Self Psychology and Diagnostic Assessment - Identifying Selfobject Functions Through Psychological Testing PDF
Self Psychology and Diagnostic Assessment - Identifying Selfobject Functions Through Psychological Testing PDF
AND DIAGNOSTIC
ASSESSMENT
♦♦♦
IDENTIFYING SELFOBJECT
FUNCTIONS THROUGH
PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING
This page intentionally left blank
SELF PSYCHOLOGY
AND DIAGNOSTIC
ASSESSMENT
♦♦♦
IDENTIFYING SELFOBJECT
FUNCTIONS THROUGH
PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING
Marshall L. Silverstein
Long Island University
| J Routledge
cj Taylor &. Francis Group
www.routledgementalhealth.com
Copyright © 1999 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in
any form, by photostat, microfilm, retrieval system, or any other
means, without prior written permission of the publisher.
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
For my devoted parents,
Jack
(1891-1963 )
and Gertrude
(1915-1998 )
who, despite all odds,
provided the kernel fo r a resilient self
and fo r Marian Tolpin, M.D.,
who fortified it
This page intentionally left blank
Contents
Preface ix
References 281
vii
This page intentionally left blank
Preface
ix
x PREFACE
(Kohut, 1959, 1966, 1971), but his broadened and expanded views (1977,
1984) about the self and its disorders extended well beyond narcissism. The
inherent confusion of this situation makes the discussion of self psychology
unpopular because it can seem conceptually unclear despite being clinically
palpable and vivid.
My second goal, using content analysis of projective test findings to
characterize self states, is also difficult. Phenomenologic data are always
vague and do not lead to clearly agreed-on interpretations of clinical
meaning. The analysis of test content and sequence analysis is unpopular
at a time that psychologists rightfully expect and can achieve psychometric
sophistication. Some clinicians may find the study of content distasteful
because the results of this approach are unverifiable and therefore
logically unsupportable and may even appear scientifically regressive.
These practitioners of psychodiagnostic testing are satisfied with nothing
less than valid and reliable empirical scores and should expect nothing less.
Although content analysis cannot provide this degree of rigor, it is valuable
in a different way if clinicians follow the highest standards of logical inquiry
and practice careful, clinical inferential thinking.
I intend to combine the rich insights of self psychology with an honest
attempt to approach content analysis with restraint and logical rigor. I
strive to make the best use of suboptimal clinical material to reveal deep
levels of the painfully experienced affect states that Kohut’s psychology
of the self has managed to explain. My discussion of psychoanalytic self
psychology does not disappoint in its complete and often painstaking
accuracy about central concepts, including transmuting internalization,
compensatory structure, empathic understanding, and, of course, the
crucially significant selfobject functions. This book is not an “easy
read”; it sacrifices nothing of self psychology’s complexity and nuances.
Thus, the book is not everyone’s “cup of tea”; certainly it is not for the
faint of heart.
One way of thinking about the convergence between the psychology of the
self and projective testing is by asking: What do patients convey through
projective test content about the self state, about its vitality or weakness
and its attempts to recover from injury? How do people use Rorschach
xii PREFACE
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I have chosen to discuss only the classical version of self psychology
associated with Heinz Kohut. This version is both sufficiently encompass
ing and difficult to comprehend that a discussion of other outgrowths of self
psychology (such as intersubjectivity) only diffuses my primary objective.
Although I never knew or studied with Kohut, his views have substantially
infused my own clinical practice and my thinking for more than 25 years.
In preparing this book, I have benefitted from the helpful comments of
many colleagues, most of whom are at the Chicago Institute for Psycho
analysis, Michael Reese Medical Center, and Long Island University. The
preparation of this book was facilitated by a sabbatical leave from Long
Island University and by a grant from the C. W. Post Research Committee
of Long Island University. Irving Weiner, Susan Milmoe, Nadine Simms,
and the production staff of Lawrence Erlbaum Associates provided steady
guidance and clarity during the production stage. I am particularly grateful
to Joanne Marengo, Michael Simon, and Marian Tolpin, for reading several
drafts of the manuscript and for their enduring support of this effort.
I trust that my thinking about psychological testing will enrich the
ongoing study of the psychology of the self, and I also believe that the
contributions of self psychology will foster a deepening understanding of
personality as revealed through psychodiagnostic testing.
Parti
Psychoanalytic Self Psychology
This page intentionally left blank
1 The Shift From Classical Drive
Theory To Self Psychology
In the introduction to the final installment of The Search fo r the Self \ the
definitive collection of Heinz Kohut’s papers and letters, Omstein (1990)
succinctly captured one of Kohut’s principal contributions. He wrote: “Ko-
hut maintained from early on that there were areas of human experience that
could not be adequately explored with the aid of drive psychology and ego
psychology and considered this fact as one of the most compelling reasons
for introducing self psychology” (p. 9).
In this statement, Omstein conveyed that self psychology had evolved to
become a theory in its own right, one that was more than an expansion or
development of classical drive theory. Nevertheless, it is difficult to think of
another conceptual system in psychoanalysis that is as incompletely under
stood as is the psychology of the self. Nevertheless, self psychology has
achieved a position of importance despite criticisms of its most central
features. This chapter and the two chapters that follow outline the progres
sion of self psychology.
In the present chapter, I describe the development of Kohut’s thinking,
beginning with his initial formulations about narcissism and the clinical
phenomena that gave rise to his ideas about the selfobject functions of
mirroring, idealization, and twinship. In Chapter 2 ,1 discuss Kohut’s formu
lations of the crucial concepts of empathy, compensatory structure, and
transmuting internalization. Chapter 2 also includes Kohut’s reformulated
understanding of dreams and the oedipal situation and his attempt to define
the self and its properties as a mental structure. Finally, the third and final
chapter on the central principles of self psychology is entirely devoted to a
comprehensive description of the selfobject functions, which represent the
conceptual anchor for the self psychological approach to diagnostic psycho
logical testing, the chief focus of the remainder of this book.
THEORETICAL ORIGINS OF SELF PSYCHOLOGY
In his initial formulation of narcissism, a formulation that he subsequently
developed into an expanded psychology of the self by 1977, Kohut (1971)
3
4 CHAPTER 1
viewed his ideas as an extension of the predominant drive (id) and ego
psychology of the day. This view was particularly prominent in his earliest
writings on narcissism, beginning in 1966 and culminating in The Analysis
o f the Self (1911). The 1971 book was Kohut’s first comprehensive state
ment about the analytic understanding and treatment of narcissistic person
ality and behavior disorders although Kohut’s important 1959 paper on the
empathic-introspective method of obtaining analytic data and on their
clinical understanding foreshadowed the book.
In several respects, the impetus for Kohut’s views came from shortcom
ings in classical analytic treatment, which Kohut believed was limited in
its ability to reach major areas of patients’ distress and internal experience.
(Ornstein’s [1990] statement, quoted previously, addressed exactly this
point.) Kohut thus discovered that an enfeebled self needed to be acknow
ledged or responded to in a way that was more accepting of people’s
discomfort and that did not leave patients feeling undermined by analytic
treatment, including analysts’ interpretations that patients perceived as
assaults or criticisms.
Kohut (1977) believed that although clinical improvement occurred, at
least from the patients’ viewpoint, many people often felt unfulfilled or
dissatisfied with their lives so that some aspects of the experience of the
self were left essentially untouched. His well-known paper The Two
Analyses of Mr. Z (1979) is an excellent illustration of exactly this problem.
In this paper, Kohut described in detail the outcome of an analysis, con
ducted in a traditional manner, followed 5 years later by a second period
of analysis informed by a self psychological viewpoint. Despite an other
wise successful treatment that produced substantial amelioration of symp
toms, Kohut maintained that the first analysis could not have produced the
results that the second analysis had.
Kohut’s (1971) early thinking about the self emphasized clinical
description of narcissistic personality disorders and the ways that psy
choanalysts treated these conditions. The description of the transferences
that emerged in treating such patients became the centerpiece of Kohut’s
attempt to understand this form of psychopathology. His theorizing
extended the idea of separate lines of development from classical drive
theory to include a developmental line for the self based on his new view
of narcissism. Kohut saw his views as a natural extension of drive theory,
an “adjacent territory” as Ornstein (1978, p. 98) put it, rather than a
variant of the object relations schools.
SHIFT FROM DRIVE THEORY TO SELF PSYCHOLOGY 5
The more one knows, the more you have a grasp of the totality of the life
histories and of the basic disturbance in the personality, the more you will
be able, with a variety of configurations in your mind, to watch and see
and find out what area the pathology finally falls into. All these variants do
occur. If I have contributed something to analysis, it is not just to have
replaced one conceptual thing with another... .Such individuals do some
times secondarily retreat from these oedipal positions into narcissistic
vulnerability. That is perfectly true. But the secondary narcissistic vulner
abilities or the secondary oral dependency attitudes you see in some
individuals are a defense against the deeper going, more deeply situated
anxieties of the oedipal period. (Kohut, 1996, pp. 118-119)
the side of the dangerous drives and has to be defended against. The
patient’s rage is “narcissistic rage.”
... Concretely speaking, whenever a patient reacts with rage to the
analyst’s interpretations, he has experienced him, from the point of view
of the archaic self that has been activated in analysis, as a nonempathic
attacker of the integrity of the self. The analyst does not witness the
emergence of a primary primitive-aggressive drive, he witnesses the
disintegration of the preceding primary configuration, the breakup of the
primary self-experience in which, in the child’s perception, the child and
the empathic self-object are one. (Kohut, 1977, pp. 90, 91)
SELF PSYCHOLOGY:
EARLY CLINICAL CONSIDERATIONS
realized that the principal reason for treatment failure resulted from misin
terpreting mirroring by using a conceptual framework based on regressions
from oedipal conflicts and libidinal and aggressive drives. He discovered
that a revival of mirroring in treatment fulfilled the usual criteria for a
genuine transference: inappropriateness to the ongoing work, its function
as a resistance, and its genetic basis. Accordingly, Kohut believed that
mirroring represented a transference reaction that could be analyzed with
out modifying standard technique. The meaning of the transference reac
tion, of course, had to be broadened to include a mirroring need rather than
what was formerly considered a transference revival of libidinal or aggres
sive wishes.
From clinical material such as his work with Miss F. and his observa
tions in other analyses, Kohut formulated the concept of the selfobject
function. The transference represented the way that selfobject functions
appear clinically; thus, he proposed the term self object transference. An
analyst or psychotherapist had to perform a particular function for the
patient’s weakened self that could not carry out this function. The selfobject
function is first and foremost a mental representation of what the under
mined self requires to restore optimal functioning. This function is per
ceived and responded to as a needed or vital extension of the patient. One
important feature of the selfobject, therefore, is its restorative nature: It
exists for repairing devalued or injured self-esteem. The selfobject can refer
to the person who serves or fulfills the function, and it is usually experienced
in reference to another person. The person who provides the needed
self-reparative function is not the selfobject, however, this person embodies
the selfobject function. People do not experience the selfobject as having
its own center of initiative with its own wishes and needs and they do not
react to it as an object of love or hate.
A selfobject transference may be detected in experiences such as an
analyst’s or therapist’s sense that there is only one person in the room. In
this experience, a therapist’s presence is felt to be mostly incidental to a
vulnerable patient’s need for someone to be there listening. The therapist is
not experienced primarily as a person with his or her own needs; he or she
does not really matter much beyond affirming some vitally needed function
to buoy up or vitalize the patient who experiences vulnerable self-cohesion.
The patient is not, however, oblivious to the analyst’s existence. Indeed,
quite the reverse: Intense affects often surround the therapist’s lapses or
absence. The need is clearly present, but it is not reciprocal; it goes in just
one direction. It is similar to casual or everyday experiences in which two
SHIFT FROM DRIVE THEORY TO SELF PSYCHOLOGY 11
people converse, and one person feels incidental or irrelevant beyond the
other’s need for a responsive, listening presence. The interaction is one
sided and unreciprocated: One person exists not in his or her own right, but
only insofar as the other person needs him or her.
If this description resembles the conventional definition of selfishness
or feeling “used,” it is merely coincidental. Selfobject functions rarely
represent self-preoccupation or self-aggrandizement. The appearance of
selfishness or neglect of another’s needs or feelings may accompany but
does not define what is fundamentally a need for the other person to shore
up a deeply felt disturbance of self-esteem or self-cohesion.
As conceptualized metapsychologically, selfobject transferences are dif
ferentiated from classical transference neuroses in that the selfobject is not
cathected with object libido. Kohut (1971) regarded narcissism as a separate
line of development in contrast to that of object love. It should be remem
bered that Kohut’s (1971) original work emphasized selfobject functions to
explain narcissistic pathology. With the extension of his discovery of the
narcissistic transferences to encompass the structural neuroses as well,
Kohut (1977, 1984) subsequently preferred the concept of selfobject trans
ferences or functions over that of narcissistic transferences.
It is important to avoid confusing the concept of narcissistic transfer
ences or selfobject functions with egocentricity or with the Egocentricity
Index from the Comprehensive System for the Rorschach (Exner, 1993).
The meaning of self-focusing or self-preoccupation for Exner’s index is
neutral with respect to selfobject needs or functions, in the same way that
it is neutral for general psychological traits of selfishness or grandiosity.
Idealization
The second major selfobject function that Kohut identified was the ideal
izing transference. In an early case, Kohut (1968, 1971) reported the treat
ment of a patient, Mr. A., who turned repeatedly to older men or senior male
colleagues for signs of approval and praise for his work. This praise was
necessary for Mr. A. to feel capable and whole, and his search for approval
was re-enacted in the transference.
Although the patient sought treatment because of disturbing homosexual
preoccupations, these were thought of as secondary. Fundamentally, the
patient turned to men for a feeling of invigorated self-esteem. This behavior
had apparently met with welcoming acceptance by Mr. A.’s father, and it
was revived in the transference as well. Mr. A. had however experienced
12 CHAPTER 1
CLASSICAL TRANSFERENCES
AND SELFOBJECT TRANSFERENCES
in treatment, although he did not presume that one selfobject function was
more primitive or archaic than another.
Kohut developed his formulations of self psychology by attempting to
analyze patients who were characteristically difficult to treat, but he came
to believe that his theoretical views could be applied just as readily to
treating the usual patients who sought analysis or psychotherapy. According
to this point of view, therapists could also clinically understand patients
with a structural neurosis and could treat them according to the principal
concepts of self psychology. Therefore, Kohut’s formulations about selfob
ject functions could be applied to equal or superior advantage in patients
with no prominent developmental arrests or propensity for regression and
with the capacity for object love.
The cohesiveness of the self is the crucial clinical problem in the
differential diagnosis of narcissistic disorders (currently usually referred to
as self disorders) and structural (oedipal) neuroses or borderline disorders.
The issue of evaluating the robustness (cohesion) of the self as a mental
structure entails more than simply distinguishing prominent clinical char
acteristics such as diminished enthusiasm or zest (Goldberg, 1978). Self
cohesion is judged or assessed by the stability of the self in the face of
regression. Clinical manifestations are important considerations in this
determination, including the degree of devitalization or emptiness, the
presence of perverse activities and addictions, and the severity of transient
fragmentation states (M. Tolpin, 1978). It is the evaluation of cohesiveness
that remains central.
For example, dependency in the transference can mean something other
than oral longings or fear of object loss. It can represent reliance on a
selfobject to sustain self-cohesion. The patients whom Kohut originally
studied and treated, those with narcissistic personality disorders and nar
cissistic behavior disorders, were markedly dependent on the analyst’s
responsiveness or presence despite high levels of functioning. Such patients
appeared independent in many external aspects of their lives, however. A
clinging quality to their dependency was either absent or not prominent.
These patients were prone to react with rage, subsyndromal depression, or
sometimes haughty or cold withdrawal occasioned by even minor em-
pathic failures. Their sensitivity to feeling slighted extended to interrup
tions of appointment or vacation schedules as well as to the therapist’s
imperfect memory for details about the patient’s life. Kohut saw these
patients as different in kind from those with dependency-passivity as
conceptualized in drive theory or ego psychology.
SHIFT FROM DRIVE THEORY TO SELF PSYCHOLOGY 15
Differential Diagnosis
The clinical appearance of severe self disorders can be indistinguishable
from that of borderline disorders or psychotic presentations. Kohut’s deci
sive test was observing a patient’s reaction to the therapist’s preliminary or
trial interpretations of defenses (Goldberg, 1978). Either the emergence of
oedipal material, for example, or self-protective measures to rescue an
injured self from fragmentation or regression normally decided the issue.
Verbalizations are diagnostically noncontributory; a self disorder can
readily be mistaken on the surface for what appears to be a phallic-oedipal
conflict when a therapist assumes one-to-one correspondence between
verbal contents and specific meanings. On interview, a therapist can catalog
descriptions of symptoms much as Kemberg (1975) did for borderline
pathology. Kohut (1971), however, preferred to wait for the decisive trans
ference configuration to emerge clearly and thus to minimize unreliable or
premature diagnostic closure. The same caution applies to interpreting
surface contents of projective test responses, particularly when the inquiry
was too conservative or insufficient. For example, on the Rorschach, a
percept of blood sometimes represents rage, sometimes guilt, and at other
times devitalization of the self. Only a larger context extending beyond the
level of specific or discrete responses ultimately settles the issue.
Goldberg (1978) also cautioned against premature decisions about trans
ference interpretations because a patient can perceive incorrect appraisals
of the nature of the transference made too early as narcissistic injuries. The
patient may then conceal the true selfobject transference. This reaction
intensifies defenses iatrogenically and sometimes gives rise to sexualiza-
tions or other extemalizations outside the treatment proper. The early
appearance of a mirroring transference can also quickly give way to a
predominant idealizing transference—another reason to view early trans
ference manifestations as tentative or preliminary. Therapists must wait for
the central transference pattern to become established and should offer
minimal interference.
This point is also pertinent in reference to the use of diagnostic psycho
logical testing to identify selfobject functions. Because several selfobject
needs can often appear, examiners must consider the total protocol to
determine the predominant pattern without forgetting that multiple selfob
ject functions are frequently present in many patients. For example, a
helmet percept on the Rorschach can suggest a vigorous self seeking
admiring, mirroring selfobject responsiveness. Alternatively, the helmet
16 CHAPTER 1
20
SELF PSYCHOLOGY: MAJOR CONCEPTS 21
mental system whose clinical relevance has had greater appeal than has its
metapsychological precision (Gedo & Goldberg, 1973).
Although Kohut did not see a therapeutic need for undue warmth,
feigned interest, or sympathy, he did not intend treating therapists to act
coldly. Indeed, few clinicians disagree that people in treatment do not
24 CHAPTER 2
[0]n the whole, self psychologists tend to work in a more relaxed fashion,
are more easygoing with their patients, have fewer misgivings about
making themselves emotionally available to their patients if the need
arises, and generally behave in a (comparatively speaking) less reserved
manner than the majority of analysts. (Kohut, 1984, p. 81)
Kohut understood that patients also improve when therapists use tech
niques that do not emphasize empathic listening or understanding. Empathy,
as described by Kohut and his colleagues, simply refers to therapists’ listening
to the communications of patients in a particular way. Empathy is a way of
introspectively gathering data from which therapists attempt to explain to
patients what a therapist understands about crucial aspects of a patient’s life.
Empathy is the way that analysts comprehend the impact of crucial figures on
others’ development.
This idea is not a radical departure from major principles of analytic
treatment, at least for the predominant psychodynamic therapies. The critical
differences materialize in the way that therapists understand, conceptualize,
and interpret patients’ communications. Empathic listening refers to the way
that analysts become sustainedly immersed in what patients say. Analysts or
psychotherapists reconstruct what they hear, not in an “experience-distant”
manner (from the vantage point of a higher order abstract theory about mental
processes), but in an “experience-near” manner (remaining close to the pa
tient’s phenomenological experience).
Kohut compared this mode of listening to the basic psychological bond
that allows people to grasp one another’s intentions or motives and to make
them intelligible. He saw empathy as “the capacity to think and feel oneself
into the inner life of another person. It is our lifelong ability to experience
what another person experiences” (Kohut, 1984, p. 82). Kohut stated that
empathy is present at all levels of development as early as a mother’s
accurate, empathic grasp of her infant’s states of satisfaction and distress.
Empathy includes a mother’s psychological capacity to respond adequately
and thus to provide the substrate for the nuclear or emerging self of infancy.
Freud (1921/1955) considered empathy to be the mechanism through
which individuals can assume any attitude at all about another person.
Kohut (1996) provided a good example of empathy serving as a basis for
calming, when it is accurately attuned, or for exacerbating an already
distressed self state. A person who is upset by something turns to another
SELF PSYCHOLOGY: MAJOR CONCEPTS 25
The mother picks the child up. What does the friend do? The friend puts
an arm around the other’s shoulder and imitates the picking up. Although
he may feel like a small baby, you put your arm around him and say “I
know how you must feel. ... Feeling your calmness while you are united
with the other person allows this person now to merge with you .... A
mother who says, “Oh, for goodness sake, don’t cry, there’s nothing to be
anxious about,” does not allow this particular kind of merger and does not
help the child. She rejects and pushes the child away. A mother who goes
to pieces over the child’s anxieties also doesn’t encourage the merger.
Why? Because why do you want to merge into something that is as
anxious as you yourself are, or even more so? A mother who misunder
stands the child and thinks that he’s anxious when he’s in pain or in pain
when he’s anxious is also no good. (p. 7)
cians. Even the term itself is unfortunate, because of its surplus meanings.
Kohut frequently struggled with others’ misunderstanding of his meaning
of empathy as a primary tool of investigation. In an effort to clarify
empathic understanding, Kohut (1984) wrote:
My answer is that the greater relaxation of the self psychologist, his greater
freedom to respond with deeply reverberating understanding and reso
nant emotionality, and the generally calmer and friendlier atmosphere of
self psychological treatment—these developments do not rest on the self
psychologist’s increased use of empathy, on the fact that he is “more
empathic” than his non-self psychological colleagues. They rest instead
on the expanded scope of empathy that is the product of the self
psychologist’s expanded theoretical understanding, (p. 82)
Let me remind you that you don’t have to mirror the patient to be effective
as his analyst. That is really a total mistake. The meaning of mirroring, the
essence of that concept, is not that you have to play-act with your patient
and praise him and respond to him and say that he is wonderful. No such
nonsense. But you do have to show the patient over and over again how
he defensively retreats because he expects that he will not get what he
wants and that he doesn’t dare to let himself know what he wants. (Kohut,
1996, p. 373)
The analyst does not actively soothe; he interprets the analysand’s yearn
ing to be soothed. The analyst does not actively mirror; he interprets the
need for confirming responses. The analyst does not actively admire or
approve grandiose expectations; he explains their role in the psychic
economy. The analyst does not fall into passive silence; he explains why
his interventions are felt to be intrusive. Of course, the analyst’s mere
presence, or the fact that he understands, all have soothing and self-con
firming effects on the patient, and they are so interpreted. Thus, the analytic
ambience that makes analytic work possible becomes itself an object for
analytic interpretation. The whole analytic process in this way blocks
exploitation for mere gratification.
What one does for the patient is really not giving him what he has missed.
As a matter of fact, most patients are very sensitive about that and dislike
it intensely. If you do that they feel patronized; they feel treated like
children, as though you have given to them as to a beggar. I think it is
much more sophisticated, and much more a real gift, when one recognizes
the need and explains it from one adult to another ... .He [the patient]
can occasionally say, “Yes, I know that, but really I do need something.”
Then maybe from time to time one can, recognizing an enormous need,
give something that one knows is, for the time being, necessary. I have a
nice phrase for it: I call it the “reluctant compliance with the childhood
wish.” (Elson, 1987, p. 39)
If the patient gets ... silent, oddly unanswering treatment, he will really
have been misled and betrayed, and he will, of course, be deeply
disappointed and enraged. This is not the expression of repressed or
otherwise neurotically defended-against or characterologically deter
mined rage, which now, because of the skillful therapeutic mobilization
of his central conflicts, erupts to reveal his true enraged self and the
consequent guilt ... That is all utterly spurious; they are all artifacts.
(Kohut, 1996, pp. 250-251)
TRANSMUTING INTERNALIZATION
If empathic understanding is the way that therapists identify patients’
concerns or injuries, the primary therapeutic technique for conveying these
insights remains interpretation. The therapeutic objective is to repair the
self disorder by interpretations emphasizing momentary disruptions of
empathic attunement and its destabilizing effects. Through the gradual
internalization of these insights, patients rebuild or fortify the injured or
devitalized self. Kohut proposed the term transmuting internalization to
refer to this process.
Interpretive work is rarely based on resolving intrapsychic conflict.
Patients establish a selfobject transference that inevitably becomes dis
rupted in the normal course of treatment by the empathic unavailability of
the analyst or psychotherapist. Patients experience this unavailability as
an injury to self-esteem. Therapists attempt to convey what they understand
to have taken place, including the genetic origins of the patient’s reaction.
Frustration produced by selfobject failure is inevitable in the treatment
relationship. It reveals the pre-existing state of self-cohesion before under
taking treatment and may lead to regression based on a patient’s specific
form of psychopathology.
In the invariable frustrations experienced in treatment, patients are re-ex
posed in the transference to empathic failures of childhood. This experience
particularly revives early failures or traumatic disappointments of parental
empathic attunement or responsiveness to the selfobject needs of the young
child. The many ways in which these early injuries are remobilized by the
momentary empathic misattunements or poorly timed interpretations are
part and parcel of analytic or psychotherapeutic treatment.
That these remobilizations are inevitable misunderstandings or misteps
of therapists or analysts is to be expected. Misunderstandings occur when
therapists attempt to grapple with patients’ unconscious dynamics, particu
larly early in treatment. That patients experience these misunderstandings
SELF PSYCHOLOGY: MAJOR CONCEPTS 31
COMPENSATORY STRUCTURES
The concept of compensatory structures is less familiar than other concepts
in self psychology. Clinicians and theorists whose primary acquaintance
with self psychology came from writings before and including The Analysis
o f the Self( Kohut, 1971) may not be very aware of this concept, which may
SELF PSYCHOLOGY: MAJOR CONCEPTS 33
also hold particular interest for those concerned with diagnostic psycho
logical assessment. Many clinical examples that I present in chapters 4
through 8 illustrate that successful and unsuccessful attempts to develop
compensatory structures can be identified by using diagnostic testing meth
ods. Psychodiagnostic tests can demonstrate the relative balance among
primary, defensive, and compensatory structures.
Kohut first described the concept of compensatory structures in The
Restoration o f the Self(\911). The concept centers on attempting to secure
another route to achieve a firmed-up sense of self when the primary pole or
sector of the self has been injured in early development. Thus, it is possible
to repair self-cohesion if another sector of the self can provide the selfobject
responsiveness of the original primary structure that was deficient. Typi
cally, the compromised primary structure is the grandiose-exhibitionistic
pole and its associated mirroring selfobject function. The idealized parent
imago pole can also be the defective primary structure, although this
situation is less frequent. In either circumstance, Kohut believed that
children tried to repair the deficit in the impaired primary structure, if the
injury was not too extensive, by seeking an alternative route to strengthen
ing the self. Compensatory structure represents a second chance to restore
self-cohesion and usually appears as idealization in an attempt to compen
sate for derailed or chronically interrupted mirroring.
In contrast, defenses operate in much the same way as in ego psycho
logical theory. That is, defensive operations function to protect an injured
or devitalized self that has been limited in acquiring compensatory struc
tures. Defenses, therefore, do not provide the possibility of a self-reparative
mechanism: the appearance of defenses signifies the failure of compensa
tory structures.
Defensive structures interfere with self-cohesion by limiting the po
tential to promote a consolidation of the self. In contrast, compensatory
structures represent the possibility of reinvigorated self-cohesion and
thus permit development to move forward. Attaining compensatory
structures allows self-cohesion to become strengthened and produces a
functional rehabilitation of the self. If a compensatory structure does not
succeed, then defenses are the best compromise solutions possible.
Kohut (1977) suggested that the work of treatment is to create com
pensatory structures from defenses, if self-cohesion has not been trau-
matically damaged by failures of selfobject responsiveness in the
weakened sector of the self. Failure to acquire compensatory structures
34 CHAPTER 2
Just as a tree will, within certain limits, be able to grow around an obstacle
so that it can ultimately expose its leaves to the life-sustaining rays of the
sun, so will the self in its developmental search abandon the effort to
36 CHAPTER 2
They become fixated on drives because their budding selves were over
looked, were not responded to. They turned to drive-gratification (and
later remained fixated on it) because they tried to relieve their depres
sion—they tried to escape the horrible feeling that nobody was respond
ing to them. Such people may have had mothers who satisfied their drives
continuously, yet failed to respond to the whole child who expected
mirroring responses; they failed to respond with pride and pleasure to the
child’s increasingly independent self. (Kohut, 1996, pp. 208-209)
Thus, it was not the emergence of libidinal and aggressive drives that
produced conflict in an oedipal child. Rather, the unresponded-to or empa-
thically unattuned response to the emerging self represented the primary
threat. Conflict, the cornerstone of psychopathology according to the struc
tural theory, was secondary to impaired self-cohesion. This position is a
main reason that many psychoanalytic clinicians have considered the psy
chology of the self to have a “misplaced (and largely incorrect) emphasis”
(Wallerstein, 1986/1995).
From the standpoint of the psychology of the self, a pathogenic oedipal
phase is not an inevitable, necessary, or universal part of normal develop
ment. Such a phase may appear when there has been appreciable empathic
failure at this stage of relatively normal development. Kohut reserved the
term Oedipus complex for pathological disturbances arising from the par
ents’ unattuned or unempathic reactions to children’s needs for selfobject
responsiveness at this stage of development.
Kohut did not consider pathological sexualizations as misplaced or
frustrated libidinal urges or view assertiveness as derived from an aggres
sive drive. He thought that many traditional analysts overestimated fear of
the parents as a neurotic manifestation of castration anxiety. Kohut believed
that attempts to treat oedipal-phase injuries to the self as structural disorders
would fail. As an example, Kohut provided the illustration of the jealous
erotized material of “pseudohysterics”:
The more you interpret the Oedipal period, the worse they get because
they are afraid those ideas will be taken away from them. Why? Because
it is the continuous activity in the romantic conflict sphere of the Oedipal
background that gives them the sense of being alive. Now, side by side,
you see indications of deeper pathology ... You must undercut the sup
posed Oedipal issues by telling the patient you think that all this romantic
falling in love probably covers a great sense of neediness ... that he really
wants something much more basic from you ... And then the patient will
feel understood, and there will be a toning down of all the romantic and
jealous and suicide threatening fireworks, and one can perhaps begin to
go to work on the depressive sense of the self. (Kohut, 1996, pp. 116-117)
40 CHAPTER 2
It is nothing else but the final paragraph of the long book of a successful
analysis. It need not be analyzed. If it is analyzed, then fine, it won’t do any
harm. It will be a little lapse, but if you have understood a person for so
many years then one final misunderstanding won’t destroy him and all the
work you’ve done. The patient will feel rebuffed because now he really
can love or really can hate you; now he is strong enough to do that. And
the fact that he can do that now is what you should recognize and
congratulate him about.” (Kohut, 1996, pp. 346-347)
In the first two chapters of this book, I showed that Kohut viewed narcis
sistic personality disorders as nonresponsive to the conventional interpre
tations of conflict and defense derived from drive theory and ego
psychology. He discovered that patients with these disorders, when cor
rectly understood, nevertheless formed stable and recognizable transfer
ences, which permitted psychoanalytic treatment to proceed without a need
for technical modifications. Kohut originally identified two basic transfer
ence patterns of mirroring and idealization, which represented the bipolar
self, and first referred to these as narcissistic transferences. His original
emphasis on narcissistic pathology and narcissistic transferences evolved
into a broad concept of the self and the self’s requirements for responsive
ness, the self-selfobject environment. Thus, he came to see mirroring and
idealization (and later, twinship) as selfobject functions that sustain and
invigorate self-esteem. The mobilization of these functions in treatment
takes the form of a selfobject transference.
“[T]hat dimension of our experience of another person relates to this
person’s functions in shoring up our self’ (Kohut, 1984, pp. 49-50). This
perception led Kohut to develop the concept of selfobject function, which
describes a psychological state that is experienced in depth, the innermost
experience of a self that is either buoyant and firm or injured and devitalized.
The actual relationship between one person and another is secondary and
frequently irrelevant.
The selfobject is necessary to sustain optimal self-cohesion or self-es
teem. Although Kohut considered selfobject functions to be necessary for
healthy self-esteem throughout life, he recognized their particular impor
tance when the self is injured or undermined. At these moments, the
primary function of the selfobject is to repair an enfeebled or devitalized
self. Through the attempt to revive or rebuild a diminished or depreciated
self state, selfobject functions are most evident clinically.
42
SELFOBJECT FUNCTIONS 43
SELFOBJECT FUNCTIONS:
CONCEPTUAL DEFINITION
selfobject functions that revive the need for a viable presence when the self
lacks sufficient resilience for sustaining itself.
How is a selfobject best defined? What is and is not a selfobject? It is
simpler to say what a selfobject is not: It is not another person. A selfobject
is a purpose or function that another person serves; it is the psychological
function. It is both correct and incorrect to think of selfobject functions as
simply experiences of need states that other persons satisfy or provide for
and that the self is unable to carry out independently in the absence of such
assistance. Another person may sometimes function in this manner, but
interpersonal relationships constitute only a simplistic view of a person’s
in-depth intrapsychic states.
Thus, to say that a selfobject is another person is correct but only to a
point. It is more accurate to consider the selfobject phenomenologically as
a function that a person needs as a substitute for a missing, defective, or
deficient aspect of the self. In this respect, the selfobject function is
reparative or restorative. A misplaced emphasis on the nature or quality of
interpersonal relationships for understanding selfobject functions (such as
cooperative, paternal, or malevolent) serves only to direct attention from
the central issue. De-emphasizing the interpersonal context promotes an
appreciation that self-cohesion is primarily an internal experience of
feeling invigorated and whole.
In reference to defining a selfobject, Basch (1994, p. 2) wrote:
urgently demanded that the analyst only restate her own opinions about
events in her life and not offer any interpretations of his own. Rather than
insisting that this transference reaction represented a reactivation of libidinal
conflict and interpreting her anger and feelings of being undermined from
that viewpoint, Kohut pursued an approach based on the fact that the patient
relegated him to little more than an impersonal function. In his attempt to
understand the meaning of this experience for Miss F., Kohut ultimately
discovered the phenomenon he came to describe as mirror transference.
A selfobject is not characterized by self-preoccupation or selfishness.
Turning to others to provide a selfobject need is not the same as being
selfish, self-absorbed, or even self-aggrandizing. Although these features
may sometimes be present, selfishness is not equivalent to selfobject need.
Selfobject needs may exist side by side with the capacity to appreciate
another person’s separate existence or needs (including selfobject needs).
Selfobject needs are not always experienced with reference to another
person. They may be realized through other means, including satisfactions
derived from literature, the arts, or abstract ideas. Selfobject functions may
even take the form of gratifications or states of relief provided by a drug or
erotized object such as a fetish, and in such cases, they are no less psycho
logically compelling.
Other self psychologists have subsequently proposed different meanings of
the term selfobject. Some of these definitions are based on the way that people
experience having selfobject functions (Stolorow, Brandschaft, & Atwood,
1987) or relationships (Bacal, 1994; Rowe, 1994). In Basch’s (1994) view,
however, the crucial characteristic of a selfobject function is the way that
another person responds affectively, acts, or otherwise fosters an experience
for the individual so as to promote a firmed-up, invigorated sense of self. In
this way, the self becomes a cohesive and enduring mental structure.
Whatever the similarity between a selfobject and an interpersonal rela
tionship, the selfobject clearly does not assume a relational meaning.
Clinicians associated with psychoanalytic self psychology have at times
casually mentioned selfobject transferences as if they were describing a
relationship. Self psychologically informed practitioners or theoreticians,
however, do not consider a selfobject to be a person (Wolf, 1988). However
tempting it is to think of selfobject functions as an interpersonal quality or
relationship, selfobject functions are still fundamentally stable psychic
structures formed as a result of optimal frustration in early development. If
the normal, ongoing internalizations of selfobject functions are interfered
46 CHAPTER 3
They [such children] feel unreal, shadowy, ghost-like, empty; their human
surroundings, their possessions, their world, become dead, devoid of
substantiality; they suffer a drop and loss in self-esteem (they feel “smaller, ”
“ordinary,” instead of proud of themselves and what they can do); they
are depleted and depressed (“underpowered”) ... the healthily proud and
jaunty aspects of the patient’s childhood self, their body-mind-self no
longer works right, (p. 430)
turned out, though often only after a prolonged search, that my rightness
was superficial whereas their rightness was profound, (p. 94)
By the end of his analysis, he had learned that most of the things he hoped
for he failed to get. Most of his expectations were never realized. Instead
he only learned that one shouldn’t expect to get certain things and then
cry one’s eyes out in disappointment or scratch out other people’s eyes in
rage ... No one ever gets as much love as he wants, no one gives as much
love as he might. Choices can be made but choice involves the assumption
of responsibility and the necessity for renunciation ... Self-improvement
will continue, autonomously, but the treatment, as such, is ended. The
parties part company. The contract has been fulfilled, (pp. 178-179)
PREDOMINANT TRANSFERENCE
CONFIGURATIONS
I now describe the three major selfobject functions that incorporate Kohut’s
(1984) final considerations on the subject. Kohut recognized that self
psychology was not a static theory; accordingly, he acknowledged that
additional selfobject functions beyond the three he described might even
tually be identified. Kohut actually discussed the mirroring and idealizing
selfobject functions as the two major forms of selfobject transferences as
early as 1966, when he noted that subtypes or variants of these functions
could be identified. Not until 1984 did he differentiate the twinship or
alter-ego selfobject function from mirroring; previously, he had considered
twinship as a subtype of the broad group of mirror transferences. Nearly 20
years later, Kohut (1984) came to view twinship as having a status inde
pendent of mirroring. Kohut and Wolf (1978) and Wolf (1988) also de
scribed several other possibilities of this sort, although at present the status
of selfobject functions remains largely confined to the three major forms
(mirroring, idealization, and twinship) described next.
Mirroring
Kohut (1971, 1977) was impressed by the importance of young children’s
wishes to experience prideful satisfaction surrounding their accomplishments.
He was particularly aware of children’s desires to have these budding steps
recognized and admired. Thus, this mirroring or echoing response (“the gleam
in the mother’s eye”) serves as the prototype for the mirroring selfobject
function, arising from the grandiose-exhibitionistic pole of the self.
Admittedly, grandiosity is an unfortunate phrase in view of the pejorative
connotation of the term narcissism. The term, however, stems from the
52 CHAPTER 3
child’s joyful enthusiasm. This view of shame differs from the familiar
object libidinal interpretation of shame as a response triggered by failure to
sufficiently satisfy the demands of the ego ideal.
Similarly, Kohut viewed narcissistic rage as an understandable reaction
to selfobject failure. His position contrasts with the predominant drive
theory interpretation of rage as derived from an inborn aggressive drive.
For some patients, narcissistic rage reactions assume primitive forms of
discharge, such as the disintegration products of fragmenting self-cohesion.
Thus, aggression not based on healthy assertion is thought of as a break
down into isolated fragments of previously structuralized aspects of the self.
The characterologic description of many patients with defects of the
grandiose-exhibitionistic self includes prominent features of coldness,
sometimes accompanied by arrogance. These manifestations of arrogance
or self-aggrandizement defensively conceal feelings of shame and depre
ciation. Often, however, self-depreciation emerges quickly and sometimes
surprisingly early in treatment, even when a patient is otherwise relatively
asymptomatic. This phenomenon is the vertical split that was Kohut’s
(1971) way of describing how the experience of grandiosity is walled off
from the experience of reality.
Kohut’s view differs from Kemberg’s (1975) interpretation of grandios
ity as a defensive reaction to conceal feelings of rage, envy, or dependency.
Kohut’s (1977,1984) subsequent expanded view of mirroring went beyond
its clinical description in psychopathology. In its broadened scope, he
emphasized the importance of in-step confirming, echoing, or affirming
responses that are crucial to mirroring. He understood these mirroring
responses to be normal, expectable, and legitimate needs, setting the stage
for realizing goals in keeping with a person’s talents and skills. Rather than
falling prey to atrophy or derailment through neglect or unattuned respon
siveness, these more or less normal needs should give rise to a firm and
vigorous sense of the self.
This normal development of the self, strengthened by the availability of
suitably responsive mirroring selfobjects, leads to the capacity to turn to the
world with assurance that one’s efforts are noticed and responded to
empathically. From such a position people derive satisfaction from their
abilities and interests. This reaction is accompanied by confidence and
sufficient vitality or enthusiasm to strive to reach life goals. The need for
mirroring extends from normal appreciation of one’s basic abilities in health
to pathological forms dominated by a profound sense of devitalization of
self-esteem, propensity for sexualization and addictions, and chronic empty
54 CHAPTER 3
Idealization
With idealization, the idealized parent imago, which is Kohut’s term for this
pole or sector of the self, emerges alongside the grandiose-exhibitionistic
pole and becomes another means of strengthening self-esteem.
Although idealization can take the form of holding others in awe,
sometimes even putting them on a pedestal, this aspect of its clinical
appearance is not invariably present. Much more common are the relatively
silent idealizations that are more subtle and less overt. This circumstance
frequently requires close examination over time before it can be defined as
genuine idealization. For this reason, psychodiagnostic testing may be
useful to detect its presence. Idealized selfobjects can be experienced as
omnipotent; when this reaction occurs in the transference, the analyst or
therapist is often revered as powerful and all-knowing. More often, ideali
zation is mobilized when the need for a calming but strong presence is
predominant. Thus, a young child’s need for both calm strength and limit
SELFOBJECT FUNCTIONS 55
ations of this type do not usually bring about the most optimal internaliza
tions; counselors, therapists, or therapeutic milieus like this are, in effect,
“swallowed whole.” As with hunger, satiation of archaic selfobject needs
requires perpetual nourishment.
Kohut (1971) identified three forms of the idealized parent imago. In the
most archaic type, a merger with the omnipotently perceived selfobject
protects the self from traumatic overstimulation leading to diffuse narcis
sistic disorganization. In a less archaic form, associated with a greater
degree of differentiation between self and selfobject, the idealized selfob
ject serves a function of strengthening the enfeebled self in the control or
neutralization of drives. Kohut described a still less archaic form of the
idealizing selfobject transference during the oedipal period of development,
associated with maximal differentiation between self and selfobject; here,
the power and perfection of the selfobject is needed for approval and for
providing guiding ideals and values.
Twinship
Kohut’s (1971) earlier work referred to the “mirroring transference in the
broad sense” and the ’’mirroring transference in the narrow sense.” He
intended the broad designation to refer to the generalized need for mirroring
in all its various forms: merger, twinship (alter ego), and mirroring in the
narrow sense. Kohut (1984) regarded the twinship selfobject function as
sufficiently important in its own right to be differentiated from mirroring.
Thus, twinship is equivalent to mirroring and idealization as an independent
selfobject function. The analysis of one particular patient initially suggested
the twinship function, and clinical observation in several other analyses
further demonstrated it.
The selfobject can be somebody (sometimes a thing or object) with
whom a person can talk or keep company; it can also exist as a silent
presence to evoke when needed. Twinship selfobject needs arise to combat
self states of depletion, loneliness, or related experiences of devitalization
of the self. Kohut considered the twinship function as a person’s experience
of being one among others and deriving a deeply felt sense of security from
this realization. Its central purpose is to represent the need for a selfobject
to operate as a twin or faithful replica of the self. Thus, a person as selfobject
acts like oneself but does not merge with one’s sense of self.
Twinship is a basis for feeling a sense of belonging or psychological
connection. LaFemina (1996) described it thus:
60 CHAPTER 3
Twinship maintains or assists the cohesion and viability of the self. Twinship
does not refer to an actual relationship between two people, nor to the
fantasies of a relationship designed to maintain defensive purposes. A
twinship relationship is established to provide specific narcissistic functions.
core of the experience that gives the twinship selfobject its urgency or
importance for a person’s self-cohesion. A child who talks to a beloved toy
or comforting companion, such as a teddy bear, as if the object were
experiencing the feelings that the child experiences, comes closer to the
spirit of twinship. The selfobject may be a person, object, or even an abstract
idea; its form is secondary because the essential function is the companion
ate or faithful replica quality.
Kohut considered the problem of distinguishing twinship from the other
selfobject functions. In the example of a friend who placed a comforting
hand on a person’s shoulder when the person is troubled or hurt, Kohut
observed that this gesture might represent a twinship selfobject function if
the friend understood exactly how injured the person felt. He dismissed this
possibility, however, by comparing the weakened or injured self that
experienced the invigorating gesture of the hand on the shoulder to an
unhappy baby picked up by the mother. Here, the mother’s ministration
permitted the baby to merge with her calmness and omnipotence. This
interpretation emphasizes that lifting the baby represents a remobilization
of a mirroring selfobject need, as does the arm around the shoulder. Kohut
noted, however, that the gesture of the hand on the back might connote a
twinship function in particular clinical circumstances.
An illustration of healthy twinship is the intense creative relationship
between the playwright Jon Robin Baitz and the actor Ron Rifkin. The depth
of intuitive understanding between them has enhanced the work of both
such that the playwright regards the actor as “my Lear” and the actor says
of the playwright: “Robbie is part of me. You know, we finish sentences for
each other. Certainly he understands something about me that I don’t” (A.
Klein, 1996). Their collaborative relationship was described in a New York
Times article as: “a rare and empathic alliance between playwright and actor,
as if a singular esthetic identity arose out of collaboration” (A. Klein, 1996).
M. Tolpin (1995) gave an illustration of a related but more subtle
twinship dynamic—the lengthy friendship between Anna Freud and
Dorothy Burlingham. Tolpin regarded Dorothy Burlingham as essential to
ensuring the lifesaving self-cohesion of Freud’s daughter. Tolpin considered
their twinship bond as crucial to consolidating what Anna Freud referred to
as her “circle of health.” In this circle, she could hold onto the unwavering,
unconditional, self-affirming regard of Dorothy Burlingham as a firmly
established internalized psychological structure. Thus, through the twinship
with Burlingham, Anna Freud was able to revitalize a profoundly injured
self state. Freud could free herself from unsuccessful attempts at mirroring
62 CHAPTER 3
65
66 CHAPTER 4
DEVELOPMENTS IN PSYCHODIAGNOSTIC
TESTING
Some patients with acute Axis I conditions also have subclinical but still
maladaptive personality disorders that aggravate the Axis I disorder. Such
patients include those with disturbances of self-esteem regulation that may
or may not be recognized as contributing factors to the overall clinical
picture. As for other personality disturbances, disruptions of self-esteem or
prominent selfobject failure are important precipitants for some Axis I
illnesses, including mood disorders, addictions, and sexual perversions.
Therefore, the identification of self disorders is often important.
Some self disorders can exist independently, of course, as the primary
condition, that is, as an Axis II disorder. Self disorders can also underlie
or precipitate many conditions not usually considered to be narcissistic
disorders or related disturbances of self-esteem regulation. These con
ditions may be secondary to an acute Axis I disorder, such as psychotic
episodes, severe episodes of affective illnesses (including manic and
hypomanic disorders), anxiety syndromes, and subsyndromal conditions
such as dysthymia, with or without a history of comorbid major depres
sion (so-called double depression) or mania (Akiskal, 1980).
Such self state disturbances often require special diagnostic chal
lenges to detect them. Careful interviewing with an eye to the clinical
CONTENT ANALYSIS OF PSYCHODIAGNOSTIC TESTING 73
CONCEPTUAL APPROACHES
TO PSYCHODIAGNOSTIC TESTING:
GENERAL PRINCIPLES
Psychological assessment of mental functions has a history over 100 years
old. Major early achievements were the instruments measuring intelligence,
notably the Binet and Wechsler scales. The development of tests for the
74 CHAPTER 4
CONCEPTUAL APPROACHES
TO PSYCHODIAGNOSTIC TESTING:
SPECIFIC PRINCIPLES
method for understanding projective test findings, the issue can benefit
from rethinking to optimize the contributions of psychometric advances
while remaining embedded in psychoanalytic theory. For many clinicians
favorably disposed to psychodynamic formulations, Schafer’s concern
about the proper approach to interpretive criteria cannot, therefore, be
dismissed as misplaced or outdated. Instead, his criteria can be applied
regardless of the psychoanalytic views that have influenced psychodiag
nostic testing, whether these be ego psychology, object relations, self
psychology, the French school of Lacan (1978), interpersonal approaches
(Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983), or intersubjectivist schools that include
offshoots of self psychology (Stolorow, Brandchaft, & Atwood, 1987).
Schafer (1954) asked: “How are we to decide whether our explorations
have led us home or astray, whether we have glittering gold in hand or just
glitter: And how are we to distinguish between thoroughness and reckless
ness?” (p. 141). He offered a solution in the form of criteria forjudging
the adequacy of interpretations. These criteria provide a useful basis for
the analysis of content and the observation of patients’ test behavior and
associated verbalizations. They are best thought of as reasonable guides
for arriving at diagnostic decisions rather than definitive, empirically
grounded rules. Schafer’s criteria, therefore, offer discipline to the in
vestigation of projective test findings based on content analysis. Written
well over 40 years ago, his work provides a cogent, compelling statement
of a systematic, logical strategy for approaching these potentially murky
areas of subjective analysis. These criteria also serve as an excellent
foundation for conceptualizing self psychological concepts through psy
chodiagnostic tests.
Sufficient Evidence
Foremost among the criteria forjudging the adequacy of clinical interpre
tation is the issue of sufficient evidence to substantiate an inference. This
criterion is usually satisfied by considering converging lines of evidence,
sometimes based on several recurrences of a particular theme, but at other
times derived from associations to particular test responses. These re
sponses characteristically serve to clarify or fine-tune the specific connota
tion underlying a motivation or drive state. Schafer (1954) wrote that
“patients usually help us out by giving not one but a number of images,
score patterns and attitude expressions that confirm, modify, offset or
de-emphasize the interpretive leads provided by one response” (p. 141).
80 CHAPTER 4
I think that you probably have a reasonably good batting average in some
very clearcut instances when the symptomatology is of chronic depression
and the parental personalities are described in a way to indicate there was
no responsiveness, or where there was the withdrawal of the father, a
depressive mother, something on that order. If long periods of loneliness
are already beginning to be described in the diagnostic interview, if there
is dream material about abandonment, if there are lonely landscapes or
snowy landscapes, that kind of thing—I mean there are certain things that
make the diagnosis iron clad. (p. 121)
For example, on Rorschach Card III, a patient might report two people
circling each other, but omit the area conventionally representing the legs
from the location. The scores reflect the inaccuracy of goodness of fit of
the specific area indicated for a human percept, but the movement score
does not capture the specific quality of circling each other as distinct from
an activity such as looking at each other. It is also not considered to represent
an activity meriting a separate code for aggressive movement, at least when
stated this way.
Similarly, to a Sentence Completion Test stem “A mother ... ,” a patient
might respond “is sometimes there when you need her.” A story might be
given to TAT Card 1 in which the violin is misidentified as a guitar or on
Card 8BM the story might ignore the rifle. The defensive aspect of these
features might be considered for its interpretive value, but just as often the
interpretation does not proceed beyond noting the perceptual inaccuracy.
This case is no different from using only the formal scores on the Rorschach
example and not assigning interpretive significance to the people circling
one another.
The problem that Schafer described as depth or level of interpretation is
no greater in the analysis of projective tests without formal scoring systems
than of tests with reliable scoring systems. The crux of the issue concerns
how to interpret responses such as people circling each other or a failure to
incorporate the large figure of the rifle into a TAT story. A scoring system
does not alleviate the problem by itself. Should circling each other, for
example, be confined to the incongruity between this response and the failure
to specify the leg area, or should it be taken to represent hostile or predatory
impulses? Should the addition of the word “sometimes” in the Sentence
Completion Test example cited be left uninterpreted, or should an inference
be entertained about a complaint of maternal neglect or indifference?
Schafer’s discussion of the depth or level of interpretation applied to just
these situations. As with the criterion for adequate evidence, he favored the
view that “the interpretation should not push below the level of defense”
(1954, p. 150). Many of Schafer’s clinical examples clearly indicated his
willingness to attempt interpretations of drives or conflict when sufficient
evidence appeared to substantiate that level of analysis. In this respect, the
criteria of depth and adequacy of evidence are closely linked. The level of
an interpretation is influenced by the sufficiency and quality of the evidence
permitting an interpretation to extend beyond the level of defense analysis.
Schafer did not leave the issue here; his next criterion for judging the
adequacy of an interpretation, the use of manifest content, continued from
88 CHAPTER 4
the point that defense analysis left off. I have combined the two criteria in
this discussion because they are interrelated. When one identifies the
conflict that underlies defenses, one deepens the level of the interpretation.
The manifest content is now considered as a basis to speculate about its
meaning. With appropriate cautions, Schafer argued that there are more than
enough times when the evidence supports at least some consideration of an
interpretation of the manifest content. He wrote: “There are in fact good
reasons to support the inclusion of this criterion, particularly if we modify
it so: ‘Whenever possible, the manifest form of the interpreted tendency
should be specified’” (Schafer, 1954, p. 151).
Schafer acknowledged that it is generally easier to understand defensive
and adaptive manifestations than drive manifestations (manifest content).
Examiners can more easily attempt interpretations about drives or conflicts,
however, when they consider the analysis of manifest content. To be sure,
the argument is strengthened when formal scores, clinical validation by
history, repetitive patterns of scores or responses, and a battery of tests can
also be relied on to buttress an interpretation under consideration. Initially,
it is preferable to consider interpretations that are based on content. The
decision to proceed beyond the level of a defense interpretation depends on
other evidence that either supports or renders uncertain a deeper level of
clinical interpretation.
Schachtel
The culmination of Ernest Schachtel’s work on and thinking about the
Rorschach is represented by his 1966 book. In this work, he presented his
views about the major determinants and scores, particularly from the
standpoint of their experiential basis and the nature of the test situation.
Schachtel’s approach has its roots in the writings of Rorschach and the
Rapaport-Schafer, Beck, and Klopfer traditions. These approaches influ
enced his synthesis of some of the finest traditions in the history of
psychodiagnostic testing.
Schachtel’s unique contributions centered on his emphasis on the expe
riential nature of the Rorschach technique. His viewpoint stemmed from
the ego psychology tradition, and he was concerned with the perceptual and
cognitive features of the Rorschach in relation to affects and motivational
states. In this respect, Schachtel’s thinking about the relationship between
personality dynamics and perception was influenced by Hartmann’s (1939)
concept of ego apparatuses of primary and secondary autonomy and Ra-
paport’s (1951) approach to the subject. His ideas about experiential proc
esses should not be confused with contemporary uses of the term, such as
those in philosophical phenomenology, the Rogerian or Gestalt psychother
apy schools, or for that matter, empathic understanding as in psychoanalytic
self psychology.
Schachtel (1966) distinguished between allocentric and autocentric
modes of perception and the influences of each on Rorschach responses. In
allocentric perception, the emphasis is on objectification of the perceptual
process. An object such as a Rorschach percept is described in terms of what
it resembles: a person “figuratively or literally, takes hold of it, tries to
‘grasp’ it” (p. 79). In contrast, the subject-centered autocentric perceptual
96 CHAPTER 4
mode stresses attending to how the feeling tone of a projective test stimulus
impinges on a person. The response creates a fusion between a sensory
quality and an affect state, a fusion that Schachtel characterized as essen
tially pleasure or displeasure.
From this view, which unites perception and affect or motivational state,
Schachtel (1966) considered major Rorschach determinants and noted that
the scoring features may be either allocentric or autocentric, even in the
same person at different times. He addressed the cognitive-perceptual
process of forming a Rorschach response to show how a percept can be
understood (i.e., how form is perceived influences the decision to produce
or withhold a particular response). Thus, Schachtel described a process in
which patients initially apprehended an ink blot in some fashion by taking
its contours and other form elements into account. The allocentric mode
leads to a tentative association with objects that resemble the ink blot. A
goodness of fit test, determined largely by an active or critical evaluation
of the likeness, is attempted, and finally the patient decides to accept or
reject the comparison. Any given response is either produced or leads to a
search for a better alternative, in keeping with a patient’s reality orientation
and capacities for critical analysis and judgment.
Schachtel (1966) noted that the autocentric mode influences the experi
ential aspect of this form-generating process. For example, affect states like
depression and boredom may interfere with perceptual processing of form.
Diminished interest constrains the vividness of the range of form like
nesses, although the accuracy of perception is usually unaltered.
To this point, Schachtel’s (1966) approach to the Rorschach response
process provided a conceptual basis for understanding how responses are
formulated and how affect or motivational states influence determinants.
The final interpretation of the full protocol, however, is another matter. In
this area, Schachtel used content analysis as a supplement to formal Ror
schach scoring. His thoughts on the subject revealed a use of content-based
material narrower than that of Rapaport and Schafer and not unlike Exner’s
(1991) view. Schachtel’s approach to content analysis was based on formal
response characteristics such as the use of particular determinants, in
contrast with the broad, albeit judicious, use of content in Schafer’s and
Lerner’s approaches. Schachtel’s approach is certainly more restrictive than
are the “dictionary” approaches that he criticized, as did Schafer (1954)
before him.
Schachtel (1966) argued that content based on verbalizations and asso
ciations is not suitable for understanding drive states, defenses, and adap
CONTENT ANALYSIS OF PSYCHODIAGNOSTIC TESTING 97
tive efforts. He thought it was difficult and “most of the time impossi
ble” (p. 259) to distinguish among these characteristics of the personality
by using content. He singled out the tendency to attribute specific
meaning to card details for special criticism and advised against inter
preting references to parental figures on Cards IV and VII, a caution
generally accepted by most Rorschach clinicians. Thus, stripped of
unsupportable inferences and associative comments about particular
percepts, the essence of Schachtel’s (1966) strategy for interpretation is
best captured in the following statement:
The data we study in Rorschach’s test are what the testee saw in the blots
and how he saw it, in the full concreteness of the percept and with all the
emotional overtones and undercurrents that color what he saw, and all
the intellectual and emotional effort, its quality, its process, its smoothness,
or conflicts which entered into the work of perceiving, associating, and
judging the fitness of the percept. From his words we try to reconstruct his
experience. The score is merely an abstraction of this experience, and the
verbal content of the responses is also an abstraction, (p. 261)
Lerner
R M. Lemer (1991) and H. D. Lemer and R M. Lemer (1988), together with
Schafer (1954) and Aronow et al. (1994), also emphasized the usefulness
of content analysis and the cautionary approach that this form of interpre
tation requires. The Lemers emphasized the importance of tracing the
inference back to the primary response data that are its source by including
each step in the process giving rise to an interpretation. R M. Lerner (1991)
emphasized searching carefully for confirmatory evidence from other test
findings and considering the internal consistency of a content-based inter
pretation in the framework of the total personality. The Lemers are cham
pions of an approach to diagnostic testing informed by contemporary
theories in psychoanalysis, including Kohut’s view of self psychology
(Lemer, 1988). In this respect, they considered internalized object relations
at least as important as drive-conflict-defense analysis.
R M. Lerner (1991) also regarded sequence analysis as a crucial aspect
of content analysis. He agreed with Schachtel’s (1966) phenomenologic
view in which sequence contains major clues to understanding a patient’s
experience or feeling state about the projective test process. Lemer (1991)
expanded on Schachtel’s (1966) and Klopfer’s (Klopfer & Kelley, 1942)
emphasis on formal Rorschach scores in sequence analysis with a broad
98 CHAPTER 4
Comprehensive System
In the 75-year history of Rorschach psychology, no attempt to bring this
instrument to a scientifically and clinically respectable level exceeded that
of Exner’s (1993) Comprehensive System. Exner’s work is anchored in
contemporary psychometric theory and benefits from concurrent advances
in diagnostic nosology, reliable measurement, and test validation. Not only
has the Rorschach achieved a needed revitalization, but the Comprehen
sive System also serves as a standard of psychological and psychometric
sophistication for other projective tests of personality. My discussion
centers on the role of content, which Exner referred to as Verbalizations and
Sequence o f Scores in his terminology.
Exner did not reject content analysis approaches to interpretation. He
considered content analysis to be potentially useful when applied judi
ciously as a secondary approach following the sequence of Comprehen
sive System scores and the clusters arising from combinations of major
scores. On this subject, Exner (1991) commented:
ous inquiry. Other TAT cards can yield better examples of selfobject
functions on the basis of their natural or evocative “pull,” such as cards that
depict more than one figure so that some type of relationship, such as a
struggle or intense emotional encounter, is powerfully evoked.
For example, Card 2 frequently contains a story line revealing how
the patient has managed the developmental step of separation from the
family of origin. The nature of the relationship between daughter and
mother may also be captured in the story. In particular, the mother who
cannot let go of the daughter raises the specter of a mother who is lonely
or fragile if left alone. This response offers useful albeit presumptive
information about the daughter’s selfobject needs from a mother who is
unable to tolerate loss or separation. The mother may be described as
though she would lose an important part of her own self, and the daughter
may be experienced as a narcissistic extension of the mother. This
description provides a hint about what the child of such a mother may
be re-exposed to on Card 2. The mother can also feel as if her maternal
function is no longer needed and she is therefore unmirrored and empty
or depleted. The mother can derive pride and sustenance from the
daughter who turns to this mother as an object of idealization. The bond
between them can be experienced as so tightly knit, from the point of
view of the mother, that the potential interruption of a possible twinship
selfobject function represents a specific threat or vulnerability.
A related if not identical pattern can also appear on TAT Card 6BM. The
fullest understanding of the psychodynamic meaning of the relationship
requires a level of inquiry that proceeds beyond simply eliciting that a
separation or rupture has occurred. The fantasied meaning of the separation
should be ascertained to understand what it represents for both characters.
A close inspection of Card 7GF, frequently seen as an interaction between
a girl and her mother, also yields an understanding of the selfobject
functions surrounding the intimate nature of this mother-child relation
ship. At one level of analysis, the girl may be described as looking away or
as absorbed in internal fantasy as the mother reads to her. At a more deeply
revealing level, the patient may note that the girl who looks away has
withdrawn or decathected from the mother. This response is quite different
from feeling content in the company of the mother. Only when a patient is
pressed does the typical story reveal whether a breach between mother and
child has exposed an empathic failure or disruption and how it is remedied.
Does the mother realize that the girl is not paying attention and attempt to
enter her world to re-engage her, or does the mother persist in reading to
CONTENT ANALYSIS OF PSYCHODIAGNOSTIC TESTING 113
the child apparently unaware if not unconcerned that the girl is not psycho
logically present? Examiners need to determine how patients perceive the
affective attunement of the mother, and by inference, the intimate figures
in their lives. Even a story indicating maternal involvement does not by
itself imply empathically attuned responsiveness. A level of responsiveness
in which the mother insists that the child listen to her or in which the child
listens obediently misses the boat if taken to mean that an optimal self-sel
fobject environment is indicated.
A telling feature of these responses is the child’s concern about securing
a needed selfobject function. This feature is typically one of seeking
admiration or affirmation from a parent who is often unaware of what the
child looks for. The story about obedience implies that the patient complies
with almost anything to win the mother’s responsive mirroring, even if the
story outcome suggests that the girl feels subdued or that she quietly
tolerates sadness or disappointment over a deficient self-selfobject milieu.
Some outcomes to this story convey similar reactions to a self-selfobject
unit that is not working optimally. The girl may be left psychologically
immobilized with no choice but to listen passively until the mother’s
“lecture” is over, or the girl’s depression may lead her to fantasize that she
will be a different kind of mother to the doll she is holding, or the girl may
become angry.
The same TAT card may also emphasize the girl’s relationship to the doll
on her lap. This feature is particularly distinctive for denoting an idealizing
or twinship selfobject function. Some common stories take the form of the
child’s imagining that she becomes as good a mother as her own, that the
mother in the picture teaches the girl how to care for her “dolly,” or that the
girl asks the mother to show her in fantasy how to minister to the doll as an
imagined baby. When such story lines do not contain veiled references to
maternal disinterest or resentment on the part of the girl, a normal or
healthy revival of the idealizing selfobject function may be indicated.
Aspects of twinship may also be present, a not unreasonable assumption if
the girl emphasizes her role as an exact replica of the mother’s attunement
or ministration, which the girl then extends by imitation to her doll.
In its most unsatisfying form, and from the standpoint of psychopathol
ogy a serious indication of a potentially untreatable self disorder, the story
may describe the mother as unresponsive to the girl’s needs, and the girl
may not manage to reinvolve the mother or, in fantasy, to work through
some way with the doll to achieve a sense of restored self-cohesion. Here,
neither mirroring nor idealization is operating properly. This response
114 CHAPTER 4
5 of Selfobject Functions:
Mirroring
GRANDIOSITY
power does not take the form of exaggerated grandiosity like the preceding
examples. Some examples are a Rorschach percept of a spaceship take-off
elaborated in a way that emphasizes that the sight is majestic, or the
abominable snowman described as impressive, or an imposing animal such
as a lion described as strong or with a grand mane. Responses such as these
emphasize the wish to be admired and sometimes have additional elabora
tion about a patient’s feeling misunderstood or unheard or otherwise indi
cating empathic failure.
Other examples are more subtle. Consider the following Rorschach
responses from a 37-year-old firefighter experiencing outbursts of anger.
The acute reaction followed his inability to prevent his father’s death in a
car accident that he witnessed. This patient produced “the stealth bomber”
as a response to Rorschach Card V. He elaborated this response on inquiry
as: “Broad wide wings, the sleekness of it, the principle that it’s an
undetectable surveillance.” Although not without connotations of secrecy,
aggression, and immunity from detection, this response can also be seen as
an image suggesting grandiose qualities such as being the best of its kind
and most powerful. The response conveys a desire to be admired for its
sleek looks. In terms of psychological development, it is not very different
from the little girl showing off her Sunday best or a little boy wanting praise
for the big fish he has caught or the proud owner of a new automobile
remarking: “Isn’t that a beauty!”
As an indication that fantasies of greatness can readily coexist with less
flattering self perceptions, this same patient gave several responses indicat
ing weakness or vulnerability. Thus, following his percept of the stealth
bomber, on Card VI this man reported: “A microorganism. I can’t relate to
this thing. Like under a microscope, looks like it’s being squashed between
two sheets of glass.” On Card VIII, the patient delivered a response of “two
rodents climbing to something, a wall, just stuck to it, a foot’s coming out
and it kind of looks like he’s falling away but yet he’s still stuck there, like
a cartoon,” and on Card IV: “A beaver laying next to a roadway, looks like
a flattened animal, the lighter color makes it look more frail like a
crusted-up dried-up dead animal.”
The flattened-out beaver percept was followed by an attempt to restore
some resiliency to the weakened undermined self, but the effort was short
lived: “It also looks like a space shuttle, the one that blew up. The Chal
lenger.” Thus, his attempt to invoke another strong, powerful image became
the vulnerable shuttle that failed to function and exploded in a well-known
disaster. In the psychopathology of self disorders, patients often give
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 119
A giant, like Jack and the Beanstalk, and this would be the beanstalk
he climbed to see if there’s a castle up there, and the giant came out
and Jack ran away and cut the beanstalk down and Jack fell from the
beanstalk; he saves the town and he’s a hero.
Heroic, yes; grandiose, perhaps; in either case, the risk was running away
from something threatening and falling down. The appearance of a noble
gesture, replete with all the greatness of being the conquering hero, is
close to an associated fantasy of falling. Later, the same patient produced
this percept: “It’s like Merlin, some sort of magician, or maybe a witch
talking out of something, it just looks like a witch, either bad or good,
helps people or doesn’t” (Card IX).
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 121
vigorous strivings of the self for definition are particularly notable in this
young woman’s projective test responses. This picture is not that of a
cohesive, invigorated self, but rather a possibly depleted shell of a self.
On Card X of the Rorschach, the patient attempted to depict lions as
a figure of strength, but the percept became “lions on a crest, or a brooch,
like my grandmother’s brooch.” Before assuming, however, that the
attempted reach for a vigorous percept of lions cannot be sustained but
instead became a design on a piece of jewelry, one must consider the
possibility that the grandmother was a source of strength to be relied on
for idealization. This possibility may also apply to the patient’s previous
Rorschach response of Merlin the magician. Nevertheless, this interpre
tation is unlikely; the patient failed to sustain an idealized wish for
strength in her tearfully delivered references to the witch-like qualities
of the percept as it was elaborated on inquiry. In addition, the previously
noted responses of a valiant prince who was not real or the heroic Jack
in the Beanstalk who was cut down are ominous. It is difficult to be
reassured that the lions on the grandmother’s brooch and the percept of
Merlin can do much to come to her aid. Rather, these responses repre
sented futile attempts to repair undermined self-cohesion. Recall again
her passive anticipation of others to pick her up at a bus stop or take her
to a place where someone comes along to “make something o f ’ her.
Thus, the possibility of idealization as a compensatory structure is
weakened by the emphasis in this patient’s associations on failure or
unavailability of potentially strong, idealizable figures such as Merlin or
the heroic prince who becomes an unreal fairy tale character. Although
the powerful lions on the grandmother’s jewelry may suggest a possibil
ity of a revivable, idealizable selfobject, the relative emphasis on help
lessness rather than on potential sources of strength favors the view that
deficient mirroring is the predominant self state.
These examples indicate that the appearance of clear or suggestive
percepts of a grandiose nature, including descriptions of powerful fig
ures, does not necessarily represent inflated self-worth. Indications of
vigor and invulnerability often give way to other percepts expressing
weakness, devaluation, or diminished self-esteem. Rather than attempt
ing to portray psychological health, responses of a grandiose nature
frequently represent ill-fated attempts to preserve whatever vitality can
be managed. As these examples illustrate, the effort is often not well-
sustained, and the patient typically caves in before very long.
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 123
ADMIRATION
ate freely. If the early associations in treatment are thought of as asking the
question “How am I doing?” the central dynamic is a need to be seen
favorably or as competent. The comparable question about beginning the
unfamiliar Rorschach test can just as easily be “Is this orchid a good enough
response?” Thus, the interpretation of a narcissistic character structure is
not crucial for understanding the orchid response but may represent the
dynamic of healthy mirroring needs striving for recognition.
The patient’s next response of a woman’s dress need also not be inter
preted as another narcissistic-decorative indication as Schafer regarded it.
Schafer considered that the dress as an adornment took precedence over the
human qualities of the person in the central detail of Card I. The focus on
the dress rather than on the person may show a continued need for recognition
or admiration originating with the orchid percept, as if the patient asked the
examiner to admire her dress or appearance as a symbol of herself. It is like
asking again “How am I doing?” without necessarily implying a deficiency
in failing to appreciate the inner qualities of the person.
By the time Card II was presented, this patient’s percept of dwarfs
attending Snow White signifies a failure of the patient’s wish to be admired
or noticed, at least from the standpoint of Kohut’s understanding of mirror
ing needs. The image of dwarfs may indeed indicate a depreciating mockery
of men, as Schafer suggested. The same imagery can also reflect the
patient’s feeling of herself as weakened. This feeling is reflected in her state
of being attended by dwarfs, in the absence of a more suitable selfobject
presence to confirm that the patient as Snow White is, in effect, the fairest
of them all. (This association is mine, not the patient’s or Schafer’s.) This
line of thinking considers the patient’s desire to be the fairest of them all,
as I view it here, not as grandiosity, but rather as simply asking for
admiration or affirmation of herself as worthwhile. The characterization of
Snow White as standing for basically normal expectations of an optimal
level of selfobject responsiveness can represent just as plausible an alter
native to the vain, hysteroid “Southern belle” of Schafer’s ego psychologi
cal interpretation.
Much of the remainder of this patient’s protocol consisted of imagery
such as heavy boots, a stuffed animal skin worked on by a taxidermist, a
rat, beetles, and a dead tree branch. By the time the patient arrived at Card
IX and reported knights in armor, it is easy to imagine that she had described
again and again her experience of an injured self in need of mirroring
selfobject responsiveness, with the suit of armor representing her need to
protect her brittle sense of self. From this viewpoint, the knights in armor
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 125
hensive System criteria for confabulation, there can be little doubt that
an integration failure has nevertheless taken place. This interpretation
casts a different light on the matter of whether references to budding and
newness represent rejuvenated self states in the same way as in the
previous examples.
The poor quality of the response, in its utilization of affect (C or CF),
form demand, and cognitive organization, may compromise this view
about the self state. The response should at least cause examiners to
consider comorbid psychopathology. Schafer pointed out that the re
sponse comes as a surprise because of the emptiness and depression
suggested by the rest of the Rorschach protocol. Thus, examiners must
take into account the possibility of hypomanic denial in evaluating this
response, which provides a useful reminder that the total record, includ
ing the assignment of major scores for form quality, affect regulation,
and cognitive organization, cannot be ignored in the analysis of content.
This point is crucial to guard against the danger of unrestrained interpre
tations considered in isolation of the total record.
In contrast, another response with exuberant affect, but without a
cognitive-affective integration failure, and also to Card X, was reported
by Athey (1986, p. 36):
Martian creatures ... overall, it’s very colorful and conveys to me a feeling
of celebration, even jubilance or flight.... I guess I’m thinking of fireworks
like on the Fourth of July ... like the burst of color and light of fireworks,
and there is an overall feel of lightness and airiness to it as well.
Unlike Schafer’s patient, Athey noted that this potential pure C response
did not inappropriately combine with form-based components, and the
associative process remained task focused. There were no thought dis
order or cognitive integration scores. Thus, there was no apparent per
ceptual-cognitive failure in this response, which permitted an analysis
of the joyful affect unspoiled by a response of poor form quality. This
response is more similar to that of the flower bud of Schafer’s first
patient, but not to the response of the flower bud from the hypomanic
patient just noted.
The contrast between these examples also serves as a useful reminder
that the content analytic approach I am emphasizing throughout this book
follows after formal scoring. It is not my intention to argue that content
analysis should represent the sole or even the primary basis for clinical
interpretation.
128 CHAPTER 5
This fellow has been working in the lab trying to find a new form of
gasoline that will revolutionize the market. After 165 trials he finds it; an
oil company buys it, but the new gas is shelved, and the man realizes ...
that he will have to start over again. He is disillusioned with science which
has no more to offer him. He has given up the idea of benefiting man.
He is a broken man.
Holt did not indicate TAT card numbers, so it is unclear whether this story
precedes the previous story. It is not crucial, however, insofar as the main
point remains unchanged: After much effort, the product of the man’s work
is “shelved,” as are all his hopes for sustaining self-esteem. Unmirrored and
unresponded to with the vitalization he needs to support a buoyant, alive
sense of the self, his ambitions (of science and benefiting mankind) and
self-cohesion (the reference to a broken man) fail to take root. Although not
inconsistent with Holt’s characterization of this TAT story as a lowering of
the ego ideal, Kohut’s enhanced description of a healthy, viable self as a
130 CHAPTER 5
One typical way in which mirroring needs may appear is in the form of
criticism, faultfinding, or arrogant superiority. These represent aspects of
grandiosity and appear sometimes with only a superficial defensive veneer
that barely conceals an underlying aggrandized sense of self. In fact, on
closer analysis, the arrogance appears to mask feelings of diminished
importance. The haughtiness or sense of entitlement is often the surface
manifestation disguising that patients with self disorders can feel deval
ued, sometimes painfully so.
Although Kemberg (1975) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual o f
Mental Disorders (DSM-IV)-fourth edition criteria for narcissistic person
ality disorder accorded entitlement a prominent place among symptomatic
features, Kohut did not emphasize this quality in the self disorders. Kohut’s
preference for directing attention to cold aloofness or arrogance rather than
feelings of entitlement did not minimize the importance of entitlement in
his thinking on the subject, however. Arrogant faultfinding is not seen as
originating from envy, as does the British object relations theoretical
preference or Kemberg’s position on devaluation. Rather, the self psycho
logical emphasis is on vulnerability or self-denigration. Thus, consider a
response such as “two pigs dancing snout to sno ut... or Middle European
peasants dancing in a folk dance—women probably, peasants” as produced
on Card II by a Hispanic man (R M. Lemer, 1988, p. 286) or similar
examples such as content involving “talking down to” or giving “dictation”
to the examiner as in several examples provided by Schafer (1954) and R
M. Lemer (1991). Responses such as these are understood in the self
psychological framework as revealing a patient’s vulnerable, undermined
self-esteem rather than as arising from a primarily aggressive impulse or
from a sadistic or envious introject.
Following an object relations interpretation of psychological dynamics,
R M. Lemer (1991) also considered devaluation as a major clinical phe
nomenon seen on projective tests. Devaluation, from this viewpoint, may
represent envy. Some of Lerner’s examples included distortions of the
human form, such as a sinister-looking figure, a headless figure, evil
witches, or figures from outer space. These examples characterize a self-
image of incompleteness, distortion, or failure. These aspects of the expe
rience of the self are not the same as Kohut’s concept of devaluation. In
Kohut’s view, devaluation appeared as a defensive means of protecting the
132 CHAPTER 5
self from further injury by concealing how the patient experiences compro
mised self-esteem.
The following vignettes are from a 53-year-old depressed woman
executive currently in her third lifetime hospitalization for a suicide
attempt. The patient had a history of flight of ideas and decreased need for
sleep, although there were no clearly documented manic or hypomanic
episodes. Her initial Rorschach response was “a butterfly” (Card I), and
on inquiry she stated:
The dominant thing is the wings, not a perfect rendition of wings. This
thing is not interesting, but calling it a butterfly gives it more validity even
though it’s not perfect, so I gave it its best name. Looking at it now I see
these hands like maybe it’s drowning, but at first I didn’t see that.
Her next response to the same card is a frog, subsequently elaborated on
inquiry by the comment: “Frogs seem like ugly witless things to me,”
which is then followed by a bat, to which she notes, on inquiry, that “it
looks blind, no intelligence there.” This same patient reported two butlers
as her first of five responses to Card III; however her fifth response was
“creatures talking to the butlers, they seem to know what they’re doing,
though, not like that woman we just drew, sort of a dumb ox” [the human
figure drawings preceded the Rorschach]. She began her inquiry to this
response by commenting: “It’s like you’d just better do it right, you fellows
[referring to the creatures talking to the butlers], this is a very important
affair so don’t screw up.”
Responses characterized by haughty arrogance frequently coexist with
other percepts denoting self-depreciation and finding oneself lacking. Thus,
this woman produced percepts such as “antennae of a bat, something very
sensitive, the vital center of the bat, the nerve center. If you destroy that you
destroy the whole thing” (Card V), and “like an animal, perched, balancing”
(Card VII), to which she elaborated on inquiry: “like all this kinetic stuff. I
must be very lacking in fantasy. There must be this big void in me that I’m
not that fanciful.”
Even on Card III (between the butlers and the creatures talking to the
butlers), the patient reported percepts of gnomes and “a decoration to fill in
the picture. I could say it looks like a tie but that’s not very interesting ...
it’s not doing anything, just hanging there.” One final example from this
same patient’s record is a response to Card IV of “dry leaves, very thin and
curled” to which she added in the inquiry: “I see a burst of glory, very
dramatic, wondrous and warm, while everyone else sees it as everything
dying.” This response may provide a good illustration of hypomanic denial.
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 133
It is at the same time a good representation of the lengths to which she may
need to go to preserve a vibrant, buoyant self in the face of a self state more
accurately experienced as diminished.
This patient’s combination of bravado and devaluation extends beyond
the Rorschach. Several instructive illustrations from her TAT and figure
drawings continued the same themes noted on her Rorschach. Thus, elabo
rating on her drawing of a person, this patient stated:
It’s devoid of a projector who could project it well. This poor person is not
attractive and not very interesting and she certainly has no style. That’s a
feminine line there but I don’t know how to make her pretty, she’s sort of
stuck with this funny body. Sort of a stupid expression, a flat, featureless
face: the cortisone look.
By her critical, mocking tone, this patient attempted to distance herself from
this projection of her depreciated self. Her response was consistent with the
previously noted Rorschach percepts in which the patient attempted to stand
above her percepts, such as an uninteresting tie or an imperfect butterfly.
Her effort was not entirely successful, as in her remarks about a void in
herself so that she was not “fanciful” in her imagination. In the projective
drawing, she tried to avoid identifying with the person who was unattrac
tive, but her only recourse was to fault the projector who is not able to project
the figure well. On Card 1 of the TAT, the patient told the story of a boy
who is told to play the violin but “he couldn’t care less, so he pulls out the
violin in both boredom and exasperation, he puzzles how he can be expected
to get anything out of this contraption.” Her outcome was that the boy breaks
the violin “with his hands in his pocket and a shrug of defeat ... although
the mother is disappointed.”
After inquiring about the mother’s disappointment, the patient changed
her tune: She concluded her story with the boy “dejected” rather than the
previous defensive shrug of defeat. Once again, and by now on three
different projective tests, this patient showed that a defensive bravado,
critical arrogance, or distancing herself from shortcomings readily evoked
feelings of shame or disparagement. Her vulnerable self-esteem was repeat
edly seen through images of diminished intellect, attractiveness, or talent.
Schafer (1954) reported the Rorschach records of two patients in which
a similar dynamic appears to be evident. One patient’s (Repression, Case
3, p. 213) response to Card III is that of two little men, about which Schafer
noted the condescension, but which he also conceptualized from the drive
theory position of oedipal rivalry or masculine strivings. Similarly, another
134 CHAPTER 5
patient’s (Denial, Case 1, pp. 255-268) response to Card I was the sinister-
looking image of a figure with a small head, big body, and “short hands
lifted up like this, as though he needed elongation for strength ... with no
mentality: that’s why you could go back almost to prehistoric ages with it.”
On Card III, the same patient reported dead limbs of trees, “an old hobo ...
gives the impression of rags and tatters,” and a monkey hanging from a tree.
Commenting on these responses, Schafer considered the reference to
the male figure seeking strength on Card I as the patient’s unstable
conception about masculinity and devaluation of the male figure. He noted:
“She may be unsure whether masculinity implies strength or weakness, i.e.,
whether to be awed by it or to sneer at it” (Schafer, 1954, p. 256). He
continued in this vein by interpreting the hobo-caricature figure as an
indication that men are ineffectual failures. Schafer included the dead limb
response as a further representation of masculine deterioration and the
inadequacy of men as providers. Clearly, although devaluation by no means
went unnoticed in Schafer’s interpretive remarks, these themes are cast
squarely in a drive theory framework. From this perspective, oedipal
rivalry is superimposed on oral hostility in the patient’s complaints about
the inadequacy of men as nurturant providers.
Schafer’s patient continued to deliver responses of a similar nature, with
percepts such as “little gnomes” and a “horrible old man” (Card V), a “weird
figure” and “the Old Man of the Mountains carved out in the rock, not by a
good sculptor” (Card VI), “the face of a baboon” (Card IX), and “a child’s
drawing ... but the house is too small” and “an old telegraph pole that was
partly cut down and grayed with age; not painted the way modem ones are”
(Card X). The devaluative content and tone of many of these responses
revealed this patient’s preoccupation with underpowered figures. In this re
gard, the projected aspects of her feeling concerned her own ineffectualness.
This same patient displayed indications of idealizing selfobject functions
alongside her responses with a devaluing tone. I return later to the clinical
significance of mirroring and idealizing selfobject functions coexisting in the
same person. For the moment, it is sufficient to note that these selfobject
functions need not be mutually exclusive. Indeed, the relative balance between
them and the capacity for one selfobject function to achieve self-esteem
regulation more successfully than another are further important considerations
in the interpretation of projective test findings.
In regard to Schafer’s patient, Kohut’s view would not ignore the possibility
that men are seen as incompetent, although the dead tree limb, the hobo in rags
and tatters, and the figure with the small head in need of elongation need not
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 135
and defiant, is listening to papa but it is too late. He is sent to a prison term ...
for further details, see page three.” Schafer certainly noted the “sarcasm and
detachment concerning human involvement and morality” (p. 125). It is clear,
however, that the central interpretive points were “intense fear of destructive,
authoritarian punishment for the aggressive, sexual, and oral aspects of oedipal
strivings, and fear of being cut off from maternal supplies, of which he feels
very much in need” (p. 127).
The focus of a self psychological interpretation is rather different. The
grandiose bravado is a thinly disguised smokescreen attempting to conceal the
injury from an unavailable, unresponsive, or unattuned selfobject milieu. The
story’s description of an understanding father whose advice arrives too late to
help the boy betrayed the absence of a selfobject environment in which
normal exhibitionistic strivings were tolerated and affirmed. Injuries resulting
from momentary failures might have been eased by timely, empathically
responsive calming, which might have forestalled the patient’s need to develop
such hypermetabolized grandiosity and sarcasm. The patient’s need to depre
ciate much of what he came in contact with preserved his faltering self-esteem.
It should be apparent from all these illustrations that grandiose images are
closely related to depreciation. On suitably vigorous inquiry, these grandiose
responses readily expose the self injury that undermines a cohesive, firmed-
up mental structure. The inferential strategy assumes that a reference to the
condition of an object seen in a Rorschach percept alters the nature of the
response itself in a psychologically important way.
The same observation is true of descriptions of the mental or physical state
of a character in a TAT story or of a human figure, house, or tree drawn on
command. The description does not refer only to an object with good or poor
fit to the form demand of the inkblot, and the perceptual function is not seen
as the primary task. The elaboration about the condition of the object or person
is taken as a projection of some aspect of the patient’s internal state. In
particular, the experience of the self is crucial insofar as it is felt to be
cohesive, vigorous, or able to withstand threat. The predominant selfobject
need is typically one in search of a potentially available or responsive
selfobject to repair defective mirroring.
woman changed when she was queried about what she meant by the man
whom she drew as unattainable. She commented: “Girls would have to feel
like you have to get someone, something I always felt I’d never attain. He’s
looking away, not interested in what I’m doing.” It is of more than passing
interest that her devalued self-esteem came forward in her elaboration of
the male drawing. This response casts doubt on her original statements
about the female drawing. This skepticism about the healthy self-image
based on the female drawing would not have arisen in the absence of the
additional comment that emerged after a thorough inquiry about the male
drawing. This patient reported a number of other responses that revealed
the same self state so that the interpretation is not tied to a single response.
The patients who can conceal deep internal states are precisely the patients
who require special efforts to provocatively reveal what an examiner needs
to understand about this level of the personality.
The TAT continued these concerns about failure, measuring up, and
attempting to assert her own wishes: The boy of Card 1 “failed to do as
well as he should have,” the girl of Card 2 “feels left out because she’s
obviously not part of the more simple farm life,” and the character of
Card 3BM is “like a puppet who fell. All her strings got cut. In the end,
she stands up again.” The predominant imagery of failure and isolation
is also associated with the feeling that “her mother is not into what the
daughter is doing” (Card 2) and “the girl wants to do something but the
mother is hovering over her being overclawing” (Card 7GF). These
responses suggest deficient mirroring of the patient’s autonomous
wishes. The mother’s selfobject failures, lacking in empathic responsive
ness as these appear to be, contributed to the patient’s concern about
measuring up. Although a few of these elaborations were supplied
spontaneously by the patient, just as many appeared only after a follow-
up question. It is well worth repeating that it takes time to conduct a
thoughtful, careful inquiry.
The following examples are taken from the Rorschach of a 16-year-old
girl whose parents were divorced; she had been hospitalized following a
suicide attempt precipitated by her father’s recent remarriage. In each
instance, the patient was asked for an association by way of testing the limits
after the scoreable response features were obtained in the standard manner
of administration. On Card I, the patient reported “a dog, a Brittany spaniel,
a puppy,” to which she added upon prompting after the formal inquiry for
determinants: “Looks dead or sick or unhappy, just kind of there, no
emotion.” On Card II, she responded with “a big diamond in the middle,
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 139
This section addresses the matter of drawing inferences about self states,
particularly those arising out of defective mirroring. I discuss differentiating
between interpretations derived from self psychology and ego psychology
by using projective test content and begin with a discussion of some
problems about how mirroring may be accurately identified.
Mirroring
The first example illustrates how a self psychological explanation of a
Rorschach response may be misapplied. This vignette is taken from one of
the few reports in the published literature about a self psychological
approach to projective testing. It provides a particularly good illustration of
the need to exercise care in clinical interpretation. At the same time, this
report by Amow and Cooper (1988, p. 58) contributes several excellent
examples of a self psychologically informed interpretive approach.
Amow and Cooper (1988) provided the following Rorschach response
from Card VII: “Two statues facing each other but without much expression
on their faces.” Their interpretation of this response centered on the formality
and coldness of the statues. They emphasized the patient’s experience of the
mother’s difficulty in providing sufficiently enthusiastic mirroring. Amow
and Cooper suggested that the patient hid behind a controlled exterior. They
also suggested that this response characterized one of Kohut and Wolf’s
(1978) typologies, referred to as the contact-shunning personality.
Amow and Cooper’s (1988) interpretation of this response should be
seen as tentative. Although the interpretation is appealing, the response of
statues without expression does not unambiguously characterize mirroring.
The affectively removed statue figures may well refer to an important aspect
of the patient’s experience of his selfobject environment, however a more
compelling case could have been made if the patient’s verbalization in
volved turning to the statue percept for affirmation that was rebuffed. A
percept of a statue may sometimes connote unavailability when the statue
is specifically referred to as expressionless as in this example. Unavailabil
ity is not, however, the same thing as deficient mirroring, particularly in the
absence of an indication that a need is frustrated. Furthermore, the clinical
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 143
validity and conceptual utility of Kohut and Wolf’s (1978) subtypes have
not been uniformly acknowledged.
This fact does not necessarily weaken the basis for assuming that a
mirroring selfobject need for empathic responsiveness is present. The
percept as it stands, however, is too ambiguous for one to conclude that a
mirroring need is mobilized for the patient at this time. In treatment, the
selfobject need is less certain when it is inferred from its absence. Selfobject
functions are most clear when seen through rage reactions and depression
or depletion anxiety arising from frustration of the selfobject need. These
functions are perhaps best illustrated by Kohut’s (1971) case of Miss F.,
summarized in chapter 1.
The same consideration about inferring selfobject functions through the
absence of a reaction or evidence for disintegration products is applicable
in psychodiagnostic testing situations. The most reliable indications of
faulty mirroring are references to a rebuff of the need for mirroring selfob
ject responsiveness. The response to that rejection (such as somebody or
something’s being depleted, in ruins, or rotting away) becomes crucial for
identifying the selfobject need. Responses based on absence of a reaction,
even stiffness or coldness, are less reliable because they are nonspecific to
mirroring. They may still be accurate, but other substantive evidence is
needed to make a compelling case.
The problem with Amow and Cooper’s example is compounded by
attributing the mirroring deficiency specifically to a maternal figure. Per
haps their reason was that the response occurs on Card VII, but this
gratuitous interpretive link is a well-established error in clinical inference.
Their attribution may be applicable to the patient on the basis of the total
protocol, but as a clinical vignette considered out of context, this example
is ill advised. It is also unclear whether Amow and Cooper regarded the
statue, with its associations of reserve, as an indication of an unresponsive
mother, the patient’s defensive coldness, or both. Finally, Arnow and
Cooper’s (1988, p. 59) view about the patient’s defensive reserve may not
be well-supported by the available evidence. Kohut did not inevitably
imply that a surface veneer of coldness or a controlling manner represents
a defensive transformation of defective mirroring.
In contrast, Amow and Cooper (1988) provided a better reasoned exam
ple of a Rorschach percept of a chandelier coming close to the mark: “Very,
very warm, of the past. Grandmother’s house. To bask in the warm light of
a radiant smile.” This response sustained their interpretive hypothesis of a
mirroring need, represented by the chandelier as something to be admired.
144 CHAPTER 5
contain more than the mere mention of mirrors and mirror images. There
are sufficient elements of an experience in depth with regard to the sense
of the self and its requirement for a mirroring selfobject function to sustain
feeling firmed up.
though not too sad. His eyes are somewhat blank ... reading a book without
any eyes or knowing what is in the book without reading it.”
The boy disregarded the music and fell asleep reading the book. Holt
noted the theme of passive-resistant counteraggression in the boy’s not
studying what the parents wanted him to. He also introduced the theme of
castration based on failure to refer to the violin in the picture. Holt com
mented on the boy’s difficulty in sustaining interest in the adventure story
that he chose to read and felt that his parents would not mind. Holt also
noted that the story concludes in withdrawal, as the boy fell asleep.
Apart from the conflict-defense interpretive framework favored by Holt,
it is also possible to approach this story from the standpoint of self
psychology. In this view, the boy’s seeking an adventure story represents
his attempt to revive an uninvigorated self, in view of the predominant affect
state of being sick, alone in bed, missing school, and assigned an uninter
esting task to study. The absence of the parents in an empathically involved
way cannot go unnoticed. The boy is thus left to his own devices to provide
the needed stimulation to an uninvigorated self in this context of parental
selfobject failure. In the end, the boy’s efforts to revitalize himself ulti
mately fail, he cannot sustain the interest any longer, and he withdraws into
sleep. The deficiency in selfobject mirroring responsiveness can presum
ably account for the boy’s ennui or inner emptiness.
Later, this patient told a story to Card 7BM in which the young man,
depressed by poor health and feeling hopeless, was told by his father to
find the will to cure himself because otherwise the father and mother must
care for the young man’s wife and children. The outcome was that the
young man moved to a healthier climate with his family, but still failed to
recover. His children were old enough to support their mother, and the
young man had no further contact with his father. Holt emphasized the
moralistic position of the father in addition to his guilt and lack of
nurturance. This situation accounted for the son’s feeling that he can resort
to passive rejection and aggression only by discontinuing any further
contact with the father.
From the viewpoint of the psychology of the self, there is hardly a clearer
illustration of empathic unresponsiveness. Not only does the father fail to
notice the son’s distress, but he lectures him about his responsibilities and
further ignores the son’s need by making sure that the son realizes the burden
that the father himself might have to bear. This interpretation about failed
empathic mirroring is not based on whichever aspect of the moral position
152 CHAPTER 5
one addresses, but rather on the father’s responding to the son without any
apparent awareness of the son’s precarious state.
Even more telling, in the present context of diminished expectation of
selfobject availability, there is no indication that the son attempts to per
suade the father to attend to his injured self. The son’s passivity is not so
much a rejection of the father as a reflection of the son’s inability to stand
on his own. He fails to recover from his illness and does not thrive. The
illness of the young man represents the devitalizing self disorder from which
he suffers and reveals compellingly how he has come to expect continuing
and ongoing selfobject failure. Like the previous story in which the boy
withdrew into sleep, the young man of this story withdraws from his
unempathic father who fails to provide the encouragement or mirroring to
revitalize the young man’s faltering self-esteem.
Holt’s patient continued with another TAT story. On Card 13B, he related
a story set in “a very poor farmhouse in an isolated part of the country” in
which the boy’s mother is about to deliver a baby and the boy “has been
told to stay out of the way.” He turned to the father but “since he cannot get
much affection from his father who is too busy working in the fields to
even be kind to him, the boy, in his small mind, comes to the conclusion
that it’s a hopeless case.” At this point, the mother needed the boy’s help,
“which makes him feel very elated and he decides he will never leave home.
He had really done more than his father.”
Holt used the term oedipal fulfillment to refer to the “elated” victory the
boy “in his small mind” ultimately achieved. Feeling needed after the initial
rejection by his parents counteracts the boy’s feeling that he is unloved.
From a self psychological position, however, the boy seems to tell the
examiner that he feels forgotten about and must keep out of his overbur
dened mother’s way. Understandably, patients who feel themselves to be a
burden can hardly expect to experience the gleam in the mother’s eye, one
of Kohut’s favorite metaphors for the developmental origin of normal
mirroring. The fact that this boy recovers so dramatically, notwithstanding
the underlying meaning of his wish to never leave home, points to the level
of selfobject admiration that he craves.
It is of more than passing interest that in the face of deficient mirroring
from one parent, the person experiencing faulty empathic responsiveness
attempts to turn to another potential selfobject. Compensatory structures to
counteract mirroring selfobject failure may emerge in this way. In this
story, unlike the previous two stories from Holt’s patient, the young man
shows some indication of what he seeks in the way of selfobject respon
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 153
Entirely reliant upon themselves and no one was reliant upon them.
Because of the reason that they were alone was also the reason that they
came to their doom. If they had had one line of contact... they could have
perhaps been saved.
of the paper and they took the paper and smashed it together because it’s
the same on both sides” (Card I).
Faultfinding responses such as these reflect patients’ complaints that the
world has “done them wrong” or mistreated them. These patients’ role in
life is to collect grievances that show how they have been slighted. At one
level, they are correct insofar as accurate empathic responsiveness has
been lacking, but correcting wrongs is hardly ever therapeutic, although
the patients’ understanding their feelings of being shortchanged may lead
to a salutary outcome. As for the psychodiagnostic use of projective test
responses of this type, faultfinding complaints are often best understood as
deriving from a history of mirroring selfobject failures.
Other subtle examples of faultfinding may be seen in responses such as
“the shadow of a tree, it has no leaves and no branches, because the color’s
fading here” (Card VIII); “an old bone, the bone is chipped and it’s got little
holes” (Card IX); “a daisy. I don’t like daisies, they look too plain, I like
more color” (Card III). Criticisms or complaints such as these nuances of
projective test content can provide indications about self states that are
otherwise difficult to discern in a clinical interview. Many patients manage
to conceal character pathology through a well-defended and compensated
clinical presentation, but projective test comments about irregularities
suggest the underlying devaluation or self-depreciation. The veiled criti
cism conceals an inner feeling of diminished self-esteem, not unlike the
way that people feel after purchasing store models or “demos.” Although
these products are not necessarily damaged goods, many people still con
sider them less than optimal. Patients with subclinical self disorders feel
this way internally, and this feeling arises from deficient mirroring selfob
ject experiences.
Another subtle feature of projective test content related to mirroring
selfobject experience takes the form of creative, novel, or even playful
responses. Consider, for example, the following responses: “A turtle, looks
like it has little socks on. Maybe turtles get cold feet, too, but the rest of his
body’s kept warm by his shell. Needs to keep his feet warm, too” (Card II);
“a flower, but the stem’s not big enough for the flower. Either the flower
has to shrink or the stem has to get bigger” (Card II); “a butterfly. His wings
are down like he’s tired. He’s been through a lot” (Card V). In the absence
of hypomanic indications, responses of this nature may be only one step
removed from a deep concern for the integrity of the self.
Finally, a need for mirroring may be contained in responses in which an
amotivational state predominates. For example, Schafer (1967, p. 139)
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: MIRRORING 155
reported the following TAT story to Card 20: “Well, it looks like somebody
taking a walk in the park. Maybe he was sitting at home and got tired of
doing nothing and wanted some fresh air and went out for a walk.”
Schafer’s comment about this story centered on the motivational state
of feeling bored and inactive. From the vantage point of the psychology
of the self, the search for fresh air is an indication that the understimulated
self is seeking revitalization to enliven or invigorate itself. Although not
specifically denoting a self state in need of mirroring selfobject responsive
ness, an unsatisfied mirroring need often turns out to be involved in many
patients’ amotivational states.
All these instances deserve note because they portray elements of impor
tant self states, albeit understated and subtly expressed. The self states in
these responses may not often be the most prominent characteristics of
clinical psychopathology, but they are no less important because self-es-
teem regulation is of concern in a broad range of clinical conditions,
including psychotic reactions, mood disturbances, character disorders, and
even normality. Either a self disorder or a muted vulnerability to disturbed
self-cohesion may constitute the central psychopathology.
A disorder of the self can appear as a primary disturbance in narcissistic
personality or behavior disorders. It may also be secondary to other
psychological disorders with a self-esteem disturbance as one important
aspect. In both instances, subtle manifestations of self states can be readily
detected in projective test responses. Psychodiagnostic clinicians may find
utility in being alert to their presence in the content analysis of projective
test material.
6 Clinical Indications
of Selfobject Functions:
Idealization and Twinship
IDEALIZATION
Theoretical Considerations
The second major form of selfobject functions that Kohut (1971, 1977)
elaborated was idealization. I begin this section of projective testing illus
trations of idealization with a summary of the principal conceptual and
clinical features of this selfobject function. The points I have selected to
restate are particularly salient for interpreting the vignettes from the psy
chodiagnostic test material that follows.
Idealization appears clinically in a variety of forms, including admiring
or looking up to others, holding them in awe, or turning to others for
calming strength or to restore equilibrium. The center of greatness shifts
from oneself to an idealized other to whom a child or adult turns with the
hope of bolstering self-esteem. Idealization does not imply attributing
omnipotence or excessive greatness to the idealized selfobject. Thus Ror
schach or TAT responses can describe an admired figure, like a parent or
favored teacher, without attributing exaggerated qualities of greatness to
the figure.
Idealization may ultimately strengthen ideals or values, although the
primary psychological function of idealization is restoring calm or giving
solace. Projective test content referring to soothing or protective figures is
likely to connote idealization, albeit subtly and sometimes silently. A
calming presence is probably more common or clinically relevant for
156
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 157
Clinical Examples
Consider the following example from a 55-year-old photographer-writer
hospitalized for a depressive episode. This episode was precipitated by the
patient’s wife’s recent onset of illness. On figure drawings, his drawing of
a man was elaborated as follows:
The next drawing, that of a woman, was described as the patient’s wife:
[She] tends to feel pretty good about herself, coping with cancer much
better than I am. She’s a professional woman, a speech therapist, she
enjoys her work and manages her job on a professional basis. Her
relationships with her students and fellow therapists are good. She’s
observant, extremely honest, affectionate, and complimentary.
I chose this example to open the clinical illustrations for this section on
idealization because it demonstrates a fundamental premise about what
people seek in an idealized selfobject function. This example, which
contrasts the description of the two figures, is deliberately not the clearest
or most self-evident I could have chosen, because it does not highlight an
exalted person or a quality of exaggerated greatness or importance. Rather,
it illustrates a subtle but crucial point about idealization.
The patient cast himself as floundering and his ill wife as managing
gracefully despite a life-threatening illness. He did not describe his wife
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 159
A phony, he staged the whole thing, he just seemed like a crook, he has
to be something other than what he is, a big facade, like my father. Now
he’s caught, everyone thought he was so great. He seemed like a real nice
man, some softness under the scary image he portrayed.
A girl who wishes she could be a ballerina, her mother reads her a story
about a ballerina, she fantasizes obsessionally about being a ballerina, she
tries on her first pair of shoes and dances as if she had magic shoes and
dreams about a beautiful ballerina she imagines. (Card 7GF)
The story concluded with the girl “sad now, she hates everything about
everything. She’s just depressed, stares a lot.”
As these examples indicate, idealized selfobject needs may appear
alongside expressions of failed responsiveness and sometimes unavailabil
ity or loss. The idealization is frequently revealed to be transitory and
fragile. The selfobject need is clear, but the unavailability or inability of the
selfobject to provide the needed vigor has been transformed into disappoint
ment, depreciation, or rage, as these responses suggest.
The previously discussed patient’s responses are not rare or uncommon,
particularly in those with severe conditions such as borderline personality
disorder. Idealizing selfobject needs frequently appear in the form of their
breakdown rather than in anticipation of being achieved. The failure or
interruption of the selfobject function indicates the presence of the need.
At other times, the idealization is sufficient to sustain the possibility
that the need may be responded to and the idealized figure remains
available. The need for carefully probing inquiry should not be minimized,
both when the idealization is self-evident and when a surface appearance
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 161
A woman meditating so she can deal with the world. She’s getting ready
to go to work, very happy with her job, it’s tedious work, she has a lot of
responsibility, but she gets paid very well. Life is beautiful, but there can
be trouble, it can be empty. Her motivation in life can be pleasant. She’s
not married, has no children, she could have an easy life. She donates her
money to charity to help others.
her very happy and she could do whatever she wants. He’s dominant,
arrogant, self-centered. She’ll look up to him.
coherent theory of personality. Just as the optimal qualities that make for
good psychotherapists have yet to be described, the definitive charac
teristics combining meaningfully sensitive and clearheaded thinking are yet
to be specified for clinicians engaged in psychodiagnostic evaluation.
For the moment and in the present context, I simply comment that the
clinical identification of idealization and twinship selfobject functions with
projective tests poses a particularly challenging test of this dilemma, one far
more difficult to resolve than is the case with the generally more transparent
or easily elicited projective test indications of mirroring selfobject needs.
Other examples from Schafer’s patient included small male lions, an
other temple “from a distance,” still another temple as drawn by a child,
and two torches from the days of Charlemagne or King Alfred as “carried
by a man but the man is not here.” These percepts are consistent with the
impression of misguided attempts to mobilize potentially idealizable sel
fobjects. The patient’s efforts failed to produce a sufficient degree of vigor
to sustain an overwhelmed and vulnerable self state.
In chapter 5 ,1cited examples of defective mirroring by this same patient.
At that time, I commented that the appearance of both mirroring and
idealization selfobject needs was not conceptually inconsistent but can
illustrate the appearance of one type of selfobject function as an attempt to
create a compensatory structure when another selfobject function (usually
mirroring) had failed to bolster an injured self. In the present example,
however, Schafer’s patient provides an illustration of her apparent failure
to secure mirroring and idealization selfobjects to sustain a weakened self.
This idea conveys the seriousness of the self disorder when compensatory
structures fail to take hold or when there is chronic or massive empathic
unresponsiveness in more than one sector of the self. The case of Ms. T.,
reported in detail in chapter 7, also illustrates defective mirroring with
unsuccesful efforts to develop idealizing selfobject functions as a compen
satory structure.
The following example illustrates a sequence of three Rorschach re
sponses to Card VI, in which the middle response apparently represents an
idealizing selfobject need if considered out of context. The sequence of
these responses unambiguously shows that an interpretation of this sort is
incorrect. A 34-year-old female inpatient produced this sequence of re
sponses to Card VI: “A thing, a living object, I’m beginning to get to know
this ink blot better. The head of a snake, a penis,” followed by “it’s a star
now, a six-pointed star,” which was spontaneously elaborated on inquiry
as: “Judaism, the Star of David. My faith is more spiritual versus religious.
I believe in a divine plan but I don’t go to temple.” This patient concluded
Card VI with the percept of “an ass,” which, on inquiry, was seen because
of the appearance of a split that gave rise to an association of constipa
tion. By itself, this patient’s Jewish star percept, with her associative
elaborations about faith and spirituality, suggests mobilization of an
idealization selfobject need. Preceded and followed by the snake-penis
and anality responses, however, this interpretation of the Star of David
response may be implausible.
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 171
There’s something inside of it. A person. She’s in this thing and can’t come
out until someone helps her out. It’s cramped, there’s no room to breathe.
Someone comes by, rubs the lamp and she’s free for a while until she goes
back in the lamp.
The patient elaborated that the woman inside the lamp was sensitive
or hurting and was being punished by being trapped inside the lamp
because she had done something terrible. Apart from the implication of
wrongdoing of potentially delusional intensity, the crucial reference to
idealization, however, pertained to the wish that some magical, benevo
lent figure would emerge to ease her distress.
For the same reason, this patient described “a flying animal” (Card
VI), about which she said upon inquiry: “I guess he’s connected to
something, maybe a bigger butterfly, a mother butterfly or something.
[Connected]1 He needs him for support, he can’t do it by himself.”
This patient’s TAT stories complemented and expanded on the ideal
izing selfobject needs suggested in her Rorschach responses. On Card 1,
the patient described “a little boy that loved to play violin, he played it
wherever he went. One day his teacher said he couldn’t play it anymore
because he wasn’t doing his other work. It made him very sad.” When asked
what sad meant, the patient stated that “he’s trying to understand what she’s
talking about.” Thus, the affect of sadness results from disappointment in
a potential idealized selfobject, represented by the teacher, who has let the
boy down by failing to recognize his need. This interpretation may be more
decisive alongside that of a teacher who is also seen as depriving or punitive.
The teacher need not be seen as great, powerful, or on a pedestal for the
idealizing selfobject function to be present.
This same patient continued with a story on TAT Card 2:
[A] girl. .. loved to go to school but they lived in this house, it needed a
lot of work. The mother was pregnant so she couldn’t do a lot of things.
So the mother made her do chores every day and look after the other kids.
This went on for seven years, then she ran away to the relatives’ house,
and there she obtained the education she needed.
In the next card (3BM), this patient reported that “a child’s been abused.
He’s praying to God to help him stop being abused and to stop his mother
from doing it, but still she abused the child and he just got up and ran away.”
That these stories contain references to abuse or neglect is a matter that
I am sidestepping because it is secondary to understanding the patient’s
depth psychology. These two stories contain clear references to the central
figure’s feeling psychologically ignored, and the self remains unresponded
to in the context of neglect or abuse. Without trivializing or dismissing the
sociological or public policy question of child abuse, I emphasize in the
present context that the more important psychological issue is the patient’s
hope for an idealized figure to come to her aid to restore self-cohesion. The
patient needed the selfobject function of a strong presence to turn to, not so
much for protection as for repairing self-esteem.
These themes continued in the patient’s stories to Cards 7GF and 7BM,
in which the girl of Card 7GF was consoled by a neighbor and the young
man of Card 7BM was consoled by his grandfather. That the idealizing
selfobject function repeatedly failed this patient may be detected in her
resolution to these stories, as well as in the stories cited earlier. Thus, the
girl cannot be reassured by the neighbor because “she learned adults lie ...
she doesn’t trust the neighbor,” and the young man does not believe his
grandfather: “The grandfather may be genuinely concerned about the boy
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 173
but the boy doesn’t know that, he don’t [s/c] know where to go or who to
turn to.”
In his expanded formulation of idealization, Kohut considered idealizing
selfobject functions to include fostering or bringing about a vigorous and
buoyant sense of self-esteem. When sustaining optimal self-esteem is
compromised, TAT responses such as these represent the failure of an
idealizing selfobject need. The patient turns to someone in the hope of the
person’s providing this restorative or rebuilding function, but the need
either goes unrecognized or is rebuffed. The patient then feels abandoned,
ignored, and ultimately diminished. This interpretation is ultimately more
important than are descriptions of figures as strong, exalted, or powerful.
The following case also illustrates the wish for idealizable selfobjects as
seen through either empathic unavailability or failed responsiveness of the
figures to whom the patient turned for repair of his self disorder. The patient
was a 31-year-old male who developed a depressive episode after he had
been discovered in a pattern of chronic lying in which he greatly exaggerated
facts about his education and work. On TAT Card 1, this patient described:
A little boy, he looks frustrated, he can’t understand his instructor and why
he can’t play it. He’s not asking advice from his parents because they think
he should know how to do it, he doesn’t know where to turn. He asks his
parents if he can give it up, and he gives it up, they don’t care.
That little boy is me. Lots of times I’d go in my room and just cry because
I didn’t know what was going on in school that day. I couldn’t go to my
parents. I didn’t think they’d understand, that’s why I left school.
Later on, he commented about Card 7BM: “That’s not our home but it’s a
father giving the son advice. He never showed me how to do things that a
man should know. This man looks like he’s giving the son good advice and
the son appreciates it.”
The noteworthy aspects of these responses concern the predominant need
for an idealizable figure who is portrayed in a position of teaching, advising,
or consoling. In each instance, the sought-after figure failed to fulfill the
idealizing selfobject need that was aroused. This situation predisposes to a
self disorder with a symptomatic expression in depression, characterized
throughout many projective test protocols as devitalization or defeat. It is
not the grandiosity or the prominent wish to be admired, adored, or other
174 CHAPTER 6
wise mirrored that the patient longs for or seeks. The problem illustrated in
these responses is the empathic failure of a figure to serve a selfobject
function of bolstering undermined self-esteem. The idealization selfobject
need remains, therefore, driven underground, thwarted or unrecognized.
The following two Rorschach responses may seem to represent idealiza
tion, but only one of these examples properly reflects this selfobject
function. The case is from two adjacent Rorschach responses to the same
card (Card III) from a 15-year-old adolescent, hospitalized in connection
with his unmarried mother’s inability to manage his behavior at home and
school. The first response was “two little spirits,” which this young man
elaborated in the inquiry as:
Conan the Barbarian. They put all kinds of paints on him to keep the spirits
from taking him to wherever he was going to go in his next life, heaven,
like the utopia, where he’s destined to go. They loved him, didn’t think he
deserved to die yet. This girl sacrificed herself to the gods so that he’d live.
They’re like misty, like coming up out of their bottle, and form into some
humanistic thing as they go up their body. Some magical type of thing or
person, another type of being, almost like from another world or some
thing, like this fantasy kind of thing. They’re almost like gods. Something
that doesn’t conform is a type of god, mythologically. These don’t look too
honest, trying to cheat at cards, neither one likes to lose.
Contrasted with the previous response from this adolescent boy, his
percept of a genie seems to suggest that the patient may admire the figure
for its ability to cheat. The genie is also couched in imagery that the patient
described as magical and god-like in quality, unlike his previous spirits
percept in which he had turned to them for calming reassurance or vitaliza-
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 175
tion of the self. His response is also different from the genie response of
another patient (cited previously), in which someone helping her was the
crucial aspect bearing on idealization.
These two responses from the adolescent boy highlight that his responses
required attention to the verbalization that distinguished them with regard to
the specific or unique idealizing selfobject function that each response identi
fied. The spirits provided a self-restorative function but the genies were not
seen as needed in a similar way. They might be, and the possibility of
idealization should not be ruled out entirely. Nevertheless, the contrast between
these two responses in respect to the selfobject function is still instructive.
TWINSHIP
Theoretical Considerations
The twinship selfobject function is the most difficult to discern both clini
cally and on psychodiagnostic testing. Kohut (1971, 1984) originally con
sidered twinship to be a subtype of mirroring, but in his final thoughts on the
subject, he regarded twinship as a special function deserving its own status.
The difficulty in identifying twinship selfobject needs is not that of
detecting possible signs or indicators such as responses related to twin
figures or symmetrical objects. These indications are easy to note on
projective tests. The problem, however, is in deciding whether superficial
references of this sort truly represent twinship selfobject needs. There is no
lack of Rorschach percepts about twins or symmetry of the inkblots. Even
TAT stories have references to perceived or fantasied twin relationships.
The overriding consideration must always be one of determining whether
a selfobject function is present. To be judged as present, there must be an
indication that the selfobject exists fundamentally to sustain or bolster the
cohesiveness of the self.
The rarity of this selfobject function is one reason that Kohut’s use of the
term twinship leads to confusion, especially when interpreting projective
test content. The many references to twins that arise, particularly on the
symmetrical Rorschach blots, can more often mislead than amplify Kohut’s
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 177
Clinical Indications
It just is. [Q] I guess they are. They’re menstruating. How long have you
been a psychologist? [Q] They’re friends and they know what’s going on,
they’re combining ideas, they’re giving each other the same idea. [Q] They
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 179
understand each other’s life experiences, they’re very happy. [Q: Menstru
ating?] It’s just about their sexuality. Actually, it’s not menstruation, it’s
more like the feeling they share. Oneness.
Siamese twin animals, like puppy dogs. I can see their snouts. They’re cute.
They’re joined at the nose. They could be thinking the same thing. These
are maybe thought waves. They could be kissing. There’s a smile.
Here’s the feet, body, ears, eye, snouts. These snouts have a horned effect
which I don’t want to look at, because it makes them look like they’re not
real, like from a fairy tale. They’re cute. [Q: Joined?] They’re probably very
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 181
the symmetry of the inkblots. I close this section with an illustration of two
responses that demonstrate this point. These Rorschach percepts described
twins but lacked the psychological quality of alikeness; as a result, the
responses cannot by themselves represent twinship selfobject functions.
The first response, to Card II, was: “A couple of bears fighting things
out. My twin brother and myself.” A second example was given to Card
VII by the same patient: “Female twins. Probably in the kitchen, using a
microwave oven for the first time.”
Neither response contained inquiry material revealing anything of note
beyond the standard information about location and determinants. These
two responses might well have been viewed as examples of twinship before
the discussion in this chapter, but examples like these do not meet the crucial
test. The responses require a more urgent quality of alikeness, shared
similarity, or bonding as an exact or faithful replica to genuinely represent
the in-depth sense of twinship that Kohut had in mind. The content must
give clear indications beyond a reference to identical or twin objects.
Because of this requirement, the twinship selfobject function is seen only
infrequently in psychodiagnostic testing.
This quality provided a potent psychological moment in the New York
stage production of Side Show, a musical about the autonomous but tragi
cally conflicted strivings of a pair of real-life conjoined twins. The twins
realized that their separate and different aspirations could not possibly
succeed if they were to remain as one consolidated living unit. Indeed, their
ultimate strength was their synchrony as a unit. Despite this rebuff to the
twins’ individual hopes, their immutable need for each other provided the
strength that sustained them psychologically. Here is the essential quality
of Kohut’s twinship selfobject function: It is not being similar or identical
that really matters, but the mutuality of need or psychological oneness.
Similarly on the Rorschach: Percepts of twins or conjoined twins must
clearly indicate mutuality in the need for the twin for the person’s psycho
logical survival or sustenance (that is, self-cohesion).
Their hands in the background, heads, like sort of contemplating. [Q] Just
their face. I do that with my friends sometimes. Just for fun. We already
know each other’s thoughts, but sometimes we’ll put our hands together
to try to understand it better. And we’re usually right, that’s because we
understand each other so well.
This is Sandra and she has a very close relationship with her girlfriend
Sally, but they’re not sexual. They understand each other so well, that
could best be understood as like one-minded. She begins to depend on
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 185
Sally too much as the only person who can understand her, so she
deliberately cuts off from her other friends to be with Sally. But Sally has
other friends too and doesn’t want to devote her time purely to Sandra,
and she confronts Sandra and tells her she can’t deal with Sandra being
too dependent on her and says she’s sorry but she can’t be friends with
her anymore. Sandra gets depressed and contemplates suicide; there’s
the gun. She’ll shoot herself because only her friend could understand her
instead of anyone else and she latched on to that. They’re two distinct
people but they think the same. That’s what she needs, a feeling of total
understanding.
It’s Jane and her mom. Jane doesn’t have any friends so she holds on to
her doll and pretends it’s a real person. One day her mother told her she
didn’t think she should use the doll as a friend. Jane said she didn’t want
any friends. One day the mother threw the doll away, Jane got angry with
her mother and said she’d run away. She forgave her mother because on
the way she met a new girl who’s now her best friend.
This example is more subtle. The story is somewhat uncommon for this
card, but is not particularly unusual. The notion of using a doll to compen
sate for lack of friends or inner emptiness is not exceptional. Holding onto
the doll with such exclusivity or for dear life, as it were, suggests that the
doll has come to substitute for a friend or has become (in an intimate, vital
sense), her friend. It raises the question that a twinship selfobject need may
be operating in maintaining this adolescent patient’s self-esteem. Although
the story also suggests regressive elements, it reveals a capacity to replace
a detached, inanimate relationship with a real one. Clinicians must wonder
about the nature of the selfobject need that predominates in that relationship.
This example is not as evident an indication of a twinship selfobject need
as is the previous example of the boy putting his hand against that of his
friend to intensify their level of intimacy. Nevertheless, I have included this
girl’s story alongside that of the boy to highlight the exclusivity that is
expressed. The story represents an important indicator of a twinship selfob
ject need. It also shows, as in the previous examples of the depressed adult
patients, that a range of certainty about the presence of twinship selfobject
functions in projective test responses is frequently more evident than is the
case with other selfobject functions. This range of confidence about the
186 CHAPTER 6
This girl found out her best friend died. She thought she couldn’t live
without her best friend. She’s thinking of killing herself. But instead, she
cut her hair so she’d look ugly and always be left alone by others. She finds
another best friend and is happy again. [Q] They were close, they told
each other everything, she didn’t have anybody else she could count on.
Her friend did everything for her, like a mother.
The mother has shocking news for the son. His father was killed in the war.
The son can’t see how he’d live without his father who he’s admired
through all the years. He finds a woman to take the father’s place.
The second illustration from this patient was her story to TAT Card 1:
Looks like my older son when he was taking Suzuki violin lessons. He hated
it. The first thing he said was I don’t want to do that, and someone said it
was what I wanted to do and not him, and I shouldn’t have forced him. I
wanted to play the violin, my father does, he’s very accomplished, he’s
very perfect. I’m more slipshod, I don’t like doing things perfectly, it would
be too much of a strain because everything has to be done rush, rush.
[Patient continues in this hypomanic digression until asked for the out
come to her story.] He doesn’t take any more violin lessons and he’s glad
and he’s happy. He’s glad he could finally voice an opinion.
188 CHAPTER 6
In these two examples, the need for the selfobject to function as a faithful
replica resembles twinship, but there is some question whether this inter
pretation is accurate. In earlier examples of the patients discussed pre
viously, the twinship selfobject function was characterized by a quality of
kinship or a close bond as the basis for the selfobject responsiveness sought
by those patients. In the present examples, the two figures were seen as
exactly alike, as if they were the same person. The wish for shared
alikeness or similarity is more than a perception of resemblance based on
looking alike or thinking the same way. It seems to run deeper than surface
similarity and is close to the core of the experience of the self. No matter
that the patient dismissed her selfobject need as silly or trivial; that
problem is different and suggests that she might feel unworthy, ashamed,
or undeserving. More to the point, however, is the question of whether the
response expressed the possibility of admiring another person without
having to become that person.
Herein lies the difficulty in the differential diagnosis: Is this response
essentially or primarily a twinship selfobject function, or is it a need for a
level of selfobject responsiveness based on idealization to buttress self-co
hesion? The answer to this question requires the full projective protocol to
decide the clinical issue accurately. Rarely, if ever, does a single response
contain a sufficiently clear or unambiguous basis for deciding among
various possibilities. The issue of a twinship or idealization selfobject
function is the critical question that an examiner must consider by using the
entire protocol to decide the matter and to determine how to view this
patient’s first response.
The patient’s second response suggests a vicarious experience of at
tempting to enhance self-esteem. Vicarious gratifications or wishes on
projective tests are not rare. They usually imply some degree of using
another person in the service of fulfilling an unmet need, thereby sustaining
self-esteem. Sometimes, the narcissistic extension is seen as exactly like
the patient (the faithful replica). It may also appear closely bound up with
the patient’s need to achieve a goal by imitation or by becoming a duplicate
of someone else. Both mechanisms should be considered to represent
potential indications of twinship when the heart of the experience is based
on the need for merging with the selfobject.
The patient’s response in which her son was expected to become an
accomplished violinist in the way that her father was but that she could not
achieve comes close to meeting the criterion of shared similarity. In the end,
however, it falls short because the focus of the response is the patient’s
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 189
failed expectation of herself rather than the compelling need for her son to
be like her or like what she wished herself to become. The vicarious
experience must go beyond someone who does something for the patient.
The selfobject must in some central or deeply felt way embody or be the
same as oneself. This experience should not be confused with merging or
loss of self-other boundaries, as occurs in transient psychotic states.
It is always possible that this patient did indeed hope that her son, by
taking up the violin, would become an extension of herself or that his need
would parallel her own need. Through the son, the patient might earn her
father’s admiration, despite the story outcome in which the son managed to
discontinue lessons and the patient allowed him his wish. Her hypomanic
digression in the midst of her delivery of the response adds further uncer
tainty as to where she might have gone with her story. The answer to the
clinical question at hand remains uncertain; the questions I have posed,
however, illustrate the clinical issue that examiners must decide about this
response as an accurate representation of twinship or some other selfobject
function that has become mobilized.
The patient’s primary concern centered on her own failure as she went
off the point of her story about the son, but her story does not afford an
examiner a clear indication about the central selfobject function. On the
basis of this single response, even in light of the patient’s Rorschach
response noted previously, the most conservative approach to interpreting
Card 1 does not rest on a twinship selfobject function. The possibility of
twinship is worth noting, but the ultimate basis for an interpretation of this
type requires scrutinizing the entire protocol for more persuasive evidence
to sustain the idea of a twinship need.
The following example illustrates that a projective test response signify
ing a particular selfobject function prepares the way for understanding other
test responses. These subsequent responses might not otherwise have led to
a particular interpretation of a selfobject function. The example is from the
projective testing of a 44-year-old male anesthesiologist hospitalized with
an acute depression. This patient produced an agitated Rorschach protocol,
beginning with “two priests holding up their hands in some kind of bene
diction” (Card I), which he elaborated further on inquiry by stating that
“benediction is wishing well, it makes me feel good, part of a community,
connected with others.”
This same patient made a figure drawing of a man that he described thus:
190 CHAPTER 6
[A] high school student, with girls and cars on his mind, he probably
works part time at McDonald’s or some such place. He’s very much
worried about his self-image, what he looks like, he just wants to look like
everyone else, to be just one of the guys.
He also made a female drawing of someone “ten years older than him, out
of high school, happily married, responsible, looking forward to sharing a
life together. Most people that age are optimistic that everything will work
out just fine.”
The Rorschach response illustrated here is consistent with a twinship
selfobject need surrounding the desire for kinship and the calming strength
that this provides. As in several of the examples previously considered, a
reference to twins, identical figures, or even similarly appearing figures is
not essential for the identification of the twinship selfobject need. The crucial
characteristic in this response is the reference to belonging to a community
from which he derived a sense of human connection, regardless of the
reference to the priests. This response prepares the way for understanding
the two figure drawing responses in a way that was not otherwise apparent.
Without this patient’s response about priests communing, the examiner
would very likely consider an interpretation of the male drawing as being
centered on regression, immaturity, or insecure identity when a middle-aged
adult, who is a successful physician, appears to identify with a teenager
working at McDonald’s preoccupied with girls and cars. This impression is
pronounced in view of the sharp contrast between this patient’s drawing of
an adolescent boy and that of a mature woman undertaking expected adult
responsibilities. Understanding this patient’s twinship selfobject need, de
rived from the Rorschach response, provides an insight about that aspect of
the figure drawing inquiry pertaining to the boy’s desire to be accepted by
his peers. This interpretation offers another aspect to consider, as an alterna
tive to or alongside that of a regressive pull or a problem in achieving mature
adult identification.
One can readily see this man’s difficulty as his struggle to sustain a
twinship selfobject need. His struggle addresses his need for a meaningful
connection with others, a connection through which he could draw on this
resource to feel at one with or at home with the people in his surroundings.
As a result, self-esteem regulation and the repair of an injured, undermined
self can be strengthened by needed twinship selfobject wishes. From this
viewpoint as well, the figure drawing of the older mature woman meeting
adult responsibilities that he felt incapable of facing also suggests the
CLINICAL INDICATIONS: IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 191
192
MS. T.: MIRRORING 193
work are noted briefly, if at all, to keep the focus of the clinical illustrations
centered on the insights derived from Kohut’s formulations.
Ms. T. was a 30-year-old White, single, female inpatient in her first
lifetime psychiatric hospitalization. She was hospitalized to treat a depres
sive episode with prominent suicidal ideation, accompanied by associated
problems of decreased sleep, significant weight gain, bulimia, and disso
ciation. Ms. T. had trouble concentrating on her work when she was
employed as a production coordinator in charge of publishing an industry
periodical. She felt herself excluded from social interactions with co-work
ers. The patient had a college education and had no previous psychiatric
history. At the time of diagnostic psychological testing, the patient was
maintained off psychotropic medications.
Let me start over:All we have is clothes with no person yet. I always knew I
didn't have a very good body concept. A very icky hair style. The only thing
that’s sturdy about this whole thing is the outfit. She’s a real disaster. [Why
is that?]1 Because I don’t know how to fix it any better. As long as you said
it’s okay, it’s your mess. Nothing’s real clear, it’s very sketchy, nothing’s real
organized here. Everything’s out ofproportion, everything’s awful. And ugly.
[Awful?] The fingers aren’t right. I know how to make fingers. [Describe the
personality] It’s like a zombie. No expression on her face. [Doing?] On this
side, she’s leaning on a rail, holding on to something, she can’t support
herself So she doesn’t have everything fall apart and collapse.
(at least in the patient’s eyes, because the drawing itself was not seriously
compromised) was accompanied by her perception of the figure as out of
proportion. Her response revealed a self state as devalued as the “awful and
ugly” representation that Ms. T. described.
The “zombie” reference to the personality, together with the figure’s
difficulty in supporting herself to prevent her from collapsing, suggests
inner depletion, diminished self-worth, and a mirroring deficit even though
there is no specific reference to failure to provide affirming or comforting
sustenance. Such indications appeared later; a markedly devitalized self
state such as this verbalization implies that insufficient mirroring is in
volved in producing a self state so devalued.
A depleted self state expressed in this way suggests that the absence of
a sense of the self as vigorous or buoyant results from an inadequately
responsive selfobject environment. The patient may struggle to conceal the
self state from others because “the only thing that’s sturdy about this whole
thing is the outfit.” Thus, what the world sees externally barely concealed
the internal state of disrepair that is not “sturdy.”
This description of an injured self is just the beginning, however. Kohut
would argue that it is also important to make sense of how the patient went
about repairing the devaluation that was experienced so acutely. Through
understanding selfobject needs, one can understand how the patient at
tempted to restore the enfeebled self. In continuing to analyze the ongoing
clinical material, it is crucial to bear in mind that Kohut’s understanding of
the self as struggling to preserve its vitality extends beyond simply charac
terizing the phenomenology, such as that in the previous description. In this
clinical example, the salient question becomes not so much one of identi
fying the self state (which is already intuitively obvious), but rather of
discovering where the patient turns to replenish what appears to be miss
ing. This level of understanding requires reconstructing what went wrong
with the selfobject environment that faltered for the patient.
Considered in this light, Ms. T. appeared not to seek out anyone beyond
herself. The best she could manage was to steady her footing by leaning
against a rail to prevent herself from “falling apart and collapsing.” At first,
she revealed her own underpowered abilities, and there was no indication
of her anticipating that her distress could be empathically understood, let
alone of expecting that any need for support or cohesion could be accurately
mirrored. It is not that the patient was depleted and therefore bereft of
resources. Rather, it is a question of what selfobject resources were poten
tially available to Ms. T., and what did these provide, or fail to provide, to
MS. T.: MIRRORING 195
bolster her devalued self state. The way that this patient made use of her
selfobject milieu is a continuing focus of this diagnostic study.
Ms. T.’s second human figure drawing, a person of the opposite sex, was
described as follows:
Neither one of my people has very good hands. I’ll just eliminate the problem
because I’m going to put this guy’s hand in his pocket. I’d do these stereotypi
cal things like a tie, which is too wide, a shirt and trousers. [Describe his
personality.] Probably a professional person, he looks like a preppie. He thinks
he’s going to decide what he’s going to do and do it. He’s got big feet so he
must be pretty well-established. [Hou/s that?] Because he’s standing pretty
firmly on the ground. Since you said draw someone of the other sex, I gave
him a beard since not many women have beards.
Her playfulness about the beard and her handling of what she considered
the problem of concealing his hand by drawing it in the pocket are probably
ways to appear engaging. Yet, the defensive solution of eliminating the
offending body part (deficient hand) is transparent. Ms. T. seemed to feel
that there were more options for repairing the drawing of the male figure
than were available for her drawing of the female (by inference, of course,
representing herself). This second drawing depicted a man who was deci
sive, sure of himself, and firmly rooted—the very picture of healthy
self-esteem by anyone’s definition. The experience of herself in her pre
vious drawing as enfeebled, unable to overturn an injured self state, and
with no resources besides a “rail” to keep her from “collapsing” contrasted
sharply with the surefooted representation of the male figure.
The patient’s second drawing may also indicate that a potential revitali
zation of the self was possible. Ms. T.’s sense that there was a way out and
that one need not hold on to a rail for dear life implied at least a possibility
that she could turn somewhere to prevent collapse and to find the means by
which the self could become revitalized as a secure structure.
scores are not provided, to emphasize the augmentation that content analy
sis provides. Thus, the self psychological viewpoint is intended to be an
addition to the main formal analysis of scores, ratios, and cluster interpre
tation strategies, to provide an expanded conceptual view.
Card I
1. An angel leading a chorus, Maybe because Christmas is com
but she lost her head. (W) ing up. Trying to get everyone to
sing together so it turns out right.
2. Two elephants drinking out Two ears, the tail, their nose. It
o f a fountain. (W) looks like Dumbo. [Dumbo?]
They're cute little pink elephants. I
think Dumbo couldfly, he could go
wherever he wanted. Actually he
was afreak and he had to get out of
there.
3. A Christmas bell and Christ The white is the branches. Every
mas tree. (D4) body's supposed to be happy.
Card II
4. Two people dancing. Here's They have on kimonos, like in Fan
their heads and their hands tasia. [Q] It's magical, having
touching. (D6) everything come to life and match
the music. Everything!s well organ
ized and orchestrated.
5. A top in the middle. (DS5) How it's shaped here. I had one
when I was little.
6. Or a rocket with the jet pro A jet blasting off. All the smoke and
pulsion. (DS5 + D3) debris. The rocket's off to places
unknown. [Rocket?] No one ever
thought rockets would be blasting
off. You never know what will hap
pen. Maybe like the one E. T. came
in.
7. A seamstress' model. This A sketch for a dress. It's all ready
manikin doesn’t have a head. for somebody to take it, for some
A very long neck. It's a pretty thing real special. A special occa
dress. Mainly to show the sion for a special person. It would
dress off. (DS5) have to be for a skinny person, so
it's not for me.
8. A monument in Arizona: a Like in the artfairs. A desert scene.
monolith. Very pretty colors. [Pretty colors, a desert scene?] It's
It looks like a Navajo paint- foreboding, isolated. Bleak.
ing. (D2)
198 CHAPTER 7
The responses to Card II are very productive. Evidently, Ms. T.’s percep
tual processes and imagination were compellingly engaged here, particularly
by the center white space, because three of her five responses emphasized
the DS5 area of the blot. Beginning with a percept about festive dancing and
ending in a bleak desert scene, Ms. T. revealed wide oscillations of affect.
These could easily be considered to reflect the lability of a borderline
disorder or, in a previous diagnostic era, hysteria. Nevertheless, the diagnos
tic issue is a separate question from that of the psychodynamic interplay of
conflict, anxiety, and defenses. A self psychological view of labile affect is
understood as indicating that the self is in a state of unstable cohesion. This
view reveals as much about defective selfobject responsiveness as about the
diagnostic condition giving rise to this degree of affect dysregulation.
From this vantage point, Ms. T.’s percept of people dancing with their
hands touching evoked her desire for a stabilized self, “well-organized and
orchestrated.” The “magical” fantasy reference could partially stem from
her final response to Card I (a Christmas tree), as a continuation of the hope
for securing a revitalized self. Like the succeeding response of a top and its
association to a childhood memory, this percept of well-organized dancing
can be seen as a wish for a vulnerable self to be repaired. This view is at
variance with a typical interpretation of regressive wishes.
The theme of her need for restoring the self persisted, despite her third
response about a rocket blasting off and leaving a trail of debris and smoke.
The association on inquiry was not to falling apart or destruction; rather,
she marveled about the achievements of space science. This response
repeated the wish to restore a sense of calming stability. (It is an error to
automatically equate an image of debris and smoke with an interpretation
of a distintegration product.) The desire for stabilization holds even though
it is experienced with the child-like, wondrous appeal of the familiar
benevolent character of E. T.
This same sequence of responses might well be viewed differently by
followers of drive theory; they would emphasize the extensive defensive
layering to contain the hostile images of a rocket creating debris. The view
of self psychology, however, is that interpretations about aggression or rage
are misguided. The ego psychological view fails to take full account of the
patient’s expression of a self that she attempted to preserve as viable or
animated. This patient sought to restore some measure of calmness in the
aftermath of a disruption to self-cohesion as inferred from her association
to debris. It is not the rage that is fundamental, at least in Kohut’s view; the
rage represents a byproduct of the failed attempt to preserve self-cohesion
MS. T.: MIRRORING 199
when the self state is threatened. Ms. T. was not blowing up; she has been
blown away.
Her response of a seamstress’ model of a headless manikin that displayed
a pretty dress “for a special person ... not for me” began a downward spiral
from her earlier percepts. These had included optimistic wishes such as
dancing in a magical setting, a top like one she had as a child, and a
wondrous appreciation of rocketry and the benevolent imagery of E. T.
Later, she failed to see herself as the intended owner of the pretty dress and
was no longer the special person she might wish to be; ultimately she ended
with the self state conveyed in the isolation of the bleak desert scene. As
Ira Gershwin wrote in the song lyric “They’re writing songs of love but not
for me”, so, too, does Ms. T. convey this sentiment by her comment that the
dress was for someone special “but not for me.”
Ms. T.’s capacity to present a stable, invigorated self continued to waver,
as already noted on her figure drawings and on Card I. Indeed, by the end
of Card II, little was left but bleak isolation of an unstimulated self as
represented by the desert monolith percept. Even her attempt to introduce
“very pretty colors ... like in the art fairs” failed to work well for her. Her
inquiry response was dominated by the desolation suggested by this percept.
Whether hypomanic denial or an indication of her symptomatic dysphoria,
the response pointed to the difficulty that Ms. T. experienced in her effort
to preserve a consistent, ongoing level of self-esteem.
Her faltering effort in this regard implies that the desolation in these
responses represented a disintegration product. In this context, the rocket’s
debris may now be considered to be part of this self state of desolation, but
not for the reason that it connotes enraged destruction. Instead, debris as a
disintegration product might belong with the devastated self state domi
nated by affects such as bleak emptiness and isolation.
With regard to the question of selfobject function, in this sequence of
five responses, potential saviors of her faltering self were unavailable or
impotent. Thus, the headless angel of Card I, the headless seamstress’
manikin, and the benevolent figure of E. T. cannot apparently prevent her
final percept of the bleak desert monolith, which was the manifestation of
unavailable or inadequate responsiveness to legitimate selfobject needs. It
is, by inference, profoundly deficient mirroring, despite a feeble attempt to
seek idealizing selfobjects such as the imposing rocket, a magical, Fanta
sia-like quality, or the angel of Card I. Her rocket ended in debris, and her
angel was headless. Even the animated Fantasia scene and the top could not
prevent her final response of the bleak desert scene.
200 CHAPTER 7
Card III
9. That's stereotypical: two Af It's just the bodies of the two peo
rican people, pointed face, ple, beating on drums right here.
the Afro hair styles. Beating
on drums, trying to get a mes
sage. (Dl)
10. Neanderthal people. They They're cave people. [How does it
both fell out of a tree. This is look that way?] It looks like a cave
what they were hanging on painting. They're hunched over
to, it probably broke. (D2) like gorillas. They 're primitive, not
very smart. That's why they fell out
o f the tree.
11. A bust o f Beethoven. A big I'm looking at a negative image.
red bow tie so it'd be cute as Beethoven because his hair is stick
a button. (D3) ing out. He's all jazzed up with the
bow tie. [How does it look that
way?] You mean sophisticated,
class and culture? A desired goal.
In the midst of this primitive stuff:
Neanderthals falling out of a tree
because they're stupid.
Card IV
12. A big monster. Here's his His hands fell off. Here's his tail.
head and his hands. An ac [Hands fell off?] The worst thing
tion shot because his feet go he can do is step on you. His head
up and down. (W) is pretty small.
vl3. A medieval castle. All this This is how it would look here s the
protection. It's very danger top right here.
ous down there. To keep peo
ple out. (Dd99; lower half of
Dl)
202 CHAPTER 7
vl4. Like Georgia O'Keefe, it’s The nasal cavity and horns. It's real
reminiscent o f some of her neat. A dead buffalo and they made
works. It reminds me of a buf- something pretty out o f it.
falo skull in the desert. (W)
vl5. A king. His crown, his head, His outfit, the beard, his head and
all this is pomp and circum- hair. It reminds me of the emperor's
stance. (W) new clothes. They were all just
fooling him.
Ms. T. did not describe her monster as threatening, although she referred
to his large size from the perspective of an “action shot.” She wasted no
time in immobilizing the monster, first by removing his hands and later by
referring to his small head. Perhaps severing the hands was sufficiently
immobilizing so that this figure did not also need to be beheaded, as Ms. T.
had already done on Cards I and II, but she had not thereby protected herself
from injury because the monster could still “step on you.”
There followed a percept of a medieval castle, fortified but still not invul
nerable. In needing “to keep people out,” Ms. T. had expended so much mental
effort on protecting herself and staving off injuries to the self that she became
deadened in the process by the time she reached the percept of the buffalo skull.
No matter that she tried to defensively represent this state of depletion and
mental exhaustion as an artistic product. Even her closing percept to this card,
that of a king, was undermined, because the power expected from a king was
weakened and illusory. Ultimately, the king too became an unreliable basis for
establishing idealization as a compensatory structure. The percept continued
her reference to the bigger-than-life Beethoven as diminished and “cute as a
button.” As with her association to the fairy tale about the emperor’s new
clothes, one is not fooled by her diversionary kings, castles, Beethoven, and
big monsters. She could not conceal that the king was just “pomp and
circumstance,” the castle held danger, the monster’s hands fell off, and
Beethoven was reduced to a “cute as a button” statue.
Card V
16. Two people sleeping. (W) The legs here, the elbow. Like at the
beach, resting. It's calm.
17. A mythical animal getting It's mythical because there's no ani-
readyfor a take-off. (W) mal like this. His feet are getting
ready to run and take off. He's fine
once he gets going, but taking off is
hard.
MS. T.: MIRRORING 203
Ms. T. began her responses to Card V with the appearance of what seemed
like a recovery from the preceding upheaval. By psychologically “sleeping,”
she managed to feel “calm.” Inasmuch as her vigilant efforts to protect the self
from depletion consumed so much of her psychological resources, she seemed
to be alone in this attempt. There is barely any indication of selfobject
responsiveness that she could rely on. Her mythical animal percept could refer
to a feeling that no one else seems to have to work this hard (“there’s no animal
like this ... taking off is hard”).
She might be sleeping her way through Card V, but her comment about not
liking the card became, in effect, a powerful quasiresponse: She did not like
the quietude that was the price she paid for feeling calm. Thus, hardly a
“sleeper,” this response was a good example of a situation on the Rorschach
when less is more.
The excitement that she missed here perhaps reminded her too much of
the inner deadness suggested by the buffalo skull of Card IV and the desert
scene of Card II. If so, did this reminder re-expose an unresponded-to self to
a sense of dread about the threat of depletion? Could a sense of calmness
also be intolerable because feeling comforted might prove to be illusory or
undependable? These possibilities must remain tentative, however compel
ling they are at this point in the protocol. Speculation about these dynamics
raises the question of what happens when it becomes necessary to self-pro-
tectively drive one’s selfobject needs underground to protect the self from
further devitalization.
This phenomenon may represent a milder form of the pronounced with
drawal states associated with the schizoid or depressive positions to which
patients defensively retreat to protect themselves, similar to the states empha
sized in the writings of Melanie Klein (1935/1975) and Guntrip (1969).
Kohut did not attach the same significance to this phenomenon that Klein
and Guntrip did, nor did he conceptualize clinical material in the same way.
Nevertheless, there may exist some commonality between these points of view.
This patient’s comment about the lack of excitement on Card V, coupled
with her earlier responses of the desert scene and the buffalo skull of a Georgia
O’Keefe painting, revived the fear of dissolution of the self. This reaction is
reminiscent of the disintegration anxiety that M. Tolpin & Kohut (1980)
204 CHAPTER 7
Card VI
vl8. A buffalo skin hanging on a The front, his back legs, the spine
wall for decoration. (Dl) I is curved, the tail here. I feel sorry
don ’tfeel like doing this any for the buffalo: Where does that
more. leave him? You’re supposed to be
nice to them, they’re an endan
gered species.
19. Waves coming in, slopping The foam hitting the beach. Soon
all over the place. (Dl) the water will be all over, there
won’t be any land left. [How do you
see it?] The way thefoam goes over
the beach. It’s like writing mes
sages in a sand castle and they’re
erased away, they’re only left in
your memory. The waves wash
everything away. It’s night.
20. A kiwi fruit that somebody This black looks like the stripe
splashed. All the seeds ex down the middle of a kiwi fruit.
ploded all over, just gushed [Stripe?] It’s something special,
out. (D ll) unusual. It’s just coincidental.
I thought these were in
colors.
Ms. T.’s remark at the end of Card V was restated even more strongly
after her first response to Card VI. Evidently, she was becoming increas
ingly perturbed by some quality of the blots that made her wish to
withdraw from the testing situation. Her earlier response of a buffalo
skull now became a buffalo skin, and the mythical animal of Card V
became endangered. She seemed to complain that the buffalo, an endan
gered species, was maltreated. Even her attempt to make the skin a wall
decoration was no more convincing than her earlier attempt to turn her
bleak desert scene into a Navajo painting.
MS. T.: MIRRORING 205
At the conclusion of Card VI, Ms. T. complained further of her need for
color in the cards, a response perhaps signifying that she was growing
overburdened by the bleak task that the Rorschach had become for her. Her
response seemed to capture how she experienced much of her life. The
understimulation signified a self that was desolate, unresponded to, and
ultimately devoid of the buoyancy required to sustain itself in a psychologi
cally alive manner.
She felt drained (“I don’t feel like doing this anymore”) and perhaps
frustrated with an examiner who continually re-exposed her to the devitali
zation of her self-esteem. She appealed to me and expected understanding
or sympathy (“I feel sorry for the buffalo; where does that leave him?”) as
if to say to me: “What’s wrong with you? Can’t you see how much I’m
hurting? You’re supposed to be nice to someone like me who feels so
endangered.” My restatement of her inquiry comments practically defined
this patient’s complaint about an unresponsive or unavailable mirroring
selfobject environment that did not notice her distress and needs.
The theme of an understimulated, unresponded-to self seemed to pervade
the rest of Card VI and was captured in the melancholy tone of lost,
unpreserved, or erased “messages.” This theme foreshadowed a forgotten-
about self in danger of eroding or being “washed away.” It was reminiscent
of her other castle (Card IV) that was in danger. Is the danger that something
imposing or vigorous is elusive or transitory? Can it be “washed away”?
Note also her comment about the castle on Card IV (“this is how it would
look”) as if she were reconstructing something from a memory; she
repeated this image in her “memory” of the sand castle on Card VI.
Even the “special” quality of the kiwi fruit was problematic: A black
stripe “exploded ... gushed.” As much as she craved it, is gushing excite
ment or enlivenment too hot for her to handle (“exploded”)? Perhaps
self-protectively, Ms. T. contained this upsurge of affect by converting
something “special ... unusual” into something merely “coincidental.”
(Recall again how her dress from Card II for someone special to show off
is “not for me.”) Perhaps, alternatively, the kiwi, with its associations of
tropical colors, revived the wish for the enlivening of a depleted self. Its
failure or disappointment might have disposed Ms. T. to comment about
missing the color on the ink blots. Was she giving the examiner another
chance to enliven her, only to have her hopes dashed?
On this card, characterized as it was by the most compelling indication
of decompensation to this point, there was still no indication of a clearly
stated selfobject need. This patient craved mirroring, complained about its
206 CHAPTER 7
absence, but finally felt that mirroring was too unsafe or unreliable to
tolerate, and withdrew into a self state of depletion depression. This is the fate
of the unmirrored self (M. Tolpin, 1993). There is some risk in making a point
by reasoning from the absence of a function rather than from an interpretation
about an actual percept. I nevertheless argue that what is seen here is a self state
experienced as forgotten about and depreciated. It seems as if this patient could
only complain about continuing the Rorschach at this point or wonder about
why there was no color in the blots. This reaction is either a defensive
withdrawal from a situation that has become too burdensome to bear or an
expression of a veiled statement expressing a need for a selfobject to become
available to enliven, revive, or otherwise add “color” to her depleted self.
That the selfobject need is expressed in only a veiled manner does not,
however, mean that it is absent. If it is concealed by being driven underground,
the examiner’s task becomes more difficult; he or she must make something
of so little. Sometimes, the selfobject function is not contained in the elabora
tion of the percept itself but may appear in a patient’s indirect, associated
comments in reference to the task itself. Thus, Ms. T.’s remarks about not
liking a card, not wanting to go on, or asking for cards with color managed to
convey what the response proper did not. Transference manifestations
directed to the examiner could reflect the selfobject needs that characterized
much of her life. Undergoing projective testing represented a microcosm
of the patient’s life and re-exposed her to the draining ordeal of her devitalized
existence. When Ms. T. said in effect, “I don’t like this any more, I want to
stop, you don’t give me anything with more color to work with,” she perhaps
expressed a wish for a selfobject to bring her back to life.
One does not see here the specific selfobject transference represented by
mirroring, idealization, or twinship, but rather the general manifestation of
a self state in disrepair. The specific selfobject function may not often be
clearly indicated on the Rorschach but may appear vividly on a task such
as the TAT, which evokes relationships among figures and ways that one
figure fails to provide some selfobject function for another. Similarly,
figure drawings may suggest depreciation of the figure and sometimes also
contain an indication of what is needed to repair the defect in self-esteem.
The Rorschach, on the other hand, more often captures the push and pull of
conflicts, defenses, and anxiety, and reveals more about ego resiliency
in the process. In a similar fashion, the Rorschach may often indicate
more about the self state than about particular selfobject needs.
This is the case with Ms. T., whose clear manifestations of selfobject needs
do become evident on the TAT. I describe this process more fully later, in the
MS. T.: MIRRORING 207
discussion of the material about Ms. T.’s TAT. The tentative speculations
noted here, however, provide a way of thinking about the probable selfob
ject functions underlying the self states that are so richly evident in the
content analysis findings from this Rorschach protocol.
Card VII
v21. Two people bending over They ’re women, on tippy-toe so you
backw ards. Their arms know they’ll fall over for sure. The
reaching out. (W) arms have to be out. You balance
so not to fall over. Also, they’re
balancing on their brains because
that’s the only thing that’s touching.
v22. Napoleon. His head, collar, The hat part. He was a ruthless
the funny hat. (DS7) leader, a military genius, and pow
erful. A little guy who got where he
wanted to, even though he ended in
exile. So, he gave it a go, and got
as far as he could.
23. Two people balancing on the Here’s the ponytail. Balancing on
very edge of the top of a rock. the base so they don ’tfall over.
They’re trying to communi
cate either by using their
arms for gestures or their
heads coming together. (W)
By now, there was little new. Most responses essentially restated earlier
themes. Beginning with people precariously balanced to avoid falling down,
Ms. T. proceeded to the diminutive but powerful Napoleon who wound up
defeated. Thus, she ended as insecurely balanced as she began. Ms. T. never
managed to gain a solid foothold for her precarious self state. Her elaboration
at the end of the inquiry to Response 21 was unclear; in an otherwise
non-thought-disordered record, she seemed to struggle with headless or brain
less imagery, and with desperation. This statement is another instance of a self
state that lacks what is needed for its cohesion or stability.
Card VIII
v24. Somebody’s brain. Some of The pons, midbrain, cerebrum, me-
the parts are rotted out. A lot dulla oblongata,
o f the parts are missing. (W)
208 CHAPTER 7
25. Noah's ark. Rocking on the It doesn't exist. The boat that was
edge of a mountain top, kind supposed to save everybody.
o f tipping. Even though it's
been there thousands of years
I think it's going to fall off.
Everything there is petrified.
(D4)
26. Two bears climbing up the They're supposed to have big
side with their hand reaching strong hands, so they yre nonfunc
out but it doesn't look like a tional, sickly.
bear hand. The fingers don't
look good. (Dl)
v27. Two unicorns. Maybe just be Here's the horses' heads, the sad
cause th e y're white. I dles. They're comforting, rocking
changed it to rocking horses. back and forth.
(Dds99=D5 + Dd33 + DdS28)
her Dumbo figure, offered little more than minimal comfort as she contin
ued to experience considerable desperation in her effort to repair an enfee
bled self.
Card IX
28. Two witches over a big pot, A looking up shot so you can't see
throwing all sorts o f stuff in. what's in there, but everyone knows
It's all poison. (D3) witches always make poison.
v29. A pelvis area. (D ll) Its shape. Now I can see the organs
and muscles (points to D6 area).
30. The back view of somebody's From the back. [Pretty?] The ele
shoulders with a real pretty gance, fanciness. Wealth.
fancy evening gown. But you
can't see the rest of her head,
but it's a very nice hair style.
(Dd99: lower half of D3 and
upper projection o fD l area)
31. Two women with long The two white things. Like a myth
dresses. The wind is blowing where the people weren't supposed
th eir hair and dresses. to do something and they turned to
(DdS99: two larger white stone and got petrified. That was
space areas on either side of their punishment.
Dd30)
Card X
v32. Bugs, like in a movie I saw. The colors and squishiness, like a
They ate the brain out. The caterpillar that crawled and slith
Wrath of Khan. Could trans ered. I clean my ears twice a day
mit their thoughts. They because I don Ylike icky things.
didn't have any control of
their actions when the bug’s
in their ear. (W)
33. The Fontainebleu and sea The way there's statues around the
food restaurant in Paris. The fountain. When I used to go on va
Eiffel Tower is nearby. (D2 + cation, I went wherever I wanted
D ll) and did what I wanted.
34. Crustaceans. (Dl) The shape they have here.
MS. T.: MIRRORING 211
35. Disneyland. When the Magic That's how it looks. Everyone has
Kingdom comes out at night. to clap their handsfor Tinkerbell so
(DIO) they wouldn't die.
36. Two pterodactyls. They kiss The bodies, their fancy tails. They
and there's a blue heart. But kissed and their heart grew.
they're extinct, so it doesn't They're prehistoric birds. Like a
make any difference. (D6) cartoon thing. The heart means
they kissed.
Summary
In Ms. T.’s Rorschach findings, a consistent picture emerged of a dimin
ished self, unable to sustain a robust state of vigor to enable the patient
to turn pridefully to others in anticipation of being admired or under
stood. Again and again, Ms. T. appeared to experience the depletion of
a self that could not hold itself up to the world. She could not expect the
selfobject responsiveness that she required to assist her in preserving self-es
teem and was easily devastated as her efforts to seek admiration went unheard.
As a result, assertive selfobject-seeking attempts at restoring self-cohesion
were driven underground.
Ms. T. attempted to turn to idealizable figures for their imagined strength to
buoy the chronic selfobject failures of mirroring to which she was continually
re-exposed. Persisting disappointment and disintegration products permeated
this protocol. Her attempt to establish idealization as an alternative pathway to
self-cohesion was Ms. T.’s effort to revive faltering self-esteem. In this way,
she tried to build a compensatory structure that might lead to the restoration of
the self. That her attempt characteristically led to her sense that idealized
selfobjects were impotent, immobilized, or illusory only further undermined
MS. T.: MIRRORING 213
Ms. T.’s self disorder and intensified the prevailing beaten-down adaptation
at the core of her experience of the self. She was unable to emerge from this
state sufficiently revived.
A compensatory structure to take the place of chronic, ongoing, deficient
mirroring is not the same thing as filling a gap. The goal of selfobject
responsiveness, or optimal treatment for that matter, is structure formation
rather than feeding, sustenance, or any other variation of supplying temporary
palliatives. None of these is particularly therapeutic as far as analysis or
intensive psychotherapy is concerned. The repair of empathic failure is accom
plished by creating a new, efficacious pathway through which the self becomes
firmed up. For this patient of poor or marginal prognosis, the outlook for repair
of the self cannot be particularly optimistic. This guarded outlook leaves little
room for the hopeful indicators on her Rorschach protocol to survive strongly
enough to help her very much. Her protocol results offered a picture of chronic,
unrelenting mirroring selfobject failure with far more defensive than compen
satory structures. Despite her insightful and richly productive percepts, the
nature of the psychopathology and the impoverishment of self-cohesion that
characterize this patient’s personality structure cannot sustain an exploratory
treatment effort beyond support. To attempt much more is arguably misguided
and clinically ill-advised, although she could derive benefit from a cautiously
supportive treatment.
Card 1.
It’s time for the boy to practice his music lesson. He doesn't want to but he
knows he's supposed to or he'll get in big trouble. He didn't want to play but
his parents decided he would play. Everyone else is having fun playing
outside. [Outcome] He sits there for 3 hours and stares at the violin. He gets
so disgusted, and he puts in his 1 hour of practice. So he didn't get to play
with his friends, and wasted 3 hours just staring at the thing. He doesn't have
much say in whatever he does even if he's miserable.
The basic theme of the story is typical and the resolution of giving in
to the parents’ demand not uncommon. Spending 3 hours staring at the
violin could be either a passive-aggressive position or an attempt by the
boy to assert his own desire. A better understanding is gained, however,
by attending to the affect state of disgust or misery in the boy’s futile
effort to assert his own will. Although Ms. T. did not use the word
214 CHAPTER 7
defeated in her story, it is not difficult to imagine that she felt that way. Here
is another instance, as in several of her Rorschach responses, in which she
felt undermined after attempting to stand up for herself. She ultimately gave
in to defeat and had no impact on the parents. In this respect, feeling
“miserable ... disgusted” was a euphemism for feeling devitalized.
When the self was not responded to, Ms. T. gave up in defeat.
In the story cited here, the boy appeared to seek some sense of
understanding by or meeting of the minds with his parents, but there is
no indication that the parents (at least in the storyteller’s eyes) showed
a capacity for recognizing the boy’s plight. Thus, any sense of compro
mise, understanding of the boy’s conflicting wishes, or even an aware
ness of his conflicts was lacking. His selfobject need was at least to be
understood or to have his needs recognized. The failure at this level gave
rise to a self state characterized by feeling impotent and devitalized in
the face of unresponsiveness from the selfobject environment. The
ensuing depletion of the self, expressed in the story by the affects of
“disgusted ... miserable” resulted from this lack of responsiveness or
selfobject failure.
Card 2.
You're not paying attention to me, so I wont pay attention to your cards. All
these people live on the farm. She has to go away to school It looks like she
doesn't want to. He works the farm. He gets visual—he gets feedback what
he's done. So everything he does has some obvious causal effect. She doesn't
look happyf also her dress is crooked, so she doesn't take very good care how
she's dressed. It looks sad, I don't know what she wants to do. [Led up?]
These two people just figured she should go to school. [Who are they?] It
doesn't really matter. [Outcome?] Even though she doesn't want to, she does
what everyone wants her to do. Even though she's miserable and her
sweater's on crooked.
ment that dismissed her need for mirroring. This fact predisposed her to the
depletion depression or devitalization that typified much of her ongoing
experience (“even though she’s still miserable”). Although it may sound
flippant when she followed this statement about feeling miserable by
commenting “and her sweater’s on crooked,” it no longer seems so when
considered from the viewpoint that her sense of herself is devalued.
Card 3BM.
1don’t know. There's tons of stuff to do and so many tasks to finish. They're
so tired, they put their head down for a minute. Then they'd have enough
energy to finish. They'll fall asleep and won't get it done and get in trouble.
[Outcome?] They'll be disappointed she didn't finish it.
Card 7GF.
The perspective is off on this. The little girl and the lady are both waiting for
something. The little girl's distracted, looking to a spot where she hopes
something will happen, but she looks disinterested like there's got to be
something else better. So she obediently sits there waiting, wishing to do what
she wants. They told her to just sit there and wait. [The lady?] She doesn't
even know the little girl. They're just waiting for some unknown reason. She
has something productive to do while waiting. The little girl's just hopelessly
aimlessly waiting.
MS. T.: MIRRORING 217
The major themes were much the same as on the previous cards. Her
initial comment about the “perspective” suggested being caught off guard
when presented with this card. The threat to self-cohesion by the marked
selfobject failure was triggered from the start because she seemed taken
aback that the “perspective is off.” She proceeded to tell a story as if the two
people were sitting in different spaces (the lady “doesn’t even know the little
girl”). The activity of waiting “for some unknown reason” remained the same
for both. The two figures could hardly be more uninvolved with one another
as they were described in this story. Indeed, one could almost imagine that
there were two completely separate cards with the same story.
The girl was “disinterested,” Ms. T.’s metaphor, like “miserable ...
disgusted ... so tired,” for disengagement in the face of mirroring selfobject
failure. She “obediently” complied with what was asked of her, as in the
previous stories of not enjoying practicing the violin or not wanting to go
to school. She felt emptiness and lack of enthusiasm for what she was
expected to do. These feelings arose from a selfobject environment in which
parental figures were uninvolved with Ms. T.’s needs. Consequently, she
revealed a self state of emptiness, unresponded to by the depersonalized
mother figure referred to as just “the lady.” The girl was “hopelessly
aimlessly waiting” for this “lady” to come to life for her in an important
psychological sense. The impersonal, psychologically bereft, and empty
quality contained in this response is the sort of material that plays by
David Mamet capture in a raw and gripping way by the bleakness and the
starkness of their stagecraft.
Card 7BM.
It’s the middle of a serious conversation. This is just a break in the topic.
Either an insurance policy or money matters. [Who are they?] They’re
relatives. [Make up a story.] It makes no difference. Otherwise they
wouldn’t be that close to each other. [What led up?] Some acute situation.
Now they have to band together and decide what to do. [Outcome?] They
come to a consensus.
This card seemed more difficult for Ms. T. to tell a story about. It required
more prompting to elicit important story features, and even with this amount
of inquiry there was less specificity compared with her other TAT stories. She
was reluctant to indicate exactly what relationship these relatives had. There
was some degree of closeness or need to “band together” in this story, but who
they were and how close they were to each other were irrelevant. Indeed, as
218 CHAPTER 7
she said, “It makes no difference.” The closeness was apparently based on
the consensus of equals or nearequals, without any indication of the guiding
parental function frequently elicited on this card.
On the previous card, Card 7GF, the girl did not feel attached enough to
consider initiating contact with the “lady” who “doesn’t even know the little
girl.” The present card was also notable for the absence of a selfobject
function characterizing the relationship between the two men. There was
distance despite the fact that the men were relatives. There was also an affective
estrangement to the “acute situation” that momentarily brought them together
to reach a consensus. Although perhaps not totally distant, cold, or business
like, this relationship was not particularly warm, benevolent, or characterized
by paternalistic concern. Ms. T. was again driving underground her need for
selfobject responsiveness, which could occur only with an “acute situation.”
An empathically attuned response should be one of concern or at least a
sense that one party feels threatened or that self-cohesion is in danger. The
response represented here is different: A dispassionate “serious conversation,”
even though it may lead to a resolution (“they come to a consensus”). Ms. T.
might have been turning from the mother who did not know the girl (that is,
the patient herself) or her need for mirroring selfobject responsiveness (Card
7GF) to the “relative” of Card 7BM. The somewhat more involved relation
ship between these male figures led to a more satisfactory resolution by
“consensus” compared to the “hopelessly aimlessly waiting” of Card 7GF.
Still, there was the perception of these men as “relatives,” but “it makes no
difference” just who they were. It is difficult to see this response as even a
remote indication of idealization. It is also doubtful that this attempt to
compensate for what was unavailable by mirroring was psychologically
satisfying or that Ms. T. felt sufficiently invigorated to revive an enfeebled,
understimulated self.
Card 18GF.
That's odd. Two people, a lady holding another person up. Something must have
happened. It looks like something pretty sad or serious. [Led up?] The person
collapsed or got sick or something. She’s probably been taking care of this person
for a while. [Who are these women?] I didn't say two women. I was very careful
to say a woman and a person. Ones a caretaker, the other one is just a person.
[Male or female?] Neither. I can't tell. Maybe an old man. She's taking care of
her father or a relative or something. [How does she feel?] All worn out and
frazzled. He's dependent on the lady. She's pretty sad and distraught. The
person's sick and needs her help. Because it's no fun to have to be responsible
and take care of someone else.
MS. T.: MIRRORING 219
Again, this patient was momentarily thrown off guard by something about
the card, possibly its stark bluntness connoting distress. This reaction appeared
in her opening comment of “that’s odd” and later in her insistent correction of
the examiner’s error in thinking that both figures were women. Furthermore,
her terse closing of the story much sooner than most of her other stories also
required a greater degree of inquiry to elicit important details. Even at that,
Ms. T. was uncharacteristically guarded about revealing too much. She
grudgingly allowed that the person in distress was an old man and was
surprisingly reticent about deciding whether the man was a “father or a relative
or something.” After that, one is still in the dark about this point. A self
psychologically informed interpretation of her “worn out and frazzled ... sad
and distraught” feeling indicates the same self state already revealed consis
tently across the TAT cards. The chronically lacking acknowledgment that she
was asked to assume so much responsibility is noteworthy; she felt
depleted or “all worn out.”
Card 13MF.
The next morning. He just got up out of bed, got dressed and ready to go.
[Who are they?] They have a steady relationship. He’s nice enough not to
wake her up in the morning.
Card 14.
The inside of a building. He’s busy working all day, and just took a minute
for time out to get some fresh air for some relief.
These two cards contributed little to the clinical picture. The responses
added nothing noteworthy to what Ms. T. already said.
tory structures. It also shows what happens on projective testing when that
attempt is thwarted and when the mirroring sector of the self has been as
chronically and seriously damaged as it has been for this patient.
As I have already indicated, it is necessary to consider the self state and
attempts to repair defective self structure together with patients’ experience
of their external world or selfobject environment. Through selfobject re
sponsiveness, people manage to re-establish or revive what they need to
preserve self-cohesion. With Ms. T., the mirroring selfobject function
necessary to respond vigorously to her need to be kept “afloat” was again
and again deficient. The repeated empathic failures that she experienced
left her chronically disappointed. As so many of her TAT stories and
Rorschach responses suggested, she felt unresponded to in her attempts to
secure mirroring, and she was ultimately defeated. Attempts at establishing
another route to repair the self by forming idealizing selfobject transfer
ences were either halfhearted or did not go far enough. She quickly
abandoned hope that there was anything there for her “in the midst of this
primitive stuff’ beyond the “pomp and circumstance.” Her confidence was
undermined: Ms. T. referred to a “Napoleon ... who got where he wanted
to,” but he “ended in exile,” or “Noah’s ark ... that was supposed to save
everybody” but “doesn’t exist” and is “petrified.” Consequently, a compen
satory structure as an alternate route to repair a devitalized self failed to
develop reliably for her.
Beyond her diminished self-esteem, Ms. T. indicated in her projective
test protocol the central significance of the buoyancy or vitality of the
experience of the self. A normal expectation of mirroring is essential for
ensuring self-cohesion, and building compensatory structures protects
against devitalization of the self by attempting to establish another route to
secure selfobject responsiveness. This protocol shows the failure of mirror
ing, and it also reveals idealization as a compensatory structure. The
characteristic experience or phenomenology of the devitalized unmirrored
self is conveyed most tellingly in this patient’s projective test content,
which depicted the remains of a self so devastated by both deficient
mirroring and idealizing selfobject responsiveness that it was unable to
sustain itself for long. Ms. T.’s self markedly lacked a quality perhaps best
denoted as resilience or hardiness.
In the next chapter, I present the projective testing protocol of a patient
who demonstrated more interplay among selfobject functions than did
Ms. T. Like Ms. T., this patient showed a basic mirroring deficiency, and
he also attempted to establish compensatory structures centered on
222 CHAPTER 7
idealization and, to an extent, twinship. Ms. T.’s self disorder was more
severe; as a result, her feeble attempt to establish an idealizing selfobject
transference quickly failed. Thus, her protocol illustrates a more pre
dominant mirroring selfobject disturbance than does the protocol dis
cussed next. The patient whose projective test findings are presented in
chapter 8 repeatedly attempted to seek idealizing and twinship selfobject
functions, although in the end he too was unable to secure them with any
greater success than did Ms. T.
The contrast between the two cases demonstrates the differences in level
of psychopathology of the self (despite comparable overt symptomatic
disturbances), differential utilization of selfobject functions and attempts to
establish compensatory structures, and approximately similar outcomes as
far as the success of these efforts.
8 Mr. L.: Idealization
and Twinship
The second case that I present in its entirety is of interest for several
reasons. This psychodiagnostic protocol has elements of the three major
selfobject functions that Kohut identified, and therefore represents a
typical clinical presentation of selfobject functions. Not a textbook case of
clear idealization or twinship, it shows the difficulty in distinguishing these
selfobject transferences from each other. The patient, Mr. L., alternated
among selfobject functions to find a viable pathway for the repair of
self-cohesion and like many other patients, attempted to establish a com
pensatory structure in the face of chronic mirroring selfobject failures.
Mr. L.’s case illustrates a point that Kohut came to see over time as his
work progressed: Selfobject functions are not entirely independent of each
other. With the possible exception of mirroring needs, which occur most
often, other selfobject transferences predominate at different times. They
come forward at some points and recede at others, often when an attempt
at restoring self-esteem is either blocked or unsuccessful. Such vacillation
is not an instance of inconsistency or failure to establish a predominant
selfobject function; selfobject functions do not represent formes frustes.
Admixtures frequently appear.
The case reported here is a good illustration of this clinical situation. I
discuss it by considering all three selfobject functions and emphasize
idealization and twinship as efforts to repair self-cohesion when mirroring
has become unavailable or unsuccessful. The patient showed a degree of
overt psychopathology comparable to Ms. T.; the degree of symptomatic
disturbance is approximately equal in the two cases. Mr. L., however, had
a more favorable premorbid adjustment. His disorder was more reactive
and less chronic than the characterologically entrenched pathology of
Ms. T.’s disorder.
223
224 CHAPTER 8
Mr. L. was also more resilient than Ms. T. insofar as he could seek out
idealization and twinship selfobjects to somewhat better advantage than
did Ms. T. Consequently, the admixture of selfobject functions in Mr. L.’s
protocol may appear to be confusing, only because Ms. T. displayed a
clearly demarcated or predominant selfobject need. The arrested develop
ment in Ms. T.’s self disorder testifies to the difference in degree of
psychopathology in these patients’ disorders. The constriction that charac
terized the pronounced mirroring deficit of Ms. T.’s condition produced a
more pathological clinical picture and prognosis than did that of Mr. L. In
the end, however, despite Mr. L.’s persistent attempt to secure selfobject
responsiveness through idealization and twinship, the efficacious repair of
a self disorder was no more successful for him than it was for Ms. T.
This case illustrates that the clinical identification of selfobject transfer
ences varies according to psychological test or procedure. For Mr. L., the
twinship and idealization selfobject functions appeared more distinctly on
the Rorschach yet were barely evident at all on the TAT and figure
drawings. Had the TAT and drawings been performed without the Ror
schach, a clinician would not have detected evidence for idealization or
twinship selfobject functions, and a disturbance of mirroring would have
been viewed as central. Had the Rorschach been administered without the
TAT or figure drawings, deficient mirroring might not have appeared to
the extent revealed on the TAT and human figure drawings.
Mr. L.’s case demonstrates, therefore, that the interplay of selfobject
functions, like defenses and conflicts, can frequently require an entire
protocol. A complete and balanced diagnostic testing protocol becomes
crucial to identify predominant selfobject patterns and central self psy
chological dynamics like compensatory structures, as these ideas were
thought about and developed by Kohut.
Mr. L., a 45-year-old married White man, was admitted to the inpatient
psychiatry service of a general hospital following a suicide attempt precipi
tated by family difficulties. He had been an accomplished learning disabili
ties supervisor and administrator, who had become enraged when his
adolescent son was diagnosed with an attention deficit syndrome by col
leagues at a hospital in which Mr. L. worked. He became furious with his
son and argued with his wife about her overprotectiveness of the boy. Mr.
L. experienced an intensification of anxiety, accompanied by shortness of
breath and chest pain; the anxiety had developed in the past 2 years. He
began drinking heavily, although not so much as to raise concerns about
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 225
A young person going out for a walk. He’s excited that the air is clean and
fresh. He's looking forward to the new things hell find as he explores.
Basically, it's the woods, nature, being alone, smelling the trees and flowers.
[Describe personality]1 Very changeable, moodwise. Sometimes he’s very
happy, sometimes he's very sad. When the mood changes he likes to be alone.
Basically, he's very free, left to his own thoughts. [Q] There's a contradiction
between emotional involvement and intellectual involvement. He's very
sensitive to the needs of others. It makes him drained, unable to deal with his
own needs. Then he realizes his own inadequacies, because he’s very angry
and oppositional. [Q] Being able to be in total control of his environment and
himself. Wanting to do what he wants to do. He doesn't like being controlled
by authority in his job, by people who know less than he does. It bothers him
when he's unable to obtain what he wants, when he knows he’s right and
he’s misunderstood. When obstacles, such as money, block his goals in life.
[Goals?] Living, not wealthy, but achieving comfortable things.
That's a mother-like figure, sweet and gentle, calling her children in to the
house. Very warm and loving. She might appear simple but is a deep
thinker, she's hurt very easily but doesn't show it. She gets a lot of
satisfaction from her family. The family takes advantage of her. Though
she resents it, she'll always have a smile covering up the hurts, with her
arms stretched. She's also there to hold the family together. [Fears or
worries?] That she's not able to protect them the way she should. A lot of
responsibility falls on to her. She's unable to deal with them at times. [Sad
or depressed?] Failure of her children to conform the way she wants them
to, because of her high expectations that's sometimes unrealistic. Failure
also of her husband's inability to take over a lot of chores thrown on to
her. Incomplete communication, feeling the family might be against her.
They're going in different directions with different goals. So, basically she's
given up. [Angry?] Very little. Disagreements with her husband as to how
to deal with the children. Over money, being taken advantage of, that she
has no control.
The description of this female drawing, like his male drawing, began
with a picture of serenity but soon changed to an elaboration of inner
distress taking the form of feeling overburdened with responsibility and
failing to realistically meet obligations. This patient’s elaboration of the
figure drawing conveyed his feeling that other people disappointed by
ignoring what the figure needed in the way of understanding or recogni
tion. Thus, the woman failed to win adequate mirroring; “basically she’s
given up.” The depletion or diminished enthusiasm gave way to feeling
taken advantage of and lacking control.
Although drawings of same-sex figures frequently offer a good initial
indication of the self state, it is difficult to interpret verbalizations about
opposite-sex drawings. Should the commentary on inquiry be under
stood as an idealized self representation when the patient feels too
undermined or depleted? Should the female drawing be taken to repre
sent the patient’s own sense of depreciation of someone important in his
life, perhaps as a defensive effort to protect the self from further injury?
Should this inquiry be thought of as a restatement of the devalued self
228 CHAPTER 8
continued from the previous drawing? The guidelines are rarely clear on
these questions, and examiners must proceed carefully with their interpre
tations.
In the case at hand, Mr. L.’s description on inquiry to this drawing did not
certainly indicate whom he was speaking about. Nevertheless, the overriding
themes of being overburdened or unresponded to cannot be ignored. He
ultimately gave up in defeat and could not sustain a sufficiently buoyed-up
feeling of self-esteem. This interpretation points to the way that Mr. L. might
experience both his own self state and the selfobject environment he turned
to for assistance in sustaining self-esteem. Whether or not he felt himself to
be faltering and diminished, he continually experienced indifference or
unresponsiveness to legitimate mirroring needs. This lack deprived him of
feeling capable and enthusiastic; instead, he felt devitalized.
Rorschach Content
Card I
1. Two people sitting over a ta Here's the table, their heads, rear
ble having dinner. Discuss ends.
ing the events o f the day. Both
are coming from work. It's a
pub-like atmosphere. Their
discussion is based on the in
eptness they came across that
day. They're in disagree
ment, one trying to quiet
down the other by the raising
o f the hand. (W)
2. A butterfly. (W) Can I turn it The body, the wings here.
around?
v3. Two people dancing around a They're holding hands around it.
maypole. One seems very
happy, the other's very sad.
When they're happy it's more
energetic a movement, when
it's sad, you can tell by the
head being down that he
doesn't want to participate.
(W)
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 229
<4. Two donkeys. Actually, one Looking down, big ears, here’s the
donkey with a reflection in reflection.
the water. Sees his own re-
flection. (W)
5. An Oriental woman right Theface, hair, standing right on the
here. (D4) ground. Not as gentle as most.
Looks hostile.
Mr. L.’s atypical approach to the inkblot was characterized from the
outset by penetrating behind the imagery of his opening response. His
response was more like a TAT story than a typical Rorschach percept, and
his first response most likely typifies the real-life dilemma that preoccupied
him. Despite the convivial letting-one’s-hair-down mood that he wished to
convey by setting the discussion in a pub, the predominant themes of
criticism or devaluation (“the ineptness they came across that day”), dis
agreement, and attempts to restore calm interfered with the congeniality he
was striving for.
The predominant self state was characterized by disharmony by seek
ing calm to re-establish cohesion (“one trying to quiet down the other”). A
specific selfobject need has not yet emerged, but the affective state of
disruption or disequilibrium was clear. The need to restore a state of calm
intimacy or communion is more central than is the wish to discharge a
hostile or critical impulse.
The serene image of a butterfly, coupled with his asking permission to
turn the card, might suggest dependency or fragility. Following the tense
disagreement disrupting the initially harmonious mood of the previous
response, the butterfly and the request to turn the card could also be a
restatement of the need to restore calm. The next percept is dancing around
a maypole, as innocent-sounding as the butterfly, but it quickly becomes
associated with the bittersweet lachen und weinen (“laughter and tears”) of
an affect state that alternates between these moods but does not settle into
either one. The happy affect was experienced as an “energetic” quality of
movement that may connote the vitality of the self state that Mr. L. was seeking.
In similar fashion, the sad affect was represented by the “head being down,”
metaphorically standing for the devitalization of the self that cannot hold its
own, that “doesn’t want to participate” or hold its head up with pride and vigor.
In some clinical conditions in which alternating mood states are promi
nent, such a response can imply a bipolar mood disorder. Fluctuating affect
states suggested by projective test responses like these may also represent
230 CHAPTER 8
Card II
6. Two people, possibly monks, The heads, bodies. Pressing hands,
having a conversation. like they're having fun, they're in
They're much more compat accord, pushing together.
ible as they're touching
hands. (W)
7. Two elephants nuzzling each The same place without the top red.
other's trunks. (D6 + D3) Their trunks and ears here.
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 231
<8. A rat, down flat, sprawled Looking down at it. A different per
out. Used like a rug, like a spective. The shape o f the head.
bearskin rug. (D6) The anatomy, the fur. [Fur?] The
roughness o f the surface and the
shading. [Sprawled out?] It’s very
flat.
v9. A bird, a duck in flight. I'm The beak, head, the wings. Like
taking one white part with the looking down from an airplane.
black as background. (DS5 + [Association] Running away. Free
D4) dom. The softness. Escape.
vlO. Two birds, like little hum The little beaks. The rest of the card
mingbirds. Taking nectar is the flower. Stamen, pistils, the
from a flower. (W) petals. The stem. [Association]
Very cheery. Free.
The response of monks on the first chromatic blot of the Rorschach cards
may be surprising, all the more so because the all-red color on Card II
frequently elicits percepts about blood. It is always questionable to assume
that examiners’ preconceived ideas about the meaning of specific cards
parallels patients’ responses. Schafer (1954), R M. Lemer (1991), and most
commentators on Rorschach interpretation stressed this point, which was
discussed in detail in chapter 4. With that in mind, it is advisable to
de-emphasize interpreting this response of monks seeking compatibility as
a defensive denial or reaction formation against hostile wishes. The rela
tively serene percept of monks as gentle figures, touching hands in a gesture
of accord or compatibility, can be construed as an excessive effort to portray
a conciliatory or cooperative relationship.
This interpretation follows from a drive theory view about the underlying
hostility suggested by the red color presumed to be the driving force behind
the response. This line of thinking applies regardless of whether a reference
to red color or blood is articulated. Although this assumption is under
standable, most responsible projective testing clinicians probably concur
with the need for caution about this interpretation, regardless of whether the
impression is supported by corroborating evidence throughout the entire
projective testing protocol. The hypothesis-generating potential of this form
of interpretation, at this stage in the workup, must be viewed as tentative.
One need not be similarly cautious about making a self psychological
interpretation of the response of monks in conversation because this
interpretation is not based primarily on the red-blood-aggression link.
232 CHAPTER 8
The next percept, of a rat sprawled out like a rug, was all the more unusual
because Comprehensive System criteria were satisfied for coding both
texture and form dimension. Thus, the psychological significance of affec-
tional longing and self-awareness was suggested in this response of a furry
rat. The incongruity of a rat as a decorative rug, with its distasteful qualities
rarely associated with warmth and furriness, suggests that Mr. L. looked in
far-flung places for what he needed to restore either a sense of psychological
connectedness or the calm reassurance to buttress faltering self-cohesion.
These responses are a further indication of incongruity between what Mr.
L. needed and what was possible for him. Was he indicating that he searched
in the most remote places (furry rat rugs, elephants nuzzling) for what the
self needed for its viability? Did he feel that his efforts led nowhere? Most
of his responses to this point, on both Cards I and II, converge on this
possibility. Unremarkably, the rat percept was followed by a bird in flight,
in which his associations of “running away, freedom, softness, escape”
suggest that protecting the self from injury or threat was as much as he
could manage. His association of “softness” recalls the textural quality of
the furry rat, and the softness embedded among associations about escaping
to safety may also indicate the fate of his needs for intimacy or closeness.
In the matter of eliciting associations following the formal inquiry, it is
often difficult to decide when a judicious use of testing limits for select
responses is appropriate. Although advising caution in this regard, Exner
(1993) did not exclude the possibility of testing the limits after the formal
Comprehensive System inquiry but advised against eliciting associations
as an appropriate use of testing limits, as is the case for the response now
being considered, as well as for several others following. Although Exner’s
reasons are compelling, there are arguments in favor of testing limits by
eliciting associations. Mr. L.’s references to escape and softness would not
have emerged without what can be seen as a provocative question that
specifically elicited associations.
Without the associations, the bird in flight would have been thought about
as calm or serene, like the following percept of hummingbirds taking nectar.
It remains to be determined whether one impression was more accurate than
the other or whether these images represented different, conflictual aspects
of the personality structure. Ultimately, the issue of administration proce
dure is one consideration in the risk-benefit balance involved in this test
administration decision. Whether the dynamic of escape or safety would
have emerged in the absence of obtaining associations as a form of testing
limits must also be determined.
234 CHAPTER 8
Mr. L.’s final response to Card II, hummingbirds taking nectar from a
flower, also has connotations of gentleness and oral dependency. It follows
the hostile Oriental woman, his closing response to Card I, and the furry
rat; the incongruity of these images can suggest the unavailability of a
deeply experienced need state. From the viewpoint of self psychology,
frustrated or interrupted mirroring responsiveness is a probable candidate
for what was amiss. Hostility or anger is less likely; this picture is not so
much one of a drive seeking discharge as it is of a need state that is
profoundly blocked or closed off.
Card III
11. Two men. They appear very The bodies here. They appear just
stagnant even though their to look formal there.
bodies are very free flowing.
They seem stiff and formal.
(D9)
vl2. Two African natives with The shape of the head: negroid.
their backs toward each Doing some type of ritual around a
other. Doing a religious fire. [Fire ?] The red. The color sug
dance. (Dl + D3) gested fire. [Association] Values,
rituals, tradition. [Q] Conformity.
13. Very segmented parts. Parts The red is the butterfly, the other
o f rats, monkeys, butterflies. red is little monkeys. Fish here, also
Nothing is coming together, two birds like eagles. It could also
though. (W) look like the abominable snowman.
Or a polar bear. [Association:
abominable snowman] I t ’s un
known. Does it exist? But also the
softness and power.
The stiffness denoted in the initial response contrasted with the reference
to the bodies as “free flowing.” It is yet another inconsistency or incongruity
emerging with increasing clarity as a central feature of Mr. L.’s personality
makeup. First glances, he seemed to tell the examiner, do not convey the
full intensity of what he experienced in depth. Free-flowing people are stiff,
birds in flight run away, a rat is characterized by its furriness, and the
convivial atmosphere of a pub-like, intimate conversation or dancing
around a maypole gives way to disagreement or disharmony. From the point
of view of the predominant self state, it grew increasingly clear that Mr. L.
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 235
felt keenly that the world was out of kilter for him. His opening response
to Card III of stagnant people revealed that he felt that way himself and that
he needed to recover. He wanted to feel “free flowing,” that is, more
psychologically enlivened or connected, both to others as well as to the
depths of his own internal experience.
Mr. L. craved the ability to feel vibrant or allied with what was important
to him. He felt repeatedly rebuffed or shut out from what he wanted and
had to escape from unpleasant situations with his psychological wounds
exposed like a raw nerve. He struggled to get the psychological oxygen he
wanted even if he seemed to return to the same devitalizing experiences
(expressed in all the incongruous percepts previously noted) that ulti
mately undermined the self.
Unlike Ms. T., this patient was able to seek out either an idealizing or
twinship selfobject function to help repair the self. For both patients,
however, the attempt came with considerable difficulty. Ms. T. retreated
almost immediately from the possibility of acquiring compensatory struc
ture through attempts at idealization. Mr. L. continued to try, however,
rather than giving up in defeat that might lead to the devastation of
self-esteem that was so prominent a feature of Ms. T.’s record.
Mr. L. initially attempted to see himself as psychologically alive (“free
flowing”) on Card III but quickly became “stiff and formal.” His next
response aimed to restore some bond or connection in the form of a religious
ritual, although the figures had their backs toward each other. The color
used to indicate the fire could stand for the vibrancy or enlivened feeling
he hoped to establish through the psychological connection of the ritual. It
faded, ultimately, into a routine of conformity and perhaps left him feeling
emotionally flattened.
This self state in which the psychological oxygen or enlivenment he
sought was insufficient, evoked his third response to Card III, segmented
parts of animals with “nothing coming together.” Starting with rats and
monkeys, he managed to end with the possibility that one of the animal
species is an eagle (soaring and majestic by implication, although not
actually stated by Mr. L.). The oscillation again represents his resiliency
in bouncing back after an injury to the self and attempting to restore
self-cohesion.
His final associations evoked the additional response of the abominable
snowman. Again, this powerful image is “unknown,” and Mr. L. asked:
“Does it exist?” When the reference to softness reappeared, it probably
denoted a longed-for calming or soothing to help repair the self state.
236 CHAPTER 8
Card IV
14. The head of a dragon. Bend The head, long neck, end part and
ing over, his head's touching back legs. [Sniffing ?] I don't know.
the ground. As if sniffing out Something dangerous.
something. Ready to spurt
fire. (W)
v 15. Two dogs, like puppies. Each The dark shaded area is the cliff.
is on top of a cliff, looking Looking down, giving you height.
down into the valley. (D2) [Association] Just overlooking
everything. The vastness. It's very
comforting, peaceful.
16. Part of a giant, half a part. Here's the boot. [Jack and the
From the waist down. Almost Beanstalk?] Being chased by
like Jack and the Beanstalk. something frightening but able to
Walking toward Jack, with getaway from it. Running, trying to
his boots on. (D7) get away, to find something, trying
to manipulate, outsmart. Going
into the unknown.
17. Now I see the whole giant. The whole thing is the giant. Car
The head's small, distorted. rying the goose here. [Goose ...
Accentuated on the feet, the golden eggs?] Being able to mone
boots. He's carrying the tarily afford everything you want.
goose that laid the golden Money growing on trees type of
eggs, you see the head of the thing.
goose. (W)
ineptness of others, the monks, and the religious ritual. These powerful or
awe-inspiring figures (giants, monks, the abominable snowman) suggested
that this patient was inclined to turn to strong, idealizable figures for
firming up the self when it was undermined. Likewise, imagery connoting
communion, such as the religious rite or the people in the pub, tentatively
suggested a twinship or companionate selfobject function. The feeling of
being at one with or like others can bolster self-cohesion when the sense of
the self is experienced as weakened, injured, or vulnerable.
Mr. L.’s idealizing and possibly his twinship selfobject needs were
revealed in ways that indicated that these selfobject functions were not
operating effectively. His initial response to Card IV was a dragon, but he
described it in an atypical way as bending over with its head touching the
ground, as if it were investigating a potentially dangerous situation. His
dragon was like a detective, “sniffing out something,” and it was ready to
attack (“spurt fire”) should the danger be confirmed. Rather than standing
up straight, exposing its powerful size for all to see and admire, Mr. L.’s
dragon was in a potentially threatened or vulnerable position from which it
must be vigilant and ready to protect itself.
This image of a dragon was not likely to be experienced as vigorous or
admirable in its great size, nor could it give this patient the comforting
feeling that there is someone in his corner to whom he can turn for
protection or strength. Mr. L. designated the idealized selfobject as an
imposing dragon, which does not exist in reality; he characterized his
dragon as threatened and consequently needing to be watchful (“sniffing
out”). Recall his additional percept of the previous card, the soft but still
powerful abominable snowman, which he also noted to be an imaginary
reference. Did he harbor concerns about whether idealized selfobjects were
really there for him in a sufficiently reliable or dependable manner?
This patient reported puppies perched on top of a cliff, seen with the
benefit of the shading on the blot. The puppies were overlooking a vast
terrain, and the response carried a serene affective tone. These diminutive
puppy dogs, themselves in need of being protected, were depicted as safe.
Before too quickly concluding that this percept connoted vulnerability that
can be calmed or soothed, the following two percepts of giants, the first of
which was “half a part, from the waist down,” were disquieting. One
association included a spontaneous reference to the Jack and the Beanstalk
tale, in which the boy is chased by the giant, tries to run away to safety, but
is “going into the unknown.” The danger of the dragon’s “sniffing out
something” has returned and re-exposed Mr. L. to the dangerous “un
238 CHAPTER 8
known.” That the giant was headless and the boy of this percept tried to
“outsmart” the giant highlighted Mr. L.’s reliance on intellectual defenses
or resources to protect himself. He therefore turned inward to his own
abilities or talents to safeguard a vulnerable sense of cohesion of the self.
Strong, idealizable selfobjects are vulnerable, weak, nonexistent, or endan
gered; he must be the strong person with his wits about him.
Idealizable selfobjects may be too unreliable for him to trust that they
can protect him. He thus saw himself once more as the small, powerless
boy, like the puppies of the previous response, who turned to a responsive
and dependably available selfobject environment. Mr. L. found such help
either lacking or too uncertain to last long enough to ensure a sustained
calming, as may be seen when the danger of the giant returned immedi
ately after the puppies on the cliff who felt comfortable. Note that the
selfobject need (in this instance, idealization) was revealed by its falter
ing or malfunction.
Mr. L. concluded Card IV with a second giant, this time with the benefit
of a head, but a head compromised by being “small and distorted.” This
giant, bearing the goose with the golden eggs, represented Mr. L.’s hope for
everything he wanted. His hope for the reassurance of an idealizable
selfobject might be illusory, insofar as it expressed the misdirected notion
that whenever the self felt endangered, he needed only the goose that laid
a golden (that is, protective) egg. The giant of this percept, who bore the
magic goose, distorted head notwithstanding, was another reminder that
the golden eggs that Mr. L. hoped to secure for himself probably fail him
in the end. Unlike Ms. T., however, he continued to search for a responsive
idealizing selfobject.
Unsuccessful mirroring selfobject responsiveness may have set the stage
for the readiness of the self to feel itself undermined or threatened. This
patient’s figure drawings had already indicated that Mr. L. felt devitalized
and experienced his existence as out of kilter and lacking equilibrium. Thus,
it is not difficult to understand this patient’s desire for a strong, resilient
figure to emerge as an idealizable selfobject, to assist his overtaxed capacity
to preserve self-cohesion. Puppies overlooking the peaceful expanse of land
and the association to money growing on trees are characteristic indications
of his need for calm security. Mr. L. was trying to find a way to keep a
vulnerable self sustained or afloat.
Card V
18. A bat, flying. (W) The wings. The body of it.
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 239
v 19. A bush. The legs on two sides The dark area is the bush. Leaping
o f two animals, rabbits. Run in for cover. [Q] They jump, hop,
ning behind the bush. They and they're swift. They run fast.
escaped from something, They're usually hunted.
possibly a hunter. (W)
20. A ballerina. Arms raised, and The head, chest, arms raised as in
leaping into the air. (W) a swan. Flight into the air. Freedom
of flight. The body's very graceful.
v21. A person diving into a swim The body; legs here, and the head.
ming pool. Right in the midst [Association] Being swallowed up.
o f the dive. Both arms and Encompassed by the water.
legs extended. (Dd99: W ex
cluding D6 area)
After the conventional bat, Mr. L. returned to what has been emerging
as the predominant theme of seeking safety from threat. In his response of
a scared rabbit, he commented on the swiftness of the rabbits, perhaps
another indication of his resiliency at times of danger or anxiety, reminis
cent of his percept on Card IV of the boy who used his skills and talents to
“outsmart” the foreboding giant. Mr. L. knew what his resources were, and
did not feel as defeated and defenseless as Ms. T. By this point, however, it
was becoming clear that it was a great strain for Mr. L. to sustain this effort.
There was hardly a moment’s respite, and he constantly needed to be
watchful and vigilant, like his rabbit “leaping for cover ... they’re usually
hunted.”
There followed another serene image, this time, a ballerina. Also por
trayed as a leaping figure, the ballerina was not leaping like the rabbit to
save her life from a hunter. Rather, she had “freedom of flight.” This pattern
was seen previously, on Card IV, on which the dragon spewing fire when
he sensed danger led to a puppy overlooking a peaceful terrain. Now, on
Card V, a rabbit using its wits by leaping quickly and agilely to save itself
was followed by a serene percept of a graceful ballerina calmly and freely
leaping. This calmness, however, was again transitory. The following
response represented a graceful dive, but on a probing association on
inquiry, Mr. L. confirmed the resurgence of threatened self-cohesion in his
comment about the diver’s being swallowed up. The incongruous furry rat,
hostile Oriental woman, and nuzzling elephants percepts, with their conno
tations of closed-off affectional need, are also relevant in the present
context.
240 CHAPTER 8
Card VI
22. An insect coming out o f a co The wings. Freedom.
coon, trying to fly. (W)
<23. A man lying down. Along a The nose, arms raised, his legs.
pool of water. The image be [Association] Restful, peaceful,
ing reflected, too. (Dl) but he knows he shouldn't be be
cause o f the rigidity o f the arm
extended. He's doing it but knows
he shouldn't be.
24. Two monkeys back to back. Just their bodies.
They're very happy. (Dl)
<25. A bird's nest. The heads of The nest is over here, and the
birds. Chirping, waiting to be birds are in it.
fed. (D4)
26. Two little boys, trying to The legs here, they're unable to
reach or grab something. get what they want on the coat
Their hands are extended up. rack.
Possibly from a coat rack.
One foot on the ground, one
foot up trying to reach what
ever 's on the coat rack. (Dl)
Despite the rich productivity of Card VI, it revealed little new. The
significance of an insect emerging from a cocoon and attempting to fly is
familiar enough to clinicians, and hardly requires much discussion. In
consideration of the self psychological view of the emergence of an invigo
rated self, it is useful only to point out that this self materializes through
transmuting internalizations in small increments, such as the percept of a
cocoon represents. Therefore, projective test imagery such as a cocoon is
misleading for an interpretation of genuinely firmed-up or stabilized self
cohesion. There is little in Mr. L.’s record to suggest anything different. The
image’s appearance now was merely a hope or wish that a strengthened or
cohesive self might still develop.
The image of a cocoon developing wings may also express this patient’s
need for a reliable selfobject to help consolidate more than a fleeting sense of
self-cohesion. That Mr. L.’s last response on the previous card involved a diver
being swallowed up emphasizes how crucial this need had become. His
response certainly indicated nothing about feeling that he was close to
reconstituting self-esteem.
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 241
The following response was notable first for its indication about passiv
ity. Moreover, this water scene captured the disparity between the theme of
peacefulness conveyed by the restful figure and the rigidity of the arm
position suggesting tension and distress. This incongruity is consistent with
several similar responses examined thus far in Mr. L.’s Rorschach record.
He seemed to grasp how little the serenity of the pool image indicated about
his internal self-experience. His comment about “he’s doing it but he
knows he shouldn’t be” may convey this experience.
Happy monkeys back to back, his next response, seemed relatively
innocuous and diagnostically noncontributory as well. This response was
followed by the bird’s nest with the young waiting to be fed, another passive
reference, with no further associative elaboration. Mr. L. seemed to want to
take a break on Card VI, to yield to passivity and to convey the impression
of not wanting to get too worked up or provoked by this Rorschach blot. At
the same time, he recognized that the picture of calm or peaceful images
did not reflect his actual internal state. His closing percept, describing two
little boys’ inability to get what they want, was in contrast to the nesting
birds being fed. This response suggested the association between passive
dependency and frustration of need, but it added little to what has already
been noted about the self state and predominant selfobject functions.
Card VII
27. Two little imps looking at The heads, hands, rear ends.
each other. Very mischievous. [Imps?] The shape of the face. [As
Planning what to do to have sociation] Childish-like, fun.
some fun. (W) Devil-may-care. Sitting on rocks.
v28. Two women doing the Char The movement or positioning of the
leston. (D2) body. [Association] Devil-may-
care.
v29. A mother dog and two baby The head and body. Nuzzling one
dogs. (W) baby dog and the other one coming
toward her.
v30. Two dogs, pulling, tearing Instead of this as the mother from
some material. Fighting over before, it's like a washcloth or
it, pulling it apart. (W) something. They're tearing it. It's
not a whole anymore, it's being
torn.
242 CHAPTER 8
Card VIII
31. A group o f animals going in It's circular. They're all holding
circles, like ring-around-a- hands,
rosy. (W)
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 243
32. I'm blocking. Cow's heads. How they're shaped here. [Asso
(D2) ciation] Milkyfood.
33. A rat. (Dl) The body, its shape.
34. A parrot. I can't find it now. [Association]
They're talkative, imitative. Stable
on his perch, staying in one place.
I also see two birds within a nest.
It's allfragmented, doesn't go
together.
of these four responses. Perhaps the color was destabilizing, and Mr. L.’s
avoidance of its mention represented his adaptive defense to minimize
the apparent distress that this blot provoked. Consistent with this inter
pretation favoring a self-protective dampening down of disturbing affect
was Mr. L .’s description of the parrot as “stable on his perch” as an effort
to prevent an affective breakthrough. A quiescent parrot was preferable
to sudden eruptions of loud, angry squawking, not unlike the equivalent
of a pure C response.
Thus, the parrot was reined in, dampened down, and in control. In this
respect, from the standpoint of the psychology of the self, color (or its
notable absence) can be understood as a manifestation of a self state in
varying stages of cohesion. The optimal form-dominant (FC) response
might imply good or at least reasonable self-cohesion, whereas progres
sively less stabilized degrees of self-cohesion or infirmity of the self might
be associated with the CF and C codes.
Absence of color might be understood as this patient’s attempt to find a
tolerable level of affective experience compatible with preserving optimal
self-cohesion. Avoidance of color could be understood as protecting a
vulnerable self state. Mr. L. might have too easily yielded to the overstimu
lating pull of color by giving in to the less well-integrated CF and C
manifestations of color on the Rorschach blots. The price that he paid was
a damping down of affect, a process that drove the self into hiding, as it
were. This reaction resulted in a lack of enthusiasm or vigor and captured
the affective deadness or mechanical plodding through life seen in many
patients with prominent pathology of the self. In this view, the absence of
color does not signify the defensive withdrawal from drive states but the
self-protective attempt of an injured or vulnerable self to ensure whatever
degree of cohesiveness it can manage to preserve.
Mr. L.’s overall protocol has not been characterized by affective empti
ness suggestive of depletion. The record revealed distancing from disrup
tive affects threatening to undermine his attempt to preserve self-cohesion.
Being “stable on his perch” in the midst of a world that was “all fragmented”
was the best that this patient could manage. Responsiveness to color was
disruptive to his effort. This degree of minimization of color may also
suggest that Mr. L. experienced the selfobject environment around him as
unavailable or unresponsive to his needs and thus failing to serve as a source
of assistance to buttress a vulnerable self in the face of threat or disrepair.
Mr. L.’s breakdown or loss of composure on Card VIII needs to be
understood in the contexts of the emergence of the passive-dependent mani
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 245
Card IX
<35. The color ’s hard to deal with. The father, with a moustache, nose,
A family. Thefather, sitting at the eyes. These objects make it look
the table eating dinner. He like he's not alone, but encom
doesn 't seem happy, he seems passed by the other figures.
very tired and angry. (Dl +
D4 + area adjacent to D4 up
to the midline)
36. Animals grabbing a fish in A dog who grabbed itfor food. The
their mouth. (D12) image o f it looks like a reflection.
246 CHAPTER 8
v37. Two ladies having a conver The heads, pony tails, the bodies.
sation together, dressed to Standing nose to nose. [Nose to
tally alike. (W) nose?] That's why they're dressed
alike, like they're in some type of
disagreement, like “Why are you
wearing this same thing?" [Asso
ciation: dressed alike] Imitation,
trying to be different and unable to
do so.
v38. An owl. Staring straight The white area with the shading.
ahead. (DS8) The eyes, the tail. [Association]
Wise, watchful. Just seeing every
thing but being very watchful,
questioning. Being noncommittal.
Card X
39. A king and his court. His sub The king, his cape like the King of
jects are looking at him. (W) Siam. The crown, his hair. All the
way around are his subjects. His
arms are outstretched. [Associa
tion] Total control as if he'll grant
unto thee what he wants to grant
unto thee. Very powerful. Very
decadent and ostentatious. His sub
jects are very plain and ordinary.
v40. A group o f sea horses. Fish swimming here, fast enough to
There's fis h swim m ing keep each one in its place. [Associa
around them. Each one is tion] Nothing really. They also
separated by the fish as if carry around their young, protect
they're captive. And crabs, ing its young.
but again I see things in iso
lation, nothing together. Like
the crabs alone, the animals
alone, nothing together. (W)
family, represented here metaphorically as the smaller fish, who are pow
erful enough in their resourcefulness to keep the sea horse immobilized.
Many parents do indeed feel just this way about their adolescent children,
particularly if the children are in trouble. Coupled with the need to be
“protecting its young,” this patient seemed to express here his own conflict
about asserting his control and being protective of his “subjects,” but
feeling undermined and impotent in the struggle.
In the end, he felt “in isolation” (like his monks of Card II); “nothing is
together.” Again, Mr. L. conveyed the self state of an injured, devitalized,
and immobilized man doing whatever he can to preserve self-cohesion. He
was “in isolation” in a selfobject environment that seemed to frustrate him,
and consequently, he experienced himself and his world as one in which
“nothing is together.” This isolation is not that of schizoid detachment.
The course of the Rorschach protocol shows the progressive devitaliza
tion of the self. Beginning with two people on Card I attempting but
ultimately failing “in disagreement” to establish a viable self-selfobject
unit, Mr. L. came to the end of the protocol with a family in disarray, two
women “in some type of disagreement” over looking alike, and finally
feeling captive by his conflict between “protecting his young” and domi
nating them in a powerful yet controlling manner.
Card 1.
That’s a young boy looking at his violin, very confused. He didn’t know what
to do with it because it’s broken. He thinks it through and decides to repair
it. He has to get glue and glue it together so no one would realize it was
damaged. [Led up?] He was playing around, it didn’t belong to him, and he
was too rough, so he broke off the top part. [How does he feel?] Puzzled and
unhappy. [Outcome?] He’s relieved. No one would find out.
the violin, but he cannot feel pride in making the repair because he was
intent on concealing what he felt was misunderstood as wrongdoing.
The self psychological view of Mr. L .’s story to Card 1 recast the
oedipal theme as a concern that more or less normal inquisitive or
exploratory urges may go awry. He needed to summon the resourceful
ness to quietly restore the situation to normal by himself. Most important,
this patient felt that others failed to understand his wishes and faulted
him for them or in some manner showed displeasure toward him.
Card 2.
example, Mr. L.’s first drawing was that of a young man stimulated by
nature, and Card I of the Rorschach began with the pub-like atmosphere for
a congenial talk, leading up to two figures dancing around a maypole.
These responses soon became dominated by the emerging need of pro
tecting self-esteem. These enthusiastic openings very shortly dampened
down, to be replaced by feelings of being misunderstood and having to
surmount obstacles. On the previous TAT card, the boy of Card 1 was
able to repair the violin and was “puzzled and unhappy”; his sense of
relief came only from escaping detection rather than from satisfaction.
Now, on Card 2, people accepted their lot in life, ignored one another’s
needs, and existed in “total isolation.” The emptiness and devitalization
were certainly palpable, but so too was the glimmer of what Mr. L.
needed in selfobject responsiveness to find some way out of his dilemma.
Like his monks of Card II, Mr. L. did not seek an isolated existence apart
from a selfobject milieu; rather, he asked only that his environment
should be responsive to his mirroring and idealization needs.
That mirroring has been deficient or undermined was rather evident
by this point, but how Mr. L. attempted to get what he needed to sustain
self-cohesion was not entirely clear. It is important to determine whether
stable compensatory structures became established or whether idealiza
tion or twinship were viable alternatives to restore his self-esteem. It is
critical to see how the selfobject environment materialized for him as
the TAT continued, in view of indications from the Rorschach about
tentative attempts to secure either idealization or twinship selfobject
functions.
Card 3BM.
A young person, female I believe. Some tragedy just happened. She’s totally
distraught, not knowing what to do. A solution wont be found. [Led up?] A
death. Her husband. [How does she feel?] Distraught, she doesn’t know what
to do next. uHow can I take over, everything’s unmanageable.”[Outcome?]
She picks up after awhile and life goes on. She fits the pieces of her life back
together and possibly remarries.
After the initial reaction to the inability to find a solution, the patient
showed a capacity to regain composure. This resourcefulness was seen
previously, most tellingly following Card VIII on the Rorschach. Moreover,
the figure on Card 3BM recovered from a state of distress by herself, without
any indication that a particular selfobject function was involved in the
260 CHAPTER 8
repair of the self state. The memory of the deceased husband or the prospect
of a remarriage was not compelling enough here to form the basis for a
potential selfobject function. Even a memory might qualify, because as
Kohut emphasized, a selfobject is not synonymous with a person; it is the
function of the object that is crucial, whether or not it takes the form of an
existing person. No one was available or remembered or sought to help the
person. Rather, the person was left alone with her despair.
In a similar fashion, on Card 2, all the figures went their own way,
oblivious to and unconcerned about one another. Previously, on Card 1,
the boy who broke the violin was left to his own devices to find a way
out of his dilemma. Mr. L. seemed to go it alone, but not from a
masochistic need or feeling of unworthiness. Instead, this patient did not
expect someone to become available to whom he could turn to bolster
self-esteem. By itself, this indicates some measure of resiliency or
self-sufficiency and suggests what might be a psychological asset. In a
self psychological view, this apparent strength was simultaneously a
liability insofar as it indicates that the patient existed in a less-than-op-
timal selfobject environment. Why did the patient present a picture of
“toughing it out” of this magnitude?
One answer to this question is derived from the advantage of a test
battery in which findings across several tests combine to highlight a
fuller picture of the personality compared to isolated aspects on single
test measures. Thus, one knows from Mr. L .’s Rorschach that he with
drew from reaching out to his selfobject environment when he experienced
disappointment or empathic failure. As a result, he felt alone too often with
his own distress to risk seeking self-restorative selfobjects.
As with the dragon sniffing danger (on Card IV from the Rorschach)
when he reduced a powerful image to one that is itself threatened and needs
to protect itself, Cards 3BM and 1 of the TAT showed Mr. L. left to his own
devices to find his way out of difficult straits. The dragon on Card IV was
followed by a boy needing to outsmart a giant to protect himself from
potential harm; on TAT Card 1, the boy thought of a solution by gluing the
broken violin himself; on Card 3BM, the despondent figure also reconsti
tuted by fitting the pieces of her life together on her own.
Mr. L. seemed to indicate that he has managed to rebuild self-cohesion
by either gluing broken pieces or by slowly fitting pieces of a shattered
life together. Thus, the violin of Card 1 and the distraught situation on
Card 3BM represented an injured and diminished self state. One pathway
to repair was through idealization or twinship, as noted previously from
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 261
Card 4.
A woman is trying to hold her husband back from fighting a person who
insulted her. She tried to do it through reason, but reason’s no longer there.
262 CHAPTER 8
He’s too angry. [Outcome?] He breaks loose and ends up in a big brawl, but
he comes out on top.
On Card 4, for the first time thus far, one figure depicted in the picture
appeared willing to come to the aid of another. The paradox, however, was
that the woman whose honor was being defended was unable to make a
sufficient psychological impact on her protector because “reason’s no
longer there.” This response is not appreciably different from Mr. L.’s
response to Rorschach Card I in which two people tried to talk in a congenial
atmosphere but ended in disagreement. Card 4 resembles other Rorschach
responses, notably the family with the tired and angry father (Card IX), two
ladies in conversation but also in disagreement (Card IX), and the decadent
and ostentatious king surveying his subjects (Card X). As Mr. L. developed
his story to Card 4, it became evident that the woman was quickly forgotten
and receded into the background, whereas the man was consumed by anger,
unreasonableness, and the need to be victorious.
It is difficult to know whether the figure representing the central object
of identification for Mr. L. was the woman whose needs were ignored or
the man whose needs for winning were central. People were not seen as on
his side or acting truly out of concern for his best interest. Previously, on
Card 2, other people were indifferent, if not actually withholding; on Cards
1 and 3BM, other people were unavailable or unempathically responsive.
Mr. L.’s story to Card 4 again showed that he expected little beneficial
concern from others. Even the appearance of coming to his aid was not, in
the long run, truly real or genuinely expressed for him. As conceptualized
in the self psychological framework, Mr. L.’s response revealed a self
experience in which he was ignored. Those selfobject functions, such as
mirroring or idealization, that could help a flagging self, ultimately disap
pointed because they fail to take account of what he needed to restore
self-cohesion, submerged as they were by others’ needs for power, vindi
cation, or “coming out on top.”
Card 6BM.
The woman’s first concern was her own failure that contributed to the
son’s wayward behavior. A picture of estrangement was emphasized remi
niscent of the family scene of Card 2. Although the mother was not depicted
as indifferent to the son’s death, neither was her concern the central
emphasis of Mr. L.’s story. The mother asked what she did wrong and
conveyed herself as being nurturant, but the tone of the story suggests that
the problem lay with the son who did not permit the mother to know him
intimately. Once again, the focus is on protecting her self-esteem, just as
the man of Card 4 was more concerned about his self-esteem in “coming
out on top” rather than about protecting the woman.
Did Mr. L. function in a world in which he saw others as too self-absorbed
to notice him, or was he himself so greatly in need of protecting his own
self-esteem that he has little interest in others? An examiner can identify a
need or feeling state, but cannot always tell whether the need is either
experienced internally or externalized in the form of a projection. The
question is fundamental to the clinical workup of psychodiagnostic testing,
regardless of the theoretical persuasion that any examiner brings to the
interpretation of this material. This element of interpretation has been
stressed by Schafer (1954), Schachtel (1966), and P. M. Lerner (1991).
Regardless of the uncertainty about the answer to this question, the
indications in the clinical material of being unresponded to, ignored, or
depreciated frequently convey salient features about the patient’s self-es
teem. In the example at hand, one can at best guess at Mr. L.’s capacity for
interest in others, but it is possible to conclude that he very much feels
himself to be ignored or forgotten about.
It is perhaps no accident that references to protective nurturing appear
more directly on Card 6BM than on the previous TAT cards; maternal
responsiveness was strongly evoked on this card. Mr. L. seemed to be
dispassionately wondering aloud: Where did the maternal care go wrong,
or where was it lacking? The theme of the death of a child is powerful,
rarely portrayed more vividly than in Mahler’s Kindertotenlieder, his
setting of haunting elegies to dead children. This extremely rare theme on
Card 6BM is even more gripping than the story about the death of the
husband on Card 3BM, which was dealt with nonchalantly. Granted such a
powerful theme, what is it about a mother who sees herself as nurturant that
leads a child to turn away to the extent that there appears to be so little
internalization of the mother? On Card 6BM, the story ended simply with
the comment about the difference in values between the mother and son and
264 CHAPTER 8
how their lives diverged as a result. This remark is defensive, of course, but
also affectively distanced.
The story outcome recalls the key dynamic of Mr. L.’s story to Card 2,
most notably the marked deficiency of mirroring selfobject responsive
ness. As with Card 2 and the ensuing TAT responses, the central issue is not
so much the question about defective mirroring but about the potential for
compensatory structure, such as idealization or twinship, to take over as a
viable pathway for repairing and restoring self-esteem.
Card 7BM.
A young man totally rejecting the opinion of his father who is very stem, hard,
and very rigid, uncaring. [Led up?] They never saw or understood each other.
No real interaction. The boy turned his back on his father. [Outcome?] The
father dies, and the son goes his own way. [Why a rift?] The father didn't
have time for his son. He has no concern. He never wanted children.
Card 12M.
A woman just died. A priest comes to give her the last rites. She died alone,
with no one around her. The priest was her only friend.
Mr. L.’s story to Card 12M was atypical. Usually, a man is said to be lying
on a bed, and the intention of the figure hovering above becomes the focus
of the story. Here, the figure was mistaken for a woman, and instead of being
ill or asleep, as customarily, the woman was dead. The priest represented the
“psychological oxygen” needed to revive a depleted self, if it is understood
that the figure of the dead person stood for a self in a state of devitalization.
The image of a priest giving last rites recalled Mr. L.’s Rorschach percept of
monks touching hands, from Card II.
Here the priest was cast in the role of supreme comforter. This percept
contrasted with the benign communing of the withdrawn monks, an order
characterized by extreme self-denial and turning away from the world.
Although not quite the same as a savior, here the implication was that the
potentially idealizable priest could not revive the dying person. This pa
tient’s attempts at idealization or twinship as seen on the Rorschach
became too undependable to adequately do the job.
In some respects, therefore, Card 12M may be viewed as a continuation
of Cards 6BM and 7BM. On Card 6BM, the son has died and the mother
was turned away from; on Card 7BM, the father was emotionally indifferent
and unavailable for genuine responsiveness. Now, on Card 12M, the figure
died alone and without support; her only comfort arrived too late.
The stories for these three cards considered in succession suggest that
the self lacked adequate mirroring of the need to be seen as valued,
important, or as a source of pleasure. The mother of Card 6BM was a figure
who turned away from the unmirrored self, as if to say “You are not my
child; your values are alien to me.” Subsequently, an attempt to revitalize
266 CHAPTER 8
the self through some other potential selfobject function, such as idealiza
tion or twinship, was spurned by the father of Card 7BM who showed no
interest or availability for that purpose. Thus, the possibility of the father’s
serving a protective or revitalizing selfobject function was virtually oblit
erated. Accordingly, there was little hope of restoring self-esteem by
developing a viable compensatory structure. As a result, the self was left in
a devitalized state, in effect bereft and dying as on Card 12M, with its only
means of sustaining itself too late to the rescue.
On Mr. L.’s Rorschach, the hope for idealization or twinship as a
means to sustain flagging self-cohesion ultimately became too unreli
able. This state gave rise to the passivity and “going in circles” that
emerged there. Modestly reconstituting the self allowed him to hold his
own, “stable on his perch,” albeit at some cost to firmly secured self-co-
hesion. It may be anticipated that Mr. L. will find a way to emerge from
the bleak picture of a self in ruin, because this capability was previously
detected on the Rorschach.
Card 13MF.
After making love, a man realized that in the act of love he killed her. He is
totally distraught in what he did, and the passions that led up to it. So he kills
himself.
Mr. L. seemed to say here that strong passions or needs can, in effect,
kill. It is well known that Card 13MF is an emotionally provocative card
for all but the most affectively distanced individuals. That Mr. L. could find
a way for the mother to emotionally wall off the death of her son on Card
6BM points to this patient’s adept way of keeping emotions in check. The
same emotional distance appeared in the marked coldness in Mr. L.’s story
on Card 7BM about a father-son relationship. There, he managed to change
lack of support and emotional indifference to a near virtue, as he did earlier
on Card 2. Now, on Card 13MF, the stimulus may be too overpowering and
may cost him more effort to withdraw affectively. He seemed able to
accomplish this withdrawal only by killing himself off. It is really the
intensity of his affective arousal that was killed off or obliterated.
These responses alongside the passivity and affective flattening seen on
his Rorschach protocol indicate that Mr. L. needed to go to great lengths to
keep himself “stable on his perch.” The self psychological implication,
however, of this degree of emotional withdrawal is that this man was left
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 267
with a self state that was killed off or driven underground. There seem to
be few real available selfobject functions for him to draw on to sustain a
cohesive self without having to dampen it down to this extent.
Card 18GF.
A mother nurturing her child who's been hurt by falling down the stairs.
From very early on, what a child needs is empathic responses to him as a
self (or at least as an anticipated self). Not to his drives. Every time a mother
gives milk to the baby, she is feeding her hungry child—she is not feeding
a drive. A mother, an empathic mother, an empathic mothering environ
ment never responds to a drive; it responds to a child.
Discussion of Mr. L.
This patient presented for treatment with a depressive syndrome clinically
similar to that of Ms. T. The chief complaint and mental status findings
revealed a comparable degree of severity of symptomatic distress. The
268 CHAPTER 8
precipitant was more identifiable in Mr. L.’s case and, in addition, he had a
somewhat more favorable premorbid psychosocial adjustment in compari
son to Ms. T. In generally similar clinical syndromes of comparable severity,
the productive responses that these two patients gave illustrate similarities
and differences in psychodynamic makeup. Both cases illustrate that a self
psychological viewpoint may be applied to projective test interpretation as
an alternative to classical drive theory or ego psychological theory.
Devalued self-esteem with ensuing disruption of cohesiveness of the self
was a prominent feature throughout the projective test protocols. The ways
that these patients attempted to repair a devitalized self centered on the
differential capacity to make use of selfobject functions of idealization or
twinship. These selfobject functions operate as potential compensatory
structures to assist in recovery from a self disorder. They represent alterna
tive pathways to repair self-esteem in the face of pathogenic unempathic
responsiveness to normal mirroring needs.
Whether or not one follows a self psychological theoretical position as
a basis for interpretation, it is not difficult to see that Mr. L. experienced
almost constant rebuffs or disappointment wherever he turned. One dis
tinctive contribution of self psychology to understanding the experiences
of a patient like Mr. L. follows from the theory’s emphasis on empathic
failure or unresponsiveness leading to feeling undermined and devitalized.
Diminished or devalued self-cohesion was the basis for Mr. L.’s depression.
It is not difficult to discern how this mechanism operates from the
content analysis of Mr. L.’s projective test responses.
I previously pointed out that several of Mr. L.’s projective test responses
could be conceptualized from the standpoint of an ego psychological
approach to drive theory. In particular, defensive and adaptive aspects of
oedipal dynamics are clear from the patient’s references to strong, overpow
ering figures on Card IV on the Rorschach, for example, and from his
preoccupation with secrecy on TAT Card 1. Even the oral wishes previously
noted after Mr. L.’s breakdown on the last chromatic cards on the Rorschach
could be viewed as an oedipal regression. I have attempted to show in my
discussion of this material, however, that the same themes may be concep
tualized from the viewpoint of Kohut’s psychology of the self, specifically,
the relation between the oedipal situation and devitalization of the self.
Kohut thought that oedipal children expect parents to admire or affirm
them; the children’s desires are not primarily drive dominated or sexualized.
If a parent of the opposite sex is capable of resonating with the child’s wish
for acknowledgment in an empathically attuned way, the experience echoes
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 269
or mirrors the child’s primary need. If the parent is unable to provide this
response to the oedipal child’s essentially normal developmental assertive
or prideful urges on a regular basis, the child again and again is re-exposed
to the empathic failures of the unresponsive parent. This experience inter
rupts maintaining a buoyant or joyfully expectant self and sets the stage for
chronic injury to self-cohesion. This experience forms the substrate for
diminished self-esteem and predisposes to a self disorder, and as a result,
the child may feel crushed or insignificant. This process is evident in the
sequence of Mr. L.’s anticipation of a welcoming or exuberant response,
which is then followed by confusion, disappointment, and the sense that the
bottom has dropped out. This feature is more telling than the more usual
interpretation of oedipal material.
For example, the design of Card IV was initially elaborated as powerful
and vigorous, but this response was soon undermined and ultimately
became linked with danger. Some caution is necessary before one assumes
that the towering percepts on Card IV represent a disguised oedipal conflict.
In a similar fashion, an interpretation of superego conflict might be pre
ferred on the basis of phallic-oedipal wishes, such as those suggested on
TAT Card 1 by the broken instrument and the need for secrecy. A genetic
reconstruction based on the oedipal situation is not even uncritically ac
cepted as a necessary or universal psychodynamic configuration in con
temporary psychoanalysis.
Without attempting to argue the issue of the fate of the oedipal situation
in psychoanalytic theory, I want to point out that although some clinicians
interpret contents about simultaneously threatening and powerful phallic-
sounding imagery, wrongdoing, or the need for secrecy as derived from
oedipal dynamics, these same contents might also be conceptualized from
a self psychological interpretation of the oedipal situation. Feelings of
shame, smallness, or the inability to stand up for oneself may result from
parents’ inability to enjoy or applaud an oedipal child’s assertive urges,
perhaps because of the parents’ empathic limitations, depression, or vulner
ability to feeling threatened by assertion. Thus, reactions of devitalization
and depletion depression may be what an oedipal child is left with after his
or her developmentally appropriate stirrings are misunderstood, criticized,
or otherwise undermined. As a result, feelings of wrongdoing or secrecy
might represent what is more accurately the shame or humiliation of
someone who is made to feel insignificant or worthless.
The more important question, however, centers on Mr. L.’s attempts to
repair frequent rebuffs to self-esteem. What did this patient attempt to effect
270 CHAPTER 8
in his world and with others to recover from the injuries to the self?
Identifying the interplay between available selfobject functions and com
pensatory structures is one way to answer this question.
This approach is not that different from the ego psychological approach
that conceptualizes defenses as attempts to protect the person from anxiety
resulting from superego conflict related to drive discharge. The psychologi
cal objective for adaptation may differ in the ego psychological and self
psychological viewpoints, in which either the self-reparative or defensive
function becomes a primary focus of the diagnostic study. In the ego
psychological framework, the emphasis is on the identification of defenses
and evaluating their effectiveness or resiliency. From the point of view of
the psychology of the self, the question becomes one of identifying selfob
ject functions in the service of restoring self-esteem.
This central question forms the basis for the balance of my discussion
about Mr. L. The essential feature of this patient’s self disorder began with
his attempt to turn to others with the expectation of being responded to;
instead, his efforts were ignored. Mr. L. saw himself like the disjointed
family members of Card 2 on the TAT; they went their own ways, unaware
of and unconcerned about one another. As on Cards 1 and 3BM, Mr. L. had
to make his way through life under his own power. He must pick up the
pieces of emotional disarray in whatever way he can and with little antici
pation of emotional support.
It was difficult for Mr. L. to depend on a selfobject environment to
provide affirming or empathic recognition when he was injured. He could
not expect to receive admiration for his accomplishments or attributes in a
reliably empathic manner. Consequently, mirroring selfobject functions
were driven underground. The hints of budding mirroring longings are best
understood as tentative attempts to secure this function, but these are at
best fragments or glimmers of a selfobject need probably long abandoned.
Any vestiges of mirroring selfobject needs are best construed as remnants
of, or undermined efforts to achieve, sustained mirroring. These needs
were not an established or viable part of Mr. L.’s psychological orientation
to the world.
This fact clearly appeared on the first Rorschach card. After the initial
revival of joyous hopefulness, there followed the re-emergence of the
devitalization he evidently experienced at the hands of a selfobject environ
ment that failed to respond to his animation. Thus, his needs were unmet
and were empathically misunderstood. For the same reason, the boy of Card
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 271
1 on the TAT never turned elsewhere for guidance or direction; he had only
his own resources to rely on to find a way out of his dilemma.
Against this backdrop of insufficient mirroring perpetuating a devalued
self, one sees the attempt to seek other selfobject functions to restore
self-esteem. Thus, Mr. L. first indicated his needs, the ways that these were
ignored or rebuffed, and his feeling that mirroring selfobject responsiveness
was not there for him. He conveyed that his attempts to achieve idealization
and twinship selfobject functions might help him fortify self-cohesion in
the absence of adequate mirroring. At first, he invoked images that provided
either a calming function or a degree of cooperative understanding. In this
way, Mr. L. expressed the wish to feel buoyed up or connected with
someone, out of which the possibility of being understood might emerge.
The patient’s urgent wish to feel himself psychologically enlivened
repeatedly met with frustration or rebuffs from the selfobject environ
ment as seen most clearly on the early Rorschach blots. He continued to
turn to potentially strong, idealizable figures as well as to figures
providing a companionate function, and mirroring selfobjects appeared
as a distant memory from which he has withdrawn. Their chronic failure
to operate reliably led Mr. L. to instead attempt to secure idealization
and twinship selfobject functions that have evidently not become so
totally shut off for him.
His effort was an uphill struggle, and he turned hopefully to idealization
and twinship selfobjects, but had to retreat from these as well. Mr. L. did
not, however, abandon the possibility that either twinship or idealization
could serve as potential routes to repairing the devitalized self state. And
so it went, beginning with Card II and oscillating back and forth in this way
until at least Card VIII.
This process is the way by which, if successful, compensatory structure
is built up, as an alternative pathway to sustaining a cohesive self that is
injured or whose stability becomes compromised. This point is also the most
critical difference between Mr. L. and Ms. T. Unlike Ms. T., who was
probably too damaged to be able to maintain the effort to acquire compen
satory structure through idealization, Mr. L. showed a relatively less severe
degree of devitalization resulting from inadequate mirroring. He had more
resilience and attempted to seek idealization or twinship selfobject respon
siveness. Ms. T., in contrast, could make only the most tentative and
short-lived attempt at idealization. Passivity appeared increasingly as Mr.
L. experienced rebuff or empathic failure at nearly every turn. Although
272 CHAPTER 8
better able than was Ms. T. to stay the course, he had only marginal success
in his effort to secure this type of compensatory structure.
By the time he reached Card VIII, this patient showed the first significant
psychological testing indication of what Kohut described as a disintegration
product. He was sufficiently destabilized to abandon the effort to seek
idealization or twinship selfobjects. As a result, he had diminished ability
to continue pursuing the vigor that he sought in a responsive selfobject to
revive a flagging, devitalized self. The disappointment that the selfobject
environment did not respond empathically drove the self, in effect, into
hiding and disposed him to a joyless, mechanical plodding through an
existence in which expecting to be understood, affirmed, or responded to
was considerably diminished.
This deadening of affective arousal produced the ennui or listlessness
that impeded Mr. L. from enthusiastically taking part in life. It appeared
superficially as depression, signifying the failure of selfobjects to function
optimally. It foreshadowed the depletion of the self and its protective
shutting down or withdrawal. The passivity and increased dependency
indications seen before Card VIII did not therefore represent features of
depression or even the ingrained characterologic predisposition to a depres
sive personality. Rather, the passivity can be understood as this patient’s
best effort to be “stable on his perch.”
Mr. L. briefly revived the effort to sustain self-cohesion, but it led him
nowhere and he ultimately returned to a position of being undermined and
immobilized. He was “in isolation” from a selfobject environment that has
not functioned well for him. When he said “nothing is together,” it became
his metaphor for feeling depleted and worn down.
Failing to restimulate a viable selfobject environment through compen
satory structures of idealization and twinship, he grew increasingly adrift,
passively withdrawn, and consequently trying to get by in whatever way he
could manage. With a markedly dampened-down affect life, Mr. L. experi
enced the joylessness and understimulation of someone defeated and had
little reason to struggle in an unresponsive selfobject environment.
It is hardly surprising that Mr. L.’s TAT responses were dominated by
stories in which relationships were characterized by puzzlement and indif
ference. The predominant feeling tone was one of being alone with his
problems, without considering that someone else could be sufficiently
understanding. The figures in Mr. L.’s stories solved problems alone,
without anticipation of support. The most striking feature of these interac
tions was not that others failed to comprehend or disappointed in their
IDEALIZATION AND TWINSHIP 273
availability, but rather that nobody ever expected anyone to come to another
person’s assistance.
The main indications about the interplay of selfobject functions appeared
in Mr. L.’s Rorschach responses. Although evidence for faulty mirroring
was consistent throughout all the projective protocols, the emergence of
idealization and twinship occurred predominantly on the Rorschach. There
does not seem to be an affinity between selfobject functions and particular
projective tests. In general, the fact that an injury to self-esteem has occurred
defines the self state. It is the self state that is consistently detected on
projective psychological tests. There may be less regularity regarding the
appearance of idealization and twinship selfobject functions on one or
another projective test.
Twinship and idealization selfobject functions were weakly apparent in
the previous case of Ms. T., but were more vigorously pursued by Mr. L. in
an effort to establish compensatory structures, at least until he became too
undermined to sustain this effort with any success. Regardless of the
viability of idealization or twinship as durable selfobject functions, these
functions do not seem to be characteristically associated with any specific
projective test instrument.
POSTSCRIPT:
SUMMARY AND REFLECTIONS
274
POSTSCRIPT 275
satisfied, or the clinician can probe the associations that (dare I say it)
remind patients or make then think of such responses containing an M code.
It is the difference between ringing someone’s doorbell to see whether the
person is visibly at home and examining the person’s diaries or personal
papers to understand what “floats his boat.”
None of what I have said is new. Of course, it goes without saying that
any psychodiagnostic testing examiner needs to understand which method
or combination of methods is compatible with his or her temperamental
makeup. Like inspecting TAT cards or Rorschach blots, there is no right or
wrong answer to this problem; it is largely a restatement of the age-old
question about how clinicians understand what is inside the black box of a
person’s psychological existence. All that is new about restating this con
troversy is that examiners who are interested in understanding the interior
life of self states and self disorders will need to probe for a degree of
depth with sufficient intensity and persistence. For the moment, scales of
narcissism or self-esteem simply do not pass muster.
If a clinician seeks answers to questions about problems that are fairly
close to the surface, a probing examination may be unwarranted. If the
problem is deeply concealed, the investigation may need to be aggressive.
I have selected clinical examples that lead to straightforward inferences
about self states. Understandably, the interpretive value of records that are
less well elaborated is not as evident. Indeed, most Rorschach records are
more banal and frequently not as distinctive as many of those reproduced
in these pages.
Thus, there is a difference in kind between the quality of responses or
verbalizations obtained from rich, productive protocols, and those ob
tained from ordinary or banal records. This difference can be reduced,
however, if examiners are so inclined, by obtaining carefully elicited
associations about motivational states at appropriate points in the inquiry.
Examiners must be diligent in following up salient comments. For exam
ple, ascertaining a beginning, middle, and outcome to TAT stories usually
does not suffice; provocative comments about the story must be pursued
cautiously but vigorously. The reserve necessary to conduct a careful,
unobtrusive Rorschach inquiry need not be regarded as a model when
applied to the TAT or figure drawings.
Lest my intention be misunderstood, I am not arguing to supplant the
unobtrusive Rorschach inquiry. Rather, I argue that something can be
added by augmenting the standard Comprehensive System inquiry and
moving beyond a perceptual problem solving function to permit the unfold
POSTSCRIPT 277
ing of the predominant self state. Admittedly, not all clinicians are disposed
to use inquiry data in this way. In the final analysis, the techniques of
administering projective tests ultimately remain a matter of clinical and
theoretical persuasion. Likewise, clinicians need not fear that conducting
probing inquiries inevitably contaminates clinical findings any more than
they should feel they must artificially guard against influencing patients’
responses by looking patients in the eye and thereby revealing the examin
ers’ reactions to the clinical material. Being well-trained means that clini
cians have the necessary internal self-discipline to control their reactions
without having to resort to playing “musical chairs” or other contrived
devices to do this for them. Examiners who have difficulty with this
fundamental clinical temperament might find greater satisfaction in other
endeavors. In this context, it is worth remembering that the reason Freud
advocated using the couch was not primarily to stimulate regression or to
facilitate free association. The real reason is because he could not stand being
looked at or scrutinized searchingly throughout the day by his patients.
I argued the case for conducting a detailed and vigorous inquiry at some
length in chapter 4. The extended case study of Mr. L. (chap. 8) demon
strated this clinical situation compellingly, in a relatively well-compensated
patient. On the other hand, severely disorganized patients, many with
borderline personality disorder, more readily reveal their psychological
distress, which can include states of profoundly disturbed self-cohesion.
Such patients characteristically lack the defensive resilience to protect
themselves effectively. Thus, their painfully experienced affect states and
compromised reality testing emerge in a seemingly uninhibited way, leak
ing or oozing out, because they cannot prevent this discharge. The case of
Ms. T. (chap. 7) illustrated this form of psychopathology.
Many people have less pronounced self disorders than did Ms. T. and
Mr. L. Many show a level of resilience, despite pronounced disorders of
self-cohesion, intermediate between severe and mild self disorders. These
patients not infrequently manage to elude themselves, the clinicians who
treat them, and the psychodiagnosticians examining their personalities.
Their disturbances are often manifested in subtle nuances and require a
sufficiently probing inquiry.
In chapters 5 and 6, I discussed a number of clinical examples of
devitalized self states that were derived from subtle indications. These
examples require appropriately rigorous restraint to avoid overinterpreting
their meanings. Schafer’s criteria are a helpful guide in this effort. Never
theless, figures described as standing or looking around with nothing to do,
278 POSTSCRIPT
281
282 REFERENCES
Levine, F. J. (1979). On the clinical application of Kohut’s psychology of the self: Comments
on some recently published case studies. Journal of the Philadelphia Association for
Psychoanalysis, 6, 1-19.
Lichtenberg, J. D. (1991). What is a selfobject? Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 1, 455-479.
Loewald, H. W. (1980). Papers on psychoanalysis. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Lovett, C. G. (1988). The Rorschach assessment of internalization mechanisms in depres
sion. In H. D. Lemer & P. M. Lerner (Eds.), Primitive mental states and the Rorschach
(pp. 107-153). Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Machover, K. (1949). Personality projection in the drawing of the humanfigure. Springfield,
IL: Thomas.
Mahler, M. S. (1968). On human symbiosis and vicissitudes of individuation: Vol. 1. Infantile
psychosis. New York: International Universities Press.
Malin, A. (1988). A short history of the psychoanalytic approach to the treatment of
psychotic disorders. In A. Goldberg (Ed.), Progress in self psychology: Vol. 3, Frontiers
in self psychology (pp. 81-86). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Mayman, M. (1967). Object-representations and object-relationships in Rorschach re
sponses. Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 31, 17-24.
Mayman, M. (1970). Reality contact, defense effectiveness, and psychopathology in Ror
schach form level scores. In B. Klopfer, M. Meyer, & F Brawer (Eds.), Developments
in Rorschach technique, (Vol. Ill, pp. 11-44). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Menninger, K. (1958). Theory of psychoanalytic technique. New York: Harper & Row.
Morrison, A. P. (1984). Shame and the psychology of the self. In P. E. Stepansky & A.
Goldberg (Eds.), Kohut’s legacy: Contributions to selfpsychology (pp. 71-90). Hillsdale,
NJ: Analytic Press.
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press.
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test: Manual. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Ornstein, P. H. (1978). The evolution of Heinz Kohut’s psychoanalytic psychology of the
self. In P. H. Ornstein (Ed.), The search for the self: Selected writings o f Heinz Kohut:
1950-1978. (Vol. 1, pp. 1-106). New York: International Universities Press.
Ornstein, P. H. (1987). On self-state dreams in the psychoanalytic treatment process. In A.
Rothstein (Ed.), The interpretation of dreams in clinical work (pp. 87-104). Madison,
CT: International Universities Press.
Ornstein, P. H. (1990). The unfolding and completion of Heinz Kohut’s paradigm of
psychoanalysis. In P. H. Ornstein (Ed.), The searchfor the self: Selected writings of Heinz
Kohut: 1978-1981. (Vol. 3, pp. 1-82). New York: International Universities Press.
Pine, F. (1988). The four psychologies of psychoanalysis and their place in clinical work.
Journal o f the American Psychoanalytic Association, 36, 571-596.
Rapaport, D. (1951). The autonomy of the ego. Bulletin o f the Menninger Clinic, 15,
113-123.
Rapaport, D., Gill, M. M., & Schafer, R. (1945). Diagnostic psychological testing. Chicago:
Year Book.
Rapaport, D., Gill, M. M., & Schafer, R. (1968). Diagnostic psychological testing: Revised
edition. (R. R. Holt, Ed.). New York: International Universities Press.
286 REFERENCES
A E
H M
P S