Benitez & Justicia 2006 - Bullying Description and Analysis of The Phenomenon
Benitez & Justicia 2006 - Bullying Description and Analysis of The Phenomenon
Benitez & Justicia 2006 - Bullying Description and Analysis of The Phenomenon
net/publication/228377096
CITATIONS READS
25 739
2 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Fernando Justicia on 31 May 2014.
Spain
[email protected]
[email protected]
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170 - 151 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
Abstract
This article purports to present this Special Issue about Bullying and, at the same time, to
introduce the phenomenon of bullying in order to facilitate readers an updated vision about
the problem that will be worked from different perspectives by researchers from national and
international scope. With this purpose, we present some controversial aspects in the charac-
terization of bullying related to definition, incidence and prevalence of bullying and the influ-
ence of specific variables like age and gender. At the same time, it is done a revision of the
more frequent types of bullying behaviours, the characterization of involved agents, as well as
the analysis of the most important risk factosr those are underliying problem’s genesis. The
final goal is to provide information about bullying for facilitating the characterization and
comprehension, more specifically, of a real problem that is interesting and important into the
educational centres.
- 152 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
Introduction
Bullying is not a new problem for schools, since it has been present for a long time;
however, only in recent years is its importance being recognized. We speak of a specific phe-
nomenon of school violence which affects schools around the world regardless of national
borders, geography or politics (Debarbieux, 2003). Ever since the initial pioneer studies by
Olweus in Scandinavian countries, many other studies have followed. In the first phase of
research, most studies focused on an attempt to define the problem (Olweus, 1993; Rivers &
Smith, 1994; Crick, Casas & Ku, 1999), giving way to other studies addressing the incidence
of the problem (Boulton, 1993; Olweus, 1996; Smith, Morita, Junger-Tas, Olweus, Catalano
& Slee, 1999; Defensor del Pueblo, AA.VV., 1999), an aspect which still concerns us today
and is reflected by specific studies published in the last five years (Carney & Merrel, 2001;
Solberg & Olweus, 2003; Toldos, 2005; Avilés & Monjas, 2005; Cerezo & Ato, 2005;
Ramírez, 2006). The detailed description of the phenomenon then encouraged the appearance
of studies concerned with describing the agents involved (Rigby, 1997; Monks, Smith &
Swettenham, 2003; Veenstra, Lindenberg, Oldehinkel, De Winter, Verhulst & Ormel, 2005;
Camodeca & Goossens, 2005; Perren & Alsaker, 2006), with analyzing the problem’s risk
factors (Lahey, Waldman & McBurnett, 1999; Kokkinos & Panayiotou, 2004; Farrington,
2005), and analyzing the effects of the problem particularly among its victims (Crick & Grot-
peter, 1995; Perren & Alsaker, 2006). As a final result of all the prior research as well as re-
sults from current studies, there is now an increase in research focused on the design, devel-
opment and systematic evaluation of intervention programs (Cowie & Olaffson, 2000; Trianes
& García, 2002; Elinoff, Chafouleas & Sassu, 2004; Nordhagen, Nielsen, Stigum & Köhler,
2005; Benítez, Almeida & Justicia, in press).
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 153 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
Beginning with this common characteristic, we find the definition offered by Olweus
(1993), where bullying is the set of physical and/or verbal behaviors that a person or group of
persons directs against a peer, in hostile, repetitive and ongoing fashion, abusing real or ficti-
tious power, with the intent to cause harm to the victim. This definition establishes fulfill-
ment of certain criteria in order for the behavior exhibited to be defined as bullying:
(a) an imbalance of power between the victim and the aggressor, to be understood as a
dishonest, domineering, opportunistic and illegitimate use of power over one’s opponent;
(b) incidence and duration of the bullying situation, with a minimum incidence of once
per week and a minimum duration of six months;
(c) intentionality and proactive character of the aggression, since one is seeking to obtain
some social, material or personal benefit, without prior provocation; and,
(d) the intent to do harm.
However, Olweus’s definition must be expanded with regard to the nature of the be-
haviors exhibited. Some authors, including Olweus himself, distinguish between direct and
indirect aggressions of the bullying phenomenon (Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz & Kaukianen, 1992;
Olweus, 1993) or overt as compared to covert aggressions (Crick, Casas & Ku, 1999). Among
direct or overt aggressions, we find both physical (kicks, punches, pushes, threats with weap-
ons, etc.) and verbal (insults, blackmail, etc.). Similarly, among indirect or covert aggressions
we find those of a physical nature (hiding property, damaging materials, stealing, etc.) and
those of a verbal nature (name-calling, spreading rumors). Nonetheless, we have yet to in-
clude within the range of bullying behaviors those relational aggressions which are only indi-
rect or covert. These aggressions seek to socially discredit the victims, provoking their isola-
tion from the peer group and a progressive social exclusion (Griffin & Gross, 2004).
If we fulfill with all the aspects mentioned above, we can come to a clearer, more con-
cise definition of bullying. Such a definition would take into consideration the intentionality
of causing harm without prior provocation, incidence and duration of the bullying situation,
power asymmetry between victim and aggressor, as well as the direct or indirect nature (ver-
bal, physical or relational) of the behaviors exhibited. As we can observe, hostile behaviors
displayed by aggressors go beyond mere harassment –a label used by the media for referring
to the phenomenon– since, in most cases, they are undisguised behaviors where the aggressor
- 154 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
neither hides nor remains anonymous, and where direct and indirect aggressions can be ver-
bal, psychological or involve social exclusion.
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 155 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
In Spain, many studies have been carried out and offer data on the phenomenon. We
begin with noting the Report on School Violence put forward by the Ombudsman (AA.VV.,
1999). The study was carried out on a national level with students from Secondary Educa-
tion, with a victim rate near 7%, and aggressors nearing 5%. Avilés and Monjas (2005), who
performed a study with student participants between the ages of 12 and 15 years, indicated
that 11.6% of participating students had been involved in abuse situations. Of these, 5.7%
were victims and 5.9% aggressors. The study sponsored by the Reina Sofia Center (Serrano &
Iborra, 2005), also on a national level and with students between the ages of 12 and 16 years
of age, shows greater indices of persons affected. That is, authors claim that the percentage of
victims comes to 12.5%, while percentage of aggressors rises to 7.6%. Finally, in a study per-
formed among Primary and Secondary students in the autonomous city of Ceuta (Ramírez,
2006), similar levels are expressed, placing the rate of those affected by bullying at 10.5%:
6.4% victims, 3.1% aggressors and 1% aggressor-victims.
- 156 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
group which is extremely aggressive and tends to provoke attacks from other pupils. Mem-
bers of this group suffer more social rejection, they confront both aggressors and passive vic-
tims and they are known as provocative victims (Olweus, 1993). The latter group of victims
belongs to that set of students who, as a function of contextual and/or situational variables,
take on the role of victim or aggressor, giving rise to the figure of aggressor/victim (Griffin &
Gross, 2004).
Passive victims are characterized as possessing low self-esteem and internalization of
problems such as anxiety and depression, having few friends, rejected and socially isolated by
their classmates (Olweus, 1993). While provocative victims tend to show hyperactive traits,
strong temperament and are aggressive (Venstra et al., 2005; Perren & Alsaker, 2006), pas-
sive victims are more sensitive, cautious and unassertive (Olweus, 1993). Both types of vic-
tims are usually unable to control their feelings or to solicit the attention of their classmates.
Victims differ from others in how they process the social information they perceive
(Venstra et al., 2005). Submissive children, when solving conflicts, more highly value sub-
missive options and they undervalue aggressive options. Likewise, they predict better conse-
quences for submissive options even when they don’t like them. In this sense, Troy and
Sroufe (1987) suggest that victims become vulnerable in certain contexts since they tend to
aggravate their difficulties by appearing more needy than others: they need more time to be
accepted, when they are excluded they continue to make ineffective attempts at social interac-
tion, etc., indicating poor social skills that contribute to aggravation of the bullying problem.
As for personality, aggressors tend to show low levels of empathy toward their class-
mates, they value violence as a tool for getting what they want, and they show aggressive ten-
dencies not only towards peers but also towards teachers, parents and siblings (Carney &
Merrell, 2001). They are impulsive, unempathetic, hostile, dominant, uncooperative and un-
friendly; moreover, they appear to have low levels of anxiety and insecurity (Carney &
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 157 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
Merrell, 2001; Veenstra et al., 2005). Aggressors as a whole show poor adjustment in school,
low academic performance, and they perceive that they are less supported by their teachers
(Nansel, Craig, Overpeek, Daluja & Ruan, 2004)
Like victims, aggressors differ in how they process the social information that comes
to them. Aggressive students tend to show hostile attributions when they find themselves in
ambiguous social situations, perceiving them as intentionally negative towards them, and tre-
sponding to these situations aggressively (Griffin & Gross, 2004). This social problem-
solving style, accompanied by aggressiveness and impulsive temperament, seems to contrib-
ute to the pattern of antisocial behavior that places aggressors at risk of suffering other behav-
ior problems, such as consuming drugs or delinquency.
Olweus (1993) affirms that, over the course of studies he has carried out, rates of vic-
timization among students decrease as age increases, and that furthermore, physical aggres-
sions occur with less frequency. However, despite this decrease in the rate of victimization, it
is not statistically significant. On the other hand, as for the aggressors themselves, their ten-
dency to abuse increases, or at least is maintained as age increases (Solberg & Olweus, 2003;
Ramirez, 2006).
In a retrospective study led by Eslea & Rees (2001), it is claimed that the largest num-
ber of victims is found between the ages of 11 and 13 years. Such results are similar to those
found by Ramïrez (2006), who also indicates this age range in his study as the range with
highest rates of students victimized. This turning point in age coincides with the passage
from Primary to Secondary Education, which can be especially difficult for some children due
to the appearance of effects of puberty, changes in the social hierarchy around them, and a
decrease in social support being received, perhaps explaining the increase in victimization
rates at these ages (Pelegrini & Long, 2002). Nonetheless, several studies have shown the
inconsistency of these data since the analyses performed provide contradictory data, thus in-
dicating a need to continue investigating the relationship between bullying and age.
Comparing the age of aggressors and victims, Solberg & Olweus (2003) note that vic-
tims are generally younger than their aggressors, given that victims indicate they are attacked
- 158 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
more frequently and commonly by students older than themselves. This age difference is ac-
centuated in Primary school and is weaker during Secondary school, just as is supported by
other studies (Olweus 1993; Rigby, 1997; Nansel et al., 2001)
In the case of gender and type of abuse being used, the idea that boys use direct ag-
gression, not covert, seems to be confirmed, while girls opt for indirect aggression of a rela-
tional type. Some data suggest that while boys tend to practice direct verbal and physical
abuse more frequently, both sexes appear equal when it comes to indirect abuse, such as so-
cial exclusion. In a study by Toldos (2005) with Spanish adolescents, differences as a func-
tion of gender and type of aggression displayed were only observed with regard to direct
physical and verbal aggression (more common among boys), while no significant differences
were seen between the genders with regard to indirect aggressions, despite being more com-
mon among girls.
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 159 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
- 160 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
ior. Longitudinal studies have shown the relationship between low verbal intelligence, low
school performance, lack of problem-solving skill and precarious social skills with the risk of
being aggressive and behaving violently (Moffit, 1993; Eron & Huesmann, 1993).
A low verbal quotient is also related to poor school performance, and both can predict
development of violent behaviors (Farrington, 2005). Children who do not do well in school
more easily become those who tend to commit delinquent acts; even early indifference toward
schooling is related to becoming delinquent. When children do not attend or are often absent
from school, they join with others who have either been delinquent themselves, or who, like
themselves, hate school, or have even been expelled. These deviant groups form part of a lar-
ger group rejected by their peers, and offer an opportunity for involving more young people in
delinquent or violent acts. The opportunity is more likely found in these groups than in oth-
ers where the members attend class and feel identified with their school (Patterson, Reid &
Dishion, 1992).
Family destructuring: change in traditional roles, absence of one parent, lack of at-
tention, etc.
Abuse and violent modeling in the nuclear family, where the child learns to solve
conflicts using physical harm or verbal aggression.
Family models where one learns that power is exerted by being the strongest, with
a lack of negotiation and dialogue.
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 161 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
Children are socialized from a very young age, they are taught how to handle frustra-
tion, to react in certain situations and to solve problems effectively. Most of this early so-
cialization takes place at the child’s home, in the family nest, and the evidence is clear: par-
ents of aggressive children punish them more frequently, more inconsistently and ineffec-
tively. They also tend to be coercive and manipulative with their children, and they fail to
reinforce their children’s positive, prosocial behaviors. A coercive style in the parent/child
relationship leads parents to unconsciously reinforce coercive behavior in their children, since
the latter are rewarded when they stop bothering or manipulating their parents. These chil-
dren learn that aggressive behavior usually leads them to getting what they want.
Parents who are careless, who reject their children or who are negligent also have a
high risk that their children become involved in violent acts. Neglect, or lack of parental fol-
low-up of their children, has been labeled as a factor which increases the risk of delinquency,
resentment in the child, etc., which can be expressed through poor school performance and
antisocial behaviors. Parents who effectively supervise their children and are more actively
involved in their children’s activities produce children who are more competent from the so-
cial point of view.
Children whose parents are antisocial also have a high risk of falling into delinquency
and violence. Part of this risk is related to the violent or criminal behavior of their parents, or
it can also be related to an inherited temperament.
Finally, we must note that children who are victims of abuse by their parents during
childhood have a higher risk of being involved in violent acts during adolescence (Farrington,
2005). In the study by Thornberry (1994), 38% of young people from non-violent families
admitted having been involved at some time in violent acts. This percentage increases to 60%
when we speak of children who belong to families where any type of violent acts occurs (do-
mestic violence, hostile family atmosphere, child abuse) and even 78% when children of these
families are under the influence of all three types of violence. Results from this study suggest
a significant influence from ongoing exposure to acts of violence and victimization as an
underlying factor in development of violence in the child.
- 162 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
Influence of the peer group, the school and the social context
As indicated earlier, having a delinquent peer group is an indicator which precedes
development of violent behaviors (Lipsey & Derzon, 1998). In this sense, it is clearly appar-
ent that violent young people associate with peers who have behavior problems, and that the
latter reinforce their antisocial behavior. As we said, a peer group with behavior problems
also has a negative influence regarding school, as expressed by: school absenteeism, lack of
identification with other students and with teachers, lack of commitment to one’s studies, etc.
On the other hand, there are factors internal to the school institution itself which en-
courage development of violence, since the school stratum itself presupposes a certain format
and certain basic principles of socialization. Furthermore, we must consider that school is
founded on an internal hierarchy and organization which in itself harbors strain and conflict.
Fernández (1998) indicates the most significant traits that can provoke appearance of abusive
conduct: (a) lack of common reference criteria among the teachers; (b) problems of school
organization; (c) appearance of different cultural values due to immigration; (d) roles and re-
lationships between teachers and students; (e) relationships among the students; (f) size of the
school facilities; and, (g) punitive and sanctioning strategies used by the schools to address
violent acts among students.
Outside of school, and without going into much detail, numerous studies show the re-
lationship between violence and certain community indicators: lack of neighborhood organi-
zation, changes of family residence, lack of parental supervision of their children’s behavior,
strong presence of drugs and gangs, precarious socioeconomic contexts, etc. (Coulton, Korbin
& Su, 1998). Violence at school is to a large degree a reflection of what happens in the
neighborhood and in the social context where the individual lives, to the extent that a signifi-
cant relationship has been established between the quantity of violence in the neighborhood
where the child lives and the level of violence seen at his or her school (Ascher, 1994).
The Media
The media are being questioned as a first catalyst of information. The content of the
media message presents violence as something immediate, ordinary and frequent. The level
of physical violence in cartoons is being discussed by everyone in public debate. Further-
more, children and adolescents are frequently exposed to intense levels of televised violence
whether through films, music channels, videogames, cell phone messages, newspapers, news
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 163 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
broadcasts, etc. Several studies have reached conclusions that exposure to violent acts is
strongly associated with the risk of suffering or being involved in aggressive or violent behav-
iors (Derksen & Strasburger, 1996).
There seem to be three clear effects of violence in the media. First, children exposed
to high levels of violence in the media more easily accept aggressive attitudes, and after wit-
nessing violent acts, they begin to behave more aggressively with their peers. Second,
chronic exposures extended over a period of time can desensitize the child toward violence
and its consequences. Third, children accustomed to seeing violence in the media perceive a
violent world, where one must fight to subsist; they have a growing fear of becoming a victim
in that world and they develop the need to fight and abuse others so as not to become victims
themselves.
It is true that not all children who grow up watching large quantities of televised vio-
lence end up becoming violent adolescents or adults; however, they do show more aggression
when they are small and especially after they have watched violence. By contrast, children
who watch programs with prosocial content are less aggressive, more cooperative and have
more desire to share their things with other children.
Conclusions
Bullying is a problem at school: its consequences affect all agents involved, and indi-
rectly, the rest of the educational community that must coexist with the effects being pro-
duced. Research on this problem has covered much ground, and yet there is still a long way
to go for researchers focused on analysis of the problem.
First, we must reach once and for all an agreed-on definition, accepted by the greater
part of researchers, which would allow all of us to be speaking about the same phenomenon.
Along these lines, methodological limitations related to the definition of the construct must be
resolved, as well as time periods of frequency and duration established in the definition of the
problem. Not having a common reference point will continue giving rise to methodological
limitations relating to instruments used for collecting information, and making it difficult to
compare studies carried out in different countries.
- 164 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
Research on the influence of certain variables such as gender and age on these behav-
iors needs to go deeper. The discrepancy between results obtained by different studies reveals
this need. Likewise, there is a need to perform studies on bullying during childhood. As we
have seen, there are indicators which allow us to foresee the development of antisocial behav-
iors starting at three years of age. However, the few studies that have been carried out in this
area require that conclusions be ratified. These aspects are closely connected to putting effec-
tive intervention measures into practice. We require intervention programs, designed and
implemented, which are accompanied by powerful evaluation designs that make it possible to
show validity and effectiveness from the results obtained. In this sense, it is of vital impor-
tance to gather as much reliable information as possible regarding the problem. Thus, in the
case of early intervention, studies carried out with students of Preschool and Primary Educa-
tion would have the best yield. In the case of Secondary or Tertiary interventions, we must
make the most of studies related to exploring risk factors that provoke the problem: from our
knowledge base we will have the keys for being able to intervene.
References
AA.VV. (1999). (1999). Informe del Defensor del Pueblo sobre la Violencia Escolar. [Om-
budsman’s Report on School Violence.] Madrid: Ministerio del Interior.
Ascher, C. (1994). Gaining control of violence: A view from the field. NY: Teachers College,
ERIC Clearinghouse on Urban Education.
Avilés, J. M. & Monjas, I. (2005). Estudio de la incidencia de la intimidación y el maltrato
entre iguales en la educación secundaria obligatoria mediante el cuestionario CIMEI
(Avilés, 1999) – Cuestionario sobre Intimidación y Maltrato Entre Iguales –. [Study
on the incidence of intimidation and bullying in compulsory secondary education, us-
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 165 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
- 166 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
Derksen, D.J. & Strasberger, V.C. (1996). Media and television violence: Effects on violence,
aggression and antisocial behaviours in children. In A.M. Hoffman (Ed.), Schools,
violence and society. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Elinoff, M., Chafouleas, S. & Sassu, K. (2004). Bullying: considerations for defining and in-
tervening in school settings. Psychology in the Schools, 41(8), 887-897.
Eron, L.D. & Huesmann, R. (1993). The stability of aggressive behaviour even unto the third
generation. In M. Lewis & S.M. Miller (eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopa-
thology (pp. 147-156). NY: Plenum.
Eslea, M. & Rees, J. (2001). At what age are children most likely to be bullied at school? Ag-
gressive Behavior, 27, 419-429.
Espelage, D. & Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on school bullying and victimization : what
have we learned and where do we go from here ? School Pshychology Review, 32,
365-383.
Espelage, D., Mebane, S. & Adams, R. (2004). Empathy, caring, and bullying: Toward an
understanding of complex associations. In D. Spelage & M. Swarer (Eds.). Bullying in
American Schools: a social-ecological perspective on prevention and intervention.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Farrington, D. (2005). Childhood origins of Antisocial Behavior. Clinical Psychology and
Psychotherapy, 12, 177-190.
Fernández, I. (1998). Prevención de la violencia y resolución de conflictos. El clima escolar
como factor de calidad. [Prevention of violence and conflict resolution. School cli-
mate as a quality factor.] Madrid: Narcea.
Griffin, R. & Gross A. (2004). Childhood bullying: current empirical findings and future di-
rections for research. Aggressive and violent behaviour, 9, 379-400.
Harris, A. & Reid, J. B. (1981). The consistency of a class of coercive child behaviours
across school settings for individual subjects. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 9, 219-227.
Kokkinos, C. & Panayiotou, G. (2004). Predicting bullying and victimization among early
adolescents: associations with disruptive behavior disorders. Aggressive Behavior, 30,
520-533.
Lahey, B., Waldman, I. & McBurnett, K. (1999). Annotation: The development of antisocial
behaviour, an integrative causal model. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
40(5), 669-682.
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 167 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
- 168 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170
J. L. Benítez & F. Justicia
Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. PP: 151-170 - 169 -
Bullying: description and analysis of the phenomenon
Troy, M. & Sroufe, L .A. (1987). Victimization among preschoolers: Role of attachment re-
lationship history. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
chiatry, 26, 166-172.
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Oldehinkel, A. J., De Winter, A. F., Verhulst, F. C. & Ormel, J.
(2005). Bullying and victimization in elementary schools: a comparison of bullies,
victims, bully/victims, and uninvolved preadolescents. Developmental Psychology,
41(4), 672-682.
Whitney, I. & Smith, P. K. (1993). A survey of the nature and extent of bullying in jun-
ior/middle and secondary schools. Educational Research, 35, 3-25.
- 170 - Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology. No 9. Vol 4 (2), 2006. ISSN: 1696-2095. pp: 151-170