Bluck2004 PDF
Bluck2004 PDF
Susan Bluck
University of Florida
Judith Glück
University of Vienna, Austria
Defining Wisdom:
Validity of the Wisdom-of-Experience Procedure
Wisdom has been defined in multiple ways and measured by various
criteria (e.g., Hershey & Farrell, 1997; Smith et al., 1989; Sternberg,
1998). The definition of wisdom that we employ is based on this past
literature, which, in convergence, suggests that wisdom is an adap-
tive form of life judgment (Kramer, 2000) that involves not what but
how one thinks. It is a combination of experiential knowledge,
cognition, affect, and action that sometimes occurs in social context
(e.g., Ardelt, 1997; Clayton & Birren, 1980; Labouvie-Vief, 1990;
Sternberg, 1998). Wisdom is defined as a personal resource that is
used to negotiate fundamental life changes and challenges and is
often directed toward the goals of living a good life or striving for the
common good (Baltes & Staudinger 2000; Kekes, 1983). The
literature shows agreement concerning these features of a basic
definition of wisdom.
546 Bluck & Glück
METHOD
Participants
The data were collected in Berlin, Germany, as part of a larger project
(Glück & Baltes, 2002). Participants who were 15 to 20, 30 to 40, or 60 to
70 years old were recruited for the study through newspaper advertise-
ments. Additional adolescents were contacted through flyers distributed
at sports facilities. We obtained a sample that was balanced in profes-
sional status and gender; all participants were Caucasian Germans.
Participants were paid DM 80 (about U.S. $40) for participation. Only
procedures and measures relevant to the present study are described here.
We began interviews with 92 participants. Two adolescents, three
younger adults, and one older adult reported that they could not remember
a wisdom-related event. The final sample included 86 participants:
Wisdom-of-Experience Narratives 551
Procedure
The data come from a semi-structured autobiographical interview per-
formed by one of six trained interviewers. At the beginning of the
wisdom-of-experience interview, the researcher informed participants
that we would like to talk to them about their own lives (the measures
collected for the larger project involved standard assessments with
no autobiographical component). The participant was given a sheet
with 15 numbered lines and asked to ‘‘write down as many situations
as possible from your life in which you said, did, or thought something
that was wise in some way.’’ Participants were given two minutes and
wrote key words for each situation. The interviewer emphasized that most
people reported fewer than 15 situations, but that the participant should
list as many situations as possible. The participant then selected the one
situation in which he or she had been wisest. This procedure was used to
allow the person to review his or her life’s events more carefully, as
opposed to being interviewed about the first wisdom-related event that
came to mind.
With consent, the interviewer switched on a tape recorder saying
‘‘Now let’s talk about this situation. What was it about, and what did you
consider and do?’’ The participant spoke for as long as he or she wanted.
If participants said little or nothing, the interviewer probed by asking,
‘‘What were your considerations in this situation?’’ Unless participants
explicitly said what had been wise, the interviewer asked, ‘‘In what way
would you say you were wise?’’ Next, the interviewer asked whether the
participant had learned a lesson from the event, and if yes, what. Some
participants were quite emotional in retelling their wisdom memories. The
interviewer gave the participant the opportunity to talk informally about
the event when the session was over. Note that this was an open-ended
interview. Thus the emergence of material in the narratives that was
coded is not due to forced responses to individual questions but to
characteristic ways in which individuals recall and retell autobiographical
wisdom experiences.
552 Bluck & Glück
Coded Variables
The 86 tape-recorded interviews were transcribed. Fifty additional inter-
views were collected and transcribed. Twenty of these 50 were used, in
addition to theoretical considerations, in developing the categories of the
coding scheme. The remaining 30 protocols were used to establish inter-
rater reliability before coding the study protocols. The practice protocols
were coded by the second author and two senior psychology students.
Disagreements were used to further refine coding categories. All 50
protocols were then used to train two other senior students who served
as final coders. When the 86 study protocols had been coded indepen-
dently by both coders, disagreements were resolved by discussion. We
only report variables for which the coders reached at least 80% agreement
before discussing disagreements. Other variables not presented here
included coded forms of experienced wisdom, age at wisdom-related
event, and self-ratings of experienced wisdom (see Glück & Bluck, 2002).
The next section is structured in four parts. The first concerns the
validity of our wisdom-of-experience interview. The second concerns the
role of experienced wisdom in ‘‘making things better,’’ that is, transform-
ing proximal aspects of a life situation. The third concerns the relation of
the reported event to the participant’s life story. The fourth concerns
learning a lesson about one’s self or life. Table 1 lists examples of all
coded variables, including both Kappa coefficients and exact percent
agreements as measures of inter-rater reliability.
RESULTS
The results are presented in the order followed in the Methods section
(also see Table 1). We present results pertaining to the validity of the
wisdom-of-experience procedure, then those that refer to the events
that elicit experienced wisdom and to their outcomes, how the event
sits in temporal and causal relation to the life story, and finally, if a
lesson was learned from the event, and if so, its nature.
Chi-square tests against theoretical distributions are used to test
questions concerning the study variables. Tests against a uniform
distribution do not make sense in examining the validity criteria
because for these indicators a uniform distribution is not theoreti-
cally justified. Therefore, for the validity variables only, the results
Wisdom-of-Experience Narratives 555
Valence of Eliciting Event positive finding out that someone is in love with one
Kappa 5 .81 negative having a serious accident
% agreement 5 90.7 both positive and negative falling in love with someone when already married
neither positive nor negative participant does not mention valence
Valence of the Outcome of the Event positive constructively dealing with spouse’s death
Kappa 5 .62 negative marriage breaking up
% agreement 5 82.6 both moving in with a new partner and having to leave the
neither previous partner
participant does not mention valence
Time Frame of The Narrative single event deciding between two jobs
Kappa 5 .82 generic event learning to live with one’s depressive episodes
% agreement 5 91.9 extended event participant describes entire life period
Causal Coherence in the Narrative none no mention of relation to any later events or life path
Kappa 5 .64 specific events ‘‘And one year later, he told me that my decision to
% agreement 5 84.9 just listen to him instead of blaming him was the right
thing to do. He hasn’t stolen anything sincey’’
life in general ‘‘That decision has made it easier for me with other
things in life that are also important, to listen more to
myself, to what I want, not what others do, or what
you ought to do y’’
Subjective Perception of a Lesson yes ‘‘With this situation in my mother’s life, I think I have
Kappa 5 .94 learned something for myself about the human
psyche.’’
Table 1 (cont.)
% agreement 5 97.7 no ‘‘No, people don’t learn from experience. I’d make
the same mistake again.’’
maybe ‘‘Well, I didn’t really learn something, but then, given
that I was able to express the main point in two
wordsy’’
Type of Lesson Learned no generalization ‘‘Yes, I’ve learned that in this case a rather drastic
Kappa 5 .72 solution that you would not use normally was really
the best one.’’
% agreement 5 82.1 factual or procedural ‘‘I have learned that you have no idea how much of
knowledge the things that happened to you in your early
childhood come back later because you didn’t get
over them.’’
life philosophy ‘‘Stay calm. That’s what I’ve learned and that helps
me a lot now. Small or big things, no problem
anymore. That has helped me a lot.’’
lesson had been learned ‘‘That I learned something from that situation? I
before don’t think so, I think I would have acted the same
way before.’’
Wisdom-of-Experience Narratives 559
Table 2
Cross-Tabulation of Eliciting Event and Outcome
Outcome
Positive 1 0 0 1
Eliciting Event Both 5 2 0 7
Negative 37 3 1 41
43 5 1 49
mentioned both positive and negative aspects, and one had a positive
event as elicitor. This distribution across the three categories is
clearly not equal, w2 (2, N 5 67) 5 86.54, po.01. Thus, of partici-
pants who included an eliciting event in their experienced wisdom
narrative, most remembered negative situations that required wise
responses. Positive situations are rarely viewed as requiring wisdom.
Outcome of the situation was not mentioned in 16 narratives, and
in eight cases the outcome was not described in terms of its valence.
Of the remaining 62 narratives, 55 had a positive outcome; in six
cases the outcome had both positive and negative aspects, and one
narrative had a negative outcome, w2 (1, N 5 62) 5 86.16, po.01.
Table 2 gives a cross-tabulation of eliciting events and outcomes
for those 49 situations in which both outcome and eliciting event
could be coded as positive, negative, or both. As the table shows, the
most typical pattern (37 of 49 cases) is that of a negative eliciting
event and a positive outcome. The table as a whole is markedly
asymmetrical; there is not a single case in which the outcome is more
negative than the eliciting event. In 91.8% of the cases, the outcome
is more positive than the eliciting event or situation. Four cases show
no change between valence of eliciting event and outcome. In sum,
the stories participants told of autobiographical wisdom-related
events most often involved times in which they viewed their
thoughts, feelings, and actions as having changed negative events
or situations into more positive outcomes.
taught people a lesson about life. The problem with this interpreta-
tion, as mentioned above, is with the demand characteristics of the
interview. To address this, we coded whether participants reported
generalizing the lesson in some way by utilizing it beyond this one
event. We reasoned that if a real lesson had been learned, it would be
one that was not simply specific to the exact situation the participant
had just recalled.
Types of lessons learned. Eight participants said they had not
learned anything from the event. Of the remaining 78 (who had
Table 3
Examples of ‘‘Life Lessons’’ Learned
Well, I think I’ve learned to get along better with my parents. And if possible,
not to let problems arise in the first place. I mean, everybody has problems with
their parents, but after all I’m still living with them and there’s always a way to
compromise, I mean you don’t have to let it escalate so that in the end it erupts.
(Male, 19 years)
Yes, I’ve sworn to myself if I have a partner, I never want to have that typical
roles thing. I don’t want to find myself as a housewife. That’s a terrible concept
for me. I don’t want to be forced into that cliche´. (Female, 19 years)
[y] if somebody somehow has authority over you and tells you something, then
I’ve learned not to follow and to believe that person blindly, but to use my own
head and to ask, ‘‘Do I have to do this, or am I a free person who can do what she
wants?’’ That’s quite an important thing for me. (Female, 32 years)
I’ve also learned to live more in the here and now. Not to be wasteful, but to
sometimes allow myself things that make life more pleasant – to be a little freer
and not delay everything. You never know what the future holds. (Male, 30
years)
Yes, certainly, one learns that one is not the only person with rights. With any
considerations or decisions one has to take into account, to what degree are you
now affecting the interests of the other person? (Male, 70 years)
The moral of the story is, you have to believe in yourself, trust in yourself. And
of course, try to do your job as well as possible, but be realistic. You need to have
a healthy trust in yourself, but you must not overestimate yourself and say you
can do something but then fail. [y] you must know your own limitations.
That’s my conclusion. (Male, 69 years)
562 Bluck & Glück
DISCUSSION
Under the assumption that almost everyone has the possibility of
being wise at some time in his or her life, even if wisdom is not
necessarily part of one’s semantic self-representation, the study
examined the nature of wisdom-related autobiographical memories
in the lives of ordinary people. After validating the wisdom-of-
experience procedure, we examined the events that elicit experienced
wisdom in relation to the outcomes of those events. Next, wisdom
events were assessed for their temporal and causal links to the larger
life story, and the life lessons learned from such events were
examined. Age differences and similarities across three age groups
were noted, with particular emphasis on how adolescents might
differ from younger and older adults.
Wisdom-of-Experience Narratives 563
CONCLUSION
Recalling and retelling autobiographical experiences of having felt
wise allows individuals to remember and share a sense of self as
having been efficacious in facing difficulty or uncertainty. Such
narratives of experienced wisdom are sometimes time-delimited
episodes, but are also quite frequently linked across larger temporal
periods, becoming part of one’s identity through being coherently
integrated into the life story. Although participants of all ages
described having experienced wisdom, it seems that wisdom itself
Wisdom-of-Experience Narratives 569
REFERENCES
Ardelt, M. (1997). Wisdom and life satisfaction in old age. Journals of Gerontol-
ogy: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 52B, P15–P27.
Assmann, A. (1994). Wholesome knowledge: Concepts of wisdom in a historical
and cross-cultural perspective. In D. L. Featherman, R. M. Lerner, & M.
Perlmutter (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior. Hillsdale, N.: Erlbaum.
Baerger, D. R., & McAdams, D. P. (1999). Life story coherence and its relation to
psychological well-being. Narrative Inquiry, 9, 69–96.
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychol-
ogy: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology,
23, 611–626.
Baltes, P. B., Glück, J., & Kunzmann, U. (2002). Wisdom: Its structure and
function in regulating successful lifespan development. In C. R. Snyder & S. J.
Lopez (Eds.), The handbook of positive psychology, (pp. 327–350). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (1990). Toward a psychology of wisdom and its
ontogenesis. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and
development (pp. 87–120). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic)
to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55,
122–136.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Baumeister, R. F., & Newman, L. S. (1994). How stories make sense of personal
experiences: Motives that shape autobiographical narratives. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 676–690.
Bluck, S., & Habermas, T. (2001a). The life story schema. Motivation and
Emotion, 24, 121–147.
Bluck, S., & Levine, L. J. (1998). Reminiscence as autobiographical memory: A
catalyst for reminiscence theory development. Ageing and Society, 18, 185–208.
570 Bluck & Glück
Staudinger, U. M., Lopez, D., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). The psyhometric location of
wisdom-related performance: intelligence, personality, and more? Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1200–1214.
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General
Psychology, 2, 347–365.
Sternberg, R. J. (2000). Intelligence and wisdom. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),
Handbook of intelligence (pp. 631–649). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events. A theory of cognitive
adaptation. American Psychologist, 30, 1161–1173.
Thorne, A. (2000). Personal memory telling and personality development.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 45–56.
Webster, J. D. (2003). An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wisdom scale.
Journal of Adult Development, 10, 13–22.
Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2000). The frequency of temporal-self and social
comparisons in people’s personal appraisals. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 928–942.