Erwin Straus Upright Posture
Erwin Straus Upright Posture
Erwin Straus Upright Posture
B Y E R W I N W. STRAUS, M. D.
I. INTRODUCTION
Upright Posture, Which Distinguishes the Human Genus from All
Other Species, Pre-Establishes a Definite Mode of Being-
in-the-World
II. HUMAN KINEMATICS
a. Acquiring Upright Posture
Counteraction to Gravity Calls for Learning, Activity, Attention,
Awakeness
The Direction Upward Divides Space Into World Regions with
Specific Emotional Values
b. Standing. The Natural Stance of Man Is Resistance
Upright POsture Brings Man Into Distance:
1. From the Ground
2. From Things
3. From His Fellow-Men
Expressive Values of the Vertical
c. Walking
Human Gait Is a Continuously Arrested F a l l i n g - - ~ a n Has to
Find a Hold Within Himself
Physiological Considerations
The Freedom Gained in Upright Posture Permits the Instru-
mental Use of Limbs
III. U P R I G H T POSTURE AND T H E D E V E L O P M E N T OF T H E
HUMAN HAND AND ARM
a. The Hand as a Sensory Organ and as a Tool
Differences in Anatomical, Physiological and Psychological Cate-
gories
The Hand as a Motor-Sensory Unit
"Experienced D i s t a n c e " ~ R e l a t e d to Upright P o s t u r e - - A n In-
dispensable Condition of Epicritical Discrimination
Distance Dominates Manual Expression and Communication
b. Expebnsion of the Body Scheme by the Arms
The Acquisition of Lateral Space
The Social Significance of Lateral Space
~Reviewed in the Veterans Administration and published with the approval of the
chief medical director. The statements and conclusions published by the author aro
the result of his own study and do not necessarily reflect the opinion or policy of the
Veterans Administration.
530 TI-IE UPRIGHT POSTURE
tal joint to be moved forward toward the center of the base of the
skull, resulting in the typical configuration of the human skull, the
extension of the base, and the closing vault, which in turn provides
wider space for the orbitae. The skulls of the other primates still
show the shape characteristic of (~ther quadrupeds, in which the
head does not rest on the vertebral column but hangs down from
it. The foramen magnum accordingly is in a more caudal position;
the clivus cuts the vertical at a more obtuse angle. The other pri-
mates--as it has been sMd--are built to stand upright but not for
upright posture. ~
Because upright posture is the "Leitmotiv" in the formation of
the human organism, an individuM who has lost or is deprived of
the capacity to get up and keep himself upright depends for his
survival completely on the aid of others. Without their help, he is
doomed to die. A biologically-oriented psychology must not forget
that upright posture is an indispensable condition of man's self-
preservation. Upright we are, and we experience ourselves in
this specific relation to the world.
Men and mice do not have the same environment, even if they
share the same room. Environment is not a stage with the scenery
set as one and the same for all actors who make their entrance.
Each species has its own environment. There is a mutuM inter-
dependence between species and environment. The surrounding
world is determined b~ the organization of the species in a pro-
cess of selecting what is relevant to the function-circle of action
and reaction. 1 Upright posture pre-establishes a definite attitude
toward the world; it is a specific mode of being-in-the-world.
Relating the basic forms of human experiencing to man's upright
posture may well be called an anthropological approach, if that
term is used with its original connotation. It was not until the
middle of the nineteenth century that the meaning of "anthropol-
ogy" was confined to zoological aspects, to a study of man as an
~The comparison of mtm and other primates is ~ time-honored topic, widely discussed
among pre-Darwinistic zoologists. Most of the characteristic differences enumerated by
Weidenreich were known to the anatomists of the eighteenth century, who also con-
sidered the possibiIity of a common origin. Daubenton published, in 1764, a paper
about the different positions of the foramen magnum in man and animals (Mdmoires de
l'Ac~d~mie de Paris, 1764, quoted a f t e r Herder). Even the sentence passed on upright
posture because of its inherent evils is old enough. Moskati, in 1771, comparing the
essential differences of man and animals, came to the conclusion t h a t upright posture
disposed heart, circulation a n d intestines to ma~y defects and diseases (Vom Koerper-
lichen Wesentlichen Untersch~ede tier Thiere ~nd Mensche~, Goettingen, 1771).
ERWIiV W. STRAUSt IV[. D. 533
neither supports the trunk nor has to hold up the body, a func-
tion still assigned to the forelimb of the tree-inhabiting primates.
Small but still significant changes ensue, in the shape and position
of scapula and clavicle, in the origin and insertion of the shoulder
muscles, in the configuration of the acromio-clavicular and the
sterno-clavicular joints. All this together provides for a maxi-
mum flexibility of the arm, aiding the display of the mechanisms
of the scapulo-humeral articulation, where, because of the loose-
ness of the capsule and the shallowness of tile glenoid cavity, the
humerus can move with great ease in all directions. In the hip
joints, on the contrary, the head of the femur is deeply set in the
socket of the acetabular cavity, the capsule is tight and reinforced
by strong ligaments. While the primarily tectonic formation of
the neck of the femur also somewhat extends the range of excur-
sions, the emphasis in the hip is on stability, in the shoulder, on
flexibility.
Language, obviously inspired by phenomenological obser-
vation, takes the arm as the protoype of the articulation of
a limb and of its motions from the joint, for the root of the
word "arm" is "Var " with the meaning "to fit, to join," the same
root from which the Greek "ar-thros" and the Latin "ar-ticulc~tio"
stem.*
Within the totality of the new spatial dimensions acquired with
upright posture, lateral space is perhaps the most important one.
Through the mobility and action of arm and hand, lateral space
becomes accessible and relevant for man. In this sector, most of
the human crafts originated. Hammer and axe, scythe and sickle,
the carpenter's saw, the weaver's shuttle, the potter's wheel, the
mason's trowel, the painter's brush--they all relate to lateral
space. This li,st could b:e extended ad Iibitum but probably would
never come to an end, for lateral space is the matrix of primitive
and sophisticated skills: of spinning and sewing, stirring and iron-
ing, sowing and husking, soldering and welding, fiddling and golf-
ing, batting and discus-throwing.
The crafts of peace are followed, accompanied, or preceded, by
the techniques and weapons of war : club, sword, spear, bow, sling,
~In this paper, the writer has made frequent use of etymology, although i t is not cus-
tomary to introduce " l i n g u i s t i c evidence ~ in a biological discussion, ttowever, be.
cause the history of a word represents the sedimentation of general psychological ex-
perience, it appears to the writer to be justified to refer to etymology as an a u x i l i a r y
discipline.
552 ~:I~E UPRIGHT POSTUI~E
bow to the palm of the hand coincides with the length of the fore-
arm. The ball is released without force, speed, or accurate aim-
ing. I t enters almost immediately the descending branch of a
steep parabola. At the age of six, the girl tilts her right shoul-
der slightly, moves the left foot f o r w a r d one small step, but shows
no f u r t h e r progress. A boy of the same age, when p r e p a r i n g to
throw, stretches his right a r m sidewards a n d backwards, supinates
the forearm, ~ twists, turns and bends his trunk, and moves his
right foot backward. F r o m this stance, he can s u p p o r t his throw-
ing almost with the full s t r e n g t h of his total motorium. The ex-
cursion of his final motion reaches an angle of 180 ~. I t moves
around the left standing leg as its central axis. The radius of this
semi-circle exceeds by far the. full length of the arm. The ball
leaves the hand with considerable acceleration; it moves t o w a r d
its goal in a long fiat curve.
As this difference appears in early childhood, it cannot result
from the development of the female breast. While the l e g e n d a r y
Amazons had the right breast removed to allow the use of bow
and spear, ~ it seems certain that Nausicaa and all her companions
threw a ball just like our Bettys, and Marys and Susans. How
can we explain the difference? The little girl has no more diffi-
culty in keeping her equilibrium than the boy. I t is t r u e that she
is weaker in muscle power; but, therefore, one should expect her
to compensate for this 1.ack of strength with added p r e p a r a t o r y
excursion. Instead, we find her avoiding the t u r n into lateral
space. Maybe the masculine way of t h r o w i n g corresponds to
masculine "eccentricity," while the feminine attitude reveals a
deep-seated r e s t r a i n t and an inclination to circle around one's own
center. The difference, then, would belong to the a r e a of expres-
sion; it would not be a difference of strength and build but of gen-
eral psychological attitude in relation to the world and to space.
Thus far, lateral space has been discussed as if it were a unit, a
single whole. Indeed, in m a n y motions, we lift our arms sym-
metrically in surrounding space. However, even simultaneous
movements need not be actually symmetrical. They a p p e a r sym-
metrical; they are not so in their intention. The a r m s can be
stretched, the hands can point, in opposite directions, to the right
~Supination reaches its fullest a n d freest excursion with the horizontal abduction of
the arm.
~ A m a z o n means ' ' a - , m a z o s , ' ' without breast.
554 THE Ut)I~IGHT t)OSTURE
c. Neuropha4siological Considerations
The highest skills are contingent upon the unification of oppo-
sites, the co-ordination of relatively independent parts which are
not bound together by symmetry, homology or synergy. A good
ERWI• W. S~RAUS,M.D. 555
REFERENCES
1. v. Uexkuell, J. : Theoretical Biology. Harcourt, Brace. l~ew York. 1926.
2. Gregory, W. K. : The Humerus from Fish to Man. American Museum l~ovitates,
J a n u a r y 31, 1949.
3, Nielsen, 1~. J. : Agnosia, Apraxia, Aphasia. 2nd edition. Hoeber. New York and
London. 1946.
4. Sittig, O. : i)ber Apraxie. Berlin. 1931.
5. Kleist, K. : Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig. 1934.
6. Aristotle: De Anima. 432a 2-4.
7. Katz, D. : Der A u f b a u der Tastwelt. Leipzig. 1925.
8. Minkowski, E. : Les notions de distance v~eue. J. de Psychol., X X V I I , 15 N0-
vembre-15 Decembre 1930. LibTarie Felix Alcan.
9. Darwin, Charles: The Expressions of the Emotions. 1872. Reprinted by Appleton.
New York. 1910.
10. Schilder, Paul: The Image and Appearance of the H u m a n Body. Psychol. Mono-
graphs, No. 4, Kegan, Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., Ltd. London. 1935.
11. Browning, Robert: Andrea del Sarto.
12. Oesoll, A., et. al. : The F i r s t Five Years of Life. Harper. New York a n d Lon-
don. 1940.
13. Foerster, O.: Die Motorischen Batmen und Folder. Handbuch der Neurologie,
ed. by Bumke and Foerster, Vol. VI. Berlin. 1936.
14. Herder, G. W. : Ideen zur Philosophie der Geschichte der Menschheit. Riga. 1784.
15. : Op. cit.