CH - 3 - LPAD
CH - 3 - LPAD
Reuven Feuerstein
Louis H. Falik
Rafi Feuerstein
In 1979, the senior author, in a book entitled The Dynamic Assessment of Retarded
Performers, presented the rationale for needed alternatives to conventional
psychometric assessment, as well as a new approach to the assessment of learning
potential, the Learning Potential Assessment Device (LPAD). The LPAD was, and
continues to be, related to the development of the theory of structural cognitive
modifiability (SCM) and its applied systems – both conceptual (Mediated Learning
Experience; MLE) and programmatic (Instrumental Enrichment - IE). That formulation
of the concepts and processes of what came to be generally described as dynamic
assessment stimulated considerable research and clinical interest.
As early as 1981, Ramey and MacPhee, in a review of the Feuerstein et al (1979)
book, identified the theory and approach as representing a new paradigm with regard to
assessment, with particular impact on conventional psychometric practice. The shift
from traditional assessment methods was, they said, impelled by disenchantment with
the logical inconsistencies in the traditional system (theory and practice), by a rec-
ognition of the need to respond differently to specific segments of the population, and
by the emergence of a new conception of learning and intelligence that spurs the
development of a new "technology." That paradigm, presented by Feuerstein and his
colleagues, stimulated great interest in the development of procedures and methodology
to provide alternatives to a wide range of conventional practices.
This interest has been reflected in the development of a number of systems and
approaches to assessment that have been identified as dynamic. They have been
subjected to critical review and comparative analyses (see Campione, 1989; Jitendra &
Kameenui, 1993; Sternberg and Grigirenko, 2002) and have joined the LPAD in the
pantheon of attempts to address the acknowledged need for paradigm shifts. Among the
more systematically developed are Assisted Learning for Transfer (Campione & Brown,
1987), Testing the Limits (Carlson & Wiedl, 1978, 1979), the Continuum of
Assessment Model (Bransford, Delclos, Vye, Burns, & Hasselbring, 1987); Learning
Potential (Budoff, 1974, 1987); and Learning Tests (Guthke, 1992; Guthke & Stein,
1996). Each of these approaches has addressed aspects of the dynamic assessment
paradigm, adding important dimensions to the definitions and processes of assessment,
but – as we shall describe below – none goes far enough to implement changes in the
process to fully meet what we believe are the critical and essential requirements of the
assessment process. There is a growing literature, stimulated by our initial thinking and
operational propositions but less closely related to our perspective, that considers the
various elements, needs, methodologies, and research applications of alternative assess-
ment processes that are to some degree categorized as dynamic in their nature and
purpose (see Hamers, Sijtsma, & Ruijssenaars, 1993; Haywood & Tzuriel, 1992; Lidz,
1987; Lidz and Elliott, 2000).
*
Earlier version of this article was published as Feuerstein, R., Falik, L., & Feuerstein, Ra. (1998).
Feuerstein’s LPAD. In R.Samuda (Ed.), Advances in Cross-Cultural Assessment. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
The LPAD reflects a different view of human beings and their development. It
represents a sharp departure from practices that are based on a view of human
characteristics as fixed, immutable, and therefore subject to study by psychometric
methods of measurement. In its underlying theory, in its structure of instruments, and in
its development of procedures, the LPAD presents a radical alternative to the
statistically based, normative comparisons and predictive goals of conventional
assessment. In its simplest sense, the LPAD shifts the focus from what the individual is
able to do (at a given moment in time) to what the individual can become able to do in
the immediate time frame and in subsequent, future interactions. In the LPAD, whatever
is done, through the process of assessment and stimulation of behavioral changes,
cannot be considered as the limits of the individual's ability to benefit from the
intervention or the examiner's activity. It is the limit of what can be done at the
particular moment. Eventually, at some other time, with modified and adapted
interventions, or in some other regions of functioning, further modifiability can be
anticipated. It is this basic understanding – that we cannot reach all of the regions or
potentials of knowledge about the other without an open, adaptive posture in our
process and our instrumentation – that underlies the LPAD philosophy.
In this regard, it has become necessary to change some of our nomenclature. As the
goal of the LPAD is to discover the hidden potential of the individual, which is not
revealed by manifest levels of functioning, the use of the term potential has come to be
somewhat ambiguous and used in a limiting and restrictive way. We have pointed out
elsewhere (Feuerstein, Feuerstein, & Gross, 1996) that the construct of potential is as
limiting as the concept of intelligence to a given quantity or even quality of the
individual's functioning. We are therefore proposing the term propensity to denote
qualities of power, energy, orientation, and inclination, so as to better reflect the
individual's unrevealed innate capacities. Thus, the Learning Potential Assessment
Device, which has had an active life of over 40 years in use, becomes the Learning
Propensity Assessment Device to do greater justice to the mental construct of
intelligence as a propensity to change and adapt.
The Output Phase: Deficiencies at the output phase include those that result in
inadequate communication of final solutions. Even adequately gathered data and
appropriate elaboration can result in inappropriate expression if difficulties exist for the
individual at this phase. Specific difficulties include:
Egocentric communication modalities
Difficulty in projecting virtual relationships
Blocking
Trial and error responses
Lack, or impairment, of verbal or other tools for communicating adequately
elaborated responses
Lack, or impairment, of need for precision and accuracy in the communication of
one's responses
Deficiency in visual transport
Impulsive. random, unplanned behavior
LPAD examiners must be thoroughly familiar with the deficient cognitive functions
to detect their manifestation in the performance of the examinee; they must also know
the mediational interventions offered to correct such deficiencies. Sources of difficulties
are identified, interventions are directed toward them, and the instruments are presented,
manipulated, and interacted with to stimulate responses that elicit change and indicate
that the change is structural. The reader will become familiar with what this process
entails, as we further discuss the structure of the LPAD process and the nature of the
instruments.
Content: Each mental act can be described according to the subject matter with
which it deals and the universe of content on which it operates. Experiential and
educational background (e.g., prior learning that has been assimilated) and culturally
determined saliency (the importance and value as a factor of an individual's cultural
experience) lead to differential levels of competency in individuals.
If the content is strange to the learner – and indeed, people differ greatly as to the
specific content they are exposed to and familiar with – or if facts, events, or details of
the required performance are not within the individual's experiential repertoire, there
will need an investment in acquiring mastery before the learner can be expected to focus
on the cognitive operations that are the target of the assessment. Failure to respond,
therefore, must be considered in light of the presence or absence of relevant content
dimensions embedded in the task. Any attempt to evaluate the intelligence of the
individual without considering content as a source of success or failure is doomed to do
injustice to the individual.
The Phase of the Mental Act: The three phases of the mental act – input, elaboration,
and output – may be differentially represented in a given task. When functioning is
appropriate, it is difficult to clearly identify the contribution of each specific phase.
With failure, however, it is necessary to isolate the responsible phase and understand its
role in interfering with performance, as a basis for assessment and intervention. A task
that places too much emphasis on input from the individual may disadvantage that
individual in subsequent performance. For example, an individual's response may be
inadequate because of incomplete, imprecise data gathering, which, even if elaborated
properly, would lead to failure at the output phase.
As a dimension of the task, examiners must analyze the specific phase requirements
or emphases embedded within it to understand failures in performance, and then link
them more specifically to the cognitive dysfunctions that may be present in the
individual. If, for example, the task requires primarily input or output phase functions,
performance on the task may be more resistant to change than if elaboration is
emphasized, and this may require more investment of time and energy or focus on
structural interventions. The analysis of impaired performance in terms of phase helps
to locate deficient cognitive functions and the source of difficulties and attribute a
differential weight to success or failure. Thus, an arithmetical problem requiring the
computation of 100 additions is measurably less difficult than one requiring four types
of operations ordered in a given sequence.
Operations: A mental act may be analyzed according to the operations that are
required for its accomplishment. An operation may be understood as a group of
activities that enable information derived from internal and external sources to be
organized, transformed, manipulated, and acted upon in a way that generates new
information. In defining the nature of an operation, it is important to identify the
prerequisites necessary for its generation and application. For example, classification,
seriation, logical multiplication, or analogical, syllogistic, or inferential thinking are
more complex in the demands they place upon the individual to use cognitive functions
than recognition or comparison.
When the examinee's performance is impaired, the examiner must determine the
component elements in the task necessary for the acquisition and/or application of the
required elements and assess the presence or level of impairment in the related cognitive
functions required to achieve the operation.
Level of Complexity: The level of complexity of a task may be understood as the
quantity and quality of units of information required to be handled for its solution.
However, this in turn is contingent on the quality of the information, its degree of
novelty for the individual/and the level of conceptual organization. The more familiar
the units, and the more organized, even if they are multiple, the less complex the act; the
less familiar, or organized, the more complex the mental act. It is thus necessary to
analyze the task from three perspectives: (a) the number of units of information
contained in the task, (b) the degree of familiarity the subject has with the task and its
component elements, and (c) the degree of organization, grouping, and categories that
allows a reduction in the complexity of the task. Intervention and mediation is then
directed toward these dimensions. As these elements are modified by mediation of
organization, levels of complexity change, both within tasks and across tasks with
similar structures or modalities.
The primary modality of the task is figural and grapho-motor. Operations included in
this task include differentiation, segregation of overlapping figures, conservation of the
figure across changes in its position, articulation of the field, and representation
(interiorization).
Representational Stencil
Design Test (RSDT)
The RSDT is based on the Stencil Design Test of Grace Arthur (1930), but it differs
significantly in its structure and technique of application, primarily in its shift of the
task away from the concrete, manipulative modality toward a representational,
internalized modality. In the LPAD procedure, the design is constructed by the subject
on a purely mental level. The instrument consists of 20 designs that the subject must
deconstruct representationally by referring to a page of model "solid" and "cut-out"
stencils that must be mentally superimposed upon one another. The problems increase
in level of difficulty (on dimensions of form, color, and structure) and are organized so
that mastering simpler problems leads to the ability to solve harder ones. The procedure
of this test orients the subject to the stencil page, offers a test page of problems, and
then provides a training page to mediate various processes and strategies according to
what is observed during performance on the test page. A Parallel Test is provided to be
used following mediation. The instrument assesses the subject's ability to learn a
complex task using internalized systems of organizing, and to use acquired learning to
solve more complicated problems. Part of what is assessed in this instrument is how
readily available the learner's inner (representational) processes are and how easily and
adaptively they are used in subsequent problems of increased complexity and
abstraction. The modalities involved are figural, numerical, and verbal. The operations
involved in successful mastery of the tasks are segregation, differentiation,
representation, anticipation of transformation, encoding and decoding, and
generalization.
Figure 2. Examples of Matrix Variations I Based on the Learning Potential Assessment Device model
SOURCE: Adapted from Instrumental Enrichment. Copyright Reuven Feuerstein and the Hadassah-WIZO-Canada
Research Institute, Jerusalem, Israel. All rights reserved. Reproduction by permission.
Numerical Progressions
This test assesses the subject's capacity to understand and deal with relationships,
identify them as rules, and apply them to building new information, using numerical and
graphic modalities. The task presents progressions of numbers, related to one another
according to rules that must be deduced from the available information. At the end of a
sequence of numbers, the subject is asked to supply the two missing numbers. A correct
response suggests that the subject has understood how the numbers are related to one
another. The format is that of a pretest, a learning phase, and two forms of a posttest. In
the learning phase, the subject is encouraged to formulate and state-the rule by which
the answers were achieved. The examiner teaches relationships that are not understood
and establishes strategies according to an analysis of needs (errors and performance on
the pretest). Following mediation, a posttest is given to determine how well the subject
has learned strategies for solving the problems. The parallel form of the posttest makes
possible assessing the permanence and stability of what has been learned over time. The
operations involved in this instrument are those of basic mathematics (addition,
subtraction, multiplication, and division) and the more generalized mental operations of
differentiation, segregation, inferential thinking, and deductive reasoning.
Organizer
This instrument presents the subject with a series of verbal statements consisting of
sets of items that must be organized according to closed, logical systems. The task
involves the subject placing the items (colors, objects, people, etc.) in positions relative
to one another according to the determined attributes or conditions presented in the
statements. A series of statements or premises is presented in each task. Each premise
permits the extraction of only a part of the needed information required to determine a
full and precise placement of the items. Thus, the subject must gather available
information, develop and test hypotheses with succeeding information given, and
generate information that is not immediately available in the given propositions. The
tasks become more complex because of more units of information and the level of in-
ference needed to solve them. What is assessed in this instrument is the subject's ability
to gather new information through the use of inferential processes, formulate hypotheses
and test them according to new information or assumptions generated, and apply
strategies for discovering relationships. The instrument consists of pretest, learning, and
test phases.
The modality is verbal, with a numerical subcomponent, The operations involve
decoding, encoding, representation, inferential thinking, transitive thinking,
prepositional reasoning, negation, with a heavy loading of mnemonic (memory)
functions.
The readiness of the examinee to grasp the principle underlying the initial problem
and to solve it
The amount and nature of investment required in order to leach the examinee the
given principle
The extent to which the newly acquired principle is successfully applied in solving
problems that become progressively more different from the initial task
The differential preferences of the examinee for one or another of (the various
modalities of presentation of a given problem
The differential effects of different training strategies offered to the examinee in the
remediation of functioning, involving the criteria of novelty-complexity, language of
presentation, and types of mental operation
The use of this dynamic approach in assessment assumes that the individual
represents an open system that may undergo important modifications through exposure
to external and/or internal stimuli. However, the degree of modifiability of the
individual through direct exposure to various sources of stimulation is considered to be
a function of the quantity and quality of MLE. It is the MLE that sensitizes the human
organism to specific characteristics of the stimuli and establishes sets and modalities for
grasping and elaborating reality, vital for the appropriate integrated use of new
experience.
Static measures completely neglect separate assessment of the dimension of
modifiability because they equate the measure of manifest functioning with the true,
fixed, and immutable capacity of the individual. The dynamic approach does not deny
the fact that the functioning of the individual, as observed in the level of achievement or
general behavior, is low; but by considering this level as pertaining only to the manifest
repertoire of the individual, it takes into consideration the possibility of modifying this
repertoire by appropriate strategies of intervention.
The tasks in the LEAD instruments are shaped in such a way as to provoke the
appearance of the deficient cognitive functions viewed as responsible for the failure of
the individual to master the task and adapt to a variety of life and learning conditions. It
is the objective of the various instruments to tease out the types of deficiencies and,
through the analyses of the process, observe what is causing success or failure. The
tasks are therefore selected and constructed according to the dimensions of deficient
cognitive functions and the cognitive map. In the RSD instrument, for example, we try
to figure out the type of perception of the individual, the capacity to analyze, to create
cardinal order, to represent what is perceived abstractly. Each task, in this and all other
instruments, is presented to permit addressing certain conditions of cognitive
functioning that are related to functioning in other areas – modalities of responding,
academic areas of performance, and the like.
An additional goal determining the structure of the LPAD tasks is the search for
indicators of even the most minimal changes in the functioning of the individual, to be
used as representative samples of modifiability. For example, increased speed of
formulating responses or expressions of certainty or energy in responding, often signify
the establishment of changes at a structural level and give the examiner cues for further
or different interventions.
Examiner-Examinee Relationship
The motivation of a low-performing and/or culturally deprived examinee in the
conventional test situation is usually low because the tasks included rarely have appeal.
A reduced level of curiosity is only one reason for a lack of motivation. Another is that
the perception of novelty necessary to elicit an orienting reflex and an arousal followed
by an exploration is not always present. Perception of novelty depends upon cognitive
functions such as comparative behavior, analytic perception, and a capacity to grasp
relationships and their transformation within a constant framework. The lack of task-
intrinsic motivation is then further aggravated by the negative valence with which the
presented task may be endowed, provoking an avoidance reaction in the individual, who
associates the task with repeated experiences of failure. Failure experiences become the
source of deeply ingrained feelings of intellectual insufficiency that further increase the
negative reaction evoked by the novel tasks.
The examiner must therefore orient the relationship toward this condition of reduced
motivation about the test situation, paying particular attention to three distinct
determinants: (a) lack of curiosity resulting from deficiency in the prerequisite cognitive
conditions, (b) lack of a need system that endows successful performance with specific
meaning, and (c) the existence of a negative component – an avoidance reaction to tasks
that have been associated with repeated experiences of failure, which leads to deeply
ingrained feelings of intellectual inadequacy.
Given the lack of positive task-intrinsic motivation and the presence of aversive
qualities, one can understand that the specific weight of emotional factors in
determining the outcome of the conventional test situation is much greater than one is
led to believe by the casual mention usually made of the meaning of the examiner-
examinee relationship and the maintenance of the rapport established between them.
The presence of a neutral, even sympathetic, and yet basically unresponsive examiner
who limits the interaction with the examinee to issuing dry, standardized instructions
cannot but add a further negative valence to the test situation. The examinee's possibly
fragmentary grasp of the instructions, as well as a potential lack of motivation toward
the task, will lead either to a correspondingly vague or imprecise way of dealing with
the problem at hand, accompanied by a low level of anxiety and a "tuning-out" of the
examiner, or – to the contrary – to a high level of anxiety, involving a feeling of great
threat and low expectation of success. Thus, the lack of manifest interest on the part of
the examiner, prescribed by the standardized test procedure, is potentially interpreted by
the examinee in two different ways, both leading to negative reactions. First, "if it
doesn't matter to you, why should I be concerned with it?" This is then followed by a
tuning-out by the examinee, who no longer pays much attention to the task and proceeds
to respond in a random or casual manner. Second, the examinee may interpret the
neutrality of the examiner, even if basically benevolent, as a manifestation of hostility
and an expectation of performance failure. This reduces efficiency by lowering
motivation to cope or by energizing a countering hostility that interferes with any cog-
nitive process that might otherwise have emerged.
The LPAD technique not only allows but intentionally creates the conditions for a
radical change. This is accomplished by a shift in the roles of examiner-examinee into
the relationship between teacher (the mediator) and pupil (the mediatee). What follows
is an elimination of the neutral, indifferent role of the examiner in exchange for the
active cooperative role of the mediator, who is vitally concerned with the maximization
of the success of the pupil. It is through this shift in roles that we find both the examiner
and the examinee engaged in the same task, in a common quest for mastery of the
material. Thus, the examiner constantly intervenes – questions, orients, makes remarks,
interprets results, and gives explanations whenever and wherever they are necessary,
asks for repetition, sums up experiences, anticipates difficulties, warns the examinee
about them, and creates reflective insightful thinking in the individual, not only
concerning the task but also regarding the examinee's reactions to it. To accomplish all
this, the examiner must be alert to each reaction of the individual, and in the course of
behaving this way, the examiner acts radically different than the usual psychometrician.
"The examiner is vibrant, active, and concerned instead of aloof, distant, and neutral,
giving the examinee the feeling that the task is important, difficult, yet quite
manageable and that the examiner is committed to the examinee's success.
With the establishment of such an interactive process, we usually observe a sharp
increase in motivation. At the beginning, it is purely extrinsic, with the major motive of
the examinee being to please the examiner. At this stage, any manifestation of reduced
or discontinued interest on the part of the examiner is followed by a marked decrease in
the efficiency of the trainee. Later, as the teacher-trainee relationship develops, and
includes the task as a part of it, turning the dyad into a triad, we invariably observe a
shift from extrinsic to intrinsic motivation. That is, the examinee begins to delight in the
task itself, having grasped the deeper meaning of his or her own activity and the
successful mastery of the task.
This shift is basically produced by two factors. One is directly linked to the capacity
of the individual to perceive the nature of the problem by having integrated a series of
criteria, at the end of which the solutions that are confronted become problems. Here,
the TOTE (Test Operate-Test Exit) model is relevant in explaining the growing interest
in the task itself, following the establishment of internal standards through previous
experience (Hunt, 1961; Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960). The second factor has to
do with the development of a positive approach to problem solving through increased
mastery of tasks, especially when the sequence of tasks follows the LPAD model of
progressively increasing difficulty. Such mastery immediately raises the need in the
individual to repeat the experience. This repetition has functional value in that it
consolidates and crystallizes a successful pattern of behavior in a way similar to the
circular reactions described by Piaget, and at the same time, it raises the level of
aspiration and the achievement motivation of the examinee. At this point, it is the task
that becomes the center of interest and motivation of the examinee, and no longer is
motivation solely aroused by the examiner.
This shift in motivation, achieved by assigning meaningfulness, giving
encouragement, and ensuring the experience of success, will not suffice to make the
examinee's problem-solving behavior successful and efficient. For this, it is necessary
to provide the examinee with a constant, fine-grained feedback of this interaction with
the task that transcends the task itself and uses a variety of communicational modalities.
In the usual psychometric model, feedback is often considered valueless or deleterious
to either the examinee, to the standardized testing procedures, or to both. It is
considered deleterious if the individual is told of his or her failure, without helping and
permitting correction in a meaningful way. Even if correction is allowed in certain tests,
it does not take the form of a thorough feedback strategy, focused on helping the
examinee to master the present material in order to enable more effective performance
on future test items. In tests whose structure does not involve interitem dependency, the
task-bound feedback is considered to have negative instead of positive implications for
future test items. The individual learns only that failure has occurred, but not how or
why. Even if the examinee should be shown how or why, little or nothing is gained that
the individual cope with subsequent items because they will be very different. No
wonder the psychometrist conventionally limits the amount of feedback interaction with
the examinee. The usual static test is structurally not suited to the use of feedback
procedures.
In the case of the dynamic LPAD procedures, the feedback fulfills a variety of
functions. It is used as a constituent part of the training process. The examinee is
informed of the nature of the product (his/her responses) in a differentiated way,
allowing for an immediate correction of incorrect responses or permitting generalization
of the specific behavior employed if the response was adequate. In both cases, there is
neither an increase in anxiety nor a reduction of the optimal motivation needed to
maintain' interest in further accomplishment. Successes are acknowledged through the
conveyance of exuberance, interest, and pleasure, intended to communicate the meaning
of the experienced success. Failure, on the other hand, is acknowledged in a tone that,
although it diminishes the importance of the failure, still includes the challenge to do
better. In other cases, behavioral patterns leading to one or another result are analyzed
and explained, thus rewarding certain types of behavior as differentiated from other
facets of the response.
In summary, the personal interaction between the examiner and the examinee on the
LPAD has as its bask outcome an increase in the test-taking motivation of the examinee
by the feet that the examiner (acting as a teacher-trainer) conveys to the examinee
(responding as the pupil-trainee) the meaning of the task, the importance of mastering it,
the capacity to do so, and finally, by a process of feedback, an ability to select the
appropriate behavior leading to success. This process is also intended to produce a shift
from extrinsic to intrinsic motivation in the examinee, thus engendering more
independence and, to a certain extent, more reality orientation. We feel that in this kind
of testing the personal relationship, which entails the change in interaction patterns as
described, is a necessary condition for the appropriate assessment of the modifiability of
culturally deprived and low-functioning individuals. This has implications leading to an
emphasis on individualized testing, with one-to-one relationships, careful focus on
mediational strategies, and much care to preserve the critical characteristics of the
interactional models described above. However, it is possible to extend the process to
group situations (see below) and to other modalities of interaction, such as programmed
learning systems that may or may not be computer based. However, in such extensions,
one must argue for extreme caution and vigilance, not only as to the application but
even more so as to interpretation of the results, lest the mediational and interactive
aspects essential to the approach be lost or so diluted as to become counterproductive.
1. Information can be collected on students in situations that are similar to the real
learning experience of students, where variables can be observed that are not available
in the one-to-one interaction. These include the subject's attending to instructions and
explanations, the maintaining of performance when direct monitoring is not being pro-
vided, response to distractions, self-control and behavioral monitoring in situations of
independent work formats, the effect of peer social relationships, and the like.
2. On the basis of information collected on the group, relevant interventions for the
group as a whole can be developed. Observations of group performance, response to
mediation, and the emergence of learning and didactic strategies can be formulated into
interventional suggestions that can be transmitted to teachers for implementation. In
addition, the development of individual programs derived from and relevant to group
performance becomes possible.
3. Because group assessment requires a more standard and structured set of initial
procedures, the procedure is more amenable and useful for research purposes. The
individual LPAD varies from examinee to examinee, from examiner to examiner, and
from session to session. This lack of consistency makes comparisons difficult, even
within the same subject. The group LPAD assessment procedure is of necessity more
structured, with less variability in the mediation, scoring, and examiner interventions,
making the baseline data available more appealing for research-oriented applications,
but less clinically rich and revealing.
The degree to which these criterial elements, or as they have been called elsewhere
(Feuerstein et al, 1995) qualitative parameters of change, are present in the functioning
of the subject is an important indicator of the subject's modifiability in a structural,
rather than peripheral, manner. In the LPAD, changes in specific task performance are
continuously – at the outset and throughout the assessment process – assessed in
relation to changes in generalized, higher-order thought processes. Indeed, the
mediational interventions offered the learner are designed to build in some of these
changes so that they can be observed in subsequent performance.
The LPAD is designed to provide information so that changes in performance are
observed, described, and analyzed within domains of functioning (related to a
delineation of the cognitive functions) and along parameters of meaningful
performance. When scores are obtained, they are used as descriptive of change, from
baseline to various degrees of post-intervention performance. They are not meant to be
considered normative or comparative, which we consider to be external to the
performance of the subject being assessed. It is in this context that we express our
concern that to the extent that approaches to dynamic assessment focus on task
performance, attempt to preserve psychometric properties of the assessment, and limit
the mediational interventions, they will inevitably limit the creation of conditions for
structural cognitive change, with restrictions on criterial elements for observation and
assessment. This is reflected in the model, design, and implementation of the LPAD.
Expansion of the Battery: The battery of instruments has expanded and developed
since the first publications on the LPAD (Feuerstein et al, 1979). We have made
possible the broader and more precise assessment of cognitive modifiability and
improved the linkage from the assessment process to the identification of and focus on
prescriptive and remediational strategies. This development includes elaborating
instruments in the logico-verbal and numerical modalities, using a variety of operations
in accordance with the LPAD model.
Validity Studies: One of our early concerns was the question of in vitro versus in
vivo validity. There is no question that during the lest sessions, changes occur in
response to mediational interventions. Examinees who become modified within the test
situation quite obviously leave the concrete and task-bound level and are able to
function with an abstract, internalized, representational conceptual thinking that was
inaccurately and unjustly considered inaccessible to them previously. But to what extent
and under what conditions will modification achieved within the test situation predict
later performance in academic and real-life settings? The question of whether the LPAD
procedure can attain degrees of reliability and validity might be answered by asking
another question: Under what conditions can and should one lest for validity? We
continue to study this issue and search for relevant and meaningful answers.
References
Arthur, G. (1930). A point scale of performance tests: clinical manual (Vol.1). New
York: Commonwealth Fund.
Bransford, J. I., Delclos, V. R., Vye, N. J., Burns, M. S., & Hasselbring, T. S.
(1987). State of the art and future directions. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment:
An interactional approach to evaluating learning potential. New York: Guilford.
Budoff, M. (1974). Learning potential and educability among the educable mentally
retarded (Final Report Project No. 312312). Cambridge, MA: Research Institute for
Educational Problems, Cambridge Mental Health Association.
Budoff, M. (1987). Measures for assessing learning potential. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.),
Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach to evaluating learning potential. New
York: Guilford.
Campione J. C. & Brown, A. L. (1987) Linking dynamic assessment with school
achievement. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach to
evaluating learning potential. New York: Guilford.
Campione J. C. (1989). Assisted assessment: A taxonomy of approaches and an
outline of strengths and weakness. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 151-165.
Carlson, J. S. & Wiedl, J. H. (1978). Use of testing-the-limits procedures in the
assessment of intellectual capabilities in children with learning difficulties. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 82, 559-564.
Carlson, J. S. & Wiedl, J. H. (1979). Toward a differential testing approach: Testing-
the-limits employing the Raven matrices. Intelligence, 3, 323-344.
Feuerstein, R. et al. (1979). The dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The
Learning Potential Assessment Device, theory, instruments, and techniques. Baltimore,
MD: University Park.
Feuerstein, R. et al. (1980). Instrumental enrichment: An intervention program for
cognitive modifiability. Baltimore, MD: University Park.
Feuerstein, R. et al. (1986). LPAD manual. Jerusalem: ICELP.
Feuerstein, R. & Feuerstein, S. (1991). Mediated learning experience: A theoretical
review. In R. Feuerstein, P. Klein, & A. Tannenbaum,(Eds.), Mediated learning
experience: Theoretical, Psychosocial, and Learning Implications. Tel Aviv and
London: Freund.
Feuerstein, R. et al. (1995). Revised LPAD examiner’s manual. Jerusalem: ICELP.
Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, R. & Gross, S. (1996). The Learning Potential
Assessment Device: History, theory, application, and results. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L.
Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Beyond traditional intellectual assessment:
Contemporary and emerging theories, tests and issues. New York: Guilford.
Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein Ra. S., Schur, Y. (1997). Process as content in education of
exceptional children. In A. Costa and R.Liebman, Eds., If Process were Content:
Sustaining the Spirit of Learning. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press
Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic
Books.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Guthke, J., & Stein, H. (1996). Are learning tests the better version of intelligence
tests? European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 12, 1-13.
Guthke, J. (1992). Learning tests – The concept, main research findings, problems,
and trends. Learning and Individual Differences, 4, 213-233.
Hamers, J. H. M., Sijtsma, K. & Ruijssenaars, A. J. J. M. (Eds.). (1993). Learning
potential assessment. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Haywood, H. C. & Tzuriel, D. (Eds.). (1992). Interactive assessment . New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class
structure in American life. New York: Free Press.
Hunt, J. McV. (1961). Intelligence and experience. New York: Ronald Press.
Jensen, A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement?
Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1-123.
Jitendra, A. K., & Kameenui, E. J. (1993). Dynamic assessment as a compensatory
assessment approach: A description and analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 14
(5), 6-18.
Lidz, C. S. (Ed). (1987). Dynamic assessment: An international approach to
evaluating learning potential. New York: Guilford.
Lidz, C. and Elliott, J. (Eds.) (2000). Dynamic assessment: Prevailing models and
applications. New York: Elsevier Science.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, H. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of
behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Osterreith, P. A. (1945). Copie d’une figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie,
205-353.
Perkins, D. N. (1995). Outsmarting IQ: The emerging science of learnable intelligence.
New York: Free Press.
Ramey, C. & MacPhee, D. (1981). A new paradigm in intellectual assessment?
Contemporary Psychology, 26(7), 507-509.
Raven, J. C. (1958). Standard progressive matrices, Sets A, B, C, D, F. London: H.
K. Lewis.
Raven, J. C. (1956). Coloured progressive matrices, Sets A, Ab and B. London: H. K.
Lewis.
Raven, J. C. (1947). Progressive matrices, Sets I and II. Dumfries: The Crichton
Royal.
Rey, A. (1959). Test de copie d’use figure complexe (Manual). Paris: Centre de
Psychologie Appliquée.
Sternberg, R. (1985a). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J., Ferrari, M., Clinkenbeard, P.R., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1996).
Identification, instruction, and assessment of gifted children: A construct validation of a
triarchic model. Gifted Child Quarterly, 40, 129-137.
Sternberg, R. and Grigorenko, E. (2002). Dynamic testing. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
processes. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.
Zazzo, R. (1964). Le test de deux barrages. Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestle.
Review Questions
1. List five differences between static and dynamic assessment
methods.
2. Which of the LPAD instruments evaluate the modifiability of
memory?
3. What is the LPAD profile?
4. Which are the criteria of structural cognitive change?
5. How group LPAD assessment is different from the individual?