What Is Design Thinking
What Is Design Thinking
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/23260048?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Review of Educational Research
September 2012, Vol. 82, No. 3, pp. 330-348
DOl: 10.3102/0034654312457429
330
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
the intellectual complexities and resources demanded to support good design edu
cation (Todd & Magleby, 2004). This awareness has resulted in the improvement
of existing courses to include industry-sponsored projects where companies pro
vide real problems along with real-world expertise (Bright, 1994; Dutson, Todd,
Magleby, & Sorensen, 1997).
Design thinking has also started to receive increased attention in business set
tings. This is because the design of products and services is a major component of
business competitiveness, to the extent that many known companies have commit
ted themselves to becoming design leaders (Dunne & Martin, 2006). And although
design thinking has become an integral part of the design and engineering fields as
well as business, it can also have a positive influence on 21st century education
across disciplines because it involves creative thinking in generating solutions for
problems. That is, in academic environments, students are required to read criti
cally, think and reason logically, and solve complex problems (Rotherham &
Willingham, 2009). Thus, to help students succeed in this interconnected, digital
world we live in, educators should support students in developing and honing 21st
century skills (e.g., design thinking, systems thinking, and teamwork skills) that
enhance their problem-solving skills and prepare them for college and career
(Rotherham & Willingham, 2009; Shute & Torres, 2012).
These skills are consistent with the theoretical traditions of situated cognition
(Lave & Wenger, 1991), developmental theories (Piaget, 1972), and constructiv
ism (Bruner, 1990). What's new is the growing extent to which individual and
collective success is seen as depending on having such skills. In addition to busi
ness settings, design thinking has received a lot of attention in engineering, archi
tecture, and design majors in universities because it can change how people learn
and solve problems (e.g., Dym, Agogino, Eris, Frey, & Leifer, 2005; Fricke, 1999;
Nagai & Nagouchi, 2003). The topic of expertise in design has also been receiving
increasing attention in design research. In support of these claims, consider the
large number of research articles published on the topic of design thinking (e.g.,
Do & Gross, 2001; Goldschmidt & Weil, 1998; Owen, 2007; Stempfle & Badke
Schaube, 2002; Tang & Gero, 2001). Among these research papers, there are stud
ies of expert or experienced designers and comparisons of the processes of novice
versus expert designers (e.g., Cross & Cross, 1998; Ericsson & Smith, 1991; Ho,
2001). Within this large body of design thinking research, experimental and quasi
experimental studies are lacking. Most, if not all of the studies are qualitative.
The dual aims of this article are to (a) summarize findings from the literature of
design thinking to gain better understanding of its characteristics, processes, and
differences between novice and expert design thinkers and (b) apply the findings
from the literature regarding design thinking to our educational system. Our over
arching goal is to identify the features and characteristics of design thinking and
show its importance in promoting students' problem-solving skills needed to succeed
in the 21st century. The major questions addressed in this review include (a) What
are the characteristics of design thinking, (b) what are the differences between a
novice and an expert design thinker, and (c) why is design thinking important?
331
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 1
Databases used in searching for articles
Database and Web sites Description
Method
Many articles in the design thinking literature were identified and then col
lected. Table 1 lists and describes the online databases and Web sites that were
employed in this search-collection effort. The focus of the search was to access
full-text documents using various search terms or keywords such as design think
ing, design cognition, design behavior, design studying, design reasoning, design
process, thinking of design, visual thinking, and prototyping. The search was not
limited to a particular date range or experimental studies. However, slight prefer
ence was given to more recent research. In all, approximately 150 documents were
collected. From this set, a total of more than 45 documents met the criteria for
inclusion in the literature review. The inclusion criteria consisted of topical rele
vancy of documents to the research questions in this article (e.g., design thinking
characteristics and processes, novice vs. expert design thinker, and the importance
of design thinking). Both experimental and nonexperimental studies were included
in this article.
332
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
Literature Review
Many authors have written about the nature of and different processes underly
ing the design thinking process (e.g., Liu, 1996; Owen, 2007; Stempfle & Badke
Schaube, 2002). We now present our review of the literature of this area, starting
with a description of the nature of design thinking, its characteristics, and pro
cesses. Next, we present literature regarding expertise, expert versus novice desig
thinkers, and expertise in design. We then present our design thinking model
adapted from Shute and Torres (2012). Finally, we discuss the findings from the
literature, showing the importance of design thinking and providing suggestion
for future research.
333
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Content:
Symbolic
> Symbolic VS.
Real
Analytic Synthetic
Process: Symbolic Symbolic
> Analytic VS.
Synthetic
Analytic
/ s Synthetic
( v
Design
Analytic Synthetic
Real Real
Real
334
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
drawn as informing further design efforts (Do & Gross, 2001; Lloyd & Scott,
1995). Designing often begins with a diagrammatic depiction that is gradually
transformed to more complex graphic representations by adding detail. These
design diagrams facilitate the designer's reflection, dialogue, and self-critique and
therefore serve the purpose of representing and testing the designer intent. In other
words, diagrams serve as a primary vehicle for thinking and solving problems (Do
& Gross, 2001; Nagai & Noguchi, 2003).
Braha and Reich (2003) viewed the design process as a generic process where
designers modify either the tentative or current design or the requirements and
specifications, based on new information that has become available. This ongoing
process of modification is performed in order to remove discrepancies and estab
lish a fit between the problem space, expressed through requirements and specifi
cations, and the proposed design solution.
In 2000, Suwa, Gero, and Purcell argued that designing is a situated act, which
means that designers invent design issues or requirements in a way that is situated
in the environment in which they design. The authors found a strong bidirectional
correlation between unexpected discoveries and the invention of issues and require
ments. Unexpected discoveries are those instances when a designer perceives
something new in a previously drawn element of a solution concept. Not only do
unexpected discoveries become the driving force for the invention of issues or
requirements, but also the occurrence of invention tends to cause new unexpected
discoveries. These results emphasize the importance of rapid alternation between
different modes of activity during the design process (e.g., drawing sketches and
conceiving of design issues or requirements that are dynamically related to one
another). This also explains the opportunistic nature of design activity, as the
designer pursues issues and requirements in an evolving solution concept.
According to Dorner (1999), several forms of thinking can be observed in
designing. Design starts as a cloudy idea about how the design/product should look
like and how it should work. With time, this idea crystallizes and transforms into
a clear and complete image of the product. The cloudy idea comes from something
that the designer already knows about the product. This knowledge can be a source
of analogies. The second form of thinking involves the sketches and models that
bring the cloudy idea to a more concrete form. Sketches and models clarify the
characteristics of the product, helping to form a specific line of thought that facil
itates the development process and forms the basis for the design thinking process.
The third form of design thinking is the "picture-word cycle," which involves
putting ideas into words that helps the designer clarify and elaborate on ideas.
However, whatever the form of thinking, the design thinker should demonstrate
specific characteristics in addition to creativity.
335
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
TABLE 2
Design-thinker characteristics
Characteristics Description
Human- and environment-centered Designers must continually consider how
concern what is being created will respond to hum
needs. They should also consider envi
ronmental interests at a level with human
interests as primary constraints for the design
process.
Ability to visualize Designers work visually (i.e., depiction of
ideas).
Predisposition toward Designers should look at different/multiple
multifunctionality solutions to a problem and keep the big pic
ture of the problem in mind while focusing
on its specifics.
Systemic vision Designers should treat problems as system
problems with opportunities for systemic
solutions involving different procedures and
concepts to create a holistic solution.
Ability to use language as a Designers
tool should be able to verbally explain
their creative process forcing invention
where detail is lacking and expressing rela
tionships not obvious visually (i.e., explana
tion should go hand in hand with the creative
process).
Affinity for teamwork Designers need to develop interpersonal skills
that allow them to communicate across disci
plines and work with other people.
Avoiding the necessity of choice
Designers search competing alternatives before
moving to choice making or decision mak
ing. They try to find ways to come up with
new configurations. This process leads to a
solution that avoids decision and combines
best possible choices.
336
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
thinking: (a) preparation, (b) assimilation, and (c) strategic control. In the prepara
tion process, designers need to learn what to focus on and what is relevant. During
this phase, the specifications and constraints of the problem, reinterpretation of
ideas, visualization, problem reformulation (including situation assessment and
elaboration), and others evolve. The assimilation process involves making sense
of the proposed solution, data, and observations coming from the design environ
ment, such as feedback from experiments with prototypes. In the strategic control
process, designers must make many decisions over the course of a design (e.g.,
which idea to elaborate or adapt next, which constraints to relax, how to set pri
orities). They also move among various tasks, subproblems, and design processes
in a flexible and highly opportunistic manner.
In 2002, Stempfle and Badke-Schaube examined a theory of what design teams
actually do while designing. They looked at theories of creativity and problem
solving and cognitive theories of human decision making. The basic elements of
design thinking that the authors proposed as cognitive operations to deal with any
kind of problem were generation, exploration, comparison, and selection. The first
two elements (generation and exploration) widen a problem space whereas the last
two (comparison and selection) narrow a problem space. When widening a prob
lem, solutions are generated and then examined in relation to the goal. Then, in an
iterative process, solutions may be modified or new solutions may be developed
until an optimal solution is found. Narrowing a problem entails comparing two or
more ideas and then selecting the solutions based on specific and relevant goal
criteria. These elements represent a model that can be applied to understand
designers' thinking while working in a team. Designers working in groups have to
communicate what they are thinking, thus showing their basic thinking processes.
The researchers applied this model to three mechanical engineering teams con
sisting of four to six students. The teams were assigned to design a mechanical
concept for an optical device to project images of celestial objects. The teams
interacted with a simulated customer at three fixed points in time during their one
day working period. Team communication was recorded. Results from protocol
analysis revealed that the teams spent only 10% of their time on clarifying the goal
and spent the remaining 90% of the time planning a solution.
The Stempfle and Badke-Schaube (2002) findings described differ from those
observed by McNeill, Gero, and Warren (1998) in electronics engineers. McNeill
and colleagues reported that across the whole design episode, the designers spent
most of their time analyzing the problem; synthesizing the solution took the second
greatest amount of time, and the remaining time was spent on the evaluation of the
solution. The authors concluded that a designer begins a conceptual design session
by analyzing the functional aspects of the problem. As the session progresses, the
designer focuses on the three aspects—function, behavior, and structure—and then
engages in a cycle of analysis, synthesis, and evaluation. Toward the end of the
design session, the designer's activity is focused on synthesizing structure and
evaluating the structure's behavior. Similarly, in a team of three industrial design
ers, Goldschmidt and Weil (1998) found that the process of design thinking is
nonlinear and that designers follow a forward (breaking down) and backward
(validating) reasoning strategy. Although research is not consistent about how time
is spent during the design thinking process, findings indicate that there is a learning
337
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Razzouk & Shute
progression during the design thinking process that eventually transforms a novice
into an expert design thinker.
Expertise
Expertise is the result of a dedicated application to a specific field of interest
(Cross, 2004). According to Ericsson, Krampe, and Tesch-Romer (1993), deliber
ate practice guided toward improvement of performance is necessary to reach high
levels of performance and the acquisition of expertise. Ericsson et al. added that
the achieved level of performance of an expert is closely related to the accumulated
amount of practice. Therefore, the development of expertise passes through differ
ent phases. Something happens in the development from being a novice to becom
ing an expert.
The major difference between experts and novices is that experts have accumu
lated a large number of examples of problems and solutions in a specific domain
of interest. A key competency of an expert is the ability to mentally stand back
from the specifics of the accumulated examples and form more abstract conceptu
alizations related to their domain of expertise (Akin & Akin, 1996; Ho, 2001).
Experts are believed to be able to store and access information in larger cognitive
chunks than novices can and to recognize underlying principles rather than focus
ing on the surface features of problems (Dorner, 1999; Nigel, 2004; Purcell &
Gero, 1996; Suwa et al., 2000). Therefore, the accumulation of experience is crit
ical in the transformation from a novice to an expert.
In many areas, like sports and music, the benefits of dedicated practice are well
known and there are established programs of training for novices to help them gain
experience and expertise over time (Cross, 2004). It may be beneficial in other
areas as well to focus on the transformational phases (i.e., novice through expert),
such as in design thinking. In design education, there are well-established practices
that are presumed to help the development from novice to expert, but there is still
little understanding of the differences between novice and expert performance in
design.
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
strategies. However, both expert and novice used similar bottom-up (working
backward) problem-solving strategies.
Christiaans and Dorst (1992) conducted protocol studies of junior and senior
college students in an industrial design course. They found that some students,
mostly the juniors, got trapped gathering information rather than progressing to
solution generation, but most of the senior students did not face this difficulty. That
is, senior design students did not gather as much information, but they were able
to solve the given problem. They asked for less information, processed it directly,
and built up an image of the problem. They also prioritized activities early in the
process.
A similar finding was reported by Gunther and Ehrlenspiel (1999), who con
ducted a set of experiments with a total of 20 novice and expert designers of
mechanical devices. The researchers found that experts were able to clarify a task
in a shorter time, whereas novices had to invest much more time in clarification.
These findings (i.e., Christiaans & Dorst, 1992; Gunther & Ehrlenspiel, 1999) cor
roborate findings from Atman, Chimka, Bursic, and Nachtman (1999), who con
ducted protocol analysis studies of engineering students. They found that novices
(i.e., freshmen with no design experience) spent a large portion of their time defin
ing the problem and did not produce high-quality designs. Therefore, and similar
to the industrial design students in the Christiaans and Dorst (1992) study, some of
the freshmen engineering students in the Atman et al. study were stuck at the level
of defining the problem, which hindered their progress in the design process.
However, senior students defined the problem adequately, which in turn resulted
in good designs.
Ahmed, Wallace, and Blessing (2003) studied differences between the behav
iors of novice and experienced designers in engineering. The authors found clear
differences between the behavior of new graduate entrants (i.e., novices) to
the engineering design profession and experienced designers. The novices used
trial-and-error techniques of generating and implementing a design modification,
evaluating it, and then generating another evaluation through several iterations.
Experienced engineers, however, made a preliminary evaluation of their tentative
design decisions before implementing them and making a final evaluation. In con
trast to the novices' trial-and-error approach, the experienced designers employed
integrated design strategies.
In 2001, Seitamaa-Hakkarainen and Hakkarainen investigated the relationships
between visual and technical designing using qualitative analysis. That is, they
examined differences between two novices and two experts in the field of weaving
design. Protocol analysis results revealed that the experts integrated the visual ele
ments (e.g., color, size, patterns) and technical elements (e.g., material) of weav
ing, and generally considered them in a parallel way during the design process.
Iteration between the visual and the technical space was a significant aspect of the
experts' design process. The experts continuously moved from one design space
to another to carry out very detailed processes of search for design solutions. In
contrast, the novices organized their process around the composition space and
rarely moved to the construction space to explore how visual ideas could be real
ized in weaving.
Similarly, using data from protocol studies, Kavakli and Gero (2002) compared
the cognitive performances/actions (i.e., looking, perceptual and functional
339
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Razzouk & Shute
actions, and goals) of a novice and an expert architect. Using protocol analysis, the
researchers investigated concurrent cognitive actions of designers and found sig
nificant differences in output between novice and expert designers. The protocol
was divided into segments. A cognitive segment consisted of cognitive actions that
appeared to occur simultaneously. They found that the design protocol of the
expert included 2,916 actions (i.e., chunks) and 348 segments, whereas the nov
ice's protocol included 1,027 actions and 122 segments. Each segment consisted
of 8 cognitive actions on average. Considering that the same amount of time was
given to both participants, the expert's design protocol was 2.8 times as rich as the
novice's in terms of actions. There were also 2.8 times as many segments in the
expert designer's session as in the novice's. Therefore, the expert had more overall
fluency in relation to divergent thinking skills. The expert's cognitive actions con
tinuously rose throughout the activity, while the novice's cognitive activity started
at a peak and then declined. The authors also found that the expert seemed to have
more control of his cognitive activity compared to the novice. Because the expert's
cognitive actions are well organized, he was able to govern his performance more
efficiently than the novice.
These findings align with those by Tang and Gero (2001), who found substan
tial differences between a novice and an expert architect. Using a retrospective
protocol analysis, the authors found differences between the novice and expert
designers in relation to four design levels: (a) the physical level, which refers to
the instances that have direct relevance to the external world, comprising drawing
and looking actions; (b) the perceptual level, which concerns the instances of
attending to visual-spatial features/relationships in an automatic perceptual mech
anism; (c) the functional level, which relates to the instances of functional refer
ences mapped between visual-spatial features/relationships and abstract concepts,
including meanings and functions; and (d) the conceptual level, which represents
the instances that process abstract concepts and the instances that process physical
and perceptual actions. The expert seemed to create more meaning at the physical
and perceptual levels than the novice.
Differences between novices and experts performing design-related problems
were also studied by Goker (1997). The author examined novices and experts on
a task involving computer-simulated construction of machines. Goker found that
the experts, skilled in the use of computer simulations, did not reason toward a
design concept in an abstract way, but relied more on their experience and visual
information. In contrast, novices depended more on abstract reasoning.
340
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
to the problem type approached the design task through solution assumptions/
conjectures instead of problem analysis. This hypothesis suggests that experience
in a specific problem type enables designers to perceive the design problem in
terms of relevant solutions that they have previously encountered.
Designers also tend to change goals and constraints as they design. They are
flexible in selecting and trying different solutions. However, when designers face
unexpected difficulties and/or shortcomings in the solution concept, they tend to
stick to their principal solution concept as long as possible through the design
process. For instance, from case studies of professional architectural designers,
Rowe (1987) observed that the designers' choices for problem-solving directions
were influenced by their initial design ideas. Furthermore, the designers made
every effort to make these ideas work whenever a problem was encountered, rather
than adopting a new idea.
And although this fixedness proclivity may sound maladaptive, Ullman,
Dietterich, and Stauffer (1988) observed the same phenomenon in their protocol
studies of experienced mechanical engineering designers. Ullman and colleagues
found that experienced designers typically pursued only one design proposal. And
even when major problems had been identified, the designers preferred to modify
the initial proposal rather than rejecting it and developing a new one. Likewise,
Ball, Evans, and Dennis (1994) drew a similar conclusion from their studies of
senior electronic engineers conducting real-world projects. The researchers stated
that when the designers generated a less than satisfactory solution, they refused to
discard the original solution or spend time and effort coming up with an alternative
one. Rather, they tended to improve the solution by developing different versions
until a workable solution was achieved. Again, the designers indicated a fixation
behavior on initial concepts (Ball et al., 1994). Nonetheless, adherence to initial
concepts seems to comprise normal expert design behavior. Finally, in a study of
experienced software designers, Guindon (1990) also found that designers came
to a solution very early in the session and quickly rejected alternative solutions.
Since a problem cannot be fully understood in isolation, expert designers use
conjectures as a means of helping them to explore and understand the formulation
of the problem. From protocol studies of experienced industrial designers, Dorst
and Cross (2001) asserted that the designers start by exploring the problem and
find, discover, or recognize a partial structure. Afterwards, they use this partial
structure to generate initial ideas for the form of a design concept, then expand and
develop the partial structure. Thus, their goal is to create a matching solution to the
problem. Having more than one solution concept should stimulate a more compre
hensive evaluation and understanding of the problem (Cross, 2004). From the
analysis and synthesis of the literature, it appears that there are a number of com
petencies that designers should acquire and hone. The more experience a designer
builds in these competencies, the more he or she advances along the novice-expert
continuum.
341
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
FIGURE 2. The design thinking competency model.
Note. Adapted from "Where Streams Converge: Using Evidence-Centered Design to Assess Quest to Learn." In
M. Mayrath, J. Clarke-Midura, & D.H. Robinson (Eds.), Technology-Based Assessments for 21st Century
Skills: Theoretical and Practical Implications from Modern Research (pp. 91-124). Charlotte, NC: Information
Age Publishing.
operationalization of the design thinking construct and may also help drive the crea
tion of appropriate activities that would allow for the collection of relevant evidence
to inform variables in the model. For example, consider the variable "Iterate
Diagrams" in Figure 2. Skills associated with this variable include tinkering, creat
ing, and testing ideas via diagrams. Testing, in turn, entails initial testing of the
design idea, getting feedback, modifying the design, reevaluating it, and making a
decision to accept or reject the modeled idea. To assess students' competency levels
relative to the iterate diagrams variable, we would have to put them in a situation in
which those constituent skills could be employed, such as in a game or simulation.
Diagnostically, the model could provide the framework for evaluating the degree to
which students are demonstrating particular design thinking skills at various times
and at various grain sizes relative to the model (for more, see Shute & Torres, 2012).
342
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
The design thinking competency model is useful for assessment and diagnostic
purposes. That is, once the key knowledge and skills have been identified, then
tasks and activities can be developed in line with the model's variables. Another
relevant question concerns whether these skills are learnable. With sufficient prac
tice within meaningful environments, along with scaffolded support and formative
feedback, we believe that students can learn design thinking skills. Moreover,
pedagogical approaches that involve problem-based learning, project-based learn
ing, and inquiry-based learning can be used to enhance students' design thinking
skills within the context of evocative and consequential classroom activities (Dym
et al., 2005).
Such learner-centered approaches can help to raise students' awareness about
good design processes and generally enhance their interest in solving complex
problems. Associated activities could be designed in a way that requires students
to generate ideas/solutions, receive support for their emergent design thinking
skills, as well as ongoing feedback about the feasibility of various solutions.
Educators can support their students in developing these skills by providing them
with multiple and varied opportunities to design and create prototypes, experiment
with different ideas, collaborate with others, reflect on their learning, and repeat
the cycle while revising and improving each time.
In summary, the premise is that by improving students' design thinking skills
through having them apply processes and methods that designers use to ideate and
help them experience how designers approach problems to try to solve them, stu
dents will be more ready to face problems, think outside of the box, and come up
with innovative solutions. We believe that design thinking is more than just a skill
to be acquired and used in limited contexts. Rather, we view it as a way of thinking
and being that can potentially enhance the epistemological and ontological nature
of schooling.
343
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Razzouk & Shute
focus and consider new educationally valuable skills (e.g., design thinking, multi
tasking, digital literacy) to value, assess, and support.
As described earlier, enhancing students' design thinking skills may be achieved
through incorporating authentic and intriguing tasks into the classroom and pro
viding many opportunities to apply design processes. In our design thinking model
shown in Figure 2, imagine tasks that are designed and developed for each of the
low-level nodes. As students work on the tasks, evidence is accumulated to evalu
ate their performance. Such information can help educators monitor the student's
performance, infer current states of strength and weakness relative to design think
ing variables, and provide targeted feedback to improve the student's performance.
Our goal as educators should not focus on preparing our students to perform well
on standardized exams, but to equip them with powerful skill sets that can help
them succeed both within and outside of school.
This article presented relevant research that has provided the basis for under
standing (a) the nature of design thinking, (b) experts' behavior in design, and (c)
differences between novice and expert designers. Most of these studies were qual
itative and employed protocol analysis, which has some limitations as a research
method, especially for investigating design activities. For example, it can be a
weak method when researchers aim to capture designers' nonverbal thought pro
cesses, which are critical in design thinking. The majority of the studies we
reviewed aimed to examine either the differences between novice and expert
designers or characterize expert behavior in the designing process. However,
experimental evidence is lacking in the field of design research.
Researchers who are interested in measuring and supporting design thinking
have great opportunities to conduct a wide range of experimental studies that can
lead to important findings. For instance, researchers may examine the effects of
the design thinking process on various learning outcomes. They can also investi
gate the effects of different tasks and their complexity relative to enhancing design
thinking skills, which in turn are assumed to increase students' learning outcomes.
It would also be interesting to know if design thinking skills mediate the learning
process. In other words, design thinking skill may serve as a mediator that clarifies
the nature of the relationship between an independent variable (e.g., problem
solving skill) and a dependent variable (e.g., math test scores). So, rather than
hypothesizing a direct causal relationship between problem-solving skill and math
test scores, we may hypothesize that problem-solving skill enhances design think
ing skill, which in turn leads to an increase in math scores. Another important study
could examine the domain-specific versus domain-independent nature of design
thinking. In other words, can design thinking skill be examined independently of
particular domains (e.g., engineering vs. marketing), or is it context bound?
Currently, we have found no valid performance-based assessments of design
thinking skills. This lack adversely affects the ability to collect good evidence
about the effects of these skills on learning (Rotherham & Willingham, 2009). A
major challenge, then, is to design and develop accurate, performance-based mea
sures of these skills. Assessing these types of 21 st-century competencies is beyond
the capabilities of most traditional assessment formats (e.g., multiple-choice test,
self-report survey). Therefore, innovative assessments that aim to reliably measure
those skills should be designed and developed to assist researchers in collecting
valid and reliable evidence. We suggest employing the evidence centered design
344
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
(ECD) framework (Mislevy, Steinberg, & Almond, 2003) for designing valid per
formance-based assessments for 21 st-century skills. ECD is a systematic approach
to the design of assessments that focuses on the evidence (i.e., student performance
and products) of proficiencies as the basis for constructing assessment tasks and
making inferences about competency levels (for more, see Mislevy et al., 2003).
ECD is especially suited for assessments that involve complex problems and
dynamic, interactive environments—which are exactly the kinds of contexts
required for design problems.
There is considerable empirical work to be done to establish a full understand
ing of design thinking. The studies surveyed in this article show the characteristics
of novice and expert designers. Having good design thinking skills can assist in
solving really complex problems as well as adjusting to unexpected changes.
Although the design process involves in-depth cognitive processes—which may
help our students build their critical thinking skills (e.g., reasoning and analysis)—
it also involves personality and dispositional traits such as persistence and creativ
ity. If we are serious about preparing students to succeed in the world, we should
not require that they memorize facts and repeat them on demand; rather, we should
provide them with opportunities to interact with content, think critically about it,
and use it to create new information. Preparation for future work situations requires
teaching learners to use their minds well. To turn the tide in education that is leav
ing students "ill-prepared to tackle real-world, complex problems [we must change
our course]... we cannot directly adjust the wind (the future), but we can adjust
References
Ahmed, S., Wallace, K. M., & Blessing, L. T. M. (2003). Understanding the differences
between how novice and experienced designers approach design tasks. Research in
Engineering Design, 14, 1-11.
Akin, O., & Akin, C. (1996). Frames of reference in architectural design: Analyzing
the hyper-acclamation (aha!). Design Studies, 17, 341-361. doi: 10.1016/S0142
694X(96)00024-5
Atman, C. J., Chimka, J. R., Bursic, K. M., &Nachtmann, H. L. (1999). A comparison
of freshman and senior engineering design processes. Design Studies, 20, 131-152.
doi: 10.1016/S0142-694X(98)00031 -3
Badke-Schaub, P. (1999). Analysis of design projects. Design Studies, 20, 465-480.
doi: 10.1016/S0142-694X(99)00017-4
Ball, L. J., Evans, J., & Dennis, I. (1994). Cognitive processes in engineering design:
A longitudinal study. Ergonomics, 37,1753-1786. doi: 10.1080/00140139408964950
Braha, D., & Maimon, O. (1997). The design process: Properties, paradigms, and struc
ture. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics-Part A: Systems and
Humans, 27, 146-166. doi:10.1109/3468.554679
Braha, D., & Reich, Y. (2003). Topological structures for modeling engineering design
processes. Research in Engineering Design, 14,185-199. doi: 10.1007/s00163-003
0035-3
Bright, A. (1994). Teaching and learning in the engineering clinic program at Harvey
Mudd College. Journal of Engineering Education, 83, 113-116.
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Christiaans, H., & Dorst, C. (1992). Cognitive models in industrial design engineering:
A protocol study. In D. Taylor & D. Stauer (Eds), Design theory and methodology—
DTM92. New York, NY: American Society of Mechanical Engineers.
345
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Razzouk & Shute
346
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Design Thinking and Its Importance
Kavakli, M., & Gero, J. (2002). The structure of concurrent cognitive actions: A case
study on novice and expert designers. Design Studies, 23, 25-40. doi: 10.1016/
S0142-694X(01 )00021 -7
Kolodner, J., & Wills, L. (1996). Power of observation in creative design. Design
Studies, 17, 385—416. doi:10.1016/S0142-694X(96)00021-X
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning. Legitimate peripheral participation.
Cambridge, MA: University of Cambridge Press.
Liu, Y-T. (1996). Is designing one search or two? A model of design thinking involving
symbolism and connectionism. Design Studies, 17, 435-449. doi: 10.1016/SO142
694X(96)00018-X
Lloyd, P., & Scott, P. (1994). Discovering the design problem. Design Studies, 15,
125-140. doi: 10.1016/0142-694X(94)90020-5
Lloyd, P., & Scott, P. (1995). Difference in similarity: Interpreting the architectural
design process. Planning and Design, 22, 383-406. doi:10.1068/b220383
McNeill, T., Gero, J., & Warren, J. (1998). Understanding conceptual electronic design
using protocol analysis. Research in Engineering Design, 10,129-140. doi: 10.1007/
BF01607155
Mislevy, R. J., Steinberg, L. S., & Almond, R. G. (2003). On the structure of educa
tional assessments. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives, 1,
3-62. doi: 10.1207/S15366359MEA0101 _02
Nagai, Y., & Noguchi, H. (2003). An experimental study on the design thinking process
started from difficult keywords: Modeling the thinking process of creative design.
Journal of Engineering Design, 14,429-437. doi: 10.1080/09544820310001606911
Nigel, C. (2004). Expertise in design: An overview. Design Studies, 25, 427-441.
doi: 10.1016/j .destud.2004.06.002
Owen, C. (2007). Design thinking: Notes on its nature and use. Design Research
Quarterly, 2, 16-27.
Piaget, J. (1972). The psychology of the child. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Purcell, T., & Gero, J. (1996). Design and other types of fixation. Design Studies, 17,
363-383. doi: 10.1016/SO 142-694X(96)00023-3
Rotherham, A. J., & Willingham, D. (2009). To work, the 21 st century skills movement
will require keen attention to curriculum, teacher quality, and assessment.
Educational Leadership, 9, 15-20.
Rowe, P. (1987). Design thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Seitamaa-Hakkarainen, P., & Hakkarainen, K. (2001). Composition and construction
in experts' and novices' weaving design. Design Studies, 22, 47-66. doi: 10.1016/
S0142-694X(99)00038-1
Shute, V. J., & Becker, B. J. (2010). Innovative assessment for the 21st century. New
York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Shute, V. J., & Torres, R. (2012). Where streams converge: Using evidence-centered
design to assess Quest to Learn. In M. Mayrath, J. Clarke-Midura, & D. H. Robinson
(Eds.), Technology-based assessments for 21st century skills: Theoretical and
practical implications from modern research (pp. 91-124). Charlotte, NC:
Information Age Publishing.
Simon, H. A. (1996). The sciences of the artificial (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Stempfle, J., & Badke-Schaube, P. (2002). Thinking in design teams—an analysis of
team communication. Design Studies, 23, 473^196. doi: 10.1016/S0142
694X(02)00004-2
347
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Razzouk & Shute
Suwa, M., Gero, J., & Purcell, T. (2000). Unexpected discoveries and s-invention of
design requirements: Important vehicles for a design process. Design Studies, 21,
539-567. doi: 10.1016/S0142-694X(99)00034-4
Tang, H-H., & Gero, J. S. (2001). Sketches as affordances of meanings in the design
process. In J. S. Gero, B. Tversky, & T. Purcell (Eds.), Visual and spatial reasoning
in design II (pp. 271-282). Sydeny: University of Sydney, Australia: Key Center of
Design Computing and Cognition.
Todd, R., & Magleby, S. (2004). Evaluation and rewards for faculty involved in engi
neering design education. International Journal of Engineering, 20, 333-340.
Ullman, D., Dietterich, T., & Stauffer, L. (1988). A model of the mechanical design
process based on empirical data. Artificial Intelligence in Engineering Design and
Manufacturing, 2, 33-52.
Weth, R., Von der. (1999). Design instinct? The development of individual strategies.
Design Studies, 20,453^163. doi:10.1016/S0142-694X(99)00021-6
Authors
RIM RAZZOUK completed her PhD in instructional systems at the Florida State University
(FSU), Tallahassee, Florida. She also pursued her master's in instructional systems and
obtained a certificate in human performance technology from FSU. Her research interests
focus on the application and evaluation of different learner-centered methods and strate
gies, integration of mobile technology in education, and development of team-shared
mental models. Currently, Rim is working as the director of measurement and assessment
at Edvation in Seattle, WA; e-mail: [email protected].
VALERIE SHUTE is a professor at Florida State University. Before coming to FSU in 2007,
she was a principal research scientist at Educational Testing Service (2001-2007) where
she was involved with basic and applied research projects related to assessment, cogni
tive diagnosis, and learning from advanced instructional systems. Her general research
interests hover around the design, development, and evaluation of advanced systems to
support learning, particularly related to twenty-first-century competencies. Her current
research involves using immersive games with stealth assessment to support learning of
cognitive and noncognitive knowledge and skills.
348
This content downloaded from 103.37.201.67 on Wed, 16 Jan 2019 12:38:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms