The - Effects - of - Movement - Distance - and - Mov N
The - Effects - of - Movement - Distance - and - Mov N
The - Effects - of - Movement - Distance - and - Mov N
North-Holland
Howard N. ZELAZNIK
Purdue University, West Lafayette, USA
Brian HAWKINS
Uniuersi~ of Colorado at Boulder, USA
Lorraine KISSELBURGH
Purdue University, West Lafayette, USA
An experiment is reported that manipulated movement distance, movement time and the availabil-
ity of vision (Light-On versus Light-Off) on the accuracy of aimed hand movements. There was a
constant difference in spatial accuracy between Light-On and Light-off conditions as a function of
distance when the duration of the movement was 200 msec; when the duration of the aimed hand
movement was 400 or 600 msec the difference in spatial accuracy between Light-On and Light-Off
conditions increased as distance increased. These results were taken as support for a two-process
model of visual feedback processing in aimed hand movements, and provide converging evidence
for the rapid visual feedback processing results of Zelaznik, Hawkins, and Kisselburgh (1983).
Since the work of Keele of Posner (1968) and the work of Howarth
and colleagues (see Howarth and Beggs (1985) there has been a
renewed interest in the study of the time course of visual feedback
processing in aimed hand movements. In a large number of these
studies estimates of visual feedback processing time emerge from
examining the effects of visual feedback withdrawal, or distortion, and
* Special thanks to S. Monsell, K. Newell, and two anonymous reviewers for comments on
previous drafts of the present paper.
Requests for reprints should be sent to H.N. Zelaznik, Motor Behavior Laboratory, Dept. of
Physical Education, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA.
mm. Klapp did not control movement duration, and thus it is possible
the distance effects were mediated by changes in movement time. This
highlights a problem with most of the visual feedback processing
research, including our own (Zelaznik et al. 1983). If only one dimen-
sion is varied, such as movement time, then one is not sure as to
whether it is the movement time change per se or some concomitant
change in movement velocity that is mediating the observed effects.
It has been assumed that the limitation in processing visual feedback
is one of time. If enough time is available then subjects can utilize
feedback (see Schmidt and Russell 1972). However, this reasoning
seems to overlook the physics of corrections. Intuitively one can
imagine that movements possessing increased momentum (mass x
velocity) will be more resistant to perturbations. If one views a correc-
tion as a process that perturbs a limb from its predestined trajectory,
then the same amount of perturbation will have less of an effect on
higher momentum movements. Thus, it is possible that the minimum
time to process visual feedback, as investigated by Keele and Posner
(1968) and Zelaznik et al. (1983), was examining the minimum average
momentum that the visual feedback control system can overcome.
If the above reasoning is plausible then it is possible that if the
movement’s average velocity is large enough, even though there is
enough time to process visual feedback, the inertial properties of a
movement (as indexed by momentum) would be too large to permit the
predestined aimed hand movement trajectory to be altered by these
visual feedback processes. The present experiment provides a first-pass
test of this idea.
In the present experiment we examined visual feedback effects for
aimed hand movements of three durations, 200, 400, and 600 msec, and
for three movement distances, 10, 20, and 30 cm. Furthermore, there
are three combinations of distance and time (10-200, 20-400, and
30-600) that all have identical 50 cm/set average velocities. Thus, we
shall have a first approximation to the role of average velocity in visual
feedback effects on aimed hand movement accuracy.
Methods
Subjects
A plywood board (90 X 40 cm) covered with white posterboard was secured to the
top of a long and narrow table. A paper clip, which was used to position the target
sheets (9 X 21 cm), was inserted into the board. On each sheet there was a target cross
formed by two 0.75-cm lines, 0.5 mm in width. The sheets were oriented on the board
so that the axes of the target were in line with the axes of the board. At the end of the
board, to the subject’s right, a microswitch was placed so that the actuating arm, when
released, was flush with the top of the board. A stop was placed under the arm of this
start switch so that the total switch excursion was 2 mm. Closing the switch (by raising
the arm) engaged a latching relay and a digital msec clock. A stylus (Archer # 274-270)
formed a circuit with a pad of wire gauze, sandwiched between the target sheet and the
target board. Prior to the movement, the tip of the stylus was positioned in a
depression on the actuating arm of the microswitch. When the sharpened tip of the
stylus cut through the paper target the circuit closed and disengaged the latching relay
to stop the msec clock, providing the movement time (MT) score. The home position
was 16.5 cm from the edge of the table top. The horizontal distance from this home
position to the subject’s right shoulder was 30 cm and the vertical distance was 22.5 cm.
The subject’s right elbow angle was approximately 90° prior to movement initiation.
The aiming movement was executed from right to left in the subject’s frontal plane.
The subject’s head and eyes were located directly in line with the target; thus the initial
arm position changed as a function of movement distance.
To control the availability of visual information, an incandescent lamp (40 watts,
decay time to 10% = 20 msec), which provided the only illumination in the testing
room. was wired via the latching relay. In this manner, the light could be extinguished
coincident with the lift of the stylus from the home key, and could be turned on
coincident with the contact of the stylus with the target board. A second parallel circuit
was controlled by the experimenter for the conditions in which the light remained on
throughout the trial.
The subject’s task was to produce an aimed hand movement that would land as
close as possible to the target cross and to achieve the MT goal. Movement distance,
MT, and visual feedback were manipulated across blocks of trials, so that for any given
trial block the subject was certain about the availability of visual feedback, MT goal
and location of the target.
Procedures
The subject was instructed that the task demanded both spatial and temporal
accuracy. After the experimenter answered any questions, and the subject gave his/her
informed consent, the trial block began. The subject was seated in front of the aiming
board so that the right shoulder was in line with the start microswitch. In all
conditions, the stylus was held with the right hand, and a trial began by depressing the
tip of the stylus in the actuating arm of the microswitch. The experimenter then said
‘ready’. The subject was instructed not to react to this command, but rather to initiate
the movement when prepared. All subjects initiated their movements within two
seconds following the ‘ready’ command. In the Light-Off conditions, the lamp was
H. N. Zelaznik et al. / Visual feedback processing 185
extinguished coincident with the lift of the stylus from the home key, and remained off
until the stylus struck the surface of the aiming board. In the Light-On conditions the
lamp remained on throughout the movement. When the movement was completed, the
experimenter utilized a colored felt-tip pen to mark the small hole in the target sheet
produced by the stylus. Six colors were used in a fixed order so that a code was
generated for determining each trial’s data. Knowledge of results (KR) concerning MT
was provided after each trial. After receiving KR, subjects placed the stylus in the
home position to commence the next trial. The paper target sheet was replaced with a
new one every six trials. The intertrial interval was approximately eight seconds, and
the interval between changing target sheets was approximately 20 seconds.
Subjects performed in all 18 conditions which were formed by crossing three levels
of movement distance (10, 20, and 30 cm), MT (200. 400, and 600 msec), and two levels
of visual feedback (Light-On and Light-Off). Three of the distance-time combinations,
10-200, 20-400, and 30-600 have an average velocity of 50 cm/set, allowing for a
limited examination of the role of average velocity in visual feedback processing.
On a given experimental session the subject performed within one distance-MT
combination for two blocks of 90 trials each, one in the Light-On and the other in the
Light-Off condition. A Latin square was utilized for the presentation of distance-time
combinations across subjects. Five subjects performed the Light-On condition for the
first block of each session, while the other four subjects performed the Light-Off
condition first. The nine testing sessions spanned not less than nine nor more than
twelve days. No subject performed two sessions in one day.
Results
Treatment of data
Fig. 1 presents the root mean square error (RMSE) in MT as a function of the goal
MT, distance and visual feedback conditions. It is clear that RMSE in timing error
decreases as the movement time decreases, f(2,16) = 57.52, p < 0.01. As expected the
relative timing error remains about 10 percent (Schmidt et al. 1978). There was no
H. N. Zelarmk et al. / Visual feedback processrng
70-
60 -
50 -
G
5 40-
Ek-
----____
:
400
g 30-
_-a---__
200
-3
g__- ---
20 -
10 -
I I I
10 20 30
DISTANCE (cm)
Fig. 1. RMSE (msec) for movement time as a function of goal movement distance, movement time
and availability of vision (Light-On vs Light-Off).
Table 1
Average MT for all trials 31-90, and number of trials (Count) included in the ‘trimmed’ spatial
analysis. There was a maximum of 60 trials included.
These small effects of feedback on movement duration cast doubt on the possibility
that subjects traded off temporal accuracy for spatial accuracy in a manner dependent
upon the availability of visual feedback. Thus, we are confident that the changes in
spatial accuracy to be discussed are not the result of concomitant changes in temporal
precision.
Spatial accuracy
MT (ms)
6-
- Ilght-on + 600
---- hght-off,/ 0 400
5-
0 200
4-
3-
2-
l-
DISTANCE (cm)
Fig. 2. The left hand panel displays RMSE (mm) for spatial accuracy as a function of goal
movement distance, movement time and availability of vision (Light-On vs Light-Off). The right
hand panel displays the difference in RMSE between Light-Off and Light-On condition as a
function of movement distance and movement time. A positive difference means that the
Light-Off condition produced more RMSE than Light-On.
188 H. N. Zeluznik et al. / Vi.wulfeedback proce.wing
Discussion
The major finding of the present experiment was that for 200-msec
movements the effect of removing visual information resulted in a
constant difference between Light-On and Light-Off conditions as
distance increased, while for 400-msec and certainly for 600-msec
movements as movement distance increased the difference in spatial
accuracy between sighted and unsighted aimed hand movement in-
creased. Thus, an independent variable, movement distance, affects
limited duration movements in one fashion, and longer duration move-
ments in another fashion. This result supports our previously stated
hypothesis (Zelaznik et al. 1983) that the processes responsible for the
limited duration feedback effects are of a different nature than those
for the longer duration case.
In our previous work (Zelaznik et al. 1983) we speculated that
limited duration feedback processing might be involved in ensuring
that the motor program executes as planned. In Schmidt’s terms (1976),
feedback processing in the limited duration case corrects for errors in
execution. In the longer duration case (> 200 msec) the predominate
H. N. Zelaznik et al. / Visual feedback processing 189
References
Carlton, L.G., 1981. Processing visual information for motor control. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7, 1019-1030.
Crossman, E.R.F.W. and P.J. Goodeve, 1983. Feedback control of hand movements and Fit&’
Law. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology 35.
3-21.
H.N. Zelarnik ef al. / Visual feedback processmg 191
Howarth, C.I. and W.D.A. Beggs, 1985. ‘The control of simple movements by multisensory
information’. In: H. Heuer, U. Kleinbeck and K.-H. Schmidt (eds.), Motor behavior: program-
ming, control and acquisition. Berlin: Springer. pp. 125-151.
Keele, S.W. and M.I. Posner, 1968. Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. Journal of
Experimental Psychology 77, 155-158.
Knapp, ST., 1975. Feedback versus motor programming in the control of aimed movements.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 104, 147-153.
Meyer. D.E.. J.E.K. Smith and C.E. Wright, 1982. Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed
movements. Psychological Review 89, 449-482.
Polit, A. and E. Bizzi, 1978. Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. Science 201,
123551237.
Schmidt, R.A., 1976. Control processes in motor skills. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews 4,
2299261.
Schmidt, R.A. and C.M. McGown, 1980. Terminal accuracy or unexpectedly loaded rapid
movements: evidence for a mass-spring mechanism in programming. Journal of Motor
Behavior 12, 149-161.
Schmidt, R.A. and D.G. Russell, 1980. Movement velocity and movement time as determiners of
degree of preprogramming in simple movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology 96,
3155320.
Schmidt, R.A. H.N. Zelaznik and J.S. Frank, 1978. ‘Sources of inaccuracy in rapid movement’. In:
G.E. Stelmach (ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning. New York:
Academic Press. pp. 183-203.
Schmidt, R.A., H.N. Zelaznik, B. Hawkins, J.S. Frank and J.T. Quinn, 1979. Motor-output
variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychological Review 86, 415-451.
Smith, W.M. and K.F. Bowen, 1980. The effects of delayed and displaced visual feedback on
motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior 12, 91-101.
Thodens, U.. J. Dichgans, M. Doerr and Th. Savides. 1979. ‘Direction-specific vestibular and
visual modulation of fore- and hindlimb reflexes in cats’. In: R. Granit and 0. Pompeiano
(eds.). Reflex control of posture and movement. Amsterdam: Elsevier. pp. 211-218.
Zelaznik, H.N., B. Hawkins and L.G. Kisselburgh, 1983. Rapid visual feedback processing in
single-aiming movements. Journal of Motor Behavior 15, 217-236.
Zelaznik, H.N., D.C. Shapiro and D. McColsky, 1981. The effects of a secondary task on the
accuracy of single-aiming movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Percep-
tion and Performance 7, 100771018.