Standard English: What It Isn't: Peter Trudgill

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Standard English: what it isn’t

(Tony Bex & Richard J. Watts eds. Standard English: the widening debate. London: Routledge, 1999, 117-
128.)

Peter Trudgill

University of Lausanne

There is a reasonably clear consensus in the sociolinguistics literature about the term
standardised language: a standardised language is a language one of whose varieties has
undergone standardisation. Standardisation, too, appears to be a relatively uncontroversial
term, although the terminology employed in the discussion of this topic is by no means
uniform. I myself have defined standardisation (Trudgill, 1992) as consisting of the
processes of language determination, codification and stabilisation. Language
determination "refers to decisions which have to be taken concerning the selection of
particular languages or varieties of language for particular purposes in the society or nation
in question" (p.71). Codification is the process whereby a language variety "acquires a
publicly recognised and fixed form". The results of codification "are usually enshrined in
dictionaries and grammar books" (p.17). Stabilisation is a process whereby a formerly
diffuse variety (in the sense of Le Page and Tabouret-Keller,1985) "undergoes focussing
and takes on a more fixed and stable form" (p.70).

It is therefore somewhat surprising that there seems to be considerable confusion in the


English-speaking world, even amongst linguists, about what Standard English is. One
would think that it should be reasonably clear which of the varieties of English is the one
which has been subject to the process of standardisation, and what its characteristics are. In
fact, however, we do not even seem to be able to agree how to spell this term - with an
upper case or lower case <s> - a point which I will return to later, and the use of the term
by non-linguists appears to be even more haphazard.

In this paper, I therefore attempt a characterisation of Standard English. It should be noted


that this is indeed a characterisation rather than a strict definition - language varieties do not
readily lend themselves to definition as such. We can describe what Chinese is, for
example, in such a way as to make ourselves very well understood on the issue, but actually
to define Chinese would be another matter altogether. The characterisation will also be as
much negative as positive - a clearer idea of what Standard English is can be obtained by
saying what it is not as well as by saying what it is. My discussion of this topic will be both
a sociolinguistic and a linguistic discussion. (But it will be specifically linguistic: the word
"ideology" wil not appear again in this paper). And it will also, I hope, be informed by
references from time to time to the nature of standard and nonstandard varieties in language
situations beyond the English-speaking world.

Standard English is not a language


Standard English is often referred to as "the standard language". It is clear, however, that
Standard English is not "a language" in any meaningful sense of this term. Standard
English, whatever it is, is less than a language, since it is only one variety of English among
many. Standard English may be the most important variety of English, in all sorts of ways:
it is the variety of English normally used in writing, especially printing; it is the variety
associated with the education system in all the English-speaking countries of the world, and
is therefore the variety spoken by those who are often referred to as "educated people"; and
it is the variety taught to non-native learners. But most native speakers of English in the
world are native speakers of some nonstandard variety of the language, and English, like
other Ausbau languages (see Kloss, 1967), can be described (Chambers and Trudgill, 1997)
as consisting of an autonomous standardised variety together with all the nonstandard
varieties which are heteronomous with respect to it. Standard English is thus not the
English language but simply one variety of it.

Standard English is not an accent

There is one thing about Standard English on which most linguists, or at least British
linguists, do appear to be agreed, and that is that Standard English has nothing to do with
pronunciation. From a British perspective, we have to acknowledge that there is in Britain a
high status and widely described accent known as Received Pronunciation (RP) which is
sociolinguistically unusual when seen from a global perspective in that it is not associated
with any geographical area, being instead a purely social accent associated with speakers in
all parts of the country, or at least in England, from upper-class and upper-middle-class
backgrounds. It is widely agreed, though, that while all RP speakers also speak Standard
English, the reverse is not the case. Perhaps 9%-12% of the population of Britain (see
Trudgill and Cheshire, 1989) speak Standard English with some form of regional accent. It
is true that in most cases Standard English speakers do not have "broad" local accents i.e.
accents with large numbers of regional features which are phonologically and phonetically
very distant from RP, but it is clear that in principle we can say that, while RP is in a sense,
standardised, it is a standardised accent of English and not Standard English itself. This
point becomes even clearer from an international perspective. Standard English speakers
can be found in all English-speaking countries, and it goes without saying that they speak
this variety with different non-RP accents depending on whether they came from Scotland
or the USA or New Zealand or wherever.

Standard English is not a style

There is, however and unfortunately, considerable confusion in the minds of many
concerning the relationship between Standard English and the vocabulary associated with
formal varieties of the English language. We characterise styles (see Trudgill, 1992) as
varieties of language viewed from the point of view of formality. Styles are varieties of
language which can be ranged on a continuum ranging from very formal to very informal.
Formal styles are employed in social situations which are formal, and informal styles are
employed in social situations which are informal - which is not to say, however, that
speakers are "sociolinguistic automata" (Giles, 1973) who respond blindly to the particular
degree of formality of a particular social situation. On the contrary, speakers are able to
influence and change the degree of formality of a social situation by manipulation of
stylistic choice.

All the languages of the world would appear to demonstrate some degree of stylistic
differentiation in this sense, reflecting the wide range of social relationships and social
situations found, to a greater or lesser extent, in all human societies. I believe, with Labov
(1972) that there is no such thing as a single-style speaker, although it is obviously also the
case that the repertoire of styles available to individual speakers will be a reflection of their
social experiences and, in many cases, also their education. It is of course important here to
distinguish between individual speakers of languages and those languages themselves, but
it is clear that languages too may differ similarly in the range of styles available to their
speakers. In many areas of the world, switching from informal to formal situations also
involves switching from one language to another. In such cases, it is probable that neither
of the two languages involved will have the full range of styles available to speakers in
monolingual situations.

English as it is employed in areas where it is the major native language of the community,
such as in the British Isles, North America and Australasia, is a language which has the
fullest possible range of styles running from the most to the least formal. This obviously
does not mean to say, however, that all speakers have equal access to or ability in all styles,
and it is generally accepted that one of the objectives of mother tongue education is to give
pupils exposure to styles at the more formal end of the continuum that they might otherwise
not gain any ability in using.

Stylistic differences in English are most obvious at the level of lexis. Consider the
differences between

Father was exceedingly fatigued subsequent to his extensive peregrination.

Dad was very tired after his lengthy journey.

The old man was bloody knackered after his long trip .

Although one could argue about some of the details, we can accept that these three
sentences have more or less the same referential meaning, and thus differ only in style - and
that the stylistic differences are indicated by lexical choice. It is also clear that native
speakers are very sensitive to the fact that stylistic variation constitutes a cline: some of the
words here, such as was, his are stylistically neutral; others range in formality from the
ridiculously formal peregrination through very formal fatigued to intermediate tired to
informal trip to very informal knackered and tabooed informal bloody. It will be observed
that, as is often the case, the most informal or "slang" words are regionally restricted, being
in this case unknown or unusual in North American English. It will also be observed that
there are no strict co-occurrence restrictions here as there are in some languages - one can
say long journey and lengthy trip just as well as lengthy journey and long trip.

Formality in English is, however, by no means confined to lexis. Grammatical


constructions vary as between informal and formal English - it is often claimed, for
instance, that the passive voice is more frequent in formal than in informal styles - and, as
has been shown by many works in the Labovian secular linguistics tradition, starting with
Labov (1966), phonology is also highly sensitive to style.

As far as the relationship between style, on the one hand, and Standard English, on the
other, is concerned, we can say the following. The phonological sensitivity to stylistic
context just referred to obviously has no connection to Standard English since, as we have
noted, Standard English has no connection with phonology.

Let us then examine lexis. I would like to assert that our sentence

The old man was bloody knackered after his long trip

is clearly and unambiguously Standard English. To assert otherwise - that swear words like
bloody and very informal words like knackered are not Standard English - would get us into
a very difficult situation. Does a Standard English speaker suddenly switch out of Standard
English as soon as they start swearing? Are Standard English speakers not allowed to use
slang without switching into some nonstandard variety? My contention is that Standard
English is no different from any other (nonstandard) variety of the language. Speakers of
Standard English have a full range of styles open to them, just as speakers of other varieties
do, and can swear and use slang just like anybody else. (It will be clear that I do not agree
with the contention which is sometimes heard that "nobody speaks Standard English".)
Equally, there is no need for speakers of nonstandard varieties to switch into Standard
English in order to employ formal styles. The most logical position we can adopt on this is
as follows:

The old man was bloody knackered after his long trip

is a Standard English sentence, couched in a very informal style, while

Father were very tired after his lengthy journey

is a sentence in a nonstandard (north of England, for instance) variety of English, as


attested by the nonstandard verb form were, couched in a rather formal style. It is true that,
in most English-speaking societies there is a tendency - a social convention perhaps - for
Standard English to dominate in relatively formal social situations, but there is no necessary
connection here, and we are therefore justified in asserting the theoretical independence of
the parameter standard-nonstandard from the parameter formal-informal. This theoretical
independence becomes clearer if we observe sociolinguistic situations outside the English-
speaking world. There are many parts of the world where speakers employ the local dialect
for nearly all purposes, such as Luxembourg, Limburg in the Netherlands, and much of
Norway. In such situations, a visit to the Town Hall to discuss important local political
problems with the mayor will not elicit a switch to Standard German or Dutch or
Norwegian , but it will produce styles of greater formality than those to be found on Friday
night in the local bar amongst a group of close friends. Stylistic switching occurs within
ialects and not between them.
This theoretical independence of the notion of Standard English from style does not mean
that there are not problems in individual cases of distinguishing the two, as Hudson and
Holmes (1995) have pointed out. For example, I tend to regard the use of this as an
indefinite in narratives as in

There was this man, and he’d got this gun... etc.

as a feature of colloquial style, but other linguists might regard it as a nonstandard


grammatical feature.

Standard English is not a register

We use the term register in the sense of a variety of language determined by topic, subject
matter or activity, such as the register of mathematics, the register of medicine, or the
register of pigeon fancying. In English, this is almost exclusively a matter of lexis, although
some registers, notably the register of law, are known to have special syntactic
characteristics. It is also clear that the education system is widely regarded as having as one
of its tasks the transmission of particular registers to pupils - those academic, technical or
scientific registers which they are not likely to have had contact with outside the education
system - and of course it is a necessary part of the study of, say, physical geography to
acquire the register - the technical terms - associated with physical geography.

It is, however, an interesting question as to how far technical registers have a technical
function - that of, for example, providing well-defined unambiguous terms for dealing with
particular topics - and how far they have the more particularly sociolinguistic function of
symbolising a speaker or writer’s membership of a particular group, and of, as it were,
keeping outsiders out. Linguists will defend the use of "lexical item" rather than "word" by
saying that the former has a more rigorous definition than the latter, but it is also
undoubtedly true that employing the term "lexical item" does signal ones membership of
the group of academic linguists. And it is not entirely clear to me, as a medical outsider,
that using "clavicle" rather than "collar-bone" has any function at all other than symbolising
ones status as a doctor rather than a patient.

Here again we find confusion over the term Standard English. The British National
Curriculum document for English talks frequently about "Standard English vocabulary". It
is not at all clear what this can mean. I have argued above that it cannot mean "vocabulary
associated with formal styles". Is it perhaps supposed to mean "vocabulary associated with
academic or technical registers"? If so, this would not make sense either, since the question
of register and the question of standard versus nonstandard are also in principle entirely
separate questions. It is of course true that it is most usual in English-speaking societies to
employ Standard English when one is using scientific registers - this is the social
convention, we might say. But one can certainly acquire and use technical registers without
using Standard English, just as one can employ non-technical registers while speaking or
writing Standard English. There is, once again, no necessary connection between the two.
Thus

There was two eskers what we saw in them U-shaped valleys


is a nonstandard English sentence couched in the technical register of physical geography.

This type of combination of technical register with a nonstandard variety is much more
common in some language communities than others. In German-speaking Switzerland, for
example, most speakers use their local nonstandard dialect in nearly all social situations and
for nearly all purposes. Thus it is that one may hear, in the corridors of the University of
Berne, two philosophy professors discussing the works of Kant using all the appropriate
philosophical vocabulary while using the phonology and grammar of their local dialect.

It would of course be possible to argue that their philosophical vocabulary is not an integral
part of their native nonstandard Swiss German dialects and that the professors are
"switching" or that these words are being "borrowed" from Standard German and being
subjected, as loan words often are, to phonological integration into the local dialect. This,
however, would be very difficult to argue for with any degree of logic. All speakers acquire
new vocabulary throughout their lifetimes. There seems no reason to suppose that technical
vocabulary is the sole prerogative of standard varieties, or that while, if you are a
nonstandard dialect speaker, it is possible to acquire new non-technical words within your
own nonstandard dialect, it is sadly by definition impossible to acquire technical words
without switching to the standard variety. After all, dialects of English resemble each other
at all linguistic levels much more than they differ - otherwise interdialectal communication
would be impossible. There is no reason why they should not have most of their vocabulary
in common as well as most of their grammar and most of their phonology. If the Swiss
example tells us anything, it tells us that there is no necessary connection between Standard
English and technical registers.

So what is it then?

If Standard English is not therefore a language, an accent, a style or a register, then of


course we are obliged to say what it actually is. The answer is, as at least most British
sociolinguists are agreed, that Standard English is a dialect. As we saw above, Standard
English is simply one variety of English among many. It is a sub-variety of English. Sub-
varieties of languages are usually referred to as dialects, and languages are often described
as consisting of dialects. As a named dialect, like Cockney, or Scouse, or Yorkshire, it is
entirely normal that we should spell the name of the Standard English dialect with capital
letters.

Standard English is however of course an unusual dialect in a number of ways. It is for


example by far the most important dialect in the English-speaking world from a social,
intellectual and cultural point of view; and it does not have an associated accent.

It is also of interest that dialects of English, as of other languages, are generally


simultaneously both geographical and social dialects which combine to form both
geographical and social dialect continua. How we divide these continua up is also most
often linguistically arbitrary, although we do of course find it convenient normally to make
such divisions and use names for dialects that we happen to want to talk about for a
particular purpose as if they were discrete varieties. It is thus legitimate and usual to talk
about Yorkshire dialect, or South Yorkshire dialect, or Sheffield dialect, or middle-class
Sheffield dialect, depending on what our particular objectives are. Standard English is
unusual, seen against this background, in a number of ways. First, the distinction between
Standard English and other dialects is not arbitrary or a matter of slicing up a continuum at
some point of our own choice, although as we have seen there are some difficulties. This is
inherent in the nature of standardisation itself. There is really no continuum linking
Standard English to other dialects because the codification that forms a crucial part of the
standardisation process results in a situation where, in most cases, a feature is either
standard or it is not.

Secondly, unlike other dialects, Standard English is a purely social dialect. Because of its
unusual history and its extreme sociological importance, it is no longer a geographical
dialect, even if we can tell that its origins were originally in the southeast of England. It is
true that, in the English-speaking world as a whole, it comes in a number of different forms,
so that we can talk, if we wish to for some particular purpose, of Scottish Standard English,
or American Standard English, or English Standard English. (Bizarrely, the British
National Curriculuim document suggests that American and Australian English are not
Standard English!) And even in England we can note that there is a small amount of
geographical variation at least in spoken Standard English, such as the different tendencies
in different parts of the country to employ contractions such as He’s not as opposed to he
hasn’t. But the most salient sociolinguistic characteristic of Standard English is that it is a
social dialect.

At least two linguists have professed to find this statement controversial. Stein and Quirk
(1995) argue that Standard English is not a social class dialect because the Sun, a British
newspaper with a largely working-class readership, is written in Standard English. This
argument would appear to be a total non-sequitur, since all newspapers that are written in
English are written in Standard English, by middle-class journalist, regardless of their
readership.

Stein and Quirk also fly in the face of all the sociolinguistic research on English grammar
that has been carried out in the last quarter of the 20th century (see for example Cheshire,
1982). Standard English is a dialect which is spoken as their native variety, at least in
Britain, by about 12%-15% of the population, and this small percentage does not just
constitute a random cross-section of the population. They are very much concentrated at the
top (or, as some would prefer, "the top") of the social scale. The further down the social
scale one goes, the more nonstandard forms one finds.

Historically, we can say that Standard English was selected (though of course, unlike many
other languages, not by any overt or conscious decision) as the variety to become the
standard variety precisely because it was the variety associated with the social group with
the highest degree of power, wealth and prestige. Subsequent developments have reinforced
its social character: the fact that it has been employed as the dialect of an education to
which pupils, especially in earlier centuries, have had differential access depending on their
social class background.

So far we have not discussed grammar. When, however, it comes to discussing what are the
linguistic differences between Standard English and the nonstandard dialects, it is obvious
from our discussion above that they cannot be phonological, and that they do not appear to
be lexical either (though see below). It therefore follows that Standard English is a social
dialect which is distinguished from other dialects of the language by its grammatical forms.

Standard English is not a set of prescriptive rules

We have to make it clear, however, that these grammatical forms are not necessarily
identical with those which prescriptive grammarians have concerned themselves with over
the last few centuries. Standard English, like many other Germanic languages, most
certainly tolerates sentence-final prepositions, as in I’ve bought a new car which I’m very
pleased with. And Standard English does not exclude constructions such as It’s me or He is
taller than me.

Grammatical idiosyncrasies of Standard English

Grammatical differences between Standard English and other dialects are in fact rather few
in number, although of course they are very significant socially. This means that, as part of
our characterisation of what Standard English is, we are actually able to cite quite a high
proportion of them.

Standard English of course has most of its grammatical features in common with the other
dialects. When compared to the nonstandard dialects, however, it can be seem to have
idiosyncrasies which include the following

1. Standard English fails to distinguish between the forms of the auxiliary forms of
the verb do and its main verb forms. This is true both of present tense forms, where
many other dialects distinguish between auxiliary I do, he do and main verb I does,
he does or similar, and the past tense, where most other dialects distinguish between
auxiliary did and main verb done, as in You done it, did you?

2. Standard English has an unusual and irregular present tense verb morphology in
that only the third-person singular receives morphological marking: he goes versus
I go. Many other dialects use either zero for all persons or -s for all persons.

3. Standard English lacks multiple negation, so that no choice is available between I


don’t want none, which is not possible, and I don’t want any. Most nonstandard
dialects of English around the world permit multiple negation.

4. Standard English has an irregular formation of reflexive pronouns with some


forms based on the possessive pronouns e.g. myself, and others on the objective
pronouns e.g. himself. Most nonstandard dialects have a regular system employing
possessive forms throughout i.e. hisself, theirselves.

5. Standard English fails to distinguish between second person singular and second
person plural pronouns, having you in both cases. Many nonstandard dialects
maintain the older English distinction between thou and you, or have developed
newer distinctions such as you versusyouse.

6. Standard English has irregular forms of the verb to be both in the present tense
(am, is, are) and in the past (was, were). Many nonstandard dialects have the same
form for all persons, such as I be, you be, he be, we be, they be, and I were, you
were, he were, we were, they were.

7. In the case of many irregular verbs, Standard English redundantly distinguishes


between preterite and perfect verb forms both by the use of the auxiliary have and
by the use of distinct preterite and past participle forms: I have seen versus I saw .
Many other dialects have I have seen versus I seen.

8. Standard English has only a two-way contrast in its demonstrative system, with
this (near to the speaker) opposed to that (away from the speaker). Many other
dialects have a three-way system involving a further distinction between, for
example, that (near to the listener) and yon ( away from both speaker and listener).

Linguistic Change

There is also an interesting problem concerning which grammatical forms are and are not
Standard English which has to do with linguistic change, in general, and the fact that, in
particular, there is a tendency for forms to spread from nonstandard dialects to the standard.
Just as there are some difficulties in practice in distinguishing between features of
nonstandard dialect and features of colloquial style, as was discussed above, so there are
difficulties associated with standard versus nonstandard status and linguistic change. Given
that it is possible for nonstandard features to become standard (and vice versa), it follows
that there will be a period of time when a form’s status will be uncertain or ambiguous. For
example, most Standard English speakers are happy to accept the new status of than as a
preposition rather than a conjunction.in constructions such as

He is bigger than me

but less happy, for the time being, to do so in

He is bigger than what I am.

Similarly, American Standard English currently admits a new verb to got in

You haven’t got any money, do you?

but not (or not yet) in

You don’t got any money, do you?.

Nonstandard lexis
I have argued above that there is no necessary connection between formal vocabulary or
technical vocabulary and Standard English. That is, there is no such thing as Standard
English vocabulary. There is an interesting sense, however, in which this is not entirely
true. We can illustrate this in the following way. It is clear that there is such a thing as
nonstandard vocabulary. For instance, in the nonstandard dialect of Norwich, England,
there is a verb to blar which means to cry, weep. Not only is this verb regionally restricted,
to the dialects of this part of the country, it is also socially restricted - the small proportion
of the population of Norwich who are native speakers of Standard English do not normally
use this word, although they are perfectly well aware of what it means. This means that
there is a sense in which we can say that to cry is a Standard English word, whereas to blar
is not However, cry is by no means only a Standard English word, since there are very
many other nonstandard dialects elsewhere in which it is the only word available with this
meaning, and even in the working-class nonstandard dialect of Norwich, to cry is a
perfectly common and frequently used word. Because Standard English is not
geographically restricted to any particular region, its vocabulary is available to all. There
are in any case also, of course, many cases in which Standard English speakers in different
parts of England employ different but equivalent words, and hundreds of cases in which the
vocabulary of English Standard English and American Standard English differ, as is very
well known. The usage in the National Curriculum of the term "Standard English
vocabulary" in the sense of "vocabulary that occurs in the Standard English dialect and no
other" thus remains problematical.

Conclusion

From an educational point of view, the position of Standard English as the dialect of
English used in writing is unassailable. (We should perhaps add, however, that it has
nothing whatsoever to do with spelling or punctuation!) As far as spoken Standard English
is concerned, we could conclude that the teaching of Standard English to speakers of other
dialects may be commendable - as most would in theory agree, if for no other reason than
the discrimination which is currently exercised against nonstandard dialect speakers in most
English-speaking societies - and possible - which I am inclined, for sociolinguistic reasons
(see Trudgill, 1975) to doubt. Either way, however, there is clearly no necessary connection
at all between the teaching of formal styles and technical registers, on the one hand, and the
teaching of the standard dialect, on the other.

References

Chambers, J. and Trudgill, P. (1997) Dialectology. 2nd edition. London: Cambridge


University Press.

Cheshire, J. (1982) Variation in an English Dialect. London: Cambridge University


Press.

Giles, H. (1973) Accent mobility: a model and some data. Anthropological


Linguistics 15: 87-105.
Hudson, R. and Holmes, J. (1995) Children's use of spoken Standard English.
London: School Curriculum and Assessment Authority.

Kloss, H. (1967) Abstand languages and Ausbau languages. Anthropological


Linguistics 9: 29-41.

Labov, W. (1966) The social stratification of English in New York City.


Washington: CAL.

Labov, W. (1972) Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania


Press.

Le Page, R. and Tabouret-Keller, A. (1985) Acts of identity. London: Cambridge


University Press.

Stein, G. and Quirk, R. (1995) Standard English. The European English Messenger
4.2: xxx

Trudgill, P. (1975) Accent dialect and the school. London: Edward Arnold.

Trudgill, P. (1992) Introducing language and society. London: Penguin.

Trudgill, P. and Cheshire, J. (1989) Dialect and education in the United Kingdom.
In J. Cheshire, V. Edwards, H. Münstermann & B. Weltens (eds.), Dialect and
education: some European perspectives. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. pp. 94-
109.

Downloaded from: http://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/home/dick/SEtrudgill.htm April, 12,


2011

You might also like