Krashen (2011) Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and The Role of Strategies
Krashen (2011) Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and The Role of Strategies
Krashen (2011) Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and The Role of Strategies
The Evidence
The evidence for the hypothesis that we acquire academic language
from reading includes many studies showing the power of free
voluntary reading:
N studies of sustained silent reading, done with a wide variety of subjects of
different ages and in different parts of the world, showing that those who
No Examples of Success
There are no case histories of successful acquisition of academic
language through formal study alone. Some people might feel that
formal study was responsible for their success, but I don’t think
they realize how much academic prose they acquired through
reading. The few aspects that they consciously learned and remem-
ber are given a great deal of importance. However, they comprise a
tiny part of the language competence.
The classic approach, nevertheless, remains the basis for EAP,
and the lack of evidence supporting it is never discussed.
A PROFOUND DIFFERENCE
A profound difference between the classic approach and acquisition
of academic language via reading is that in the classic approach, the
goal in the mind of the student is mastery of academic language. In
the reading approach, acquisition of the academic language is a by-
product; the goal is understanding what is on the page. In other
words, those who have acquired the academic language did not set
out to acquire the academic language. They read for interest and
pleasure, for the content.
The result of classic instruction is a conscious knowledge of the
elements of academic language. The result of reading is a feel for
academic prose.
Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and the Role of Strategies 383
OBJECTIONS TO ACQUIRING ACADEMIC LANGUAGE
COMPETENCE VIA READING
OTHER SOURCES
Reading is not the only source of input containing academic lan-
guage. Other sources, such as classroom input, contain aspects of
academic prose. However, classroom input appears to have more in
common linguistically with face-to-face interaction than with
written academic prose (Biber, 2006).
Of course, classrooms and conversation help us solve problems
by providing information from others through interaction, but my
hunch is that most creative work is solitary. Interaction can play a
role; descriptions of eminent scientists reveal a considerable amount
Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and the Role of Strategies 387
of interaction with others, but more time spent alone. Faraday, for
example, was ‘‘in many respects a solitary scholar’’ and ‘‘usually
avoided students working under him. . . . [A]s one of his biogra-
phers put it, Faraday’s dialogue was with nature’’ (Howe, 1999, p.
104).
STRATEGIES
If the set of hypotheses presented earlier are correct, we can predict
that useful strategies are those that help provide comprehensible,
interesting text; that help make texts more comprehensible; and that
help in problem solving.
We need to distinguish two different kinds of strategies:
Innate Strategies
Some strategies don’t need to be taught; they are part of our innate
mental equipment. The strategy of prediction, for example, is
frequently taught. Anderson (2003) provides an example of stu-
dents deliberately practicing prediction:
Students specifically focused on making predictions of text
content. The students made a prediction, read a portion of the
text, and then paused to confirm or reject their prediction. They
then continued the cycle of predicting followed by confirming or
rejecting their guesses multiple times during the reading
passage. (para. 14)
Deprogramming Strategies
Some of the strategies that teachers do teach, and that seem to
actually help, are those that should develop naturally as people
read and try to solve problems. They often don’t, because of the
influence of school. Teaching these strategies amounts to de-
programming.
Among the deprogramming reading strategies are these:
Delay editing
Over-concern with accuracy, stimulated by instructional practices
as well as time constraints, motivates students to worry about
punctuation, grammar, and spelling as they write, which diverts
attention from what they are saying. Direct strategy instruction in
this case simply advises writers to delay editing until the final draft.
Peter Elbow (1973) puts it this way:
Treat grammar as a matter of very late editorial correcting: never
think about it while you are writing. Pretend you have an editor
who will fix everything for you, then don’t hire yourself for this
job until the very end. (p. 137)
390 TESOL Journal
I mention only these two as examples of instances in which
direct instruction may work very well as a means of de-
programming. There are of course others (Krashen, 2003).
The common assumption is that all we as teachers have to do is
see what strategies experts use and teach these strategies to our
students. I question this assumption, without ruling out the
possibility that some strategies are teachable and direct instruction
in these strategies is a real help to students.
CONCLUSION
Pedagogy in developing academic proficiency has been dominated
by the assumption that academic linguistic proficiency and knowl-
edge of academic content can be described and taught directly. My
goal is to reduce this axiom to the status of hypothesis: There is
strong evidence that academic language proficiency is acquired
through reading, and that knowledge of content is developed
through problem solving.
THE AUTHOR
Stephen Krashen is Emeritus Professor of Education at the
University of Southern California. He is the author of The Power of
Reading (2004); was the 1977 Incline Bench Press champion of
Venice Beach, California; and holds a black belt in Tae Kwon Do.
REFERENCES
Anderson, N. J. (2003). Metacognitive reading strategies increase L2
performance. The Language Teacher, 27(7). http://www.jalt-
publications.org/tlt
Biber, D. (2006). University language: A corpus-based study of spoken
and written registers. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
Bloom, B. (1963). Report on creativity research by the examiner’s
office of the University of Chicago. In C. Taylor & F. Barron
(Eds.), Scientific creativity: Its recognition and development (pp. 261–
264). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Crawford, J., & Krashen, S. (2007). English learners in American
classrooms: 101 questions, 101 answers. New York, NY: Scholastic.
Elbow, P. (1973). Writing without teachers. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and the Role of Strategies 391
Gardner, D. (2004). Vocabulary input through extensive reading: A
comparison of words found in children’s narrative and
expository reading materials. Applied Linguistics, 25, 1–37.
doi:10.1093/applin/25.1.1
Gardner, D. (2008). Vocabulary recycling in children’s authentic
reading materials: A corpus-based investigation of narrow
reading. Reading as a Foreign Language, 20(1), 92–122.
Howe, M. (1999). Genius explained. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language
acquisition. New York, NY: Prentice Hall.
Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in language acquisition and use: The
Taipei lectures. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Krashen, S. (2004). The power of reading. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann.
Krashen, S. (2007). Extensive reading in English as a foreign
language by adolescents and young adults: A meta-analysis.
International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 3(2), 23–29.
Krashen, S. (2010, November). Academic language proficiency:
Acquired or learned? Paper presented at the 19th International
Symposium and Book Fair on English Teaching, Taipei, Taiwan.
Krashen, S., & Brown, C. L. (2007). What is academic language
proficiency? STETS Language and Communication Review,
6(1), 1–4.
Lao, C. Y., & Krashen, S. (2000). The impact of popular literature
study on literacy development in EFL: More evidence for the
power of reading. System, 28, 261–270. doi:10.1016/S0346-
251X(00)00011-7
Mason, B. (2006). Free voluntary reading and autonomy in second
language acquisition: Improving TOEFL scores from reading
alone. International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 2(1), 2–5.
Mason, B. (2010a). Comprehension is the key to efficient foreign
language education: Self-selected reading and story-listening are
the solutions. The Bulletin of Shitennoji University, 49, 371–380.
Mason, B. (2010b, November). Impressive gains on the TOEIC after one
year of comprehensible input, with no output or grammar study.
Paper presented at the 19th International Symposium and Book
Fair on English Teaching, Taipei, Taiwan.
392 TESOL Journal
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (2004). Is form-focused vocabulary
instruction worth-while? RELC Journal, 35(2), 179–185.
doi:10.1177/003368820403500206
Mason, B., Vanata, M., Jander, K., Borsch, R., & Krashen, S. (2009).
The effects and efficiency of hearing stories on vocabulary
acquisition by students of German as a second foreign language
in Japan. Indonesian Journal of English Language Teaching,
5(1), 1–14.
Ohanian, H. (2008). Einstein’s mistakes. New York, NY: W. W.
Norton.
Schleppegrell, M., Achugar, M., & Oteiza, T. (2004). The grammar of
history: Enhancing content-based instruction through a
functional focus on language. TESOL Quarterly, 38, 67–93.
doi:10.2307/3588259
Simonton, D. K. (1988). Scientific genius: A psychology of science. New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Smith, F. (1983). The role of prediction in reading. In Essays into
literacy (pp. 35–39). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Swales, J. (1990). Genre analysis: English in academic and research
settings. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Vonnegut, K. (1981). Easter Sunday. New York, NY: Dell.
Academic Proficiency (Language and Content) and the Role of Strategies 393