Identity and Impoliteness in Computer-Mediated Reader Responses
Identity and Impoliteness in Computer-Mediated Reader Responses
SHIV R. UPADHYAY
Abstract
This paper investigates the connection between linguistic impoliteness and
identity in computer-mediated communication through the examination of
reader responses to ideas and views expressed by professional journalists
in the online media. The study finds that respondents may use linguistic
impoliteness strategically to communicate disagreements, to argue against
an out-group’s ideological views, or to discredit ideological opponents. The
analysis of reader responses within a social psychological theory of identity
indicates that the use of impoliteness is linked to the respondent’s identifica-
tion of him/herself with a group and its ideological position and his/her
willingness to act toward the achievement of a goal desired within that
group.
1. Introduction
This paper explores the link between speaker identity and the phenome-
non of linguistic impoliteness through the examination of computer-me-
diated reader responses. Such responses are comments posted online by
readers of a news story, article, or editorial. Research on the link between
identity and linguistic impoliteness is now beginning to gain momentum
as researchers realize that there are many questions that remain unan-
swered. Brown and Levinson (1987) built their politeness framework
upon the notion of face, derived from Goffman’s (1967: 5), “the positive
social value a person effectively claims for himself ”, and many subse-
quent studies have investigated the link between various attributes of
face and linguistic politeness. The link between identity and impoliteness
Example (1)
Krauthammer is the village idiot. It’s difficult to understand why
someone so gullible and without wit or intelligent thought can have a
column in a major newspaper. Seriously, you need to seek help.
(The Washington Post, September 19, 2008)
Example (2)
What’s the next thing this liar is going to do? Is he going to hide
behind Palin’s skirt? The show must go on! Where is all that experi-
ence now? Coward! (CNN’s Cafferty File, September 24, 2008)
2. The data
This study examines several tokens of impoliteness in online reader re-
sponses to ideas and views expressed in newspaper articles and editorials.
It is important to note that online reader responses result from a speaker
participation framework that is different from that of face-to-face in-
teraction or that of regular print media. When speakers come together
face-to-face to talk, they are subject to what Goffman (1967: 11) calls
“the rule of self-respect and the rule of considerateness”, which creates
a situation in which each speaker tends to save his/her own face and that
of the other. Their language use and behavior are consequently moder-
ated by these rules. On the other hand, reader responses are posted on-
line by those who do not come into face-to-face contact with the author
or with other respondents. In the absence of face-to-face contact, and
given their desire to put their views and opinions across, it is plausible
that respondents may consider the rules of self-respect and considerate-
ness to be less relevant than their urgent desire to communicate with the
intent of achieving a goal. Unlike talk, which is essentially produced as
speech when two or more speakers come face-to-face to verbally interact
in real time, reader responses are generally computer-mediated and pro-
duced as text. Most online reader responses are produced as single shots.
While reader responses are prompted by ideas and views expressed in
the media (or its online version), response writers do not usually engage
in dialogic interaction with the author of the article publicly. Even when
they address the author of an article directly in their posting, it is reason-
able to assume that their responses are meant not just for the author but
for a much larger readership. In addition, the author does not generally
post a response back to a reader’s comments unlike in a face-to-face or
synchronous conversation in which the participants can, and generally
do, continue to respond to each other. Reader responses can, however,
be influenced by the response writers’ anticipation that their postings
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 111
will be read by others, including other response writers, who may react
in agreement or disagreement with their views.
The reader responses analyzed in this study are from the online ver-
sions of the Washington Post and New York Times, two widely read
American dailies, as well as from the online version of CNN’s Cafferty
File. A total of eleven reader responses from these sources are examined.
All of the reader responses revolve around the US presidential election
of 2008 and cover the period from April to November of the same year.
The eleven reader responses examined in this study are representative of
the fifty reader responses that were initially selected out of approxi-
mately three hundred reader responses I surveyed in the three media
sources. The fifty reader responses, which were chosen because they ex-
pressed critical views about a public figure and/or the author of a media
article, served as a corpus of data from which the eleven examined in
this study were taken. The critical views communicated in these reader
responses were blatantly face-aggravating in the sense discussed earlier
on, as well as in the following section.
Each of the reader responses examined in this study was a one-shot
and from a different respondent. Many of the reader responses from the
Washington Post were posted under such pseudonyms as “Concerned2”
and “LiberalPatriot” or under made-up names consisting of a possible
human name and something else as in “daniel13715”. On the other hand,
reader responses from the New York Times and CNN’s Cafferty File
tended to reveal respondents’ first and last names or just their first
names.
A caveat about the data analyzed in this study, and also a limitation,
is that they are all about the American presidential election of 2008. It
is fair to assume that those who watched this election closely would
agree that the election generated an unusual degree of interest among
the American people of all political stripes and that it was perhaps the
most fiercely contested election in decades. Many communicated their
concerns and stakes associated with this election through their responses
to newspaper articles and editorials on various aspects of the election. A
number of those who chose to respond to such articles and editorials
did so with linguistic impoliteness; for this reason, their responses are
considered appropriate data for analysis in this study.
and Brown and Levinson (1987). Lakoff (1979: 64) views politeness as
“a device used in order to reduce friction in personal interaction”. Build-
ing upon Grice’s (1968) maxims of conversation, Lakoff (1973, 2005)
formulates politeness in terms of pragmatic rules and sub-rules that
speakers follow to be polite. Similarly, Leech’s (1983: 82) framework,
which is also built upon the Gricean Cooperative Principle, lays out a
set of conversational maxims that “maintain the social equilibrium and
the friendly relations” among interactants. Leech (1983: 105), however,
suggested that linguistic impoliteness was a rather marginal phenomenon
when he observed that “conflictive illocutions tend, thankfully, to be
rather marginal to human linguistic behaviour in normal circumstances”.
Likewise, Brown and Levinson’s (1987) framework, perhaps the most
seminal and most extensively discussed one, is essentially built upon
Goffman’s (1967) notion of face and its positive and negative needs.
Their framework explicates different positive and negative politeness
strategies based on positive and negative face needs. Brown and Levin-
son’s (1987: 69) framework, however, lists three situations in which face-
threatening acts can be performed without redress: (1) when face con-
cerns are “suspended in the interest of urgency or efficiency”; (2) when
face threat is “very small” (their italics); and (3) when the addressor is
“vastly superior in power” or can use “audience support to destroy” the
addressee’s face “without losing his own”. In other words, committing
face-threatening acts without mitigating devices in the first and second
scenarios may not necessarily result in the perception of impoliteness. In
the third scenario, while the addressor may commit face-threatening acts
without fearing retribution from the addressee because of asymmetrical
power, the addressee and onlookers may perceive the addressor as be-
ing impolite.
While Brown and Levinson’s (1987) politeness framework provides
some understanding of how impoliteness can ensue, politeness research-
ers have recently pointed out its inadequacy, particularly when it comes
to accounting for linguistic impoliteness. Watts (2008: 290) has observed
that in many politeness approaches, including Brown and Levinson’s
(1987 ), impoliteness is “hardly ever focused on”. Locher and Watts
(2005: 10) consider Brown and Levinson’s framework inadequate in that
they see it as “a theory of facework, dealing only with the mitigation of
face-threatening acts” and argue that the framework “does not account
for those situations in which face-threat mitigation is not a priority, e. g.
aggressive, abusive, or rude behavior …” Instead, they propose a
broader framework of relational work within which, as they claim, the
politeness strategies proposed in Brown and Levinson (1987) and strate-
gies that reflect impolite behavior can be accommodated. Similarly, Cul-
peper et al. (2003) argue that Brown and Levinson’s (1987) politeness
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 113
4. Attributes of identity
There are different views of identity as a relevant aspect of social behav-
ior. Spencer-Oatey (2007: 640) observes that face is linked to the notion
of self and that identity theories “can offer very useful insights for both
the conceptualization and the analysis of face.” The relationship between
face and self has been previously pointed out by Goffman (1967: 5) in
his definition of face as the “image of self ” and by Brown and Levinson
(1987: 61) in their view of face as “the public self-image that every mem-
ber wants to claim”. While the social, psychological, and cognitive na-
ture of self is suggested in these views, there is a lack of agreement in
terms of how the self is construed. Simon (2004: 26) notes that the self
in North America is conceptualized principally in “individual(istic)
terms” and that in Europe the preferred term is “identity” with its em-
phasis on “the role of group memberships and intergroup relations”.
Scollon and Scollon (1995: 36), who posit that the self is reflected as an
individual’s identity in communication, point out that in Asian societies,
including China and Japan, the self is “more collectivistic” and “con-
nected to membership in basic groups such as the family or one’s work-
ing group […]”.
The self, as conceptualized in the North American psychological tradi-
tion, has been attributed a number of characteristics some of which are
114 Shiv R. Upadhyay
selves with a social movement. Simon points out that both pathways can
work simultaneously and that, when individuals’ collective identification
with a social cause is strong, they are likely to be involved in the cause
while they overlook their individual costs and benefits. Simon (2004:
184) also observes that, when people take a strong collective identifica-
tion path, they may become “diehards who support their group in word
and deed against all odds”. Now I turn to the analysis of reader re-
sponses.
Example (3)
What Barack said was an unfortunate truth (Many people in PA
ARE frustrated, angry, and yes, sometimes bitter.) and an opinion
poorly expressed. What much of what Hillary says are outright lies.
(The Washington Post, April 15, 2008)
Example (4)
Richard, thank you for a brilliantly expressed summary of the
Obama-Clinton moral fiber differences. Your description of the Clin-
ton values: “they are expressions of treacly nostalgia and not the life
of incredible affluence and situational morality she now enjoys” is a
work of art. We are sick of this slimey [sic] old time Clinton (and
McCain) neoconian ambush campaign. I hope the good people of
Pennsylvania will recognize the goodness of Obama and will end this
madness. (The Washington Post, April 15, 2008)
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 117
Example (5)
Dick, come on man, what has ever been genuine with these two liber-
als? Oh wait a minute, there is one thing, they both are genuinely
LIBERAL. Let’s get real here bubba, we all know Hillary is a liar,
even you liberals know that, she just isn’t as good at it as Bill, but
what gets me, you will explain away everything/anything that Barack
Hussein Obama says. What he said, actually every time he opens his
mouth he reveals just a little more about who and what he is. Isn’t this
fun DICK? (The Washington Post, April 15, 2008)
Example (6)
Richard Cohen is rarely ever right, about anything.
If this schmuck is for Obama, then Obama is wrong for the country,
take that to the bank.
He pimped for the war in Iraq, the biggest debacle in the history
of our country and now he is pimping for Barack Obama, a person
who consorted with Chicago underworld slumlords and went to a
church that had an unpatriotic, racist preacher, for twenty years.
At least he is consistent. (The Washington Post, April 15, 2008)
…), thus implying the latter is untrustworthy. The respondent also labels
Obama and Clinton as unauthentic (what has ever been genuine with
these two liberals?) and characterizes Clinton as a publicly known liar
(we all know Hillary is a liar, even you liberals know that …). Further-
more, the respondent uses Obama’s middle name, Hussein, thereby asso-
ciating him with Arabs and possibly with potential terrorists in the cur-
rent US American rhetoric, and projects him as an unknown commodity
because the American people have yet to find out who Obama really is
(actually every time he opens his mouth he reveals just a little more about
who and what he is), thereby suggesting he is unfit to be president. All
these negative comments, along with the damaging implications they
make, are interpretable as negatively marked, face-aggravating linguis-
tic behavior.
In Example (6), the respondent likewise speaks of the journalist disap-
provingly. The former suggests that the latter is unreliable (Richard Co-
hen is rarely ever right, about anything.) and contemptible (If this
schmuck is for Obama). Once again, we thus witness blatant face-attack-
ing behavior addressed to the journalist. The respondent then uses his/
her own negative evaluation of the journalist as a basis for disapproving
of Obama as a candidate (If this schmuck is for Obama, then Obama is
wrong for the country […]). The condemnation of the journalist intensi-
fies as the respondent calls him a pimp for his initial support of the Iraq
war as well as his favorable treatment of Obama in the article. Similarly,
the respondent disapprovingly characterizes Obama as keeping bad com-
pany (a person who consorted with Chicago underworld slumlords and
went to a church that had an unpatriotic, racist preacher, for twenty years).
By labeling the journalist as a pimp and associating Obama with undesir-
able people, the respondent demonstrates a language behavior that is
clearly interpretable as negatively marked in its face-attacking force.
Simon’s (2004) Self-Aspect Model of Identity offers insights that help
to uncover the social psychological significance of these impolite reader
responses. Two of the functions of identity, according to Simon (2004),
are to validate an individual’s place in society and to allow individuals
to recognize themselves as important social agents. By demonstrating
his/her familiarity with the feelings and experiences of rural Pennsylva-
nians (Many people in PA ARE frustrated, angry, and yes, sometimes
bitter), the respondent in (3) appears to be presenting him/herself as
someone who is linked to the community and who can relate to its
plight. The respondent also projects him/herself as an individual who
can discerningly evaluate the two presidential candidates. Accordingly,
Obama is projected as a truth-teller (What Barack said was an unfortu-
nate truth), with the implication that he is a man of morality, whereas
Clinton is presented as a liar (What much of what Hillary says are out-
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 119
right lies), thereby implying that she lacks morality. By making a moral
distinction between the two candidates, the respondent can be seen, fol-
lowing Simon (2004), as taking on the role of a social agent who pro-
motes a “virtuous” presidential candidate. Likewise, the respondent in
(4) negatively portrays Clinton as slimey [sic] and as someone of whom
we are sick, but characterizes Obama as a man of goodness, a quality
that hopefully the good people of Pennsylvania will recognize. An implica-
tion in the portrayal of Obama as a man of “goodness” is that people
should choose him as president. The respondent, like the one in (3), can
thus be viewed within Simon’s (2004) framework as a social agent who
is involved in promoting a “qualified” presidential candidate.
Following Simon’s (2004) model, the respondents in (5) and (6) can
similarly be seen as social agents involved in the rejection of the two
Democratic presidential candidates. It is through their act of rejection
that they validate their place in society and project themselves as impor-
tant social agents. In (5), the respondent suggests that both Obama and
Clinton are disqualified to be president by framing them as unauthentic
(what has ever been genuine with these two liberals?). Clinton is charac-
terized as a publicly known liar (we all know Hillary is a liar), and
Obama is implicated as being an Arab, and hence not a typical Ameri-
can, through the mention of his middle name, Hussein. These negatively
marked characterizations can be reasons for a candidate to lose in
American presidential elections. Similarly, the respondent in (6) argues
that Obama is wrong for the country because he is propped by people
like the journalist whose character is questioned, as discussed above.
Now, let us consider the following reader responses in order to see
how they, as expressions of impoliteness, serve to reveal the respon-
dent’s identity.
Example (8)
Hey, Charlie, you and Bush must have shared some pretty potent weed
for that interview! In this context equanimity equals a good buzz.
Come on, Charles, you’ve kissed the backsides of everyone in this
administration long enough, stand up and face up to the fact that you
are nothing more than a parrot for their twisted and immoral ideology.
I can just imagine what the interview actually was like. No doubt you
120 Shiv R. Upadhyay
represented the fawning courtier who kept telling the naked king how
wonderful his clothes looked.
The final statement of unreality or THC induced euphoria was your
comparison of W to Truman. How could you possibly compare W,
narcissistic, self-absorbed, privileged (he does have Connecticut Yan-
kee roots), spoiled brat, who was bailed out of every failed venture he
ever got himself into, and unwilling to own up to any of his mistakes
to Truman, who had history thrust upon him and honorably made the
tough choices and unflinchingly took responsibility for them by actu-
ally believing that “the buck stops here?”
(The Washington Post, September 19, 2008)
Both reader comments in (7) and (8) were made in response to an article
by journalist Krauthammer (2008) in the Washington Post. In essence,
Krauthammer opined that President Bush, despite his unpopularity now,
would be re-evaluated more positively in the future just as president
Truman was, who at the time he left office was “disparaged and highly
out of favor”. The comment in (9), which was posted in response to
CNN’s journalist Cafferty’s question “In light of the financial crisis,
should the first presidential debate be postponed?”, refers to the Repub-
lican presidential candidate John McCain.
The reader responses in (7) through (9) contain expressions that are
face-aggravating for the journalists as well as the candidates talked
about. In (7), the respondent’s characterizations of the journalist as the
village idiot, as someone so gullible and without wit or intelligent thought,
and as someone who needs to seek help are interpretable as negatively
marked due to the severity of the face-attack in this context. More im-
portantly, the respondent’s negatively marked language use against the
journalist is intensified through the use of the second person singular
pronoun, “you” (you need to seek help). Similarly, in (8) the characteriza-
tions of the journalist as a drug user in a personalized manner (Hey,
Charlie, you and Bush must have shared some pretty potent weed for that
interview!), as an obsequious person (Come on, Charles, you’ve kissed the
backsides of everyone in this administration long enough … No doubt you
represented the fawning courtier who kept telling the naked king how won-
derful his clothes looked, an apparent reference to Danish author Hans
Andersen’s fairly tale The Emperor’s New Clothes), and as a coward fol-
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 121
lower of bad ideology (stand up and face up to the fact that you are
nothing more than a parrot for their twisted and immoral ideology) would
be impolite since these characterizations are likely to be interpreted as
negatively marked and inappropriate language behavior by American
readers of online reader responses. The respondent in (8) also makes an
impolite comment about President Bush through his/her negatively
marked characterization of him as narcissistic, self-absorbed, and a
spoiled brat, besides other things. The comment the respondent in (9)
makes would likewise be seen as impolite since he/she negatively labels
John McCain as a liar and coward and by suggesting that he is a timid
evader (Is he going to hide behind Palin’s skirt?).
Earlier, I discussed Simon’s (2004) dual pathway model in the context
of social movement in which people who collectively identify themselves
with a group and its ideological views can become staunch supporters
willing to act toward the achievement of their group’s desired goal. The
American presidential election of 2008 may not have been driven by a
social movement, but the socio-political environment indicated that the
country was going to witness a historic change in American society and
politics. As I pointed out earlier, the election was vigorously contested
and ideologically divisive, and it aroused an unusual level of voter inter-
est and participation, possibly reflecting the degree of concern of those
Americans who seriously wanted to socially move on, but who feared
that the chance to bring in the change they wanted could be missed.
It is in the context of the vigorously contested and ideologically divi-
sive election with an unusual level of voter participation and interest in
change that the reader responses in (7), (8), and (9) can be meaningfully
understood. Since, as a conservative journalist, Krauthammer had made
it public through his previous columns in the Washington Post that he
was a McCain supporter, it is reasonable to deduce from the comments
in (7) that the respondent is a McCain opponent and possibly an Obama
supporter. This deduction is compatible with Simon’s (2004) observation
that members of an ingroup like each other, but dislike those of an
outgroup. By simply making impolite remarks and without giving a ra-
tional argument, the respondent in (7) is making an ad hominem argu-
ment, a sub-type of emotional argument (Kienpointner 2008), to dis-
credit the journalist’s credibility and to reject his positive view of Bush.
Within Simon’s (2004) dual pathway model, the impolite remarks sug-
gest the respondent’s identification of him/herself as a supporter of the
ideological view of the political party (in this case the Democratic party)
to which the journalist is opposed and as a willing agent to work toward
the achievement of the party’s collectively desired goal. Following the
same reasoning, the respondent in (8), given his/her remarks about the
journalist (in the first paragraph) as well as his/her counter argument (in
122 Shiv R. Upadhyay
the second paragraph), can be seen as aligning him/herself with the polit-
ical party that has set its goal to get Obama, not McCain, elected.
Through his/her face-aggravating comments, the respondent in (9) is
similarly suggesting that McCain is not fit to be president. Again, within
Simon’s (2004) framework, the respondent can be seen as making a case
against McCain, thereby identifying him/herself as one of those individ-
uals who would be involved in defeating the Republican presidential can-
didate.
Finally, let us consider the following reader responses to an online
New York Times editorial of November 5, 2008, that essentially attrib-
uted Obama’s presidential victory to his understanding of “what is wrong
with this country: the utter failure of government to protect its citizens”
and to his offer to the American people of “a government that does not
try to solve every problem but will do things beyond the power of individual
citizens.” The editorial also pointed out that the new president will need
to address “many other urgent problems,” including health care and im-
migration.
Example (10)
Now that you have succeeded in getting your candidate elected, what
will the NYT do to reclaim any modicum of journalistic integrity?
(The New York Times, November 5, 2008)
Example (11)
Dear NYT,
While you have successfully managed to get your candidate of choice
elected, you have lost much much more. That is, readers’ respect in
your journalistic integrity. It will be reflected in your readership
months to come. The American public is not as stupid as you think
they are. (The New York Times, November 4, 2008)
The respondent in (10) presupposes that the New York Times, which
endorsed Obama for president, has lost its journalistic integrity. This is
evident in the respondent’s query: what will the NYT do to reclaim any
modicum of journalistic integrity? The respondent in (11) directly alleges
that the newspaper lost its journalistic integrity and implies that it
wrongly assumes the American people to be gullible (The American pub-
lic is not as stupid as you think they are). The allegations about the loss
of journalistic integrity and the implication about the underestimation
of American people’s intellect can be seen as intentional, face-aggravat-
ing, and negatively marked linguistic behavior by American viewers of
online reader responses. They can also be seen as an effort to discredit
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 123
6. Conclusions
This study examined impoliteness in computer mediated reader re-
sponses to ideas and views communicated in the media. The analysis of
online reader responses has shown that the respondents strategically
used linguistic impoliteness expressed through overtly face-aggravating
comments. The respondents were found to use blatant face-aggravation,
and hence impoliteness, to communicate disagreements, to argue against
an outgoup’s ideological views, or to discredit ideological opponents.
Within Simon’s (2004) social psychological theory of identity, their use
of impoliteness was seen as serving to identify them as individuals with
views opposed to those of outgroup members or as social agents under-
taking the task of promoting a view collectively held by ingroup mem-
bers.
The analysis of online reader responses has shown that identity can
be produced interactionally and that it can influence language behavior.
For instance, the respondents in this study revealed their identities as
either supporters or opponents of the presidential candidates and of the
ideological positions the candidates represented through their comments
made in response to views expressed in the media. My analysis would
thus concur with Haugh’s observation (in this issue) that “evaluations of
impoliteness are not made independently of the identities attributed to
the persons concerned”. On the other hand, the publicly known (hence,
cognitively based) identity of a journalist as a supporter of conservative/
Republican ideology (for example, journalist Charles Krauthammer) or
liberal/Democratic ideology (for instance, journalist Richard Cohen) ap-
pear to have influenced the face-aggravating, and hence impolite behav-
ior, of respondents. This finding would thus be in line with Spencer-
Oatey’s (2007) observation that identity is both social/interactional and
cognitive.
The analysis has also indicated that Simon’s (2004) social psychologi-
cal theory of identity can be useful in gaining a deeper understanding of
language use and behavior. While the respondents’ positioning of them-
selves as ingroup or outgroup members became apparent in their re-
sponses, Simon’s framework provided a socio-psychological explanation
of such positioning. Of course, more discourse analysis would be needed
124 Shiv R. Upadhyay
Bionote
Shiv R. Upadhyay is a faculty member at York University, Toronto,
Canada. He received his Ph. D. in Linguistics from Georgetown Univer-
sity. His research interests include sociolinguistics, critical discourse
analysis, linguistic politeness, intercultural communication, ESL and sec-
ond language acquisition. E-mail: [email protected]
Note
* I wish to thank Miriam Locher and the three anonymous reviewers as well as Brook
Bolander and Tanja Hammel for their valuable comments and suggestions on this
paper. Any remaining shortcomings are indeed mine.
References
Allport, Gordon W. 1968. Is the concept of self necessary? In Chad Gordon and
Kenneth J. Gergen (eds.), The self in social interaction: Classic and contemporary
perspectives (vol. 1), 25⫺32. New York: Wiley.
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 125
Androutsopoulos, Jannis. 2006. Introduction: Sociolinguistics and computer-mediated
communication. Journal of Sociolinguistics 10 (4). 419⫺438.
Arundale, Robert B. 2005. Face as relational and interactional: Alternative bases for
research on face, facework, and politeness. Paper presented at the 9th International
Pragmatics Association Conference, Riva del Garda, Italy, 10⫺15 July.
Baym, Nancy. 1996. Agreements and disagreements in a computer-mediated discus-
sion. Research on Language and Social Interaction 29 (4). 315⫺345.
Brown, Penelope and Stephen Levinson. 1987. Politeness: Some universals in language
usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bucholtz, Mary and Kira Hall. 2005. Identity and interaction: A socio-cultural linguis-
tic approach. Discourse Studies 7 (4⫺5). 585⫺614.
Cohen, Richard. 2008. Guns, god and gotchas. The Washington Post, April 15. http://
www.washingtonpost.com (accessed 15 April 2008).
Culpeper, Jonathan. 2008. Reflections on impoliteness, relational work and power. In
Derek Bousfield and Miriam A. Locher (eds.), Impoliteness in language: Studies
on its interplay with power in theory and practice (Language, Power and Social
Process 21), 17⫺44. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Culpeper, Jonathan, Derek Bousfield and Anne Wichmann. 2003. Impoliteness revis-
ited: With special reference to dynamic and prosodic aspects. Journal of Pragmat-
ics 35. 1545⫺1579.
Donath, Judith. 1999. Identity and deception in the virtual community. In Marc A.
Smith and Peter Kollock (eds.), Communities in cyberspace, 29⫺59. London:
Routledge.
Eelen, Gino. 2001. A critique of politeness theories. Manchester: St Jerome.
Goffman, Erving. 1967. Interaction rituals: Essays on face-to-face behavior. Garden
City, NY: Anchor.
Graham, Sage Lambert. 2007. Disagreeing to agree: Conflict, (im)politeness and iden-
tity in a computer-mediated community. Journal of Pragmatics 39. 742⫺759.
Grice, Paul. 1968. Utterer’s meaning, sentence-meaning, and word-meaning. Founda-
tions of Language 4. 1⫺18.
Herring, Susan. 2007a. The pragmatics of computer-mediated communication: Pros-
pectus for an emerging research agenda. Paper presented at the 10th International
Pragmatics Conference, Goteborg, Sweden, July 8⫺13.
Herring, Susan. 2007b. A faceted classification scheme for computer-mediated dis-
course. Language@Internet 4. 1⫺37. http://www.languageatinternet.de (accessed
13 February 2009).
Kakavá, Christina. 1993. Negotiations of disagreement by Greeks in conversations
and classroom discourse. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University unpublished
doctoral dissertation.
Kienpointner, Manfred. 2008. Impoliteness and emotional arguments. Journal of Po-
liteness Research 4 (2). 243⫺265.
Kotthoff, Helga. 1993. Disagreement and concession in disputes: On the context sensi-
tivity of preference structures. Language in Society 22 (2). 193⫺216.
Krauthammer, Charles. 2008. History will judge. The Washington Post, September 19.
http://www.washingtonpost.com (accessed 19 September 2008).
Lakoff, Robin. 1973. The logic of politeness, or minding your p’s and q’s. Chicago
Linguistics Society 9. 292⫺305.
Lakoff, Robin. 1979. Stylistic strategies within a grammar of style. In Judith Orasanu,
Mariam Slater and Leonore Adler (eds.), Language, sex, and gender, 53⫺80. New
York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Lakoff, Robin T. 2005. Civility and its discontents: Or, getting in your face. In Robin
T. Lakoff and Sachiko Ide (eds.), Broadening the horizon of linguistic politeness
126 Shiv R. Upadhyay
(Pragmatics and Beyond New Series 139), 23⫺43. Amsterdam and Philadelphia:
John Benjamins.
Leech, Geoffrey. 1983. Principle of pragmatics. London: Longman.
Lim, Tae-Sop. 1994. Facework and interpersonal relationships. In Stella Ting-Toomey
(ed.), The challenge of facework, 209⫺229. New York: State University of New
York Press.
Linville, Patricia. 1985. Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related ill-
ness and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (4). 663⫺676.
Locher, Miriam A. 2004. Power and politeness in action: Disagreements in oral com-
munication (Language, Power and Social Process 12). Berlin and New York: Mou-
ton de Gruyter.
Locher, Miriam A. and Derek Bousfield. 2008. Introduction: Impoliteness and power
in language. In Derek Bousfield and Miriam A. Locher (eds.), Impoliteness in
language: Studies on its interplay with power in theory and practice (Language,
Power and Social Process 21), 1⫺13. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Locher, Miriam A. and Richards J. Watts. 2005. Politeness theory and relational work.
Journal of Politeness Research 1 (1). 9⫺33.
Locher, Miriam A. and Richard J. Watts. 2008. Relational work and impoliteness:
Negotiating norms of linguistic behaviour. In Derek Bousfield and Miriam A.
Locher (eds.), Impoliteness in language: Studies on its interplay with power in theory
and practice (Language, Power and Social Process 21), 77⫺99. Berlin and New
York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Markus, Hazel, Jeanne Smith and Richard L. Moreland. 1985. Role of the self-concept
in the perception of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49 (6).
1494⫺1512.
Mills, Sara. 2005. Gender and impoliteness. Journal of Politeness Research 1 (2).
263⫺280.
Pomerantz, Anita. 1984. Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of
preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In John Maxwell Atkinson and John Heritage
(eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis, 75⫺101. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schlenker, Barry and Beth Pontari. 2000. The strategic control of information: Impres-
sion management and self-presentation in daily life. In Abraham Tesser, Richard
B. Felso and Jerry M. Suls (eds.), Psychological perspectives on self and identity,
199⫺232. Washington: American Psychological Association.
Scollon, Ron and Suzanne Wong Scollon. 1995. Intercultural communication. Ox-
ford: Blackwell.
Simon, Bernd. 2004. Identity in modern society: A social psychological perspective. Ox-
ford: Blackwell.
Spencer-Oatey, Helen. 2007. Theories of identity and the analysis of face. Journal of
Pragmatics 39. 639⫺656.
Terkourafi, Marina. 2008. Towards a unified theory of politeness, impoliteness, and
rudeness. In Miriam A. Locher and Derek Bousfield (eds.), Impoliteness in lan-
guage: Studies on its interplay with power in theory and practice (Language, Power
and Social Process 21), 45⫺74. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Tracy, Karen. 2008. “Reasonable Hostility”: Situation-appropriate face-attack. Jour-
nal of Politeness Research 4 (2). 169⫺191.
Waldron, Vincent and James Applegate.1994. Interpersonal construct differentiation
and conventional planning: An examination of two cognitive accounts for the
production of competent verbal disagreement tactics. Human Communication Re-
search 21. 3⫺35.
Watts, Richard. 1989. Relevance and relational work: Linguistic politeness as polite
behavior. Multilingua 8 (2⫺3). 131⫺166.
Identity and impoliteness in computer-mediated reader responses 127
Watts, Richard. 2005. Linguistic politeness research: Quo vadis? In Richard Watts,
Sachiko Ide and Konrad Ehlich (eds.), Politeness in language: Studies in its history,
theory and practice, 2nd edn, xi⫺xlvii. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Watts, Richard. 2008. Rudeness, conceptual blending theory and relational work.
Journal of Politeness Research 4 (2). 289⫺317.
Copyright of Journal of Politeness Research: Language, Behavior, Culture is the property of De Gruyter and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.