(Felis Silvestris Cams) : The Function of Allogrooming in Domestic Cats
(Felis Silvestris Cams) : The Function of Allogrooming in Domestic Cats
(Felis Silvestris Cams) : The Function of Allogrooming in Domestic Cats
16:1-i3, 1998
1The data were previously presented at the 29th International Congress of the International Society for Applied
Ethology (van den Bos, R. (1995) Allogrooming in domestic cats in confinement, Proceedings of the 29th Interna-
tional Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology, S.M. Rutter et al. (Eds.), pp. 109-110)
2Author's address: Ruud van den Bos PhD; Institute of Evolutionary and Ecological Sciences, Section Theoretical
Biology, University of Leiden, P.O. Box 9516, 2300 RA Leiden, the Netherlands
One important issue which has been addres- ships with individuals with whom it may be
sed is conflict regulation in group-living condi- advantageous to affiliate like high ranking indi-
tions (Natoli & de Vito, 1991; van den Bos & viduals m which may give support in future con-
de Cock Buning, 1994a). Cats appear to lack flictsmas has been repeatedly observed in pri-
formal dominance hierarchies: none of their mates (see e.g. Wilson, 1975; Seyfarth, 1976,
affiliative behavioural patterns shows a unique 1977, 1980; Goosen, 1980; Hemelrijk, 1990;
one-way distribution with respect to rank-orders Hemelrijk & Ek, 1991; Spruijt et al., 1992).
based on the outcome of aggressive encounters As hypothesized in a previous paper (van den
(van den Bos & de Cock Buning, 1994a,b; van Bos & de Cock Buning, 1994a) allogrooming
den Bos, 1995a,b). Still, allogrooming, i.e. may be a way of reducing tension between
grooming a conspecific, appears to be an impor- animals which are living near one another. This
tant behavioural pattern in conflict-regulation, study addresses this possibility by careful
that is it appears to play a role in forming and observation and analysis of grooming interac-
maintaining stability of groups (van den Bos & tions.
de Cock Buning, 1994a; van den Bos & de
Vries, 1996).
The following has been specifically revealed Materials and Methods
with respect to allogrooming between adults: (i)
allogrooming occurs regardless of sex Animals & Study Area
(male/female), sexual status (intact/neutered) or
living conditions (free-ranging/non flee-rang- The study colony existed since 1988. Cats
ing); (ii) the distribution of allogrooming bouts (European short hairs) were purchased from
in pairs is asymmetric; (iii) the flow of allog- different breeders and brought together while
rooming is downrank; (iv) animals receive more juvenile. Males were neutered at an age of 6-12
bouts of grooming from animals from whom months, females at an age of 12-25 months.
they also receive more bouts of offensive be- The coefficients of relatedness ranged from 0 to
haviour; (v) allogrooming is associated with be- 0.632. These coefficients were calculated from
havioural patterns expressing confidence and breeders' information on which males sired
which are more likely to be shown by high- which females (see Brown, 1993 for details on
ranking animals, such as social sniffing and snif- the genealogy of this colony). Males were
ring rear; (vi) proximity scores of pairs and fie- generally stockier and heavier than females
quencies of allogrooming in pairs correlate posi- (weight ratio male to female: 1.3; ranges: male:
tively; (vii) in groups, mean grooming rate 5-8 kg, female: 4-5.5 kg). At the start of this
(bouts/h/pair) and density (cats/sqm) correlate study eleven females and fourteen males were
positively, while mean agonistic interaction rate present. During the study, after 9 sessions, one
(interactions/h/pair) and density correlate nega- female was removed from the colony, leaving
tively, i.e. the higher the density the less ten females for the remainder of sessions.
aggression and the more allogrooming occurs This colony has been studied earlier (March-
(Brown, 1993; van den Bos & de Cock Buning, -June 1991; reported in Brown, 1993). The be-
1994a,b; van den Bos, 1995a,b; van den Bos & havioural characteristics of the animals as well
de Vries, 1996; van den Bos et al. unpublished as the rank orders for males and females are
data). therefore well documented. Cats were classified
The asymmetrical distribution of grooming in into three ranking categories (high, middle and
pairs and the effect of rank order exclude the low) based on informal observations by Brown
possibility that grooming solely serves a (1993) and formal analysis of the flow of ago-
hygienic function as has been observed in im- nistic behaviour (data in Brown, 1993; cf. van
pala's (Hart & Hart, 1992; Mooting & Hart, den Bos & de Cock Buning, 1994a).
1993). Furthermore the rank order effect ex- The group's riving quarters consisted of an in-
cludes the possibility that grooming serves as a door and outdoor enclosure. The indoor enclo-
way of getting access to and establish relation- sure consisted of two rooms (A: 23 sqm; B: 28
sqm) and a corridor (length: 10.8 m; width: 1.3 ence with theft behaviour, the cats were fami-
m; area: 14 sqm) connecting rooms A and B liarized with the presence of an observer during
and leading to the outside enclosure. Room A two 1-hour sessions before actual observations
contained shelfs with beds in which cats could started (cf. van den Bos & de Cock Buning,
sleep, lie, sit etc.. This room also contained lit- 1994a). During these sessions and throughout
ter trays (bedding: wood shavings). Room B the observations the observer did not interact
contained cages in which cats could lie, sit etc.. with the cats. The observer recorded all
The corridor contained a pole with shelfs on ongoing behaviour from one spot in the enclo-
which cats could lie, sit etc. as well as a box in sure. The camera was focussed on the area
which cats could hide. The outside enclosure most frequently visited by the cats (about one
(96 sqm) contained several elements on which third of the enclosure). Several rehabituation
cats could sit, lie (pole with shelfs) or in which sessions were included when recording sessions
they could hide (boxes). Furthermore it con- were separated too far apart in time (see be-
tained logs for cats to scratch on. The floor of low). Recording was spread out over a period
the outside enclosure was made of tiles (29 • of 6 months. Sessions were interspersed with re-
29 cm). The population density was 0.16 cording sessions in room A. The latter sessions
cat/sqm. were initiated on days different from those in
Cats had free access to the outside enclosure the outside enclosure (van den Bos, in prepara-
during working hours (08.00--17:00 h). They re- tion). The temperature in the outside enclosure
mained in the inside enclosure at night. Cats was determined after each recording session us-
were kept on a 12-12 h day-night cycle with ing an ordinary thermometer to assess the effect
lights on from 08:(D--20:00 h. They were fed dry of temperature on grooming rate (see Troisi &
(SDS; U.K.) and wet food (Whiskas (R)) once Schino, 1986).
daily in the afternoon (after observations took
place; see below). Water was available through- Interactions
out the day in bowls present at various places in
the enclosure. Temperature in the inside enclo- An interaction was considered to have started
sure was maintained at 21 degrees Celsius; when a cat clearly and unambiguously
temperature in the outside enclosure varied approached another cat--approach: moving
according to weather conditions. clearly towards another c a t - - o r invited another
cat for an interaction by e.g. rolling in front of
Observational Regimen another cat. The cat which started the interac-
tion (approaching another cat or rolling in front
For each recording session cats were moved of another cat) was labelled the initiator, its
to the outside enclosure if the weather allowed partner the recipient. If cats started to groom
so. Only during dry weather recording sessions one another while sitting or lying close together
were initiated. Furthermore the floor and the (no approach or invitation) the cat which deli-
elements on which cats could sit etc. had to be vered the first bout of grooming was called the
dry. Two litter trays were provided during re- initiator.
cording in case cats needed to urinate or defe- The distance between the cats at the start of
cate. Observations were always made between the interaction was split into two categories:
11:00 and 15:00 h. The observer was present in start from within 0.5 m (proximity: a cat's body
the enclosure and recorded the behaviour of the length; van den Bos & de Cock Buning, 1994a)
cats using a video camera recorder with zoom- and start from a distance of more than 0.5 m.
lens (Philips Explorer Autofocus CCD During the interaction four behavioural pat-
VKR6850). Previous observations showed that tems of the initiator and recipient were re-
cats behave somewhat more aggressive and less corded (Table 1A) using the programme 'The
affiliative towards one another when an obser- Observer' (v2.0; Noldus Information Technolo-
ver is present in the enclosure (Brown, 1993). gy, Wageningen, the Netherlands). One extra
Therefore, in order to avoid too much interfer- category of behaviour was added, i.e. 'other be-
Table 1. Ethogram used in this study (after van den Bos & de Cock Buning, 1994a; UK Cat Behaviour
Working Group, 1991). In parentheses the abbreviations which are used in the tables are shown.
(A): BEHAVIOURS
Behaviour Description
allogrooming (GC) licking the fur of another cat;
autogrooming (GS) cat licking its own fur (including wiping head region with paw after licking
paw);
Offensive behaviour (OF) combination of the following elements: (slowly) approaching another cat
(lowered head); running in pursuit of another cat, erect ears swiveled to
point back on the head; hairs on back and tail erected; lashing tail; pant-
ing, salivating; cuffing, growling, yowling; narrowing of the pupils (includ-
ing mounting);
Defensive behaviour (DE) combination of the following elements: lowering of hindquarters; pilo-
erection; flattening of the ears to the head such that they tend to flush
with the top of the head; hissing, spitting; growling; rolling on the dorsum
to expose claws; cuffing; dilation of the pupils (including fleeing);
(B) POSTURES
Posture Description
standing (STA) standing on all four;
sitting upright (SITUP) sitting on hindpaws with outstretched forepaws; the weight of the body is
shifted towards hindquarters;
sitting (SIT) sitting on hindpaws and frontpaws; forepaws may be tucked underneath
body or stretched out in front of the animal; the weight of the body is
supported by front- and hindquarters;
lying (LYI) lying on the side, back etc.; the weight of the body is supported by the
body rather than the paws;
haviour', which contained all other behaviours. mined whether it was directed to the head-neck
Furthermore postures of the groomer and area, the shoulder-chest area or the a b d o m e n
groomee (Table 1B) while bouts of grooming area (see insert of Fig. 1 for the body-areas).
were exchanged were recorded.
An interaction was considered to have ended
Body Are== =nd AJlogroondng
if one of the (or both) cats moved away or fled
(distance: > 0 . 5 m). If cats remained together
(distance: < = 0.5 m; proximity) the interaction
was considered to have ended if none of the be- 100~ - i
C "
havioural patterns of Table 1A was shown by 80X;
the cats for a period of 60 s.
A bout of allogrooming was defined as a
series of licking spells not interrupted by 40g - l
another behaviour, like looking round, watch
20~ -
other cats, licking the perioral region, unclearly
directed nibblings to the fur of the other cat or o,J r
a clear short pause (cf. van den Bos & de Cock
Buning, 1994a). The same principle applies to Body =rea
the definition of bouts of autogrooming, offen- ['] Bout= I I Durltlon
sive behaviour and defensive behaviour.
Fig. 1. Distribution of the number of bouts of aUog-
rooming and of total duration of allogrooming across
Data Analysis and Statistics different body-areas; Insert: Areas into which the cat's
body was split for analysis: A: head-neck area; B:
For each allogrooming bout it was deter- shoulder-chest area; C: abdomen area.
Postures of the groomer and groomee were Throughout the Results section M E A N + S E
analysed by preparing a matrix containing post- is reported unless otherwise stated. Reported p-
ures for the groomer (rows) and the groomee values are two-tailed unless otherwise stated, p-
(columns). The matrix was analysed using a Values equal to or smaller than 0.05 were cons-
Chi-square test (Sokal & Rohlf, 1981). idered to indicate statistical significant differ-
For each interaction the members of the in- ences.
teracting pair were denoted. Interactions were
labelled unidirectional if only one animal
groomed, otherwise bidirectional. Interactions RF~ULTS
between the same individuals, i.e. the same
pair, were added to obtain one score (frequen- Interactions: General Aspects
cy, duration) per behavioural pattern per mem-
ber of a pair. Thus for each member of a pair In total n = 2 8 sessions were recorded on
one score per behavioural pattern (frequency, video tape (23h45min) over a 6 month period.
duration) was obtained, whether the animal had Ninety-two interactions were taped (3.9+0.4
been an initiator or a recipient in an interac- interactions/h), but nine interactions (9.8%)
tion. could not be analysed, because interactions
Scores (frequency, duration) per behavioural were only partly on screen and because other
pattern per individual were totalled, i.e. for animals prevented reliable recording. No cor-
each individual one score (frequency, duration) relation existed between the temperature in the
per behavioural pattern was obtained, irrespec- outside enclosure and the number of grooming
tive with whom the individual had interacted interactions per h (rS=0.05, n--25, ns).
with as an initiator or as a recipient. Most often (n=78 interactions; 94%) interac-
The number of interacting male-male (MM), tions (duration: 103.9+15.7s, n=83; range
male-female (MF) and female-female (FF) pairs 5.7-945.4s) started with one animal approaching
was related to the maximal possible number of or inviting another animal; only 5 interactions
pairs for each category (MM,MF or FF pairs). (6%) started with animals sitting or lying
For the final number of different grooming together (distance: < = 0 . 5 m). In 89.7% of the
pairs an expected distribution was calculated approaches/invitations animals were more than
based on the maximal number of pairs per 0.5 m apart before the interaction started.
category. The observed distribution and the ex- Accordingly in total 84.3% (n=70) of interac-
pected distribution were subsequently compared tions started while animals were more than 0.5
using a Chi-square Goodness-of-Fit test (Sokal m apart.
& Rohlf, 1981). Subsequently, means + SE's for The majority of interactions (91.6%) was un-
the different behaviours of the MM, MF and idirectional. In case of bidirectional interactions
FF pairs were calculated. Differences between the distribution of grooming in terms of bouts
males and female in MF pairs were analysed us- (correlation coefficient: r=0.28, dr=5, ns) as
ing paired t-tests. well as duration ( r = - - 0 . 0 2 , df--5, ns) deviated
To assess the effects of kinship, for each pair from symmetry ( r = 1).
the overall scores of the different behavioural Allogrooming was most often directed to the
patterns (frequency, duration) were correlated (dorsal part of the) head-neck area both in
with the coefficient of relatedness using Spear- terms of number of bouts and duration (Fig. 1).
man rank correlation tests (Sokal & Rohlf In the majority of cases (n=58 interactions;
1981). Furthermore pairs were classified accord- 69.9%) interactions ended with one animal
ing to whether the animals were highly (r = > moving away (>0.5 m); in only a small number
0.25), intermediately (0.125 < = r < 0.25) or of interactions (n=15; 18.1%) animals re-
distantly (r < 0.125) related. Differences in fre- mained within 0.5 m of one another. Ten in-
quencies and durations of behavioural patterns teractions (12%) ended with the flight of an
between different categories were analysed us- animal.
ing one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA).
Interactions: Behavio ural patterns number of allogrooming and autogrooming
bouts as well as between their durations was
Groomers were often standing (43.6%) or sit- found for both initiators (r=0.63, dr=81,
ting upright (45.1%) whereas groomees were P<0.001 and r=0.42, df=81, P<0.001 respec-
often sitting (46.6%; Table 2). Due to the large tively) and recipients (r=0.47, df=81, P<0.001
number of cells containing values smaller than 5 and r=0.14, df=81, n.s. respectively). These
(9/16: 56.3%) a Chi-square could not be prop- same correlations held when allogrooming and
erly run. Yet, it should be noted that the cells autogrooming scores of recipients and initiators
containing the combination standing/sitting were summed to the scores for different indi-
(groomer/groomee; 23.3%) and sitting viduals ( n - 2 1 individuals: r=0.92, df=19,
uptight/sitting (18.8%) contained the highest P<0.001 and r=0.87, df=19, P<0.001 respec-
values (Table 2). tively). Bouts of allogrooming ( n = 8 3 bouts)
In 34.9% of interactions agonistic behaviour more were often followed (60.2%) than pre-
(offensive or defensive behaviour) was re- ceded (39.8%) by bouts of autogrooming when
corded. If offensive behaviour was shown by both occurred.
either an initiator or recipient of an interaction
(n--30 cases) it was more often shown by the Pairs: Males and Females
groomer (70%; n = 2 1 cases) than by the
groomee (30%; n = 9 cases). Bouts of offensive In total, 37 different pairs were observed to
behaviour shown by the groomer ( n = 2 6 bouts) groom (13.4% of all possible pairs; Table 3).
were more often preceded (80.8%; n = 2 1 The overall grooming rate was: 0.28+0.07
bouts) than followed (19.2%; n--5 bouts) by bouts/pair/h. The distribution of allogrooming
bouts of allogrooming when both occurred. across pairs was asymmetric whether in terms of
Bouts of defensive behaviour ( n = 1 8 bouts) frequencies of grooming bouts (Fig. 2A) or tot-
were more often preceded by bouts of offensive al duration of grooming (Fig. 2B).
behaviour (83.3%; n = 1 5 bouts) than by bouts The 37 pairs showed 83 interactions. Most in-
of allogrooming (16.7%; n = 3 bouts) when both teractions consisted of males interacting with
occurred. one another: MM: 54 (65.1%), MF: 26 (31.3%)
A strong positive correlation between the and FF: 3 (3.6%). In the 26 MF interactions,
Table 2. Occurrencesof combinations of postures of groomer (rows) and groomee (columns) while bouts of
grooming were delivered. Tot: totals for rows or columns. For abbreviations of behaviours see Table 1.
Table 3. Total number of different pairs interacting (n=37). Since in the majority of sessions (67.9%) 10
rather than 11 females were present the values were calculated using the smaller number of females leading
to a slight overestimation of the contribution of females. %tot: number of pairs expressed as percentage of
total number of pairs (n=37); %class: observed number of pairs per class expressed as percentage of the tot-
al number of pairs per class (MM: n=91 pairs; MF: n = 140 pairs; FF: n=45 pairs); int/pair: number of in-
teractions per pair.
0
orii-i vv
'
10 @ 2JO 9 30
l~xmd~l~lr
400
DurlUon o f / i ~ r o o m l n g
~ ~ B 300
300 +
200
250
@@
200
lO01
150
100
.IL~I-.+ L 9 J,-+.-,
vv
Z~JI'.I,_
v v i
@
- | 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 0,5 06
50 ~ ol v~lhK~mmm
-- @ 9
i
i@
T v -5~" vv i00 15o moo 250 300
Frequency of AIIogroomlng
llcolw4l kw:IM<~l~ A
Published by Japan Ethological Society, Department of Zoology, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto, 606-8502, Japan