News & Views: A Heated Response To Danger
News & Views: A Heated Response To Danger
News & Views: A Heated Response To Danger
1038/d41586-020-00873-0
A heated response
To examine the role of this region in stress
responses, the authors impaired its connec-
tion to the DMH in three ways. They blocked
Nature | 1
©
2
0
2
0
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
News & views
the MD interacts with the prefrontal cortex to when presented with an aggressive, dominant for instance — is mediated, not by the DP/DTT,
mediate complex cognitive functions, such as counterpart that has recently defeated it in a but by another region upstream of the DMH,
rule learning, abstraction, evaluation and (in stressful social interaction. the preoptic area9. Blocking the DP/DTT–
humans) imagination7. Thus, every possible Under normal conditions, a defeated animal DMH pathway would therefore be expected
stressor, from physical pain to anticipated will try to stay away from the aggressor to to leave day-to-day regulation of temperature
legal trouble, can find their way to the DP/DTT. avoid incurring further damage. By contrast, unchanged. It is early days, but manipulation
It remains unclear, however, how different naive animals that have not previously gone of the DP/DTT could potentially be a way to
stressors are encoded in the DP/DTT, whether through a social defeat show no signs of fear, curb chronic psychological stress.
the responses of the DP/DTT to stressors are and investigate the dominant rat with great
influenced by experience, and whether defi- interest. Remarkably, when the authors Dayu Lin is at the Neuroscience institute
cits in DP/DTT cells could be responsible for blocked the DP/DTT–DMH pathway in rats and Department of Psychiatry, New York
abnormal physiological responses to stress. that had been defeated, the animals behaved University School of Medicine, New York,
Future studies using electrophysiological or like naive rats. New York 10016, USA.
optical recordings of the DP/DTT cells will help Thus, the behavioural manifestation of fear, e-mail: dayu.lin@nyulangone.org
to address these questions. and perhaps the perception of fear (which
The philosopher and psychologist William can only be inferred from behaviours in rats), 1. Oka, T. J. Temp. 2, 368–378 (2015).
James suggested that fear is an interpretation depends on bodily responses to threat. These 2. Kataoka, N., Hioki, H., Kaneko, T. & Nakamura, K.
of physiological responses to threat, instead of data provide an indication of why taking a deep Cell Metab. 20, 346–358 (2014).
3. Kataoka, N., Shima, Y., Nakajima, K. & Nakamura, K.
the other way around8. In other words, rather breath before that big public speech might Science 367, 1105–1112 (2020).
than running from a bear because we are help to calm us down. The data also suggest 4. Nakamura, K. et al. J. Neurosci. 24, 5370–5380 (2004).
afraid, we are afraid because we are running that suppressing physiological responses to 5. Lkhagvasuren, B., Nakamura, Y., Oka, T., Sudo, N. &
Nakamura, K. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1442–1452 (2011).
from a bear. If James is right, rats should stop stress could be an effective way to alleviate 6. Penzo, M. A. et al. Nature 519, 455–459 (2015).
being afraid if their physiological responses to stressful feelings. Of importance in this con- 7. Parnaudeau, S., Bolkan, S. S. & Kellendonk, C.
Biol. Psychiatry 83, 648–656 (2018).
a threat are blocked. Kataoka et al. therefore text, non-stress-related thermoregulation —
8. James, W. Mind 9, 188–205 (1884).
asked whether inhibiting the DP/DTT–DMH changes in internal temperature caused by 9. Morrison, S. F., Madden, C. J. & Tupone, D. Cell Metab. 19,
pathway can suppress the fear that a rat shows infections or external temperature change, 741–756 (2014).
2 | Nature
©
2
0
2
0
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.