find this much
easier in the case o f ‘bright-blanched’ than ‘knot-grass’. Pronunciation
changes may be responsible for som e o f our problems; a disyllabic
com pound was more likely in early modern English to have a
secondary stress (see D obson, 1968, pp. 445, 8 3 0 -3 4 ), and
Renaissance poetry seem s particularly rich in com pounds that demand
two strong stresses, where we would give only one:
printing o f Sonnet 73, for instance, gives ‘twi-light’ and ‘Sun-set’ but
‘death bed’, and in Sonnet 62 both ‘selfe-love’ and ‘selfe love’ occur. In
forms o f verse in which a strong rhythm dom inates the linguistic
material, com pounds can more happily provide two beats; an example
used in Chapter 4 provides an illustration:
Y eats uses a four-beat line with the fourth beat unrealised in ‘The Cap
and B ells’ to create a strong rhythmic pulse, which is powerful enough
to push the main stress on to the second elem ent in a compound:
Bailey (1975a) recorded ten speakers reading the poem , and notes
that nine ‘appeared to m ove the stronger stress to the second elem ent’
o f ‘night-gown’ (p. 26).
The poet who wishes to use a com pound which has successive
stresses plus an additional unstressed syllable - like dog-ow ner or
land-grabber - has to pick his way even more carefully, since the
com m onest treatm ent o f the disyllabic com pound, dem otion of the
second stress, is denied him in duple metre. If the com pound can be
treated simply as a stress and two nonstresses, which will only be
possible if it has becom e familiarised and is acting as a single word,
there is no problem. Kiparsky (1 9 7 7 , p. 221) notes that in
Shakespeare words like bedfellow and torchbearer are consistently
treated in this way:
+s -s -s
(2 06) I have this twelvem onth been her bedfellow
B o B
N otes
1. Emphatic stress has posed problems for many metrical theories: see, for instance,
the disagreements between Stein (1956) and Chatman (1956b), or between Halle &
Keyser ( 1 9 6 6 ,1971b) and Magnuson & Ryder (1970), or Beaver’s shifting position
in successive studies (1968a, 1969, 1971a, 1976).
2. An interesting account, along these lines, of the rhythmic multiplicity that can be
implied by a single line of regular verse is given by Scott (1980, pp. 5 -7 ). The subject
is discussed further in 9.6 below.
3. It is interesting to note that in the four-beat verse studied by Bailey (1975b),
metrical subordination occurs occasionally in his samples from most poets before
the twentieth century, but not at all in Eliot, Auden, or Graves (p. 75, Table 11).^
4. Hatcher (1928, p. 91) calls this ‘one of the most numerous of Browning’s
variations’, and cites several examples.
5. In one of the first published discussions of English metre, this word caused problems
because of its common contraction to a monosyllable: see Harvey & Spenser
(1579-80, pp. 98-9), and Attridge (1974, pp. 146-7).
NOTES 281
Though there are several examples in Comus and the first three books of Paradise
L o st, there are no clear examples in Milton’s later work, suggesting that he became
increasingly sensitive to the rhythmic effect of this formation (and also that it cannot
be explained away by pronunciation changes) - see Bridges (1921, pp. 70 -7 3 ) and
Sprott (1953, pp. 136-7).
Since Tarlinskaja includes initial inversions in her totals, it is necessary to subtract
these in order to reach figures for stress-initial pairing.
This page intentionally left blank
Part Four : Practice
This page intentionally left blank
Chapter 9
one another and with all the other elem ents o f the poem: I shall say
scarcely anything, for instance, about the manifold ways in which
syntax and word-order contribute to impressions o f m ovem ent and
rhythm. It must also be borne in mind that literature is characterised as
much by its subversion o f prevailing norms as by its obedience to them,
and a traditional rhythmic function, like any other literary function,
can be turned on its head in the unique situation o f a particular text. A s
in the book as a w hole, I shall confine m yself almost entirely to metrical
verse, though much o f what follows will have implications for
nonmetrical verse. Though the discussion will reflect the account of
metre I have given in earlier chapters, it will not illustrate the functions
o f individual rhythmic formations and variations; many o f these will be
evident already, and the passages to be considered in the following
chapter will exem plify som e o f them further. A s an additional
limitation, I shall be concerned only with the importance of rhythm for
the reader, though there is ample evidence for its formative role in the
act o f writing.1 N or shall I summarise or make specific reference to the
mass o f critical and theoretical discussion o f this subject: that too
would demand a separate study. What follows is largely speculative,
and leaves behind the realm of empirical evidence and testable
hypothesis within which we have so far tried to confine ourselves.
Although we have observed the rootedness o f English metrical
structures in the nature o f the English language, we have also seen how
these linguistic characteristics are m odified by general principles of
rhythmic form, and by more conventional features of the literary
tradition. To exam ine the functions o f rhythm in poetry is to ask how
this process o f m odification invests the language’s rhythmic properties
with additional potency, and our attention must therefore be directed
towards the distinctiveness which metre imparts to language, rather
than to the com m on ground betw een them. W e shall consider in this
chapter a number o f respects in which verse differs from other
m anifestations o f language, making use o f a broad categorisation
whose inevitable elem ent o f arbitrariness will, I hope, be outweighed
by its expository convenience. W e can m ake an initial distinction
betw een sem antic and nonsemantic functions o f poetic rhythm, that is,
betw een those aspects which operate within the same space as the
meanings o f the poem ’s words, whether to reinforce, limit, expand, or
modify them , and those which operate on som e other axis,
contributing to the total working of the poem but not to its ‘m eaning’ in
the narrow sense. This distinction corresponds roughly to a distinction
ICONIC FUNCTIONS 287
that can be drawn betw een two ways in which verse challenges the
arbitrary but indissoluble link betw een signifier and signified on which
the linguistic sign, as envisaged by Saussure, depends: the first by
creating the illusion o f a peculiarly intimate connection betw een the
physical stuff o f language and its meanings rather than a
conventionally guaranteed coexistence, and the second by insisting on,
and taking advantage of, that arbitrariness. The first four sections o f
this chapter will deal with sem antic functions, the last two with
nonsem antic functions.
9.1 IC O N IC F U N C T IO N S
In considering the sem antic functions o f poetic rhythm, we are
enquiring into the various ways in which the substance o f language,
perceived as a dynamic phenom enon, can itself contribute to meaning,
independently o f the signifying procedures o f the words for which it
provides a physical vehicle. W e can subdivide these functions into two:
those which dirt externally oriented, and work by establishing relations
betw een the linguistic artefact and the world beyond it other than
those determ ined by the normal processes o f signification; and those
which are internally oriented, and work by highlighting or linking
elem ents within the poem and thereby m odifying its sem antic texture.
This section and the two that follow it will be concerned with the
former, and section 4 with the latter. W e shall look first at what can be
term ed iconic functions; that is, devices which depend on som e
perceptible resemblance betw een the physical properties o f language
and external reality.
Faced with the unprofitable task o f com m enting on the part played
by rhythm in the m eaning o f a poem , critics often turn to the notion o f
‘imitative form ’, but the apparent safety o f this refuge am ong the
shifting sands o f metrical analysis may exist only in the eager
imagination o f the com m entator and the lazy assent of his reader. The
monitory specim en o f such criticism given by Dr. Johnson, one o f the
few trustworthy guides in this treacherous territory, cannot be quoted
too often. Johnson imagines his straw critic, D ick Minim, praising
som e lines from H udibras:
288 THE FUNCTIONS OF POETIC RHYTHM
imperious need to create a m etatext to set beside the original text, will
often seize upon the features o f a poem m ost easily pointed to and
talked about, instead o f attending scrupulously to the act o f reading
and the mental habits on which it is based. I am not suggesting that we
exclude em blem atic interpretation from the critical repertoire, but
that we recognise it for what it is - and that we recognise further that,
since there is no limit im posed by the actual processes o f reading, the
number o f em blem atic devices which can be ascribed to a text is
infinite. Why not look for emblematic appropriateness in the number of
words in each sentence, or in the patterns made by letters with tails, or
in the use o f earlier or later sections o f the alphabet? A further
dim ension is added by metaphorical slippage, as Johnson was well
aware: ‘The fancied resem blances, I fear, arise som etim es merely from
the ambiguity o f words; there is supposed to be som e relation betw een
a so ft line and soft couch, or betw een hard syllables and hard fortune’
(Life o f P o p e). It is largely the prevailing conventions o f criticism, not
those o f literature, which render som e em blem atic interpretations
more plausible than others: we may find ourselves accepting an
emblematic analysis o f a rhythmic feature not because it tallies with our
experience o f the poem , but because it conform s to our notions of what
such an analysis is em pow ered to say. A problem remains, however, in
that our reading o f a line may be permanently altered by an
em blem atic analysis (even one we find implausible); it is partly in this
fashion, o f course, that the practice o f criticism can push iconic devices
over the dividing line betw een the em blem atic and the mimetic. One
way in which em blem atic devices can function as a genuine part of the
reading activity is by drawing attention to the role o f convention itself;
if, for instance, we were to encounter in a poem the word grass printed
in green ink, any pleasure we experienced w ould not arise because the
text represented so realistically the colours o f the real world, but
because it played with the norms o f textual representation. This would
not be a sem antic contribution to the poem , however; it is closer to the
type o f function we shall discuss in section 6 o f this chapter.
A s far as rhythm is concerned, the em blem atic function which has
been m ost important in the history o f verse is a general one, located in
the fact o f metrical organisation itself: the N eoplatonic notion that
language which obeys the rules o f a strict metre represents an ideal
reality governed by order and harmony. The heading o f the final book
o f St A ugustine’s D e Musica (tr. Taliaferro, 1947, p. 324) presents this
idea in a nutshell: ‘The mind is raised from the consideration of
290 THE FUNCTIONS OF POETIC RHYTHM
or, at m ost, call up associations with other signs within the system. If we
are to respond, say, to / s / not just as one phonem e entering into
significant relations with others, but as a noise produced by the
expulsion o f air through the teeth which may in its physical
characteristics resem ble other noises, the text must in som e way bring
this aspect o f language into the reader’s interpretative activity. Two
ways o f doing this are by strongly patterning the sounds, and by
drawing attention at the level of content to the manner in which they
are produced. N abokov uses both o f these in the opening o f Humbert
H um bert’s narration in L o lita :
-s +s -s +s -s +s -s +s - s +s
(2) When I do count the clock that tells the time
(7) The tip o f the tongue taking a trip o f three steps down the
o B o B o B o B o B o B o
palate to tap, at three, on the teeth.
B o B o B o B
A s a more typical exam ple o f mim etic rhythm, we may consider the
opening o f L aw rence’s nine-line poem , ‘Brooding G rief’:
The poem opens in a simple m etre, making use o f the base rules alone:
each stress is separated from its neighbour by one or two nonstresses,
realising a three-beat metrical pattern (or four-beat with one beat
unrealised). But the third line ends unexpectedly with two consecutive
stresses, and there is no easy way of relating these to a metrical pattern.
Either the first w ould have to be metrically subordinated, as shown,
which preserves the three-beat pattern but contradicts the sem antic
emphasis, or both w ould have to be given weight, to allow the line an
extra beat through the agency o f the m ost com plex o f deviations, an
implied offbeat. N ote how a continuation o f the previous rhythm
would have a quite different effect:
The uncertainty, which com es about only because a strict metrical set is
rapidly established, and which no manipulation o f the voice can
overcom e, is crucial to the effect: the rhythm, in a quite literal sense,
‘stands still’, as the alternating pattern is momentarily suspended,
before beginning again with even greater regularity in the following
line. Though it has no m imetic function, the double alliteration on
‘start’ - ‘stand’ - ‘still’ heightens attention to the stressed syllables
responsible for the change in m ovem ent. It will be evident that the
rhythm provides nothing but regular m otion o f a certain kind and
sudden stasis: the words as linguistic signs imbue the sequence with
specific m eaning, though this m eaning is in its turn reinforced and
perhaps generalised by the rhythm. (The halt o f ‘stand still’ also has a
structural function, closing the first section o f the poem and providing
a pivot on which the time sequence turns; this function o f rhythm is the
subject o f section 4 below .) This is not a particularly subtle instance of
rhythmic im itation, which is why it is relatively easy to discuss; as an
exam ple o f a more difficult question, one might ask whether the
placing o f ‘H ops’ after an enjam bm ent can be regarded as imitative,
since in this case the rhythmic formation would pass unremarked if it
did not em body a semantically striking word.
By their very nature, as largely unconscious elem ents in the reading
process w hose function is m erely to intensify or modify the meanings
already given by the language, true mimetic effects remain for the m ost
part inaccessible to conscious appraisal and precise analysis. In
proportion as the reader becom es aware o f them , and is able to
pinpoint their operation, so they tend towards the em blem atic. This is
especially true o f effects o f sound: the more overtly imitative they are,
the more they strike the reader’s notice, and the less directly and
im m ediately they function to suggest qualities o f the outside world. We
enjoy the heightened awareness of the sounds o f language produced by
Tennyson’s ‘murmuring o f innumerable b ees’, but it is unlikely that
any reader hears, as he pronounces the words, a distant buzzing.
(H um bert’s paean to Lolita is a special case, because in denying the
reader his habitual and com fortable unconsciousness o f the organs
whereby he speaks, the language actually heightens attention to its
subject m atter.) Rhythm ic effects can work in this way too; P ope’s
famous dem onstrations o f mim etic rhythm in the Essay on Criticism
can be savoured because they are demonstrations, momentarily
bringing to consciousness our usually automatic responses to the
m ovem ents of the spoken language. In cases like these, the degree to
which iconic resem blances fall short o f precise imitation is probably as
AFFECTIVE FUNCTIONS 295
important as the degree to which they achieve it, and they belong
properly with our consideration o f nonsem antic functions in section 6.
9.2 A F F E C T IV E F U N C T IO N S
Plato’s view o f the distinctiveness o f poetic language, it will be recalled,
was that it possesses a special power to induce em otional responses; for
him this was a reason for banishing poets from the republic, but the
notion o f metre as a m eans o f increasing the affective force o f language
has survived without pejorative overtones to becom e one o f the m ost
prevalent conceptions o f it. Y et it is difficult to account for this
com m on view in terms o f iconic functions: it is true that they add
semantic intensity and com plexity to language, but when one thinks of
the deeper levels o f mental experience with which poetry is assumed to
engage, they appear to operate at a relatively superficial level. Let us
turn yet once more to Johnson for guidance: ‘The m easure or time o f
pronouncing may be varied so as very strongly to represent, not only
the m odes o f external m otion, but the quick or slow succession of
ideas, and consequently the passions o f the m ind.’ {The R am bler, N o.
94) It is precisely the ‘passions o f the m ind’ that we are concerned with
in this section, and to which I am applying the broad term ‘affective’:
the em otions, attitudes, and m odes o f thought that constitute mental
experience (and the bodily experience that feeds and realises it). Is it
possible to develop Johnson’s suggestion that the m ovem ent of verse
can represent not only the outside world but this inner world as well?
A human voice reading a line o f poetry sounds much more like other
Jiuman voices speaking in other situations than it does any non-human
sound, such as the galloping o f a horse or the rushing o f a brook. The
m ost im m ediate kind o f representational power which poetic rhythm
possesses, therefore, is one which we ignored in our discussion of
iconic functions: the ability to reflect not the external reality being
spoken o f but the rhythms that characterise the act o f speaking itself;
and as these rhythms frequently serve to express the speaker’s mental
state, they are an obvious source o f affective signification in poetry.
Using C. S. Peirce’s well-known categorisation o f signs, one might
regard a rhythmic feature that functions in this way as an index of
extra verbal reality, rather than an icon; that is, it signifies som ething
other than itself not because it resem bles that other thing, but because
it is a direct product o f it.
296 THE FUNCTIONS OF POETIC RHYTHM
The following line eases the tension and leads into the only five-beat
line in the poem , w hose rhythmic regularity, strong alternations of
stressed and unstressed syllables (supported by alliteration and a
simple syntactic structure), and final hint o f the triple rhythm that has
played throughout the poem , all contribute, together with its unusual
length, to an isochronic insistence that em bodies a slow, steady release
of affective energy:
There may appear to be a clear distinction betw een poetry that uses
rhythm in this way to em body the mental state of a fictive speaker and
poetry that uses rhythm to imitate not the word but the world; in
practice, however, the two functions merge, since a poem that imitates
external reality in its rhythmic form may at the same time be
em bodying that very habit o f speech: we often im pose on our
utterances physical features which mimic the subject of our words - as
when we speak rapidly while talking o f a quick succession o f events, or
slowly while describing a sluggish m ovem ent. In such cases, we may
well be sim ultaneously expressing som e quality o f em otion: not just a
rapid set o f occurrences, but the associated excitem ent; not merely
leaden m otion, but the boredom it produces. So the rhythm of
Florim el’s lyrical praise o f Perdita imitates the grace o f her
m ovem ents, as is often observed, but it also em bodies his rapture;
while T ennyson’s long day waning and M arvell’s winged chariot
hurrying both represent mental states rather than the passage o f time.
The regularity o f (2) is more interesting as a reflection o f the speaker’s
m ood than o f the operation o f clockwork; and we may feel that the
rhythm o f the phrase ‘stand still’ in (3) does not merely imitate the
speaker’s m ovem ents, but adumbrates the psychological shock given
overt expression later in the poem: the irruption o f self-consciousness
into the lulling cocoon o f memory.
N or does there have to be an imagined speaker w hose speech
rhythms are em bodied in the line for it to convey mental conditions in
this way. There is more to M ilton’s description o f Satan in prospect of
Eden than a m im etic representation o f the m ovem ents o f his facial
muscles:
(5) Thus while he spake, each passion dimm ed his face
Thrice changed with pale, ire, envy, and despair,
AFFECTIVE FUNCTIONS 299
verse has - perhaps even more, since it can range more freely in its
search for imitative devices - and can em body a variety o f affective
speech rhythms, it is the approximation to regular rhythms, and the
consequent play o f arousal and satisfaction, which engages the deepest
sources o f affective behaviour: those neural and muscular periodicities
that generate all mental and physical activity.
9.3 A S S O C IA T IV E F U N C T IO N S
W hereas an iconic effect relies on som e physical resem blance betw een
language and the rest o f the world (an analogy I used earlier was a
black cross representing a crossroads), and an affective rhythm serves
as an index o f a mental state (in the way that skid-marks indicate a
dangerous bend), an associative connection - which Peirce rather
misleadingly called ‘sym bolic’ - is one which depends entirely on an
acquired disposition to relate diverse phenom ena (like the red triangle
which signifies a major road). That is to say, convention is responsible
not merely for sanctioning as meaningful one out o f many similarities
betw een a linguistic form and external reality, as is the case with iconic
representation, but for instituting a connection where there is no basis
in resem blance at all. A m ong the conventions o f language, the
equivalent distinction is betw een m otivated signs - those which make
use of onom atopoeia, for instance - and the unm otivated or arbitrary
signs that constitute the bulk of our speech; iconic effects in poetry can
be said to increase the degree o f m otivation in language, while
associative effects extend the system o f unm otivated signs. O f course,
there is a large area o f poetic signification in which the resem blances
betw een poetic devices and the reality with which we associate them
are so slight that a judgement as to whether they are merely
fortuitous, or perhaps just pegs on which, historically, conventional
associations have been hung, is im possible (and perhaps em pty).
Attem pts to ascertain exactly the degree o f m otivation in language
founder in the same zone o f uncertainty.
Because literary associations becom e so thoroughly naturalised, we
are more likely to underestim ate than overestim ate their importance,
and to think that we are responding to iconic relations when we are
simply obeying long-established habits o f association. A s an exam ple,
let us take the response which triple metre evokes in m ost readers
today : we are likely to feel that it is peculiarly suited to light, humorous
ASSOCIATIVE FUNCTIONS 301
verse, and unsuited to a serious engagem ent with the painful aspects of
experience. There seem s to be justification for this reaction in the
nature o f the rhythm itself: the double offbeats make for a light, rapid
m ovem ent with both a m imetic and an affective dim ension; the
insistence o f the rhythm tends to override natural speech patterns and
so limit em otional expressiveness; and there is a certain artificiality
about the linguistic structures that have to be used in order to avoid the
alternating stress contours that characterise the language. Cowper’s
‘Poplar-Field’ if often criticised for such reasons:
(6 ) Tw elve years have elapsed since I first took a view
O f my favourite field and the bank where they grew;
A nd now in the grass behold they are laid,
A nd the tree is my seat that once lent me a shade.
Y et we cannot simply dismiss this poem as an exam ple o f the
unfortunate results o f using a metre inappropriate to the subject. N ot
only was it much admired, it was also much imitated: H ollander, in his
suggestive study o f metrical conventions (1 9 7 5 , Ch. 9), shows that the
use o f triple metre as a medium for sober, reflective writing flourished
in the nineteenth century side by side with its use for com ic and satiric
verse. W e must conclude that the elem ent o f purely conventional
association in our response to this metre is substantial, and that there is
nothing frivolous or jaunty in a triple m ovem ent p e r se. Once these
associations are evoked, however, the iconic and affective
consequences o f the metrical characteristics already m entioned com e
into play - but we can never be certain that we are not ascribing to them
sem antic powers for which the initial associations alone are
responsible.
Rhythmic associations are therefore primarily associations with
other poem s or other m anifestations o f rhythm. A triple rhythm
sounds cheerful because we associate it with the triple rhythms o f all
the cheerful poem s in such metres that we know; if, however, current
literary tastes were different, and we were steeped in the tradition o f
‘The Poplar-Field’ and its successors, we might have very different
associations, and find them equally natural.4 The contribution made by
the rhythms o f The Prelude to the total effect o f that poem depends
crucially on their relation to the rhythms o f Paradise L ost; and The
Prelude has in its turn provided rhythmic nuances that echo through
later poetry, all the m ore powerfully, perhaps, w hen they do so without
reaching consciousness. It is likely, too, that familiarity with The
302 THE FUNCTIONS OF POETIC RHYTHM
which are receiving reinforcement by the sound. The fact that verse is
more highly patterned than prose, and som e poem s more highly
patterned than others, is less important as an attribute of ‘beauty’ than
as a feature o f organisation, and will be discussed as such in section 5;
and there are more useful analogies to be drawn with music than those
based on vague notions o f m elodious utterance, one of which will be
touched on in the same section.
9.4 E M P H A SIS A N D C O N N E C T IO N
Rhythm participates in the greater sem antic density o f poetic language
not only by establishing its own connections betw een the poem and the
physical and mental world, but also by functioning within the poem as a
formal network that acts directly upon the semantic level by
emphasising or connecting individual elem ents in the text. One
obvious m ode o f rhythmic emphasis is the use o f variations to create
local tension: when the language slips sm oothly past on the wings o f a
regular rhythm no word or sequence receives prominence (an asset in
som e kinds o f song-like verse); but a deviation in an established
rhythm thrusts itself into the reader’s attention in a way that is
impossible in prose. This practice is endem ic in poetry at every level,
from the individual syllable or word rendered salient by rhythmic
tension to the line or stanza set apart by its metrical schem e. The last
line o f the follow ing stanza forces itself on the attention not only by
virtue o f the semantic contrast with what has gone before but also by
the rhythmic shift which it performs:
Rhythmic echoes no doubt work subliminally for the most part, and
only when they are m ade obvious by such m eans as Blake uses do we
becom e conscious o f them . But it is one o f the distinguishing
characteristics o f verse, and o f the habits by which we read it, that
changes or repetitions in the rhythmic texture are experienced not as
the random and contingent by-products o f a signifying system, but as
part o f the signifying process itself.
9.5 P A T T E R N A N D C O H E SIO N
A ll the functions o f metre we have looked at so far can be broadly
classified as m odes o f semantic reinforcement or modification: the
rhythmic features operate in the same field as the meanings conveyed
by the words, whether to strengthen or to modify them . This has
proved to be the type o f metrical function m ost am enable to critical
discussion, since rhythm is thereby assimilated to a notion o f poetry as
an expression o f certain truths about the world beyond it with a
subtlety or forcefulness denied to nonpoetic language. A lthough this
approach credits poetry with a certain kind o f distinctiveness - a
specially dense use o f language signalled, and in part created, by metre
- it provides only for an intensification o f the main business o f ordinary
language: the com m unication o f meaning from one individual human
consciousness to another. W e have seen that one function o f metre is in
fact to prise m eaning away from the notion o f a single speaker, but we
need to go further than this in considering its power to distance poetry
from ordinary language and ordinary experience: we need to exam ine
the ways in which poetic rhythm might operate quite separately from
the semantic content o f the lines it marshalls, preventing us from
taking that sem antic content as a simple statem ent about a familiar
reality perceived in familiar ways. In regular verse, metre is not
som ething that is called upon only in m om ents o f expressive need, but
a constant presence, sanctifying or stigmatising the language it marks
as different. W e shall be concerned in this section and the next with two
com plem entary views of the nonsem antic functions of rhythm, derived
ultimately from two conceptions o f art: as the provider o f reassuring
experiences o f order, and as the challenger o f settled assumptions.
PATTERN AND COHESION 307
9.6 F O R E G R O U N D IN G A N D T E X T U A L IT Y
The major tradition in the discussion o f metrical function is founded on
the assumption that a work o f art is characterised, perhaps defined, by
its unusual unity and cohesion, and metre is understood as contributing
to the closeness o f the links, both betw een the forms o f language and its
meanings and among the various parts o f the poem , that establish this
satisfying unity. It is a tradition with a long history, finding expression
in terms o f order and decorum in classical rhetoric and its R enaissance
successors, remaining potent in Augustan notions o f artistic rules and
imitative effects, and receiving a powerful new im petus in the
Rom antic theories o f organic form which today still dom inate aesthetic
thought and responses to art. In this century, however, an apparently
opposed view has gained strength, one which sees metre as a means of
unsettling the fixities normally sustained by language and challenging
our assumptions o f order and cohesion in the world and in ourselves.
The approaches to rhythmically ordered language that we have
considered in the previous sections tend to treat its distinctiveness
from other uses o f language as a m eans towards or a product of its
special semantic and aesthetic status; this approach, however, regards
that distancing itself as a prime function o f poetic rhythm.
The first body o f theory to exam ine systematically the differences
betw een poetic and nonpoetic language was Russian Formalism,
which laid em phasis on verse not as a means whereby language can
transcend the ordinary world, but as a verbal practice which
reinvigorates attention to language itself, and to the way in which
language constitutes that ordinary world as part o f our experience.6
W e grow accustom ed to the speech we use and hear around us every
day, and take for granted the easy passage from words to ideas; the
strange, organised language o f poetry de-autom atises and defamiliar-
lses that response, foregrounds the language itself rather than its
FOREGROUNDING AND TEXTUALITY 311
subject, establishes a set towards the medium and not the m essage, and
interrogates the connections betw een sounds and m eanings. Poetry
represents not a minimisation o f the arbitrariness obtaining betw een
signifier and signified, as a semantically oriented approach to verse
would imply, but an enforcem ent and exploitation o f it; our rush for
meaning is im peded, and we are obliged to acknowledge the
independence and value o f the linguistic properties we are usually so
eager to leave behind.
A ll formal devices in poetry serve this function by furnishing the text
with elem ents that cannot be incorporated into the kind of
interpretation we habitually give to linguistic utterances.7 In our
literary theory and criticism we all too easily ignore this dim ension and
fall back on sem antic properties or ideological content, and the
com m on em phasis on mim etic effects o f rhythm typifies this retreat
from what is distinctive about the language o f literature. But even
within the dom ain o f formal devices, metre should not simply be
regarded as one defamiliarising feature am ong many: it counters
singleness and sim pleness o f meaning in a particularly forceful way, by
organising and foregrounding not those elem ents o f language which
have a sem antic function - words and sentences, and the phonem es or
distinctive features out o f which these are constructed - but the
presemantic carrier o f speech, the rhythmic progression o f stressed
and unstressed syllables. It is because it belongs to this fundamental
level o f language that it can function so powerfully to imitate and
em body the outer and inner world, and to focus attention or provide
cohesion within the poetic structure, but it is for this reason too that it
can challenge so effectively the unconscious ease with which we
habitually produce and consum e our language.
More recently, critical theory has witnessed a developm ent o f this
position which m akes an even greater separation betw een the
language o f literature - or more generally o f the written text - and
other m odes o f discourse, and which lays an even stronger emphasis on
its function as a subverter o f the linguistic conventions by which we
make our world, and, som e o f its proponents would argue, are made
ourselves. The post-structuralist view o f the literary text as the site o f
an unending interplay o f unsettled m eanings would seem to leave little
room for the notion o f metrical verse as the moulding o f speech
rhythms into regular forms to create a distinctive poetic voice.8
H ow ever, we have at many points in this study been obliged to
question the assumption that verse is a representation o f an
312 THE FUNCTIONS OF POETIC RHYTHM
N otes
1. The following statements are representative o f many more: ‘Now this is very
profound, what rhythm is, and goes far deeper than words. A sight, an emotion,
creates this wave in the mind, long before it makes words to fit it* (Virginia Woolf,
1926, p. 247) ; ‘I know that a poem, or a passage of a poem, may tend to realise itself
first as a particular rhythm before it reaches expression in words, and that this
rhythm may bring to birth the idea and the image’ (T. S. Eliot, 1942, p. 28); ‘I feel
that poetry comes from the basic rhythmic structures of one’s body or mind’
(Richard Eberhart, 1973, p. 42); ‘Even before it is ready to change into language, a
NOTES 315
poem may begin to assert its buried life in the mind with wordless surges o f rhythm
and counter-rhythm’ (Stanley Kunitz, 1978, p. 284).
2. The reading of classical verse in England depended for centuries on a response to
the visual features of a text whose aural manifestation was largely unmetrical; see
Attridge (1974, Part One).
3. I give the text of the poem as published in Am ores (1916); for the Collected Poems
of 1928 Lawrence changed ‘traffic’ to ‘the city street’, lengthening the final line still
further, but diminishing its rhythmic insistence.
4. Lotman (1976, pp. 5 4 -5 ) produces some interesting Russian examples of the
historical changes in associations which rhythmic forms can undergo.
5. The most valuable discussion of this aspect of poetic form is by B. H. Smith (1968,
Ch. 1 and 2); its musical analogue is fully explored by Meyer (1956).
6. With regard to the study o f verse rhythm, the most important o f the Formalists were
Jakobson, Tomashevskij, and Tynjanov; a useful survey of their work is given by
Erlich (1955, Ch. 12).
7. Other aspects o f verse which work apart from and often counter to the semantic and
rational aspect are rhyme (see Wimsatt, 1954) and the visual dimension (see
Hollander, 1975, Ch. 12). For a full discussion o f the devices o f formal artifice
which set poetry apart from ordinary discourse, see Forrest-Thomson (1978).
8. See, for instance, the defence by Donoghue (1980), in reviewing some examples of
deconstructive criticism, of the common assumption that ‘in reading a poem you
think of the words on the page as a transcription o f a voice speaking’.
9. One o f the few metrical analysts to acknowledge this is Scott (1980), who questions
the ‘peculiar assumption that the scanner is duty bound to push verse towards a
single and definitive existence, which is the recited existence of verse’ (p. 5).
Chapter 10
10.1 N O N M E T R IC A L V E R SE
(1) Gasholders, russet among fields. M illdams, marlpools that lay
unstirring. Eel-swarms. Coagulations o f frogs: once, with
NONMETRICAL VERSE 317
It is all too easy to take a piece o f nonm etrical language and find
rhythmic ingenuities in it; the characteristic mixture o f repetition and
variety in the m ovem ent o f English produces flows and eddies, echoes
and inversions, that w ould look like the work o f a skilful designer if
they were not ubiquitous. Much discussion o f the rhythms o f literary
prose or free verse falls into this trap, and it is difficult to know how to
avoid it when the only certain rhythmic effects are the obvious and
therefore uninteresting ones. O ne precaution is to attem pt such an
analysis only with writing which possesses a distinctive aural character
recognisable am ong the myriad other arrangements o f the syllables o f
English; this is true o f G eoffrey H ill’s set o f prose-poem s, Mercian
H y m n s, and the above exam ple is typical o f the sequence in its
scrupulous control o f rhythmic form.
It is obvious that nonmetrical language does not m ake use o f beats in
the same way as its rhythmically regular counterpart; nevertheless, the
peaks o f energy on stressed syllables still function as the carriers o f a
fundamentally stress-tim ed rhythm, and prose that invites careful
enunciation by its sense and sound-patterns can exhibit many o f the
functions o f rhythmic form discussed in the previous chapter. In the
opening paragraph o f this poem , the language is at its m ost
concentrated and unprosaic, blending the eighth and twentieth
centuries in a depiction o f O ffa’s boyhood haunts; this feeling o f
powerful com pression is achieved partly by the sensory density o f the
images, partly by the ellipses o f syntax, and partly by the m anagem ent
318 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
The rhythm that em erges here is a familiar one in English poetry: the
consistent separation o f beats by single or double offbeats. There is
only one instance o f a triple offbeat, and the two implied offbeats occur
naturally at strong pauses. Otherwise, the only irregularity occurs on
the word ‘C oagulations’. The scansion I give shows the prom otion of
the second syllable, which carries a secondary stress, to a beat; this is
undoubtedly how the rhythm would make itself felt in regular verse,
but as prose it probably reads m ore naturally without a beat, creating a
temporary disturbance in the steady sequence o f alternations, a
sudden stutter o f syllables testifying to the ugliness and fascination of
the scene it refers to.
The rhythmicality o f the passage contributes to its sem antic and
affective intensity, evoking the young O ffa’s absorption in the
surroundings o f his ‘kingdom ’; but its effects can be more fully
specified. W hen we exam ine the grouping o f the words, we find that
the predominant rhythm is falling: m ost o f the more significant words
begin with a stress ( ‘Coagulations’ being a notable exception), and
because o f the syntactic om issions, sentences and phrases begin
im m ediately with important nouns. This is an intensifying device used
throughout M ercian H ym n s, to the extent that a falling rhythm
becom es part o f the work’s metrical signature, contributing to its
cohesion and distinctiveness. One variety o f this rhythm is especially
noticeable in this paragraph: the com pound w hose second syllable
carries som e degree of secondary stress, preventing the falling rhythm
from picking up speed - exam ples here are ‘milldams’, ‘m arlpools’,
‘Eel-swarm s’, and perhaps ‘half-brick’. A fter its slow opening,
NONMETRICAL VERSE 319
The last six words, with their concentration o f stresses, linger over the
320 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
+S -s -s +S +S ~S +S
Crosses the brown land, unheard. The nymphs are departed.
o B o B o
But E liot does more than half-reproduce the rhythms o f the iambic
tradition, he reproduces som e o f its fam ous lines; and it is instructive to
observe how they are transformed by their new context. Spenser’s line,
in its setting in ‘Prothalam ion’, relies on two dem otions and a
prom otion for its metrical acceptability as an iambic pentameter:
Sweet Thames run softly, till I end my spng
o B 0 B 0 B 0 B o B
A s it is, the five-beat rhythm remains only a familiar ghost behind the
arras, one o f the multiplicity o f associations which constitute in large
measure the poem ’s m ode o f meaning.
10.2 F O U R -B E A T V E R S E
(3) Bidderes and beggeres faste aboute yede
Till hire bely and hire bagge were bretful ycrammed;
Flite thanne for hire foode, foughten at the ale.
In glotonye, god w oot, go thei to bedde,
A nd risen up with ribaudie as Roberdes knaves;
Sleep and sleuthe seweth hem evere.
Pilgrymes and Palmeres plighten hem togidere
For to seken Seint lam e and Seintes at Rom e;
W enten forth in hire w ey with many wise tales,
A nd hadden leve to lyen al hire lif after.
that o f the 4.3 .4 .3 stanza. Perhaps the best way o f putting it is that as
we m ove from one line to the next, the absence o f a fourth beat
produces a slight degree o f tension, and this tension contributes to the
unease that hovers about even the apparently ordinary descriptions in
the opening o f the poem . (See Ch. 4, exam ple (2 2 ), for another use o f
this form.) The variation in nonstresses betw een beats - usually two or
one, occasionally none - further diminishes the prominence o f the
rhythm, and prevents a clear triple or duple m ovem ent from emerging:
The aptness o f this metrical form for the poem is obvious. It allows
both the easy flexibility o f the detached observer’s m editation and the
intense rhythmicality o f the admirer’s incantation. It enacts the
transformation which is its subject: the quotidian becom es the
remarkable, the casual becom es the com pulsive. The local effects are
just as telling, such as the emphasis given to ‘grey’, with all that it
implies, by the tension o f the run-on already described, or the
em blem atic awkwardness o f the implied offbeat in
scansions, one derived from the strong-stress aspect of the metre, the
other from its accentual-syllabic, rhyming aspect:
Are changed, changed utterly
o B o B oB o [B]
o B o B o B [o B]
N o single reading can satisfy these contradictory dem ands, and the
resulting rhythmic tension thrusts the line into prom inence, enacts its
own difficult transition, and em bodies a strong, though unspecific,
em otional response. A line which retained the rhythmic shape o f its
predecessors would have a totally different effect:
Are changed in every way
o B o B o B
The closing line presents the results o f the change, rhythmically as well
as semantically: it is almost songlike in character, with its upbeat, triple
rhythm, firm stresses, alliteration, and perfect rhyme:
A terrible beauty is born
o B o B o B
The adjective reflects the speaker’s continuing detachm ent from the
events he is contem plating, but the rhythm informs us that at another
level all doubts are silenced.
It is the familiar 4 x 4 structure, and the triple metre is alm ost wholly
regular; that is, a strong preference is shown for double offbeats, and
little use is made o f prom otion and dem otion. (Im plied offbeats, it will
be rem em bered, are in any case foreign to triple m etre.) The metrical
pattern alternates betw een 4B and 4B o, a form with which we are
already familiar in duple verse (see Ch. 4, exam ples (4 8 ) and (5 4 )),
and one which encourages a falling rhythm, further encouraged here
by the repeated phrase in line 1. Only at the very end o f the stanza does
Hardy avail him self o f the possible m ethods o f muting a triple rhythm,
by substituting a single offbeat for the expected double offbeat. Y et the
rhythm does not dom inate the language; it is rapid, but not insistent.
One reason is that it is achieved by the use o f syntactic sequences that
fall naturally but not mechanically into triple patterns, rather than by
polysyllables with their stronger contrast betw een stress and
nonstresses (apart from two disyllables, the entire stanza is
m onosyllabic). The fact that the stanza is a single sentence also
provides som e syntactic justification for the rapid m ovem ent. The
affective speech rhythm which the verse em bodies is relaxed and
freely-flowing: it has neither the tension nor the heavy beats that
characterise different kinds o f em otional stress. The repetition at the
end o f the first line contributes to the stanza’s tone o f wonderm ent - we
say a thing twice if w e find it hard to believe - and mimetically
introduces a hint o f the repeated cry itself, with a triple rhythm that
suggests a sound dying away (echoed two lines later in an attenuated
form by m eans o f a rhyme which merely drops the phrase’s first
consonant).
FOUR-BEAT VERSE 331
A t the end o f the third line here, the stress which the sense demands on
‘then’ increases the tension (as Leavis pointed out w hen discussing the
poem in N ew Bearings), since the rules o f triple metre do not permit
dem otion in this position. There follow s a line which presents the
strongest threat to the metre so far, in the occurrence o f four
nonstresses betw een the first two beats. Either we give them all syllabic
value, which suspends the metre for this part o f the line, or we use
elision, which preserves the m etre, but creates a different kind o f
tension:
(S) “ SA
Even to the original air-blue gown!
B o B o B o B
The end o f the line deviates in the opposite direction, though less
markedly: as in all three o f the longer stanzas, the last offbeat is
monosyllabic, but in this case it involves a degree o f dem otion as well,
creating a final cadence with a slower, heavier m ovem ent.
This rhythmic change in the last line-and-a-half o f the stanza does
not have any iconic reference to the air-blue gown, though it does work
affectively: the speaker is no longer bem used by an aural image but
excited by a visual one, and the words lose their easy flow for the
sharper contours o f em otional utterance. The rhythmic tension helps
to convey a sense that the visitation is not only a cause for joy, but a
cause (and ultimately the result) o f deep sorrow; and when the vision
evaporates and the poet faces the objective reality o f his situation the
rhythm actually becom es easier again, at least for a while. Hardy
m akes the triple rhythm at the start o f the third stanza expressive o f a
kind o f feebleness o f energy; it is possible, and appropriate, to read the
first line with ten unstressed and only two fully stressed syllables,
leaving prom otion to take care o f the other beats:
332 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
+s +s
Or is it only the breeze, in its listlessness
B o B o B o B o
We have not returned to the lilting m elody o f the first stanza, however:
in the lines that follow , two strong m onosyllables require dem otion:
We are in a different rhythmic world; this is, o f course, the voice o f the
speaker in full self-consciousness, and we realise that all that has gone
before is in quotation marks. The second line is in regular duple m etre,
and provides a context within which we could have interpreted the
previous line, but now it is too late:
FOUR-BEAT VERSE 333
N or are we allow ed to settle into the new metre, because the third line
returns us to the rhythm o f the first three stanzas: triple verse with a
tense dem otion on ‘oozing’, and a single offbeat before the final beat,
like the last line in each o f the earlier stanzas:
The only deviation used is the least com plex one, prom otion, which by
preventing strong alternations, contributes to the quiet confidence o f
the utterance. N otice that the metrical pattern has initial offbeats only
in the second and fourth lines, and no final offbeats; this enhances the
rhythmic simplicity by providing each line with a final beat and each
half of the quatrain with an initial beat, and by easing the progression
from line to line within each half, while articulating the division
between the two halves.
The second part o f the stanza introduces a less com forting notion
(though its ostensible purpose is to reassure), and the steady
FOUR-BEAT VERSE 335
m ovem ent receives a slight shock. This is experienced not in the use of
rhythmic variation within the line, but by a change in the metrical
pattern to a two-beat line followed by three three-beat lines,
accompanied by a shift from an abab rhyme scheme to abba. Though it is
not until the last line that deviation is used to disturb the progress o f the
rhythm and provide a local emphasis, the quatrain as a whole has the
effect o f moving into a new key for the statement o f a second, more
sombre, subject:
Because o f the shortness o f the first line, the absence o f any full
four-beat units, and the strong run-ons,there is no clear sense o f
unrealised beats; in fact, the syntactic structure encourages the
em ergence o f a counterpointed four-beat rhythm to threaten the
grouping determ ined by the line-divisions, and there is even a
half-rhyme to reinforce the alternative alignment:
Again prom otion helps to keep the rhythm light, and the first line o f
each pair runs easily into the second. Then, once m ore, the short line
brings a sense o f modulation, as the speaker turns to his own promised
return, and this tim e a new rhythmic effect is used to undermine the
assured prediction that follows. Our expectation is that the sixth line
will begin with a single offbeat, like all the even-num bered lines so far,
and this seem s to receive confirmation in the word ‘B ut’. H ow ever, this
is follow ed im m ediately by another nonstress, implying a double
offbeat, though one which breaks the metrical rules so far observed by
being unpaired and unelidable:
A t least the line does contain the anticipated three beats - but the
following line throws even this into doubt, since it unexpectedly and
unambiguously contains four beats, and appears to begin with an initial
inversion that implies the normal iambic conventions:
The only way to match these two lines is to use an initial prom otion in
the first and elision in the second, and reinterpret the metrical pattern
as a fundamentally four-beat one, involving an unrealised beat in the
last line:
Then fear not me,
o B o B
But believe that I shall make
B o B o B o B
-s
Speedier journeys, since I take
B o B o B o B
More wings and spurs than he.
O B o B O B [o B]
It cannot be
That thou lov’st m e, as thou sayst,
B o B o B o B
i JL
If in thine my life thou waste,
B o B o B o B
Thou art the best of me.
Only in the fifth and final stanza does metrical certainty return: the
first line o f the pair is once more am biguous in precisely the same way,
but the second has four unequivocal beats, allowing the poem to close
with a revival o f its early confidence:
It is worth adding that, in spite o f the poem ’s title, both the rhythmic
shaping o f the w hole work and the local effects o f uncertainty and
tension w ould be lost in a musical setting that im posed the same
metrical framework on each stanza.
338 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
s. [s]
If hate killed m en, Brother Lawrence
o B o B o B o B o
rhythm provides the perfect vehicle. Lines 5 -7 are more regular than
the previous three lines, the only deviations being two dem oted beats,
and only the second o f these disturbs the evenness o f the alternations:
What, your myrtle-bush wants trimming?
B o B o B o B o
Oh, that rose has prior claims -
B o B o Bo B
N eeds its leaden vase filled brimming?
B o B o B o B o
Other stanzas o f the poem also use rhythmic complexity for climactic
endings; stanza 2, for instance, closes with these lines:
O ne o f the sim plest forms in English poetry - in one sense o f the word
‘sim ple’ - is the 4 x 4 duple stanza which m akes free use o f initial
offbeats and unrealised beats, and thereby resists classification as
iambic or trochaic, tetram eter or trimeter. The use o f such an
elem entary form suits this poem ’s character as a moral fable, a paring
down to essentials o f a com plex human event in order to enforce it as
an illustration o f a general truth, without taking away any o f its
complexity. That truth, I take it, is one which Blake frequently
expounds: love in all its true power is som ething one dare not frankly
show to the world; only the m ethod o f indirection, o f secrecy, m eets
with acceptance. B lake’s use o f a form in which society com m only
encapsulates its truisms - in hymns and in children’s rhymes - in order
to attack those truisms is another exam ple o f the associative function
o f metrical form: here the purpose is to expose what it brings to mind,
rather than to claim kinship.
The rules by m eans o f which this poem ’s metre can be specified are
appropriately sim ple. The base rules are used without the option
allowing double offbeats; and though prom otion occurs normally,
neither the dem otion rule nor the im plied offbeat rule apply. In other
words, the general set is cut off after the third rule, indicating a metre
that realises the underlying rhythm with unusual directness. The 4 x 4
rhythmic structure allows unrealised offbeats in the usual places in the
first and third lines, and the metrical pattern can therefore be stated as
follows:
342 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
((o )) B o B o B ( [ o B])
The rhythm clearly suits the content, but the logical connection
implied by ‘For’ is not clear to the reader: we have m oved off at a
tangent from the apparent obviousness o f the opening sentim ents into
a mysterious region which the rest o f the poem will have to account for.
A fter the generalised moral com es the exem plification in personal
experience, and the rhythm changes once more: the voice again
becom es em phatic, this tim e in an insistent rising rhythm based on
pairs o f m onosyllables mirroring the obsessive repetition, and using
initial offbeats for the first time:
/ - \ / n r f
The fourth beat we have been led to expect does not com e this time;
instead, there is a tense pause, followed by the climactic line o f the
poem:
FOUR-BEAT VERSE 343
This line is as metrically simple as the opening lines o f the poem , but
the relation o f the linguistic structures to the metrical pattern is very
different. The opening word - hardly the expected response to a
confession o f love - abruptly reverses the rising rhythm o f the previous
two lines, and in place o f the unified or medially divided structure, this
line has a break after the first offbeat, follow ed by an isolated, and
therefore prom inent, m onosyllable. Instead o f the expected rhyme
with the first line, ‘lo v e’ is answered by ‘fears’. The rhythmically simple
final line tells the inevitable consequences in equally simple language,
now in the present tense, as if the m om ent is continually relived:
But the third line destroys this dipodic rhythm com pletely, and it is
specifically on the word ‘cold’ that the dislocation occurs, since it
demands as much weight as the first stress. This helps to account for the
way in which the line, and one word in particular, stands out from the
rhythmic sequence, in spite o f the regular realisation o f the metre. The
highlighting o f the line is sealed by the reinstatement o f the dipodic
rhythm im m ediately after it, to continue strongly to the end o f the
poem .
The opening line o f the final stanza has a regular rhythm but no
words which require special em phasis, as if the trauma o f the second
344 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
stanza is now over; the events have been replaced in the past, and the
p oet’s ironic control is reasserted:
The rhythm lightens still further in the second line, which has only one
strong stress, preparing us for a repetition o f the poem ’s gentlest line,
now m oved into the pivotal third line position:
A traveller came by
o B o B o B [o B]
Silently, invisibly
Bo B o B o B
The m ysterious wind o f the first stanza has now received em bodim ent
in a type o f human individual, the secretive lover so hated by Blake.
The effect o f the rival’s very different approach needs no elaborate
statement:the metre is again reduced to essentials, and this time the
syntax with it, for the baldest enunciation of willing acceptance:
O, was no deny.
B o B o B [o B]
The silence in which the final beat occurs is as eloquent as any o f the
sounds that have preceded it.
10.3 F IV E -B E A T V E R SE
(9) They flee from me that som etim e did me seek,
With naked foot stalking in my chamber.
I have seen them gentle, tam e, and m eek,
That now are wild, and do not remember
That som etim e they put them self in danger
To take bread at my hand; and now they range,
Busily seeking with a continual change.
Thanked be fortune, it hath been otherwise
Twenty tim es better; but once in special,
In thin array after a pleasant guise,
W hen her loose gown from her shoulders did fall,
A nd she me caught in her arms long and small,
Therewithal sweetly did me kiss,
A nd softly said, ‘D ear heart, how like you this?’
FIVE-BEAT VERSE 345
It is the presence o f lines like these that unsettles the sense of regularity
established by the clear five-beat lines, and renders am biguous
other lines which might be perceived as five-beat in a more
hom ogeneous context. The line that follow s these two could be read as
a pentameter:
To take bread at my hand; and now they range
0 B 0 B o B o B o B
A nother regular pentam eter is the following, and there is no doubt that
its rocking rhythm contributes to the satisfaction it expresses:
And softly said, ‘Dear heart, how like you this?’
o B o B o B 0 B 0 B
The follow ing line, on the other hand, gets som e o f its power from its
avoidance o f a regular pentam eter rhythm: it hovers betw een a
four-beat and a five-beat pattern:
It was no dream: I lay broad waking
o Bo B o B o B o
B o B 6 B
The rising rhythm is obstructed by the initial strong stress; and the
syntactic relation o f the first word to the second, parallel adjectives
qualifying the same noun, m eans that the dem otion is in no way made
sm ooth by natural subordination. The first unstressed offbeat is
realised by a minimum of phonetic substance, since ‘ruined’ is not
strongly disyllabic, and indeed the next word has a similar com plex
FIVE-BEAT VERSE 349
The two lines that follow retain the sm ooth m ovem ent, though ‘black’
is given som e prom inence by its occurrence as an offbeat, slowing the
line down on its most om inous word:
A s after sunset fadeth in the west,
0 B 0 B 0 B 0 B o B
Which by and by black night doth take away
o Bo B o B 0 B 0 B
Then once m ore the final line o f the quatrain opens with an initial
dem otion on a word that dem ands attention, and thereby opposes the
pressure o f the metre. In addition, the clustering o f consonants forces a
pause betw een ‘D eath ’s’ and ‘second’, and alliteration and assonance
highlight the phrase still further, and echo through the remainder o f
the line:
A s in its counterpart in the first quatrain, the line m oves from a salient
three-word, tw o-beat phrase into a sm ooth, predominantly rising
rhythm, but this tim e it is not for a nostalgic vision o f the past but a look
forward to what seem s a desired consum m ation.
The third quatrain opens with another new beginning that repeats
earlier ones, this tim e an exact reproduction o f the rhythm, and many
o f the words, o f the first line o f the second quatrain; and it too is
follow ed by a regular line, using only prom otion to lighten the
m ovement:
In me thou seest the glowing of such fire
o B o B o B 0 B 0 B
That on the ashes of his youth doth lie
o B 0 B 0 B 0 B o B
But the pattern o f recurrence is broken when the idea o f death, which
has been pressing more and m ore forcefully into the poem , is
introduced a line earlier; and it occurs this tim e not with a dem otion
but a stress-final pairing. M oreover, the two beats fall on the two
com ponents o f a com pound (see the discussion above, pp. 2 7 7 -8 ),
requiring, for the modern reader at least, an unusual em phasis on the
w hole word. The rest o f the line, and the closing line o f the quatrain,
falls into a weak alternating rhythm with only three more strong
stresses, as if enacting the expiration they refer to, or, more accurately,
em bodying the affective rhythms o f a speaker losing his determination
and energy:
Instead, the rhythm creates what we might call a balanced pentam eter,
in which the first two and last two beats are realised by stresses and the
middle beat is realised by a prom oted nonstress. The balanced line is
still usually divided by a pause betw een the second and third beats,
though this is no longer an essential feature, since the three successive
nonstresses at the centre act as a structural divider. The character o f
the balanced line is quite different from that o f the sprung line: the
medial prom oted nonstress acts as a kind o f pivot, and the speed o f the
sections on either side is roughly the sam e. The effect is less that o f
winding up and release than o f thesis and antithesis, and the sense is
frequently concentrated into two significant words on each side o f the
central beat. It is a useful structure with which to close a group o f lines,
providing a firm and settled ending, and is m ore suited to this position
than the sprung pentam eter, with its quicker, unemphatic second
354 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
section. The line in question does in fact round off the first four lines o f
the paragraph (P ope is of course sensitive to the natural tendency o f
couplets to group them selves in pairs), and the next line begins a new
four-line group. The rhythm o f these four lines is highly regular, with
only a couple o f prom otions to vary the firm alternation o f stress and
nonstress; the first three m ake little use o f internal structuring, while
the group closes once m ore on a balanced line:
This regularity gives the lines a rhythmic sobriety which matches the
grimmer vision they offer.
The em otional intensity o f the poetry is growing, and the group o f
lines that follow s shows an increase in metrical variation. It begins with
a balanced line w hose rhythm acquires a distinctive colouring through
the additional emphasis given to each half, the first by m eans o f a
dem otion, and the second by means o f two contrastive stresses; the
weight on each side o f the balance is increased, as it were:
s. s.
As hags hold sabbaths, less for joy than spite
o B o B o B o B o B
The irony o f the statem ent becom es fully apparent only in the lines that
follow , however. The first o f these uses stress-final pairing (relatively
rare in this strict metrical style) to bring out the contrast beneath the
apparent parallel:
Youth is given the easy rhythm, age the distorted one. There follow
two matching sprung lines, their structure again heightened by initial
inversion and prom oted fourth beats, and, like the second line o f the
passage, by repeated phrases betw een the major beats:
The strength o f feeling cannot be m issed, but its exact quality resists
categorisation: one is tem pted to say that it is a fusion o f intense scorn
with equally intense com passion. Such a union could occur only at the
deepest level o f affective experience, well below the specific em otions
356 RHYTHM AT WORK: SOME EXAMPLES
M E T R IC A L R U L E S: T H E G E N E R A L SET
The following is a summary in verbal and formal terms o f the metrical
rules that constitute the general set for English accentual-syllabic
verse. R eferences are given to the main discussions of individual topics.
B: beat
o: offbeat
(o): optional offbeat: neutral preference
(o ): optional offbeat: preference for inclusion
((o)): optional offbeat: preference for om ission
[B]: unrealised beat
([B]): optional unrealised beat
Formal conventions
In the formal statem ents o f the rules that follow the verbal statem ents,
the following symbols are used in addition to those given above:
O ptions which include bracketed item s are ordered later than options
which do not.
Duple metre
BA SE R U LES (7.2)
(1) Beat rule
A stressed syllable may realise a beat.
+ s —►B
Expanded:
(i) (single offbeat) - s -> o (preferred)
(ii) (double offbeat) - s —s o
(For verse which varies freely betw een single and double offbeats, the
form o f the rule is —s ( —s) —> o.)
D E V IA T IO N RU LES
(4 ) D em otion rule (7 .4 )
A stressed syllable may realise an offbeat when it occurs
betw een two stressed syllables, or after a line-boundary and
before a stressed syllable.
+ s —►o / + s, # +s
Expanded:
(i) + s —> o / 4 - s +s
(ii) + s —►o / # +s
CO NDITIO NS
(1 ) Pairing conditions (7 .6 )
(a) Im plied offbeat condition
A n im plied offbeat may occur only (i) when it is
im m ediately preceded or follow ed by a non-final double
360 APPENDIX: RULES AND SCANSION
D E V IA T IO N R U LES
Expanded:
(i) + s —> O / + S — + s
(ii) + s o / # +s
(iii) —s + s —> O / + S — + s
(iv) - s + s —> o / # +s
(v) + s —s —> O / + S — + s
(vi) + s ~ s —> O / # — + s
(vii) + s + s - * o I + S — + s
(viii) + s + s —►O / # — + s
pattern
The realisation rules generate a sequence o f syllables marked + s and
- s ; however, a full description o f the syllabic characteristics involved
in the rhythmic perception o f the line includes som e further
possibilities. Other types o f syllable may replace + s or - s in the
output o f the rules, but only under the specific conditions discussed in
Chapter 8 :
s: indefinite stress; may replace either + s or —s ( 8 . 1 )
s: em phatic stress; may replace + s ( 8 .2 )
[s]: metrically subordinated stress; may replace —s (8.3)
(s): elision by contraction; the syllable is om itted (8 .4 )
+»s r i : elision by coalescence; may replace + s or - s (8.4)
These sym bols are used in the statem ent o f tint stress p a tte rn , which is a
simplification o f the linguistically determ ined stress contour brought
about by the perception o f a regular rhythm.
SCA N SIO N
o: double offbeat
b: promotion
362 APPENDIX: RULES AND SCANSION
o: d em o tio n
ft: im plied offb ea t
•ft, ft; ft*, double offbeat with dem otion
T. triple o ffb ea t
Full scansion shows the stress pattern above the line, and the metrical
pattern with deviation sym bols beneath it:
+s - s [s] +s -s +s (s)-s+ s -s -s
(1 ) G ildin g pale stream s with h eaven ly alchem y
B o B o B o B o B
The grouping o f words and syllables can also be shown above the line,
to draw attention to the presence o f rising or falling rhythm s (4.6):
/ * / ------------------------- » / --------------------- » / \ t----------------------- V
(2 ) I w ake and feel the fell o f dark, not day
Where two dates are given, the one after the author’s name is the date o f first
publication, and is used for reference in the text.
Finnegan, Ruth (1977) Oral Poetry: Its Nature, Significance and Social Context
(Cambridge)
Fodor, J. and Garrett, M. (1966) ‘Some reflections on competence and
performance’ in Psycholinguistic Papers ed. J. Lyons and R. J. Wales
(Edinburgh), 133-79
Forrest-Thomson, Veronica (1978) Poetic Artifice: A Theory o f Twentieth-
Century Poetry (Manchester)
Fowler, Carol A. (1980) ‘Coarticulation and theories of extrinsic timing’
Journal o f Phonetics 8, 113-33
Fowler, Roger (1966a) ‘ “Prose rhythm” and metre’ in Essays on Style and
Language ed. Roger Fowler (London), 82-99
Fowler, Roger (1966b) ‘Structural metrics’ in The Languages o f Literature:
Some Contributions to Criticism (London, 1971), 124-40
Fowler, Roger (1968) ‘What is metrical analysis?’ in The Languages o f
Literature: Some Contributions to Criticism (London, 1971), 141-77
Fowler, Roger (1970) ‘Against idealization: some speculations on the theory
of linguistic performance’ Linguistics 63, 19-50
Fowler, Roger (1975) ‘Language and the reader: Shakespeare’s Sonnet 7 3 ’ in
Style and Structure in Literature: Essays in the New Stylistics ed. Roger
Fowler (Oxford), 79-122
Fraser, G. S. (1970) Metre, Rhyme, and Free Verse (London)
Freeman, Donald C. (1968) ‘On the primes of metrical style’ Language and
Style 1, 63-101
Freeman, Donald C. (1969) ‘Metrical position constituency and generative
metrics’ Language and Style 2, 195-206
Freeman, Donald C. (1972) ‘Current trends in metrics’ in Current Trends in
Stylistics ed. Braj B. Kachru and H. F. W. Stahlke (Edmonton, Alberta),
67-81
Fromkin, Victoria (1968) ‘Speculations on performance m odels’ Journal o f
Linguistics 4, 4 7-68
Fry, D. B. (1964) ‘The function of the syllable’ Zeitschrift fur Phonetik 17,
215-21
Frye, Northrop (1957 ) ‘Introduction: lexis and m elos’ in Sound and Poetry ed.
Northrop Frye, English Institute Essays, 1956 (New York), ix-xxvii
Funkhouser, Linda Bradley (1979) ‘Acoustic rhythm in Randall Jarrell’s The
Death o f the Ball Turret Gunner’ Poetics 8, 381-403
368 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Jacob, Cary F. (1918) The Foundations and Nature o f Verse (New York)
Jakobson, Roman (1960) ‘Closing statement: linguistics and poetics’ in Style
in Language ed. T. A. Sebeok (Cambridge, Mass.), 350-77
Jespersen, Otto (1900) ‘Notes on metre’ in The Structure o f Verse: Modern
Essays on Prosody ed. Harvey Gross (Greenwich, Conn., 1966), 111-30
Jones, Daniel (1960) An Outline o f English Phonetics, 9 th edn. (Cambridge)
Nabokov, Vladimir (1964) Notes on Prosody and Abram Gannibal: From the
Commentary to the Author's Translation o f Pushkin's 'Eugene Onegin'
(Princeton)
Napoli, Donna Jo (1978) ‘The metrics of Italian nursery rhymes’ Language
and Style 11, 4 0 -5 8
Nash, Walter (1980) Designs in Prose: A Study o f Compositional Problems
and Methods (London)
Needier, G. H. (1941) The Lone Shieling: Origin and Authorship o f the
Blackwood *Canadian Boat-Song' (Toronto)
BIBLIOGRAPHY 371
Ramus, Petrus (1564) Libri Duo de Veris Sonis Literarum & syllabarum
(Paris)
Richards, I. A. (1924) Principles o f Literary Criticism (London, 1967)
Robertson, Jean (1960) ‘Sir Philip Sidney and his poetry’ in Elizabethan
Poetry ed. John Russell Brown and Bernard Harris, Stratford-upon-Avon
Studies, 2 (London), 111-29
Robinson, Ian (1971) Chaucer's Prosody: A Study o f the Middle English Verse
Tradition (Cambridge)
372 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Youmans, Gilbert (1974) ‘Test case for a metrical theory: “La Belle Dame
Sans Merci” ’ Language and Style 7, 283-305
BIBLIOGRAPHY 375
The following abbreviations are used: FQ - Spenser, The Faerie Queene; Son-
Shakespeare, The Sonnets; PL - Milton, Paradise Lost ; Prel - Wordsworth,
The Prelude (1850 text); D J - Byron, Don Juan ; RB - Browning, The Ring and
the Book (1 8 6 8 -9 text). Ballads are cited from Child’s collection.
Chapter 1
1 FQ I 7 i
2 Shelley, ‘To a Skylark’
3 Blake, ‘Nurse’s Song’
4 Hogg? ‘God Save the King’
5 Wordsworth, ‘The Solitary Reaper’
6 Yeats, ‘Two Songs from a Play’
7 Arnold, ‘Stanzas from the Grande Chartreuse’
8 Son 30
9 Shakespeare, ‘The Phoenix and Turtle’
10 Blake, ‘The Chimney Sweeper’ {Songs o f Experience)
11 Son 29
12 Son 55
13 Son 64
14 Marvell, ‘To His Coy Mistress’
16 Milton, ‘II Pensoroso’
Chapter 2
1 Shelley, ‘To a Skylark’
2 Donne, Holy Sonnets: ‘Batter my h eart. . . ’
5 Donne, ‘To His Mistress Going to Bed’
6 Keats, ‘How many bards . . . ’
11 Son 7
12 PL II 297
13 PL III 465
14 PL VIII 299
15 Son 147
16 Son 57
17 Son 132
SOURCES OF EXAMPLES 377
Chapter 4
1 Donne, ‘Song: “ Go, and catch a falling star” ’
2 Hogg, ‘The Skylark’
3 Traditional
4 As You Like It, V iii
5 Traditional
6 Advertising jingle
7 Traditional
8 Marlowe, ‘The Passionate Shepherd to His Love’
9 ‘The Lochmaben Harper’, Child 192A
10 Byron, ‘Stanzas: “Could love for ever . . ’
11 Burns, ‘To a Louse’
12 Traditional
13 ‘The Gay Goshawk’, Child 96A
14 Wordsworth, ‘She dwelt among the untrodden ways’
15 Grimald, ‘To his Familiar Friend’
16 Gay, ‘A New Song of New Similies’
17 Gray, ‘Ode on the Death of a Favourite Cat’
18 Coleridge, The Ancient Mariner, 91-6
19 Ditto, 446-51
20 Traditional
21 Tennyson, ‘The Lady of Shalott’
22 Roethke, ‘My Papa’s Waltz’
23 Traditional
24 ‘Mary Hamilton’, Child 173A
25 Queen Elizabeth I, ‘The doubt of future foes . . . ’
26 Traditional
27 Traditional
28 Traditional
29 ‘Clerk Colvill’, Child 42A
30 ‘The Gay Goshawk’, Child 96B
31 Campion, ‘Come, let us sound . . . ’
32 Coleridge, The Ancient Mariner, 2 9-30
33 ‘Sir Patrick Spens’, Child 58A
34 D e La Mare, ‘The Listeners’
35 Auden, ‘Victor’
36 Blake, ‘Nurse’s Song’ (Songs o f Innocence)
37 Blake, ‘The Divine Image’
38 Blake, ‘Nurse’s Song’ (Songs o f Experience)
39 Traditional
40 Traditional
41 ‘Sir Patrick Spens’, Child 58A
42 Marvell, ‘A Dialogue Between the Resolved Soul and Created Pleasure’
43 Shelley, ‘The Sensitive Plant’
378 SOURCES OF EXAMPLES
44 Traditional
45 Traditional
46 Traditional
47 Hood, ‘The Bridge of Sighs’
48 Betjeman, ‘The Irish Unionist’s Farewell to Greta Hellstrom in 1922’
49 Traditional
50 Traditional
51 Clough, ‘Say Not the Struggle Nought Availeth’
52 ‘The Wife of Usher’s Well’, Child 79A
53 Johnson, ‘On the Death of Dr. Robert Levet’
54 Johnson, ‘A Short Song of Congratulation’
55 Tennyson, In Memoriam , II
56 Sidney, Astrophil and Stella: Fourth Song
57 The Tempest, IV i
58 Longfellow, Hiawatha: ‘Hiawatha’s Departure’
59 ‘Thomas Rymer’, Child 37A
60 ‘The Wife of Usher’s Well’, Child 79A
61 Traditional
62 Chesterton, ‘The Rolling English Road’
63 Browning, ‘A Toccata of Galuppi’s’
64 Byron, ‘To Thomas M oore’
65 Ditto
66 Kipling, ‘The Long Trail’
67 Pound, ‘In a Station of the Metro’
Chapter 5
1 Chaucer, General Prologue, 14
2 Son 65
3 PL XII 645
4 Pope, ‘Epistle to Dr. Arbuthnot’, 309
5 Prel IV 327
6 Yeats, ‘Sailing to Byzantium’
7 Anon., ‘The Valiant Seaman’s Happy Return to His Love’ (C. Stone, Sea
Songs and Ballads, Oxford, 1906)
8 Gray, ‘Elegy Written in a Country Churchyard’
9 Byron, ‘The Destruction of Sennacherib’
10 Burns, ‘A e Fond Kiss’
11 Browning, ‘One Word More’
12 Jonson, Cynthia's Revels , V vi
13 Pope, ‘Elegy to the Memory of an Unfortunate Lady’
14 Emily Bronte, ‘The Old Stoic’
15 Kipling, ‘If- -’
16 Marvell, ‘The Garden’
17 Pope, ‘To Mrs M. B. on Her Birthday’
SOURCES OF EXAMPLES 379
18 Herbert, ‘Easter’
19 Auden, New Year Letter , Part Three
20 Sidney, ‘What tongue can her perfections tell?’, Arcadia, Book III
21 Traditional
22 Prince, Afterword on Rupert Brooke, I
23 Herrick, ‘To Dianeme: “Sweet, be not proud
24 Herrick, ‘When He Would Have His Verses Read’
25 Byron, Beppo, 10
26 Lovelace, ‘The Grasshopper’
Chapter 7
1 ‘Robin Hood Rescuing Three Squires’, Child 140B
2 ‘The Famous Flower of Serving-Men’, Child 106
3 DJ IX 76
4 Auden, ‘Victor’
5 Arnold, ‘Requiescat’
6 Ditto
7 Ditto
8 ‘Tam Lin’, Child 39F
9 Shelley, ‘Lines Written in the Bay of Lerici’
10 Coleridge, The Ancient Mariner, 504
11 Tennyson, ‘Ulysses’
12 The Tempest, I ii
13 Moore, ‘Hark, the Vesper Hymn is Stealing’
14 PL I 142
15 Tennyson, ‘Tithonus’
16 Hardy, ‘The Newcomer’s Wife’
17 ‘Lord Thomas and Fair A nnet’, Child 73C
18 Milton, ‘II Pensoroso’
19 Coleridge, The Ancient Mariner, 269
20 The Tempest, I ii
21 Burns, ‘To a Mouse’
22 Tennyson, Idylls o f the King : ‘The Passing of Arthur’, 34
23 FQ I iv 17
24 Dryden, Absalom and Achitophel, 869
25 Pope, ‘Epistle to Dr. Arbuthnot’, 201
26 Tennyson, ‘Tears, Idle Tears’
27 DJ I 210
28 Pope, ‘Epistle to Dr. Arbuthnot’, 182
29 Traditional
30 Traditional
31 Traditional
32 Tennyson, ‘Break, Break, Break’
33 A Midsummer Night's Dream, II i
380 SOURCES OF EXAMPLES
Chapter 8
1 Crabbe, The Village, I 111-12
2 Tennyson, Idylls o f the King : ‘Pelleas and Ettarre’, 39 3 -4
3 DJ III 2
4 King Lear , V iii
5 Crabbe, The Village, I 25
6 PL X 943
7 FQ I vii 42
8 Son 89
9 PL X 882
10 FQ I ix 40
11 Prel V 133
12 PL I 67
13 Pope, A « Essay on Criticism, 458
14 Keats, The Fall o f Hyperion, I 297
15 Ditto, I 87
16 Ditto, I 65
17 Ditto, I 202
18 Ditto, I 182
19 Ditto, I 209
20 Son 42
21 Drayton, Idea (1619), Sonnet 37, ‘Dear, why should you
com m and..
22 PL IX 944
23 FQ I ii 31
24 Son 39
25 PL IX 652
26 Jonson, ‘An Epitaph on Master Vincent Corbett’
27 Swift, ‘Verses on the Death of Dr. Swift’
28 Donne, ‘A Hymn to God the Father’
29 Son 22
30 Keats, ‘Bright st ar. . . ’
31 Donne, ‘Elegy 10: “The Dream” ’
32 Ditto
33 FQ I iv 17
34 Dryden, Absalom and Achitophel, 869
35 Pope, ‘Epistle to Dr. Arbuthnot’, 201
36 Tennyson, ‘Tears, Idle Tears’
37 Marvell, ‘To His Coy Mistress’
SOURCES OF EXAMPLES 383
83 PL IX 52
84 Macbeth, II ii
85 The Tempest, V i
86 Milton, Comus, 582
87 PL II 353
88 PL II 490
89 PL II 359
90 PL II 424
91 PL III 257
92 Arnold, The Scholar-Gipsy , 211-12
93 Son 52
94 PL IV 141-7
95 Son 64
96 Cowper, The Task , VI 908
97 Keats, ‘Ode to a Nightingale’
98 PL XII 649
99 Pope, ‘Epistle to Burlington’, 48
100 Keats, The Eve o f St. Agnes, 203
101 Prel V 80
102 Pope, The Dunciad , IV 3
103 PL IX 115
104 Pope, The Rape o f the Lock, III 136
105 RB IV 649
106 Tennyson, ‘Ulysses’
107 Wordsworth, ‘Tintern Abbey’
108 Yeats, ‘Among School Children’
109 PL IV 59 8 -9
110 PL II 621
112 Tennyson, ‘Saint Simeon Stylites’
113 Browning, ‘The Bishop Orders His Tomb’
114 Clough, ‘Say Not the Struggle Nought Availeth’
115 Keats, Hyperion, I 116
116 Tennyson, ‘Saint Simeon Stylites’
117 Prel I 309-312
118 Coleridge, ‘To William Wordsworth’
119 Son 116
120 Gay, Trivia, III 263
121 PL IX 560
122 Tennyson, ‘Lucretius’, 204
123 Marlowe, Hero and Leander, I 5
124 Ditto, I 320
125 Son 29
126 Son 140
127 PL IX 1144
SOURCES OF EXAMPLES 385
Chapter 9
1 Nabokov, Lolita
2 Son 12
3 Lawrence, ‘Brooding G rief
4 Ditto
5 PL IV 114-20
6 Cowper, ‘The Poplar-Field’
7 Hardy, ‘During Wind and Rain’
8 Blake, ‘London’
9 Edward Dyer, ‘My mind to me a kingdom is’
10 Jonson, ‘Her Triumph’, A Celebration o f Charis
Appendix
1 Son 33
2 Hopkins, ‘I wake and feel the fell of dark, not day’
3 Yeats, ‘A Dialogue of Self and Soul’
Index
Bold figures indicate main entries. Authors of examples are indexed only where
reference is made to their individual use of rhythm.