Psychopathy To Altruism: Neurobiology of The Selfish-Selfless Spectrum
Psychopathy To Altruism: Neurobiology of The Selfish-Selfless Spectrum
Psychopathy To Altruism: Neurobiology of The Selfish-Selfless Spectrum
Psychopathy to Altruism:
Neurobiology of the Selfish–Selfless
Spectrum
James W. H. Sonne 1* and Don M. Gash 2
1
Department of Health Professions, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, United States, 2 Department of Neuroscience,
University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, United States
The age-old philosophical, biological, and social debate over the basic nature of
humans as being “universally selfish” or “universally good” continues today highlighting
sharply divergent views of natural social order. Here we analyze advances in biology,
genetics and neuroscience increasing our understanding of the evolution, features
and neurocircuitry of the human brain underlying behavior in the selfish–selfless
spectrum. First, we examine evolutionary pressures for selection of altruistic traits
in species with protracted periods of dependence on parents and communities for
subsistence and acquisition of learned behaviors. Evidence supporting the concept
Edited by: that altruistic potential is a common feature in human populations is developed.
Roumen Kirov,
Institute of Neurobiology (BAS), To go into greater depth in assessing critical features of the social brain, the two
Bulgaria extremes of selfish–selfless behavior, callous unemotional psychopaths and zealous
Reviewed by: altruists who take extreme measures to help others, are compared on behavioral traits,
Sue Llewellyn,
structural/functional neural features, and the relative contributions of genetic inheritance
The University of Manchester,
United Kingdom versus acquired cognitive learning to their mindsets. Evidence from population groups
Livia Colle, ranging from newborns, adopted children, incarcerated juveniles, twins and mindfulness
Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy
meditators point to the important role of neuroplasticity and the dopaminergic reward
*Correspondence:
James W. H. Sonne systems in forming and reforming neural circuitry in response to personal experience
[email protected] and cultural influences in determining behavior in the selfish–selfless spectrum. The
underlying neural circuitry differs between psychopaths and altruists with emotional
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to processing being profoundly muted in psychopaths and significantly enhanced in
Psychopathology, altruists. But both groups are characterized by the reward system of the brain shaping
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
behavior. Instead of rigid assignment of human nature as being “universally selfish” or
Received: 04 January 2018
“universally good,” both characterizations are partial truths based on the segments of
Accepted: 05 April 2018 the selfish–selfless spectrum being examined. In addition, individuals and populations
Published: 19 April 2018 can shift in the behavioral spectrum in response to cognitive therapy and social and
Citation: cultural experience, and approaches such as mindfulness training for introspection and
Sonne JWH and Gash DM (2018)
Psychopathy to Altruism: reward-activating compassion are entering the mainstream of clinical care for managing
Neurobiology of the Selfish–Selfless pain, depression, and stress.
Spectrum. Front. Psychol. 9:575.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00575 Keywords: heredity, social, cultural, genetic, neural circuitry, emotions, empathy, compassion
Adolphs et al., 2005; Schiller et al., 2009) in conjunction with the A correlation was also observed between offers and blood-
fusiform cortex (Hadjikhani and de Gelder, 2003; Britton et al., oxygen level dependent increases in fMRI activation of the
2008). The amygdala may play a central role in regulating an primary somatosensory cortex and other associated areas during
individual’s aversion to or propensity for social interaction based the observation of the painful hand stimulation, supporting
on the activation of the brain’s reward mechanisms during the a hypothesis of empathic behavior as a form of “self-other
reading of facial expressions and the subsequent regulation of resonance” as a result of “neural resonance” between individuals.
comfort in social situations. While undergoing fMRI the subjects were then asked to imitate
An interesting sub-population is political orientation, which faces that were displayed to them. The subjects who donated
is increasingly recognized as having a genetic as well as a more money to low socio-economic players tended to exhibit
social and cultural influence (Kandler et al., 2012; Hatemi and greater levels of BOLD increases in fMRI in the left amygdala and
McDermott, 2012), including dopamine neurochemical receptor also the left fusiform cortex which is a region responsible for facial
variant expression (Settle et al., 2010). In these sub-populations processing and implicated in empathy.
we again see the role of the amygdala in making social-based Kim et al. (2010) observed a genetic variation on the oxytocin
snap judgments when presented with images of faces. Bilateral receptor gene OXTR at rs53576 between cultures (Kim et al.,
amygdalar activation recorded by fMRI positively predicts a 2010). In individualistic European Americans a guanine (G) is
participant’s snap decision to vote for a person based solely on more prevalent at this position, but in collectivist East Asians an
appearance, a phenomenon that was observed across cultures adenine (A) is more common. Oxytocin is a neurohormone that
(Rule et al., 2010). Schreiber et al. (2013) found increasing brain is primarily involved in stimulating contractions of the uterine
activity in the right amygdala amongst Republican voters versus wall during childbirth and the milk “let-down” reflex of lactation
increased anterior insular activation in Democrats, suggesting during nursing. Oxytocin also acts on the central nervous
different limbic processes are involved in reaching decisions in system for brain development and to regulate behavior including
a risk-taking task. These findings were supported in separate maternal behaviors such as infant response and protection,
studies by Kanai et al. (2011) reported that the gray matter and other social behaviors including bonding, trusting, and
volume of the right amygdala was observed to be larger in encouraging generosity (Yang et al., 2013). Nasally administered
individuals self-described as more conservative, contrasted with oxytocin reduces fear and anxiety (Kirsch et al., 2005), increases
those self-described as more liberal who exhibited greater gray trust (Kosfeld et al., 2005), reduces xenophobic outgroup
matter volume in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). rejection (Marsh et al., 2017), increases monogamous behavior
The ACC is involved in many executive level brain functions, (Scheele et al., 2012), increases empathy (Sheng et al., 2013)
including reward-based decision making, error detection, and and conformity with in-group members (Stallen et al., 2012).
conflict monitoring. An example of a task that activates the error Some benefits of oxytocin beyond promoting positive social
detection/conflict monitoring function of the ACC is the “Stroop interactions include anti-inflammatory effects and indications
task” (Pardo et al., 1990). In this task the name of a color is for quicker wound healing (Gouin et al., 2010). It is being
written in a different color of ink, for example the word “RED” investigated as a treatment for the social deficits of autism (Dadds
is written in blue ink, and the subject is asked to name the et al., 2014a), however, the efficacy reported in initial studies
word and ignore the color of the ink (or vice versa). This task has been mixed with results ranging from modest benefits to no
activates the ACC. The ACC also may serve as an evaluative role observed improvements (Young and Barrett, 2015).
after effortful error commission, producing emotional distress Vasopressin has also been identified as a possible regulator
associated with the act of producing an error (Bush et al., 2000). of compassionate behavior. Human studies of individuals that
Thus, the ACC is thought to be responsible for adapting behavior exhibit strong sibling bonding (Bachner-Melman et al., 2005) and
in response to the production of errors (Luu and Pederson, pair bonding (Walum et al., 2008) have identified that increases in
2004). The perigenual region of the ACC may also play a role the length of vasopressin 1a receptor repeat sequences 1 and 3 are
in modulating the reward mechanisms in a way perceived as linked to these behaviors in these sub-groups. Vasopressin, also
gratitude at the relief of a stressor (Fox et al., 2015) and as the called antidiuretic hormone or arginine vasopressin hormone
result of positive social interactions (Van den Bos et al., 2007). (AVP), is a neurohormone that controls the antidiuretic effect
As a part of the social species, these functions may be critical in through water reabsorption in the kidneys’ nephrons and by
maintaining alliances and raising offspring through a protracted controlling the constriction of blood vessels. In terms of social
stage of dependency. behavior, central vasopressin receptors AVPR1a in the ventral
In one study (Christoy-Moore, 2016), subjects’ donations to pallidum of the prairie vole are necessary for pair bonding and
individuals based solely on profiles listing socio-economic status partner selection (Lim and Young, 2004) by activating the reward
were recorded (called the Dictator Game) and then activity levels circuitry during mating (Pitkow et al., 2001). This and other
in the subjects’ brains were measured by functional magnetic- central AVP receptors have been shown to play critical roles in
resonance imaging (fMRI) while observing video of a human social recognition and interpretation of social cues as well as
hand being punctured by a hypodermic needle, touched by a related stress pathways in knockout mouse models (Bielsky et al.,
cotton swab, or static without stimulation (called the Needle 2004; Wersinger et al., 2004).
Test). Subjects’ offers to low socio-economic status players in In addition to the strong evidence in animal studies that
the Dictator Game were positively correlated with the subject’s oxytocin and vasopressin are two neurohormones which play
own Empathic Concern score determined by questionnaire. important roles in social behavior and the resultant reward and
stress pathways that support those behaviors, there is increasing TABLE 1 | PCI-R: psychopathy criminal focus (APA, 2013).
research supporting the hypothesis that genetic variants of OXTR
• Core features: Callous-unemotional – lacking empathy and lacking remorse
and AVPR1a are predictive of humans displaying greater degrees for mistreating others.
of altruistic, empathic and compassionate behavior traits within • Frequently breaking the law, being arrested.
population sub-groups. For example, Poulin et al. (2012) have • Pervasive dishonesty, chronic lying.
reported that the amount of individual involvement in charitable • Dysfunctional planning.
activity and civic duties correlated with genetic variants. As • Impulsive, irritable and aggressive behavior.
expected, it was found that those with the OXTR rs53576 G • Fearless, reckless and irresponsible, endangering self and others.
to A variation or AVPR1a RS1/RS3 long to short variation
were more likely to exhibit “prosocial” behavior by being more
trusting of strangers, contributing more to charitable activities
a monolithic disorder (qualitative), or is it a syndrome
and participating in more civic duties.
with multiple interacting factors determining the extent and
phenotypic expression (quantitative)? To more fully evaluate the
hypothesis of multiple interacting factors, an alternate rating scale
EXTREME SELFISHNESS: CRIMINAL to the PCL-R has been developed, the Psychopathic Personality
PSYCHOPATHY Inventory-Revised (PPI-R, see Table 2) designed and validated to
measure more affective and interpersonal traits and to be used
Callous-unemotional criminal psychopaths epitomize extreme
in both criminal and non-criminal populations without a priori
antisocial behavior. These individuals are characterized by
assumptions of antisocial and criminal behavior (Skeem et al.,
aggression and violence with a long criminal record and frequent
2011; Dutton, 2016; Sorman et al., 2016).
incarceration. Their core behavioral pattern of pervasively
While the two different rating scales overlap in measures
violating the rights of others without remorse can begin as early as
such as meanness (e.g., callous and unemotional, coldhearted),
3 years of age and continue into adulthood (Hare, 2006; Gao and
antisocial behavior (e.g., pervasive dishonesty, Machiavellian self-
Raine, 2010; APA, 2013). In the United States, they are estimated
interest) and poor planning skills, PPI-R includes the positive
to represent 16% of male prisoners (Kiehl and Hoffman, 2011).
traits of boldness (social dominance, immunity to stress and fear).
In England and Wales, the estimates are lower, close to 8% of
While all unsuccessful psychopaths are by definition criminals,
men and 2% of women (Coid et al., 2009b), perhaps due to
as mentioned before, successful psychopaths on the PPI-R scale
cultural differences between the countries. Serial killers fall into
are found in politics, medicine and business. Their actions range
this category. However, by maintaining an outwardly normal
from criminal to courageous, as evidenced by the high PPI-R
persona, they can often evade detection and arrest for periods
scores of national leaders of World War II: Franklin D. Roosevelt,
running into decades.
Winston Churchill, and Adolph Hitler (Dutton, 2016). Roosevelt
The spectrum of personality disorders classified as
and Churchill were powerful leaders whose bold, hardnosed
psychopathic is much broader than those on the extremist
(coldblooded) actions were instrumental in the survival and
criminal end. Psychopaths can be separated into two groups –
success of their social order; Hitler’s leadership epitomizing
unsuccessful and successful (Gao and Raine, 2010). The
callousness, lack of remorse and blame externalization was
unsuccessful are the callous-uncaring criminals. Successful
disastrous for all of Europe and led to notorious crimes against
psychopaths are a more diverse group ranging from ruthless con
humanity.
artists to leading statesmen (Dutton, 2016). Both unsuccessful
As successful psychopaths can intelligently conceal their
and successful psychopaths can exhibit varying combinations of
psychopathic traits, their number in the population is difficult
traits, which collectively predict their behavioral patterns. With
to detect. In one large population survey, the prevalence of
the legal and societal problems created by criminal psychopaths,
successful (i.e., no criminal record) psychopathic individuals
most research has been focused on defining their psychological
living in households in England was 0.6% (Coid et al., 2009a). In
features and neurobiology. The current criteria for determining if
the professional world of politicians, businessmen, doctors and
someone is a criminal psychopath is the Psychopathy Checklist-
lawyers, the number may be much higher. In their book Snakes
Revised (Hare, 2003; Babiak and Hare, 2006), which is crafted
in Suits: When Psychopaths go to Work, industrial psychologist
for clinical and legal use, emphasizing antisocial and criminal
behaviors. It is used worldwide and its influence is seen in
the gold standard for clinical diagnosis, the 5th Edition of the
American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical TABLE 2 | PPI-R: psychopathy positive and negative features (Skeem et al., 2011;
Dutton, 2016).
Manual of Mental Disorders (APA, 2013) where psychopathy is
described as a synonym for Antisocial Personality Disorder (see • Coldblooded
Table 1). • Powerful social dominance
As Hare developed his PCL-R as a research tool based largely • Fear immunity
on his experience in analyzing criminals, its use more broadly • Stress immunity
• Machiavellian self-interest
in formulating public policy, in business and in conducting • Rebellious non-conformity
unbiased social research is controversial (Skeem et al., 2011). • Blame externalization
Also, it has raised a major scientific issue. Is psychopathy • Carefree living (no planning)
Babiak and criminal psychologist Hare (Babiak and Hare, 2006) social processing and behavior regulation (Anderson et al.,
estimated 3.5% of professionals in business possess strong 1999; Grossman, 2013). In individuals with pronounced conduct
psychopathic traits. While some professionals with psychopathic control problems, numerous studies have shown the amygdala
traits are criminals, others benefit the social order by boldly is smaller along with less gray matter volume in the frontal and
leading changes needed for cultures to adapt to ever changing temporal cortices (Yang et al., 2010; Rogers and De Brito, 2016).
environmental, economic, and political conditions. Hypoactive amygdala responses to stimuli of others in distress
are characteristic of children with the callous-unemotional trait
and associated with aggressive behavior (Lozier et al., 2014).
NEUROBIOLOGY OF PSYCHOPATHY Humans with bilateral amygdala lesions have impaired learning
of fear and responding to eminent danger (Bach et al., 2015;
Dysfunctional emotional processing is a defining feature of Klumpers et al., 2015). Bilateral amygdala lesions in rhesus
psychopathy (Anderson et al., 2017), from lacking empathy monkeys have significantly blunted stress responses (Raper et al.,
to possessing immunity to stress and fear. Meta-analyses of 2013). These findings strongly link the behavioral traits of the
26 studies found emotional recognition of facial expressions callous-unemotional trait, boldness, and fear/stress immunity to
and vocal cues was significantly impaired in young and adult amygdala functions.
psychopaths for all of the basic emotions: anger, disgust, fear, The hypothalamus is intimately involved as the control center
happiness, sadness, and surprise (Dawel et al., 2012). Their of the brain for autonomic responses and regulation of sex
inability to recognize fear and sadness was especially pronounced, hormones and the secretion of oxytocin, cortisol, and vasopressin
but they also exhibit reduced neural response to laughter into the bloodstream. Low oxytocin levels have been linked
(O’Nions et al., 2017). Such blunted emotions affect perceptions, with callous-unemotional scores in adolescents (Levy et al.,
thought processes and actions toward others, fostering both 2017). Supporting this link are other studies indicating that
boldness and lack of remorse. Dysfunctional emotions also affect inactivation of the oxytocin receptor by DNA methylation is
another trait of criminal psychopaths – deeply flawed reasoning, correlated with an increased risk of callous-unemotional traits
including moral judgment. (Cecil et al., 2014). Children and adolescents with the callous-
Three of the sites in the brain responsible for criminal unemotional trait exhibit reduced cortisol response (von Polier
psychopathic behavior are also principal components of the et al., 2013; Grotzinger et al., 2018) perhaps explaining increased
neural circuitry for normal social-emotional processing: the boldness/impulsivity, and are at high risk for developing
prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala, and hypothalamus (Figure 2). into criminal psychopaths (Kahn et al., 2013; Kimonis et al.,
Psychopathic behavior resulting from injury or disease implicates 2016). As discussed earlier, oxytocin and vasopressin are two
the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) as a critical node for prosocial neurohormones that have essential roles in social behavior and
behavior; its dysfunction resulting in antisocial behavior the resultant reward and stress pathways that support those
(Anderson et al., 1999; Barrash et al., 2000; Trebuchon et al., behaviors (Poulin et al., 2012).
2013). The enlarged prefrontal cortex is the neocortical (i.e., The neural circuitry for the Impulsive-Antisocial dimension
evolutionarily newest) region of the human brain responsible for of psychopathy overlaps with that for Boldness, but differs in
top down, executive control; while the amygdala is the part of important details. The size of the striatum, a major component
the allocortical (i.e., evolutionarily old) cortex that, as discussed of the basal ganglia, is larger in Impulsive-Antisocial individuals
earlier, integrates sensory and acquired information, including (Korponay et al., 2017), especially the putamen and the nucleus
facial features, to assess threat levels. In the mammalian brain, accumbens in the ventral striatum, the reward center of the brain
the PFC is richly networked with the amygdala (McDonald (Figure 3).
et al., 1999), with neurons in the cerebral neocortex sending Dopamine release from ventral tegmental area dopamine
fibers to connect with neurons in the amygdala embedded in neurons innervating the nucleus accumbens is an essential
the rostral temporal lobe (Stein et al., 2007). Like most neural component of the reward circuitry (Sesack and Grace, 2010;
assemblages in the brain, it is a two-way street with amygdaloidal Kahn and Shohamy, 2013). In functional MRI studies, the
neurons sending fibers to the cerebral cortex. The amygdala in ventral striatum including the nucleus accumbens displays robust
turn is richly interconnected with the hypothalamus (Herman, activation in criminal psychopaths in game tasks involving
2012), another evolutionarily ancient brain region that regulates rewards (Pujara et al., 2014). The nucleus accumbens is a major
homeostasis and autonomic nervous system activity (Jansen et al., component of the limbic system (Morgane et al., 2005), the
1995) and controls neuroendocrine functions including secretion part of the human brain that provides emotional processing
of oxytocin and vasopressin into the systemic circulation (Carter, and motivational information to the enlarged, more deeply
2014). layered areas of the prefrontal cerebral cortex (Shnitko and
The association between medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) Robinson, 2014). Other interacting centers include the amygdala
dysfunction and psychopathic behavioral features including and hippocampus (de la Mora et al., 2010).
lack of empathy and remorse, dishonesty, and poor planning As recently emphasized by Reidy et al. (2017), reward-
and decision-making skills, has been extensively documented dominant learning and decision making in callous-unemotional
since the index case of Phineas T. Gage in 1848 (Harlow, individuals with strong Impulsive-Antisocial traits can explain
1868; Damasio et al., 1994). Since then, there has been an their extremely violent behavior, their hair-trigger response for
abundance of research supporting the role of the PFC in evoking rage or anger (the fight component of “Fight or Flight”
FIGURE 2 | Prefrontal cortex-amygdala-hypothalamic circuitry. The prefrontal cortex-amygdala-hypothalamus axis has a pivotal role in social-emotional processing.
Developmental disorders and injuries effecting its neural assemblages and circuitry can lead to antisocial behaviors characterizing psychopathy. (A) In this
parasagittal view of the human brain, the spatial relationships and neural connectivity between the prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala (Amy), and hypothalamus (Hyp)
are illustrated. Lesions involving the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) are especially disruptive of prosocial behavior. Note the central role of the amygdala in the circuitry
linking the prefrontal cortex and hypothalamus in the emotional processing network. Also see its intimate integration of the amygdala with the head of the
hippocampus (Hip), which initiates and consolidates cognitive memory and learning processes in the brain. (B) Here is a schematic based on the generic mammalian
brain to illustrate the multiple actions taken by the hypothalamus when a “Fight or Flight” response is triggered by cortical-amygdala interactions signaling high levels
of risk or immediate danger, including life or death situations. Neurons in the hypothalamus terminating in the pituitary release oxytocin (OT), vasopressin (VP) and
adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) into the systemic circulation. The sympathetic nervous system is fully activated via the hypothalamus, including by a direct
neural projection to the adrenal medulla stimulating release of epinephrine (EPI, adrenalin) and norepinephrine (NEP, noradrenalin) into the blood stream. ACTH
stimulates the release of the stress hormone cortisol (COR) from the adrenal cortex. The physiological responses include hyperarousal, focused vision, increased
heart rate and blood pressure, blood shunted to the muscles, and suppression of digestion and appetite. Illustrated by Matt Hazard.
FIGURE 3 | Dopamine reward system of the brain. (A) Dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) directly innervate the nucleus accumbens (NAc),
prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala (Amy), and hippocampus (Hip) (de la Mora et al., 2010; Kahn and Shohamy, 2013; Shnitko and Robinson, 2014). As in most
neural networks, connectivity between the nuclei goes both ways. Dopamine release from the VTA promotes feelings of satisfaction, pleasure and euphoria,
rewarding and motivating behavior. Dopamine release from the substantia nigra in the striatum modulates motor functions. (B) While the neurocircuitry modulating
the VTA and NAc is complex, major projections from the PFC, Amy and Hip to the VTA and NAc have been identified (Sesack and Grace, 2010). This is consistent
with known modulation of reward system dopaminergic activity being influenced by goal-directed behavior (PFC), emotions and feelings (Amy) and
experience/memories (Hip) (Sesack and Grace, 2010). Illustrated by Matt Hazard.
behavior). In these individuals, rage and impulsivity with reduced The absence of normal fear and stress responses is analogous
fear from the amygdala evokes pleasure – dopamine release in the to removing the brakes on a bulldozer. Boldness and greatly
nucleus accumbens. muted fear with positive reinforcement from the reward system
of the brain is the result. For criminal psychopaths, it leads of callous-unemotional behavior in their adopted child. The
to clashing violently with others and taking risks that lead to effect was dose-dependent, adoptive mothers with high positive
incarceration, debilitating injuries or death. reinforcement completely buffered the expression of the callous-
unemotional behavior at 27 months (Hyde et al., 2016; Viding and
Pingault, 2016). Follow-up studies over time will be extremely
NATURE VERSUS NURTURE IN important to assess the efficacy of early positive parenting on
PSYCHOPATHY adolescent and adult behavior patterns.
Even with the caveat that longer longitudinal studies are
Based on parent reported data on 5092 twins, genetically modeled needed, the malleability of callous-unemotional behavior in early
inheritance for the core feature of psychopathy – callousness childhood is encouraging. But as a developmental neural disorder
and unemotional trait – was 70% (Henry et al., 2016). A smaller with structural neuroanatomical abnormalities (Cope et al., 2014)
more recent study examining the reliability of parent reported and impaired functional connectivity (Harenski et al., 2018),
data refines the estimate to 47% (Moore et al., 2017). For the how long is the window of opportunity open for therapeutic
traits of Boldness and Impulsive-Antisociality, twin studies have intervention for criminal psychopaths? Working with juvenile
reported inheritance in the 40–50% range (Blonigen et al., 2005) delinquents, over half of whom had committed a serious violent
and indicating the two traits were not linked genetically and differ felony, a program at the Mendota Juvenile Treatment Center in
in their neurobiology. Wisconsin using intensive therapy balancing punishment for bad
Some progress has been made in identifying genes associated behavior with rewards for improved behavior was able to reduce
with features of psychopathy. Consistent with data reported the number of crimes by over 35% perpetrated by the trainees
throughout this review that oxytocin has a prominent role in over a 4–5 years period after release, compared to a treatment-as-
promoting prosocial human behavior, two independent studies usual control group (Caldwell and Van Rybroek, 2005; Caldwell,
have recently found a high association between an oxytocin 2011). While the effects on violent behavior were impressive,
receptor gene and callous-unemotional traits (Beitchman et al., there remained a significant risk for aggressive behavior injuring
2012; Dadds et al., 2014b). Endogenous oxytocin is important others.
in neural development including neural circuitry (Carter, 2014), The Mendota Center approach of effectively reinforcing
making it difficult to predict the effects from administration of the reward center of the brain for improved behavior, while
exogenous oxytocin to the adult brain after critical periods in providing negative reinforcement for bad behavior, is now being
brain development. Initial studies indicate nasal administration tested elsewhere for younger adolescents with strong callous-
of oxytocin increases aggressive behavior in normal adults unemotional traits (Hagerty, 2017). However, treatments for
(Ne’eman et al., 2016) and adults with antisocial personality adult criminal psychopaths have been notoriously ineffective and
disorder (Alcorn et al., 2015). upon release 90% commit another violent crime within 20 years
(Anderson and Kiehl, 2014).
Role of Neuron-Based Heredity in
Psychopathy
To what extent can neuronal-based heredity – learned EXTREME SELFLESSNESS: ZEALOUS
societal and cultural traits – compensate for strong genetic ALTRUISM
psychopathic predispositions? This acquisition of social and
cultural information begins before birth (Partanen et al., 2013) Placing the interests of others above one’s own safety occurs
as newborns recognize their mother’s voice and can distinguish so regularly in the United States and the rest of the world
it from a stranger’s voice (Beauchemin et al., 2011). Prenatal that it garners little media attention (Ricard, 2013, 2015). Only
development and functioning of neural systems is evident after when there is serious injury or death of the Good Samaritans
birth with the pattern of crying of newborns shaped by their does it make the news, such as in May 2017 when a crazed
native language (Mampe et al., 2009). Positive parental support man approached two young women on a commuter train
and maternal behavior is exceptionally important during these in Portland, Oregon brandishing a knife and screaming anti-
critical periods. Chaotic home environments, negative parental Muslim insults. Three strangers rushed to rescue the girls. Two
behavior and mothers with strong callous-unemotional traits died and the third was seriously injured (Kristof, 2017). As
can affect their fetus’s and infant’s emotional and cognitive the list of Carnegie Medal recipients for Extraordinary Civilian
development (Fontaine et al., 2011; Hyde et al., 2016; Viding and Heroism shows (CarnegieHero.Org, 2017), altruists range from
Pingault, 2016). adolescents to aged adults. Some are very young like 10 year-
In a study of 561 children adopted within several days after old Kiera Larsen who saved a 2-year old child from being run
birth where severe callous-unemotional maternal behavior was over by a car that then struck and killed her. They can be
replaced by strong positive support, major effects were found in old, like 72 year-old Louis Scharold who braved intense heat
altering behavior (Hyde et al., 2016). Having a biological mother from burning, wrecked trucks to reach through the broken
with severe callous-unemotional behavior predicted the same windshield of one vehicle to pull the dazed driver to safety.
traits in their children at 27 months of age, even though they had Twelve of the 94 Carnegie Medal Recipients in 2016 lost their
not been parenting them after adoption. Positive reinforcement lives in trying to save others. Again, routine altruistic actions
by the adoptive mother significantly mitigated the expression are commonplace and seldom make the news, but extraordinary
risks taken by some can and do lead to injury and death. and altruism. A linkage that has been repeatedly replicated
These zealous altruists on the extreme end of the spectrum are (Persson and Kajonius, 2016) and further modeled and linked
those who take extreme measures to help others, unnecessarily with oxytocin by Zak and Barraza (2013). The second factor
placing themselves in harm’s way, such as anonymous living is punishment of those violating social norms (Fowler, 2005).
kidney donors that partake in surgery to donate an organ to an The efficacy of punishment as in promoting cooperation has
unrelated and unknown recipient they will never meet (Tong been controversial. However, a meta-analysis of punishment
et al., 2012). and cooperation in 18 societies found punishment strongly
Altruism can be impulsive suggesting instinctive reactions promoted cooperation in societies with high trust levels (Balliet
as in the preceding cases, or premeditated by choosing to help and Van Lange, 2013). Also as Mussweiler and Ockenfels (2013)
others in ways that are knowingly risky indicating involvement have demonstrated, perceived similarity promotes altruistic
of executive functions, such as the actions of David Eubank, cooperation as well as evoking increased punishment for norm
the American Aid Worker who rescued a young girl, the lone violations. The result, they suggest, enhances the ability for
survivor of about 70 civilians massacred by ISIS fighters as similar individuals to build strong, stable socially cooperative
they tried to free Mosul in June of 2017. Braving sniper fire groups.
with some support from Iraqi and US Forces, Eubank ran into
the street, picked the girl up and brought her back to safety
(Yuccas, 2017). Eubank has repeatedly chosen to go to war- NEUROBIOLOGY OF ALTRUISM
ravaged areas in Asia to aid children and others in need. Less
dramatic, but equally extreme premeditated acts of generosity Implicit mind reading skills (gene-based) are present by the first
are those of altruists who donate one of their kidneys to help 4–7 months of life with normally developing infants making
an unknown anonymous patient (Marsh et al., 2014), placing face-to-face communication that activates the medial prefrontal
their own lives at risk from complications of elective surgery and cortex (Grossman et al., 2008; Kovacs et al., 2010; Urakawa et al.,
the removal of an organ. Other less risky altruistic premeditated 2015). Most or all neurocognitive mind reading skills are acquired
actions include being a blood donor or donating bone marrow for and strongly shaped by culture (neuron-based), with the early
transplantation. stages of learning similar to those of learning to read (Heyes and
Thus, the conundrum of altruism – taking risks by placing the Frith, 2014). Infants are very vulnerable, so development of the
interests of others, often strangers, beyond one’s self-interest – ability to distinguish between those who can help versus those
seems to directly violate the “survival of the fittest” principle of who may pose a risk is essential. Strong evidence from many
gene-based evolution. Darwin noted both sides of the issue in research groups indicates that the majority of infants as early as
The Descent of Man. He astutely recognized that selfishness was 6–10 months of age prefer and selectively approach individuals
a roadblock to human social evolution, “Selfish and contentious displaying intentional prosocial behavior (Hamlin and Wynn,
people will not cohere, and without coherence nothing can be 2011; Holvoet et al., 2016; Van de Vondervoort and Hamlin,
effected.” However, Darwin continued, writing, “He who was 2017). Also by 1–2 years of age, simple reciprocal interactions
ready to sacrifice his life, as many a savage has been, rather than elicit an early form of altruistic behavior, the child helping the
betray his comrades, would often leave no offspring to inherit his experimenter or a stranger obtain an object clearly out of their
noble nature” (Darwin, 1871). Darwin (1871) proposed that as reach (Barragan and Dweck, 2014). By 5 years of age, children
ancestral human reasoning and foresight powers increased, the are capable of sharing with others and anticipating reciprocation
benefits of reciprocal social assistance would become obvious and (Sebastian-Enesco and Warneken, 2015).
gradually lead to inherited reciprocal benevolence. Altruism goes well beyond reciprocation by compassionately
Darwin seems to have been justified, as just discussed there is helping a stranger with no apparent self-benefit and at some
strong evidence for both instinctive and cognitive benevolence in risk to one’s own being. Neuroimaging studies have provided
our species. This evolution has occurred thanks to a capability important insights into the neural networks underlying the
of the mind Darwin did not anticipate. Namely, our species behavioral linkage between empathy and compassion. As defined
possesses enhanced mind-reading skills to understand our own by de Waal and Preston (2017), empathy is a “term for all
thoughts and emotions and what others are thinking and feeling processes that emerge from the fact that observers understand
(Heyes and Frith, 2014; de Waal and Preston, 2017). Affective others’ states by activating their own personal, neural, and
perception of other’s emotions includes the six basic emotional mental representations of that state.” Empathy then is a
states visualized by facial expressions and body language: anger, passive state of feeling. Physical pain or distress, and empathy
fear, surprise, sadness, disgust, and happiness. Feeling and from witnessing pain both activate the same higher brain
understanding the emotions of others, the recognition of signals areas, the anterior insula and anterior to mid cingulate cortex
of need, sets the stage for perception-based actions such as (Figure 4) (Lamm et al., 2007, 2011). Depending on the
benevolence and compassion. type of distress, other brain areas such as the primary motor
With mind-reading skills at work, two major interactive and somatosensory cortices are recruited to simulate in the
factors appear to be crucial for promoting and maintaining broad observer the neural activity in distressed individual: a vivid
levels of cooperation within human populations. The first, as example of the continuing theater of the mind envisioned
hypothesized by Batson et al. (1981, 1983), Toi and Batson (1982) by the founder of modern psychology William James over
and tested in a series of studies, is a strong link between empathy 100 years ago where one’s own thoughts and sensations
FIGURE 4 | Active cortical sites for fear, distress, and empathy. Two deep cortical regions, the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex are strongly activated
when feeling fear and empathy. Both are strongly interconnected with the amygdala (Stein et al., 2007). (A) The insula lies beneath the temporal and cortical lobes
and can be seen by separating the two lobes. (B) The anterior cingulate gyrus is the deepest cortical region of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) as seen in this sagittal
section, and caps the anterior corpus callosum. Illustrated by Matt Hazard.
are blended with ongoing experience to produce emotions between Zealous Altruists (individuals who had donated one
and feelings. Empathy for another in extreme pain can be of their kidneys for transplant recipients) and Criminal
extremely painful (Fitzgibbon et al., 2010; Ricard, 2015). Psychopaths. Brain size can be larger than normal controls in
Intense and repeated exposure to distress can lead to severe zealous altruists and, even controlling for the brain volume, the
emotional and health problems including empathetic distress right amygdala is larger (Marsh et al., 2014). As reviewed earlier,
and post-traumatic stress syndrome (APA, 2013; Klimecki et al., in psychopaths with conduct control dysfunction, the amygdala
2014). is smaller along with less gray matter volume in the frontal
As opposed to the passive state of empathy, compassion is and temporal cortices (Yang et al., 2010; Pardini et al., 2014;
taking action to help others, including other species, in distress. Rogers and De Brito, 2016). In contrast to psychopathy where
Synonyms for compassion that help define it are benevolence, responses to all facial expressions of emotion are muted, altruists
kindness and sympathy. Compassionate actions activate the show an enhanced responsiveness to fearful facial expressions
reward system of the brain, the ventral striatum and the and diminished responsiveness to anger (Marsh et al., 2014).
dopaminergic ventral tegmental area, as well as the medial The neural pathways involved in compassion are similar to
orbitofrontal cortex (Klimecki et al., 2014). The positive affect those for impulsive-antisocial behavior. In this instance, prosocial
from compassion not only reinforces benevolent behavior, but compassionate behavior rather than antisocial behavior activates
can also calm painful empathetic feelings (Figure 5). the dopamine reward system of the brain. The same neural
Neuroimaging studies on adult Europeans making decisions systems are engaged (hippocampus, ventral striatum and ventral
on altruistic giving have identified two strongly engaged tegmental area, prefrontal cortex and amygdala). The major
brain areas. Activity in anterior insula predicted generosity in difference between prosocial and antisocial behavior is likely
individuals influenced by emotional empathy, while activity embedded in the perception of social order. This difference
in the temporoparietal cortical junction was associated with is wittily broached in a letter by Walpole (1769) in which he
cognitive empathetic giving (Tusche et al., 2016). Another study declared, “this world is a comedy to those that think; a tragedy
taking a similar approach testing young European adults reported to those that feel – a solution of why Democritus laughed and
that volume of gray matter in the right temporoparietal cortical Heraclitus wept.” For altruists, the full capability for processing
junction was strongly positively correlated with the maximal and integration of sensory and acquired information in the
acceptable cost for an altruistic action (Morishima et al., 2012). reading and feeling of emotions of others engenders taking
The results from both studies are supportive of the concept of positive actions – compassion – that mitigates the altruist’s own
altruistic potential being a common feature in populations and distress and reflexively activates the reward system. With the
that can be evoked by empathetic feelings. muted neurocircuitry for basic emotions in criminal psychopaths,
Another approach for identifying brain areas engaged in taking action in the form of violent aggression actives the
altruistic giving is that taken by Marsh et al. (2014) using anger/fight component of the Fight or Flight response reflexively
neuroimaging to quantify structural and functional differences activating the reward system.
FIGURE 5 | Benevolence pathway. (A) Empathy involves literally feeling another’s pain. An individual with empathic responsiveness upon seeing and or hearing
others in pain mirrors that pain in their brain, activating the same higher cortical brain areas activated by fear, the anterior insula and mid-to-anterior cingulate cortex.
Intense exposure to a stressful event or repeated exposure to stress in others can lead to burnout, empathy fatigue or PTSD. (B) Compassion activates the reward
system of the brain and can significantly calm empathetic feelings of fear and pain. Therefore compassion and benevolence elicit positive feelings. Altruism not only
benefits the recipient, but also benefits the altruist by rewarding their behavior with feelings of satisfaction. Illustrated by Matt Hazard.
NATURE VERSUS NURTURE IN have high non-social anxiety. With cognitive deficits and overly
ALTRUISM trusting of others these individuals cannot live independently.
Several hundred genes, many of them rare copy variations,
Given that altruistic behavior can be expressed by large segments have been implicated as risk factors for dysfunctions in
of the population as discussed earlier, the genes underlying communication and mind reading that characterize autism
altruism may be commonplace. This includes genes for central spectrum disorders (Pinto et al., 2014; Sahin and Sur, 2015).
dopaminergic systems engaged in the reward system and genes The large number is consistent with the broad heterogeneity of
for oxytocin receptors that promote development of the social symptoms and their expression in autism. While alterations in
brain with strong capabilities for communication and mind oxytocin stimulatory proteins, oxytocin plasma levels and genetic
reading of emotions (Donaldson and Young, 2008; Carter, 2014; variance in oxytocin receptors have been reported as risk factors
Heyes and Frith, 2014). for autism (Jacobson et al., 2014; LoParo and Waldman, 2015),
If genes underlying altruism are the norm, then it can clinical trials on the efficacy of oxytocin therapy has found only
be predicted that gene variants and mutations resulting modest benefits or no positive effects (Young and Barrett, 2015).
in dysfunctional and antisocial behavior can be used to Collectively, the genetics underlying psychopathy, Williams
identify gene networks that are good candidates for promoting disease and autism indicate that no one gene is responsible for the
altruistic behavior. With the Callous-Unemotional trait found development of the human prosocial brain, but a broad network
in individuals at high risk for violent criminal behavior, genetic of interacting genes found in normal populations set the stage for
variants in the oxytocin receptor have been identified as discussed the second system of heredity (neuron-based) to sculpt prosocial
earlier. Dysfunctional social behavior is also found with the brain functions.
overexpression of oxytocin. Williams Syndrome, which results
from the loss of 28 genes, features dysregulation of oxytocin and Role of Neuron-Based Heredity in
vasopressin secretion by the hypothalamic-pituitary system (Dai Altruism
et al., 2012). Baseline plasma levels of oxytocin are three-times The human genome containing around 20,000 protein-encoding
higher and vasopressin levels 0.30% higher in Williams Syndrome genes can provide the basic blueprint for brain development,
than in controls. Individuals with Williams Syndrome possess but training and experiences in the early years from infancy
cognitive deficits with behavior characterized by diminished through childhood are crucially important in sculpting brain
social anxiety and fearfulness, readily approaching, socially development and function. As discussed earlier, prosocial
interacting with and trusting strangers (Jarvinen et al., 2013). behavior is displayed by most infants and preschool children.
However, they experience difficulty interacting with peers and Culture plays a major role in enhancing and strengthening
prosocial behavior. Even where genetic inheritance of the callous- human subjects 5 days after training (Leiberg et al., 2011),
unemotional trait is a significant risk factor for developing suggesting long-term processing and learning through reward
criminal psychopathic behavior, especially in chaotic, uncaring mechanisms. Reflecting on gratitude increases scores on a
environments, expression of risky behavior can be muted by early self-report measure of altruistic values and coincides with
adoption and raising with strong positive parenting. increased BOLD fMRI activation in the nucleus accumbens
The typical experience for a newborn is rapid bonding with a and vmPFC (Karns et al., 2017) indicating involvement of the
loving mother providing intensive high-attentive care. When this mesolimbic dopamine pathway for reward and reinforcement
does not happen and there is profound social deprivation early in learning of altruistic behavior (Strobel et al., 2011). Further
life, such as institutionalization in an orphanage, prosocial brain supporting the role of social learning as important in the
development is severely compromised. Connectivity between the cultivation of prosocial behavior are studies linking oxytocin,
prefrontal cortex and amygdala is altered and the amygdala- previously described for its prosocial and parental functions,
hypothalamic-pituitary stress axis affected (Gee et al., 2013; with dopamine circuitry. Oxytocin receptors and dopamine
McLaughlin et al., 2014). The constellation of problems found receptors coexist in the striatum, medial PFC, substantia
in institutionalized children include: smaller brain size with nigra and ventral tegmental area (Skuse and Gallagher, 2009).
reduced cortical thickness, deficits in cognitive and language Intranasal oxytocin in normal humans appeared to increase
functions, problems with emotional regulation, and increased the reward for reciprocated cooperation through increased
risk for psychotic symptoms (Tottenham et al., 2010; McLaughlin activation of the dopamine-activated, reward-linked nucleus
et al., 2014; Trotta et al., 2015; Bick and Nelson, 2017). accumbens during repeated iterations of Prisoner’s Dilemma
From these studies we see that development plays an game (Rilling et al., 2012) which supports the role of oxytocin
important role in cultivating behavior, and that early intervention and dopamine learning pathways in trust and reciprocation
can help recover normal development, even impacting adolescent behavior.
criminals. How much of this is learning and how much is Thus social and cultural behaviors and activities are
developmental determinism? Studies of economic games such as mechanisms for not only training the brain beginning in
the Prisoner’s Dilemma indicate wide differences in the display prenatal life, they are also principal components in determining
of prosocial behavior in single and iterative play (Fehr and brain development and dopaminergic reinforcement learning
Schmidt, 2003). However, a study of 102 adults participating in in the adult. Empathy and compassion are feelings with basic
a repetitive Social Gambling Task indicated that an individual’s genetic and neural underpinnings crucial for the development of
ability to learn how their actions impacted another’s outcome large interacting social communities characterizing our species.
led to more prosocial behavior (Kwak et al., 2014). In this Successful cultures have strong and effective implicit and explicit
context, learners were defined as those who made choices mechanisms for promoting and enhancing empathetic and
resulting in economic gain, whether for themselves or others, compassionate behavior. Empathy and compassion are core
and is mathematical in nature. On a neuroscientific basis, values in most, if not all, of the world’s major religions. Buddhist
learning occurs with the release of dopamine in the reward methods for training the mind through meditation and yoga
and reinforcement centers of the brain including regions of (Tomasino et al., 2014) are an example of positive approaches
the striatum, such as the nucleus accumbens, from the ventral to enhance empathy and compassion that have been secularized
tegmental area and substantia nigra. Rodents have been observed and entered mainstream Western culture and medicine through
to learn the conditions of an experimental shock more quickly mindfulness practices (Gotink et al., 2015).
after vicariously observing other rodents receive the shock during While this review has focused on those populating the
such conditioning experiments. These rodents vicariously learn extremes of the social, behavioral, and cognitive spectrums, the
the conditions of the experimental shock more rapidly if they question remains as to whether individuals within a cultural
also have experienced the shock for themselves, regardless of norm can alter their behavior to become more compassionate
whether that shock was experienced within the context of the and display less selfish behavior. For this non-clinical cohort
conditioning experiment or not (Sanders et al., 2013; Lahvis, there exists an interesting non-clinical approach encompassed
2017). Rodents that had these vicarious learning experiences by mindfulness-based and compassion meditation. In one
while hearing pain-induced vocalizations in others exhibited BOLD fMRI based study of expert and novice compassion
increased activation of both dopamine and serotonin circuitry meditators, an increase in activity was observed in the ACC,
and the ACC (Kim et al., 2014) suggesting both empathy amygdala, and the insular cortex (among other regions) of
and learning through limbic systems and dopaminergic reward expert meditators during meditation compared with non-
mechanisms. These studies on vicarious or empathic learning meditative rest in response to negative emotional sounds (Lutz
are supported by studies in humans showing experience of a et al., 2008), indicating that compassion meditation increased
painful stimulus increases empathy in human observers (Eklund activation of these emotion-processing limbic regions that
et al., 2009; Preis and Kroener-Herwig, 2012) which coincide are connected to the prefrontal cortex (also see Fox et al.,
with BOLD fMRI data indicating that the perception of pain 2016). These include regions implicated in the selfish–selfless
in others is neurologically similar to the actual sensation of spectrum as outlined previously in this text. The degree
pain (Preis et al., 2015). Participation in the Zurich Prosocial of activation of these regions also correlated with the self-
Game, a computer-based compassion training game that requires reported depth of meditation and the degree of meditative
cooperation, has been shown to increase helping behavior in training, indicating that additional experience activates these
regions of the brain to a greater degree. Some groups have social debate over “universal selfishness” or “universal goodness”
attempted to bring this practice to the clinical psychology setting characterizing human behavior. Both positions are partial truths
(Fox et al., 2016). One study found that 12 weeks of mindfulness- based on the components of the Selfish–Selfless Spectrum being
based intervention with compassion meditation in a group with observed. As a lens into the social brain, the extremes of
social anxiety disorder resulted in a significant improvement the Selfish–Selfless Spectrum defined by callous, unemotional
in social anxiety symptoms, depression, social adjustment, and psychopaths and dynamic, zealous altruists reveal the importance
enhancement of compassion, all compared to a control group of both genetic and neuron-based heredity systems and reward
that was placed on a waiting list at the beginning of the study processes in strongly influencing actions toward others and
(Koszycki et al., 2016). cooperative behavior. Critically, individuals with genes associated
In the general population, mindfulness-based practices have with developing dangerous social disorders such as callous,
been reported to promote a plethora of effects on the whole body: unemotional traits have the potential to modify those conditions
reducing stress hormones, reducing inflammation, promoting using cognitive therapeutic interventions (e.g., strong positive
pain relief and wound healing (Hofmann et al., 2011; Lutz et al., parenting, compassion training) to change where they reside
2013; Zeidan et al., 2015; Rosenkranz et al., 2016). Other forms on the social spectrum. Evidence from population groups
of meditation including concentrative eye-gazing and controlled ranging from incarcerated juveniles, adopted children, twins,
breathing have been shown to result in voluntary activation and meditators point to the important role of neuroplasticity
of components of the sympathetic nervous system, changes in and reward learning circuitry in forming and reforming of
plasma catecholamine and serum cortisol concentrations, and neural connections that determine our behavior. Success with
an attenuation of the innate immune response as measured by treatment programs promoting positive behavior via the brain
plasma cytokines (Kox et al., 2014) and IL-6 markers (Pace et al., reward system in these diverse groups suggests promise as a
2009). The study of altruism and psychopathy has implications therapeutic approach to mitigating violent, destructive behavior.
beyond these traits, as similar brain regions are affected in Post- Approaches involving introspection and promoting acts of
Traumatic Stress Disorder (Keding and Herringa, 2016; Rinne- compassion that activate the reward system, such as mindfulness
Albers et al., 2017). Yoga and controlled eye movement therapies, training, are entering the mainstream of clinical treatment for
called Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR), pain management, depression, stress, and behavior modification.
are techniques used for post-traumatic stress (Zepeda Méndez
et al., 2018) and recognized by APA (2017) and United States
Department of Veterans Affairs (2017). This suggests meditative AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
or contemplative practices can affect acquired cognitive traits,
including promoting unselfish behavior. JS and DG contributed equally to the conception, design, writing
of the manuscript, revisions, and approval of the submitted
version.
SUMMARY
Altruism as envisioned by Auguste Comte exists in the general ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
population and in zealous altruists who anchor the benevolence
end of the Selfish–Selfless Spectrum. Advances in genetics, The authors would like to thank Matt Hazzard for contributing
psychology, and neurobiology have increased our understanding the medical illustrations and figures used in this text and Dr.
of social neurocircuitry in the human brain, providing critical Wayne Bell for the encouragement in the study of critical issues
insights into resolving ongoing philosophical, biological, and in neurotheology.
REFERENCES with functional neuroimaging. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 491–515.
doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0493-5
Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu. Rev. Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R.
Psychol. 60, 693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514 (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage
Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., and Damasio, in human prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1032–1037. doi: 10.1038/
A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala 14833
damage. Nature 433, 68–72. doi: 10.1038/nature03086 APA (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the APA (2017). Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) Therapy.
human amygdala. Nature 372, 669–672. doi: 10.1038/372669a0 Available at: http://www.apa.org/ptsd-guideline/treatments/eye-movement-
Alcorn, J. L. III, Rathnayaka, N., Swann, A. C., Moeller, F. G., and Lane, S. D. (2015). reprocessing.aspx [accessed February 15, 2018].
Effects of intranasal oxytocin on aggressive responding in antisocial personality Babiak, P., and Hare, R. D. (2006). Snakes in Suits: When Psychopaths Go to Work.
disorder. Psychol. Rec. 65, 691–703. doi: 10.1007/s440732-015-0139-y New York, NY: Regan Books.
Anderson, N. E., and Kiehl, K. A. (2014). Psychopathy: developmental perspectives Bach, D. R., Hurlemann, R., and Dolan, R. J. (2015). Impaired threat prioritisation
and their implications for treatment. Restor Neurol Neurosci 32, 103–117. after selective bilateral amygdala lesions. Cortex 63, 206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.
doi: 10.3233/RNN-139001 cortex.2014.08.017
Anderson, N. E., Steele, V. R., Maurer, J. M., Rao, V., Koenigs, M. R., Decety, J., Bachner-Melman, R., Zohar, A. H., Bacon-Schnoor, N., Elizur, Y., Nemanov, L.,
et al. (2017). Differentiating emotional processing and attention in psychopathy Gritsenko, I., et al. (2005). Link between vasopressin receptor AVPR1A
promoter region microsatellites and measures of social behavior in humans. Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., and Hare, R. D. (2009a). Prevalence and
J. Individ. Dif. 26, 2–10. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.26.1.2 correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain.
Balliet, D., and Van Lange, P. A. (2013). Trust, punishment, and cooperation across Int. J. Law Psychiatry 32, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.01.002
18 societies: a meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 363–379. doi: 10.1177/ Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., Moran, P., Bebbington, P., et al. (2009b).
1745691613488533 Psychopathy among prisoners in England and Wales. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 32,
Barragan, R. C., and Dweck, C. S. (2014). Rethinking natural altruism: simple 134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.02.008
reciprocal interactions trigger children’s benevolence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Cope, L. M., Ermer, E., Gaudet, L. M., Steele, V. R., Eckhardt, A. L., Arbabshirani,
U.S.A. 111, 17071–17074. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419408111 M. R., et al. (2014). Abnormal brain structure in youth who commit homicide.
Barrash, J., Tranel, D., and Anderson, S. W. (2000). Acquired personality Neuroimage Clin. 4, 800–807. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.002
disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal Dadds, M. R., MacDonald, E., Cauchi, A., Williams, K., Levy, F., and Brennan, J.
region. Dev. Neuropsychol. 18, 355–381. (2014a). Nasal oxytocin for social deficits in childhood autism: a randomized
Batson, C. D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T., and Birch, K. (1981). Is controlled trial. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 521–531. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-
empathetic emotion a source of altruistic motivation? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1899-3
290–302. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.2.290 Dadds, M. R., Moul, C., Cauchi, A., Dobson-Stone, C., Hawes, D. J., Brennan, J.,
Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M., and Isen, A. M. (1983). et al. (2014b). Polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with
Influence of self-reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic the development of psychopathy. Dev. Psychopathol. 26, 21–31. doi: 10.1017/
motivation to help. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 706–718. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514. S0954579413000485
45.3.706 Dai, L., Carter, C. S., Ying, J., Bellugi, U., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., and Korenberg,
Beauchemin, M., Gonzalez-Frankenberger, B., Tremblay, J., Vannasing, P., J. R. (2012). Oxytocin and vasopressin are dysregulated in Williams Syndrome,
Martinez-Montes, E., Belin, P., et al. (2011). Mother and stranger: an a genetic disorder affecting social behavior. PLoS One 7:e38513. doi: 10.1371/
electrophysiological study of voice processing in newborns. Cereb. Cortex 21, journal.pone.0038513
1705–1711. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq242 Dalai Lama, X. I. V. (2007). Preface in “Mind in Comfort and Ease”. Boston, MA:
Beitchman, J. H., Zai, C. C., Muir, K., Berall, L., Nowrouzi, B., Choi, E., Wisdom Publications.
et al. (2012). Childhood aggression, callous-unemotional traits and oxytocin Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M., and Damasio, A. R.
genes. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 21, 125–132. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012- (1994). The return of phineas gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a
0240-6 famous patient. Science 264, 1102–1105. doi: 10.1126/science.8178168
Bell, A. V., Richerson, P. J., and Mcelreath, R. (2009). Culture rather than genes Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. New York,
provides greater scope for the evolution of large-scale human prosociality. Proc. NY: D. Appleton and company.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 17671–17674. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903232106 Davidson, R. (2015). “The Four Constituents of Well-Being,” in Mindfulness &
Bick, J., and Nelson, C. A. (2017). Early experience and brain development. Wiley Well-Being at Work Conference, (Berkeley, California), 13–14.
Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 8:e1387. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1387 Dawel, A., O’kearney, R., Mckone, E., and Palermo, R. (2012). Not just fear
Bickart, K. C., Wright, C. I., Dautoff, R. J., Dickerson, B. C., and Barrett, L. F. and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits
(2011). Amygdala volume and social network size in humans. Nat. Neurosci. for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36,
14, 163–164. doi: 10.1038/nn.2724 2288–2304. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006
Bielsky, I. F., Hu, S. B., Szegda, K. L., Westphal, H., and Young, L. J. Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(2004). Profound impairment in social recognition and reduction in de la Mora, M. P., Gallegos-Cari, A., Arizmendi-García, Y., Marcellino, D., and
anxiety-like behavior in vasopressin V1a receptor knockout mice. Fuxe, K. (2010). Role of dopamine receptor mechanisms in the amygdaloid
Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 483–493. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300360 modulation of fear and anxiety: Structural and functional analysis. Prog.
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., and Iacono, W. G. Neurobiol. 90, 198–216. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.010
(2005). Psychopathic personality traits: heritability and genetic overlap with de Waal, F. B. M., and Preston, S. D. (2017). Mammalian empathy: behavioural
internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychol. Med. 35, 637–648. manifestations and neural basis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 498–509. doi: 10.1038/
doi: 10.1017/S0033291704004180 nrn.2017.72
Britton, J. C., Shin, L. M., Barret, L. F., Rauch, S. L., and Wright, C. I. (2008). Donaldson, Z. R., and Young, L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the
Amygdala and fusiform gyrus temporal dynamics: responses to negative facial neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322, 900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668
expressions. BMC Neurosci. 9:44. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-44 Dutton, K. (2016). Would you vote for a psychopath. Sci. Am. 27, 50–56.
Brown, S. L., and Brown, R. M. (2015). Connecting prosocial behavior to improved doi: 10.1038/scientificamericanmind0916-50
physical health: contributions from the neurobiology of parenting. Neurosci. Eisenberg, N., and Mussen, P. H. (1989). The Roots of Prosocial Behavior
Biobehav. Rev. 55, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j/neubiorev.2015.04.004 in Children. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/
Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences CBO9780511571121
in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364- Eklund, J., Andersson-Straberg, T., and Hansen, E. M. (2009). “I’ve also
6613(00)01483-2 experienced loss and fear”: effects of prior similar experience on empathy.
Caldwell, M. F. (2011). Treatment-related changes in behavioral outcomes of Scand. J. Psychol. 50, 65–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00673.x
psychopathy facets in adolescent offenders. Law Hum. Behav. 35, 275–287. Fehr, E., and Schmidt, K. (2003). “Theories of fairness and reciprocity: evidence
doi: 10.1007/s10979-010-9239-z and economic applications,” in Advances in Economics and Econometrics, eds
Caldwell, M. F., and Van Rybroek, G. J. (2005). Reducing violence in serious M. Dewatripont, L. P. Hansen, and S. J. Turnovsky (Cambridge: Cambridge
juvenile offenders using intensive treatment. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 28, 622–636. University Press), 208–257.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2004.07.001 Field, J., and Leadbeater, E. (2016). Cooperation between non-relatives in a
CarnegieHero.Org (2017). Carnegie Hero Fund [Online]. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie primitively eusocial paper wasp, polistes dominula. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond.
Hero Fund Commission. B. Biol. Sci. 371:20150093. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0093
Carter, C. S. (2014). Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Fitzgibbon, B. M., Giummarra, M. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Enticott, P. G., and
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 17–39. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115110 Bradshaw, J. L. (2010). Shared pain: from empathy to synaesthesia. Neurosci.
Cecil, C. A., Lysenko, L. J., Jaffee, S. R., Pingault, J. B., Smith, R. G., Relton, Biobehav. Rev. 34, 500–512. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.007
C. L., et al. (2014). Environmental risk, oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) Fontaine, N. M., Mccrory, E. J., Boivin, M., Moffitt, T. E., and Viding, E. (2011).
methylation and youth callous-unemotional traits: a 13-year longitudinal study. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and
Mol. Psychiatry 19, 1071–1077. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.95 conduct problems in childhood. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 730–742. doi: 10.1037/
Christoy-Moore, L. (2016). Self-other resonance, its control and prosocial a0022620
inclinations: brain-behavior relationships. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1544–1558. Fowler, J. H. (2005). Altruistic punishment and the origin of cooperation. Proc.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.23119 Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 7047–7049. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500938102
Fox, G. R., Kaplan, J., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. (2015). Neural correlates of Holvoet, C., Scola, C., Arciszewski, T., and Picard, D. (2016). Infants’ preference
gratitude. Front. Psychol. 6:1491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01491 for prosocial behaviors: a literature review. Infant Behav. Dev. 45, 125–139.
Fox, K. C., Dixon, M. L., Nijeboer, S., Gim, M., Floman, J. L., Lifshitz, M., et al. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.10.008
(2016). Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: a review and meta-analysis Hyde, L. W., Waller, R., Trentacosta, C. J., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban,
of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 65, J. M., et al. (2016). Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-
208–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021 unemotional behaviors. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 903–910. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.
Gao, Y., and Raine, A. (2010). Successful and unsuccessful psychopaths: a 2016.15111381
neurobiological model. Behav. Sci. Law 28, 194–210. doi: 10.1002/bsl.924 Jacobson, J. D., Ellerbeck, K. A., Kelly, K. A., Fleming, K. K., Jamison, T. R., Coffey,
Gardner, A. (2015). The genetical theory of multilevel selection. J. Evol. Biol. 28, C. W., et al. (2014). Evidence for alterations in stimulatory G proteins and
305–319. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12566 oxytocin levels in children with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 40, 159–169.
Gash, D. M., and Deane, A. S. (2015). Neuron-based heredity and human doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.11.014
evolution. Front. Neurosci. 9:209. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00209 Jansen, A. S., Nguyen, X. V., Karpitskiy, V., Mettenleiter, T. C., and Loewy, A. D.
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Humphreys, K. L., Telzer, (1995). Central command neurons of the sympathetic nervous system: basis
E. H., et al. (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala- of the fight-or-flight response. Science 270, 644–646. doi: 10.1126/science.270.
prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 5236.644
110, 15638–15643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307893110 Jarvinen, A., Korenberg, J. R., and Bellugi, U. (2013). The social phenotype of
Gotink, R. A., Chu, P., Busschbach, J. J., Benson, H., Fricchione, G. L., and Hunink, Williams syndrome. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 414–422. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.
M. G. (2015). Standardised mindfulness-based interventions in healthcare: 2012.12.006
an overview of systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs. PLoS One Kahn, I., and Shohamy, D. (2013). Intrinsic connectivity between the hippocampus,
10:e0124344. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124344 nucleus accumbens, and ventral tegmental area in humans. Hippocampus 23,
Gouin, J. P., Carter, C. S., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Glaser, R., Malarkey, W. B., 187–192. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22077
Loving, T. J., et al. (2010). Marital behavior, oxytocin, vasopressin, and wound Kahn, R. E., Byrd, A. L., and Pardini, D. A. (2013). Callous-unemotional traits
healing. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1082–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen. robustly predict future criminal offending in young men. Law Hum. Behav. 37,
2010.01.009 87–97. doi: 10.1037/b0000003
Grossman, S. E., Fontanini, A., Wieskopf, J. S., and Katz, D. B. (2008). Learning- Kanai, R., Feilden, T., Firth, C., and Rees, G. (2011). Political orientations are
related plasticity of temporal coding in simultaneously recorded amygdala- correlated with brain structure in young adults. Curr. Biol. 21, 677–680.
cortical ensembles. J. Neurosci. 28, 2864–2873. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4063- doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017
07.2008 Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., and Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political
Grossman, T. (2013). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in early social orientation: the roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative
cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:340. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013. mating, and personality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 633–645. doi: 10.1037/
00340 a0025560
Grotzinger, A. D., Mann, F. D., Patterson, M. W., Tackett, J. L., Tucker- Karns, C. M., Moore, W. E. III., and Mayr, U. (2017). The cultivation of pure
Drob, E. M., and Harden, K. P. (2018). Hair and salivary testosterone, hair altruism via gratitude: a functional MRI study of change with gratitude practice.
cortisol, and externalizing behaviors in adolescents. Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1177/ Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:599. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00599
0956797617742981 [Epub ahead of print]. Keding, T. J., and Herringa, R. J. (2016). Paradoxical prefrontal-amygdala
Hadjikhani, N., and de Gelder, B. (2003). Seeing fearful body expressions activates recruitment to angry and happy expressions in pediatric posttraumatic stress
the fusiform cortex and amygdala. Curr. Biol. 13, 2201–2205. doi: 10.1016/j.cub. disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 2903–2912. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.104
2003.11.049 Kennedy, P., Higginson, A. D., Radford, A. N., and Sumner, S. (2018). Altruism in
Hagerty, B. B. (2017). When Your Child Is a Psychopath. Available at: a volatile world. Nature 555, 359–362. doi: 10.1038/nature25965
https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2017/06/when-your-child- Kiehl, K. A., and Hoffman, M. B. (2011). The criminal psychopath: history,
is-a-psychopath/524502/ neuroscience, treatment, and economics. Jurimetrics 51, 355–397.
Hamlin, J. K., and Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial Kim, B. S., Lee, J., Bang, M., Am Seo, B., Khalid, A., Jung, M. W., et al. (2014).
others. Cogn. Dev. 26, 30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001 Differential regulation of observational fear and neural oscillations by serotonin
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health and dopamine in the mouse anterior cingulate cortex. Psychopharmacology 231,
Systems. 4371–4381. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3581-7
Hare, R. D. (2006). Psychopathy: a clinical and forensic overview. Psychiatr. Clin. Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., Sasaki, J. Y., Xu, J., Chu, T. Q., Ryu, C., et al.
North Am. 29, 709–724. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2006.04.007 (2010). Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact
Harenski, C. L., Calhoun, V. D., Bustillo, J. R., Haas, B. W., Decety, J., to influence emotional support seeking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107,
Harenski, K. A., et al. (2018). Functional connectivity during affective 1517–1521. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107
mentalizing in criminal offenders with psychotic disorders: associations with Kimonis, E. R., Kennealy, P. J., and Goulter, N. (2016). Does the self-report
clinical symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 271, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017. inventory of callous-unemotional traits predict recidivism? Psychol. Assess. 28,
11.003 1616–1624. doi: 10.1037/pas0000292
Harlow, J. M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Kirsch, P., Esslinger, C., Chen, Q., Mier, D., Lis, S., Siddhanti, S., et al.
Mass. Med. Soc. 2, 327–47. (2005). Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear
Hatemi, P. K., and McDermott, R. (2012). The genetics of politics: discovery, in humans. J. Neurosci. 25, 11489–11493. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3984-
challenges, and progress. Trends Genet. 28, 525–533. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2012. 05.2005
07.004 Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M., and Singer, T. (2014). Differential pattern
Henry, J., Pingault, J. B., Boivin, M., Rijsdijk, F., and Viding, E. (2016). of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Soc. Cogn.
Genetic and environmental aetiology of the dimensions of Callous- Affect. Neurosci. 9, 873–879. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst060
Unemotional traits. Psychol. Med. 46, 405–414. doi: 10.1017/S003329171500 Klumpers, F., Morgan, B., Terburg, D., Stein, D. J., and Van Honk, J. (2015).
1919 Impaired acquisition of classically conditioned fear-potentiated startle reflexes
Herman, J. P. (2012). Neural pathways of stress integration: relevance to alcohol in humans with focal bilateral basolateral amygdala damage. Soc. Cogn. Affect.
abuse. Alcohol Res. 34, 441–447. Neurosci. 10, 1161–1168. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu164
Heyes, C. M., and Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Korponay, C., Pujara, M., Deming, P., Philippi, C., Decety, J., Kosson, D. S.,
Science 344:1243091. doi: 10.1126/science.1243091 et al. (2017). Impulsive-antisocial dimension of psychopathy linked to
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., and Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness enlargement and abnormal functional connectivity of the striatum. Biol.
and compassion meditation: potential for psycho-logical interventions. Clin. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2, 149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.
Psychol. Rev. 31, 1126–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.003 07.004
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., and Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin rejection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 9314–9319. doi: 10.1073/pnas.
increases trust in humans. Nature 435, 673–676. doi: 10.1038/nature03701 1705853114
Koszycki, D., Thake, J., Mavounza, C., Daoust, J. P., Taljaard, M., and Bradwejn, J. Matusall, S. (2013). Social behavior in the “Age of Empathy”?-A social scientist’s
(2016). Preliminary investigation of a mindfulness-based intervention for social perspective on current trends in the behavioral sciences. Front. Hum. Neurosci.
anxiety disorder that integrates compassion meditation and mindful exposure. 7:236. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00236
J. Altern. Complement. Med. 22, 363–374. doi: 10.1089/acm.2015.0108 McDonald, A. J., Shammah-Lagnado, S. J., Shi, C., and Davis, M. (1999). Cortical
Kovacs, A. M., Teglas, E., and Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: susceptibility afferents to the extended amygdala. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877, 309–338.
to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science 330, 1830–1834. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09275.x
doi: 10.1126/science.1190792 McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., and Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood
Kox, M., van Eijk, L. T., Zwaag, J., van den Wildenberg, J., Sweep, F. C., van der adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct
Hoeven, J. G., et al. (2014). Voluntary activation of the sympathetic nervous dimensions of early experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 578–591.
system and attenuation of the innate immune response in humans. Proc. Natl. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 7379–7384. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322174111 Moore, A. A., Carney, D., Moroney, E., Machlin, L., Towbin, K. E., Brotman, M. A.,
Kristof, N. (2017). On a Portland train, the battlefield of American values. New York et al. (2017). The inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU) in children:
Times, 30th May. reliability and heritability. Behav. Genet. 47, 141–151. doi: 10.1007/s10519-016-
Kwak, Y., Pearson, J., and Huettel, S. A. (2014). Differential reward learning for self 9831-1
and others predicts self-reported altruism. PLoS One 9:e107621. doi: 10.1371/ Morgane, P. J., Galler, J. R., and Mokler, D. J. (2005). A review of systems and
journal.pone.0107621 networks of the limbic forebrain/limbic midbrain. Prog. Neurobiol. 75, 143–160.
Lahvis, G. P. (2017). Social reward and empathy as proximal contributions to doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.01.001
altruism: the camaraderie effect. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 30, 127–157. Morishima, Y., Schunk, D., Bruhin, A., Ruff, C. C., and Fehr, E. (2012). Linking
doi: 10.1007/7854_2016_449 brain structure and activation in temporoparietal junction to explain the
Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., and Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of neurobiology of human altruism. Neuron 75, 73–79. doi: 10.2013/j.neuron.
human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. 2012.05.021
Neurosci. 19, 42–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42 Mussweiler, T., and Ockenfels, A. (2013). Similarity increases altruistic punishment
Lamm, C., Decety, J., and Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 19318–19323. doi: 10.1073/pnas.
and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and 1215443110
empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010. Ne’eman, R., Perach-Barzilay, N., Fischer-Shofty, M., Atias, A., and Shamay-
10.014 Tsoory, S. G. (2016). Intranasal administration of oxytocin increases human
Leiberg, S., Klimecki, O., and Tania, S. (2011). Short-term compassion training aggressive behavior. Horm. Behav. 80, 125–131. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.
increases prosocial behavior in a newly developed prosocial game. PLoS One 01.015
6:e17798. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017798 O’Nions, E., Lima, C. F., Scott, S. K., Roberts, R., McCrory, E. J., and Viding, E.
Levy, T., Apter, A., Djalovski, A., Peskin, M., Fennig, S., Gat-Yablonski, G., (2017). Reduced laughter contagion in boys at risk for psychopathy. Curr. Biol.
et al. (2017). The reliability, concurrent validity and association with salivary 27, 3049–3055. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.062
oxytocin of the self-report version of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Pace, T. W. W., Negi, L. T., Adame, D. D., Cole, S. P., Sivilli, T. I., Brown, T. D., et al.
Traits in adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res. 256, 124–129. (2009). Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.06.028 and behavioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34,
Lim, M. M., and Young, L. J. (2004). Vasopressin-dependent neural circuits 87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.011
underlying pair bond formation in the monogamous prairie vole. Neuroscience Pardini, D. A., Raine, A., Erickson, K., and Loeber, R. (2014). Lower amygdala
125, 35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.008 volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic
LoParo, D., and Waldman, I. D. (2015). The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is traits, and future violence. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.
associated with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 2013.04.003
640–646. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.77 Pardo, J. V., Pardo, P. J., Janer, K. W., and Raichle, M. E. (1990). The anterior
Lozier, L. M., Cardinale, E. M., Vanmeter, J. W., and Marsh, A. A. (2014). Mediation cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict
of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 87, 256–259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.
by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA 1.256
Psychiatry 71, 627–636. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4540 Partanen, E., Kujala, T., Näätänen, R., Liitola, A., Sambeth, A., and Huotilainen, M.
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). (2013). Learning-induced neural plasticity of speech processing before birth.
Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 15145–15150. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302159110
of meditative expertise. PLoS One 3:e1897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897 Persson, B. N., and Kajonius, P. J. (2016). Empathy and universal values explicated
Lutz, A., McFarlin, D. R., Perlman, D. M., Salomons, T. V., and Davidson, R. J. by the empathy-altruism hypothesis. J. Soc. Psychol. 156, 610–619. doi: 10.1080/
(2013). Altered anterior insula activation during anticipation and experience of 00224545.2016.1152212
painful stimuli in expert meditators. Neuroimage 64, 538–546. doi: 10.1016/j. Pinto, D., Delaby, E., Merico, D., Barbosa, M., Merikangas, A., Klei, L., et al.
neuroimage.2012.09.030 (2014). Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism
Luu, P., and Pederson, S. M. (2004). “The anterior cingulate cortex: regulating spectrum disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 94, 677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.
actions in context,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, ed. M. I. Posner 03.018
(New York, NY: Guilford Press). Pitkow, L. J., Sharer, C. A., Ren, X., Insel, T. R., Terwilliger, E. F., and Young,
Madsen, E. A., Tunney, R. J., Fieldman, G., Plotkin, H. C., Dunbar, R. I., L. J. (2001). Facilitation of affiliation and pair-bond formation by vasopressin
Richardson, J. M., et al. (2007). Kinship and altruism: a cross-cultural receptor gene transfer into the ventral forebrain of a monogamous vole.
experimental study. Br. J. Psychol. 98, 339–359. doi: 10.1348/000712606X129213 J. Neurosci. 21, 7392–7396.
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., and Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns’ Poulin, M. J., Holman, E. A., and Buffone, A. (2012). The neurogenetics of nice:
cry melody is shaped by their native language. Curr. Biol. 19, 1994–1997. receptor genes for oxytocin and vasopressin interact with threat to predict
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.064 prosocial behavior. Psychol. Sci. 23, 446–452. doi: 10.1177/0956797611428471
Marsh, A. A., Stoycos, S. A., Brethel-Haurwitz, K. M., Robinson, P., Vanmeter, Preis, M. A., and Kroener-Herwig, B. (2012). Empathy for pain: the effects of prior
J. W., and Cardinale, E. M. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics experience and sex. Eur. J. Pain 16, 1311–1319. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.
of extraordinary altruists. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 15036–15041. 00119.x
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408440111 Preis, M. A., Kröner-Herwig, B., Schmidt-Samoa, C., Dechent, P., and Barke, A.
Marsh, N., Scheele, D., Feinstein, J. S., Gerhardt, H., Strang, S., Maier, W., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of empathy with pain show habituation effects. An
(2017). Oxytocin-enforced norm compliance reduces xenophobic outgroup fMRI study. PLoS One 10:e0137056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137056
Preston, S. D. (2013). The origins of altruism in offspring care. Psychol. Bull. 139, evoked by ventral tegmental area stimulation. Synapse 68, 131–143.
1305–1341. doi: 10.1037/a0031755 doi: 10.1002/syn.21723
Pujara, M., Motzkin, J. C., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., and Koenigs, M. (2014). Simon, B., Fletcher, J. A., and Doebeli, M. (2013). Towards a general theory
Neural correlates of reward and loss sensitivity in psychopathy. Soc. Cogn. of group selection. Evolution 67, 1561–1572. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.
Affect. Neurosci. 9, 794–801. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst054 01835.x
Raper, J., Wilson, M., Sanchez, M., Machado, C. J., and Bachevalier, J. Sinervo, B., Chaine, A., Clobert, J., Calsbeek, R., Hazard, L., Lancaster, L., et al.
(2013). Pervasive alterations of emotional and neuroendocrine responses (2006). Self-recognition, color signals, and cycles of greenbeard mutualism
to an acute stressor after neonatal amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys. and altruism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 7372–7377. doi: 10.1073/pnas.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1021–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.10.008 0510260103
Reidy, D. E., Krusemark, E., Kosson, D. S., Kearns, M. C., Smith-Darden, J., and Skeem, J. L., Polaschek, D. L., Patrick, C. J., and Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011).
Kiehl, K. A. (2017). The development of severe and chronic violence among Psychopathic personality: bridging the gap between scientific evidence
youth: the role of psychopathic traits and reward processing. Child Psychiatry and public policy. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 12, 95–162. doi: 10.1177/
Hum. Dev. 48, 967–982. doi: 10.1007/s10578-017-0720-5 1529100611426706
Ricard, M. (2013). On the Path to Enlightenment: Heart Advice from the Great Skuse, D. H., and Gallagher, L. (2009). Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in
Tibetan Masters. Boston, MA: Shambhala. the social brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.007
Ricard, M. (2015). Altruism: The Power of Compassion to Change Yourself and the Sorman, K., Nilsonne, G., Howner, K., Tamm, S., Caman, S., Wang, H. X.,
World. New York, NY: Little, Brown and Company. et al. (2016). Reliability and construct validity of the psychopathic personality
Rilling, J. K., DeMarco, A. C., Hackett, P. D., Thompson, R., Ditzen, B., Patel, R., inventory-revised in a Swedish non-criminal sample - A multimethod approach
et al. (2012). Effects of intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin on cooperative including psychophysiological correlates of empathy for pain. PLoS One
behavior and associated brain activity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 11:e0156570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0156570
447–467. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.013 Stallen, M., De Dreu, C. K., Shalvi, S., Smidts, A., and Sanfey, A. G. (2012). The
Rinne-Albers, M. A., Pannekoek, J. N., vanHoof, M. J., van Lang, N. D., Lamers- herding hormone: oxytocin stimulates in-group conformity. Psychol. Sci. 23,
Winkelman, F., Rombouts, S. A., et al. (2017). Anterior cingulate cortex grey 1288–1292. doi: 10.1177/0956797612446026
matter volume abnormalities in adolescents with PTSD after childhood sexual Stein, J. L., Wiedholz, L. M., Bassett, D. S., Weinberger, D. R., Zink, C. F., Mattay,
abuse. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 27, 1163–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro. V. S., et al. (2007). A validated network of effective amygdala connectivity.
2017.08.432 Neuroimage 36, 736–745. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.022
Robarcheck, C. A., and Robarcheck, C. J. (1992). “Cultures of war and peace: a Stewart-Williams, S. (2008). Human beings as evolved nepotists : exceptions to the
comparative study of Waorani and Semai,” in Aggression and Peacefulness in rule and effects of cost of help. Hum. Nat. 19, 414–425. doi: 10.1007/s12110-
Humans and Other Primates, eds J. Silverberg and J. P. Gray (New York, NY: 008-9048-y
Oxford University Press), 189–213. Strobel, A., Zimmerman, J., Schmitz, A., Reuter, M., Lis, S., Windmann, S., et al.
Rogers, J. C., and De Brito, S. A. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter (2011). Beyond revenge: neural and genetic bases of altruistic punishment.
volume in youths with conduct problems: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73, Neuroimage 54, 671–680. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.051
64–72. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2423 Taborsky, M., Frommen, J. G., and Riehl, C. (2016). The evolution of cooperation
Rosenkranz, M. A., Lutz, A., Perlman, D. M., Bachuber, D. R., Schuyler, B. S., based on direct fitness benefits. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci.
MacCoon, D. G., et al. (2016). Reduced stress and inflammatory responsiveness 371:20150472. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0472
in experienced meditators compared to a matched healthy control group. Toi, M., and Batson, C. D. (1982). More evidence that empathy is a source of
Psychoneuroendocrinology 68, 117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.02.013 altruistic motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 43, 281–292. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.
Rule, N. O., Freeman, J. B., Moran, J. M., Gabrieli, J. D., Adams, R. B. Jr., and 43.2.281
Ambady, N. (2010). Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across Tomasino, B., Chiesa, A., and Fabbro, F. (2014). Disentangling the neural
cultures. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 349–355. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp046 mechanisms involved in Hinduism- and Buddhism-related meditations. Brain
Sahin, M., and Sur, M. (2015). Genes, circuits, and precision therapies for autism Cogn. 90, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.03.013
and related neurodevelopmental disorders. Science 350:aab3897. doi: 10.1126/ Tong, A., Chapman, J. R., Wong, G., Kanellis, J., McCarthy, G., and Craig,
science.aab3897 J. C. (2012). The motivations and experiences of living kidney donors: a
Sanders, J., Mayford, M., and Jeste, D. (2013). Empathic fear responses in mice thematic synthesis. Am. J. Kidney Dis. 60, 15–26. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.2011.
are triggered by recognition of a shared experience. PLoS One 8:e74609. 11.043
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074609 Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., Mccarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T.,
Scheele, D., Striepens, N., Güntürkün, O., Deutschländer, S., Maier, W., Kendrick, et al. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large
K. M., et al. (2012). Oxytocin modulates social distance between males and amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev. Sci. 13, 46–61.
females. J. Neurosci. 32, 16074–16079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2755-12.2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x
Schiller, D., Freeman, J. B., Mitchell, J. P., Uleman, J. S., and Phelps, E. A. Trebuchon, A., Bartolomei, F., McGonigal, A., Laguitton, V., and Chauvel, P.
(2009). A neural mechanism of first impressions. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 508–514. (2013). Reversible antisocial behavior in ventromedial prefrontal lobe epilepsy.
doi: 10.1038/nn.2278 Epilepsy Behav. 29, 367–373. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.08.007
Schreiber, D., Fonzo, G., Simmons, A. N., Dawes, C. T., Flagan, T., Fowler, J. H., Trotta, A., Murray, R. M., and Fisher, H. L. (2015). The impact of childhood
et al. (2013). Red brain, blue brain: evaluative processes differ in Democrats and adversity on the persistence of psychotic symptoms: a systematic review
Republicans. PLoS One 8:e52970. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052970 and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 45, 2481–2498. doi: 10.1017/S00332917150
Sebastian-Enesco, C., and Warneken, F. (2015). The shadow of the future: 5-year- 00574
olds, but not 3-year-olds, adjust their sharing in anticipation of reciprocation. Tusche, A., Böckler, A., Kanske, P., Trautwein, F. M., and Singer, T. (2016).
J. Exp. Child Psychol. 129, 40–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.08.007 Decoding the charitable brain: empathy, perspective taking, and attention shifts
Sesack, S. R., and Grace, A. A. (2010). Cortico-basal ganglia reward network: differentially predict altruistic giving. J. Neurosci. 36, 4719–4732. doi: 10.1523/
microcircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 27–47. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.93 JNEUROSCI.3392-15.2016
Settle, J. E., Dawes, C. T., Christakis, N. A., and Fowler, J. H. (2010). Friendships United States Department of Veterans Affairs (2017). Eye Movement
moderate an association between a dopamine gene variants and political Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) for PTSD. Available at:
ideology. J. Polit. 72, 1189–1198. doi: 10.1017/S0022381610000617 https://www.ptsd.va.gov/public/treatment/therapy-med/emdr-for-ptsd.asp
Sheng, F., Liu, Y., Zhou, B., Zhou, W., and Han, S. (2013). Oxytocin modulates the [accessed February 15, 2018].
racial bias in neural responses to others’ suffering. Biol. Psychol. 92, 380–386. Urakawa, S., Takamoto, K., Ishikawa, A., Ono, T., and Nishijo, H. (2015). Selective
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.018 medial prefrontal cortex responses during live mutual gaze interactions in
Shnitko, T. A., and Robinson, D. L. (2014). Anatomical and pharmacological human infants: an fNIRS study. Brain Topogr. 28, 691–701. doi: 10.1007/
characterization of catecholamine transients in the medial prefrontal cortex s10548-014-0414-2
Van de Vondervoort, J. W., and Hamlin, J. K. (2017). The early emergence of Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and
sociomoral evaluation: infants prefer prosocial others. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nat.
77–81. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.014 Neurosci. 5, 277–283. doi: 10.1038/nn816
Van den Bos, W., McClure, S. M., Harris, L. T., Fiske, S. T., and Cohen, J. D. Yang, H.-P., Wang, L., Han, L., and Wang, S. C. (2013). Nonsocial functions
(2007). Dissociating affective evaluation and social cognitive processes in the of hypothalamic oxytocin. ISRN Neurosci. 2013:179272. doi: 10.1155/2013/17
ventral medial prefrontal cortex. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 337–346. 9272
doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.337 Yang, Y., Raine, A., Colletti, P., Toga, A. W., and Narr, K. L. (2010).
Viding, E., and Pingault, J. B. (2016). Can positive parental reinforcement counter Morphological alterations in the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in
genetic risk for callous-unemotional behavior? Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 862–863. unsuccessful psychopaths. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 546–554. doi: 10.1037/a001
doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16060694 9611
von Polier, G. G., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Konrad, K., Wiesler, K., Rieke, J., Young, L. J., and Barrett, C. E. (2015). Can oxytocin treat autism?: we are still at an
Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M., et al. (2013). Reduced cortisol in boys with early- early stage of assessing oxytocin-based therapy for autism spectrum disorders.
onset conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits. Biomed. Res. Int. Science 347, 825–826. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa8120
2013:349530. doi: 10.1155/2013/349530 Yuccas, J. (2017). American Aid Worker Runs Through ISIS Gunfire to Save Little
Walpole, H. (1769). Letter to Sir Horace Mann, Vol. 23. New Haven, CT: Yale Girl. Oroville, WA: CBS News.
University, 166. Available at: https://walpole.library.yale.edu/ [accessed March Zak, P. J., and Barraza, J. A. (2013). The neurobiology of collective action. Front.
02, 2018]. Neurosci. 19:211. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00211
Walum, H., Westberg, L., Henningsson, S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Reiss, D., Igl, W., Zeidan, F., Emerson, N. M., Farris, S. R., Ray, J. N., Jung, Y., McHaffie, J. G.,
et al. (2008). Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (AVPR1A) et al. (2015). Mindfulness meditation-based pain relief employs different
associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. neural mechanisms than placebo and sham mindfulness meditation-induced
106, 14153–14156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803081105 analgesia. J. Neurosci. 35, 15307–15325. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2542-15.
Weiner, R. (2012). Paul Ryan and Ayn Rand. The Washington Post, 13th August. 2015
Wersinger, S. R., Kelliher, K. R., Zufall, F., Lolait, S. J., O’Carroll, A. M., and Young, Zepeda Méndez, M., Nijdam, M. J., Ter Heide, F. J. J., van der Aa, N., and
W. S. III (2004). Social motivation is reduced in vasopressin 1b receptor null Olff, M. (2018). A five-day inpatient EMDR treatment programme for PTSD:
mice despite normal performance in an olfactory discrimination task. Horm. pilot study. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 9:1425575. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2018.142
Behav. 46, 638–645. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.07.004 5575
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., and Gardner, A. (2007). Social semantics: altruism, Zhang, R., Brennan, T. J., and Lo, A. W. (2014). Group selection as behavioral
cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. J. Evol. Biol. adaptation to system risk. PLoS ONE 9:e110848. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.
20, 415–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x 0110848
Whalen, P. J. (1999). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: initial neuroimaging studies
of the human amygdala. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 7, 177–187. doi: 10.1111/1467- Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
8721.ep10836912 conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
Whalen, P. J. (2007). The uncertainty of it all. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 499–500. be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.016
Whiting, B. B., Whiting, J. W. M., and Longabaugh, R. (1975). Children of Six Copyright © 2018 Sonne and Gash. This is an open-access article distributed
Cultures : A Psycho-Cultural Analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use,
Press. doi: 10.4159/harvard.9780674593770 distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original
Wilkinson, G. S., Carter, G. G., Bohn, K. M., and Adams, D. M. (2016). Non- author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication
kin cooperation in bats. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150095. in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use,
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0095 distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.