Morfin Vs Oxy
Morfin Vs Oxy
Morfin Vs Oxy
Pharmacology
Correspondence
Different effects of Mrs Anne Estrup Olesen MSc (Pharm)
PhD, Mech-Sense, Department of
Gastroenterology, Aalborg Hospital,
morphine and oxycodone in Aarhus University Hospital, Hobrovej
18-22, 9000 Aalborg, Denmark.
Tel.: +45 9932 6243
experimentally evoked Fax: +45 9932 6507
E-mail: [email protected]
hyperalgesia: a human
----------------------------------------------------------------------
Keywords
experimental model, hyperalgesia,
Received
11 August 2009
Anne Estrup Olesen,1,2 Camilla Staahl,1,2 Lars Arendt-Nielsen2 & Accepted
Asbjørn Mohr Drewes1,2 31 March 2010
1
Mech-Sense, Department of Gastroenterology, Aalborg Hospital, Aarhus University Hospital and
2
Center for Sensory-Motor Interactions (SMI), Department of Health Science and Technology, Aalborg
University, Aalborg, Denmark
Pain test
Oesophagus
3. Heat
4. Mechanical
5. Electrical
Muscle
2. Deep
pressure Pain tests Pain tests Pain tests Pain tests
Baseline 30 60 90 Time (min)
a. Chemical perfusion of oesophagus
b. Drug administration
Figure 1
Flow-chart for the study protocol. A schematic illustration of the pain tests is shown on the left. Pain tests were performed in the order illustrated by the
numbers. The order was similar for all stimulation series and between days and consisted of heat stimulation of the skin, mechanical stimulation of the
muscle and heat, mechanical and electrical stimulation of the oesophagus.The graph (right) shows a schematic illustration of the analgesic effect. Pain tests
were performed at baseline after which hyperalgesia was induced and the drug administered. Pain tests were performed 30, 60 and 90 min after baseline.
Drug administration included either placebo, morphine (30 mg) or oxycodone (15 mg) in randomized order
drug was given) and 30, 60 and 90 min after induction of mixture 2 mg ml-1, Hospital Pharmacies, Denmark) or
hyperalgesia and drug administration. Pain was assessed 15 mg oxycodone (Oxynorm® oral liquid mixture
from skin, muscle and oesophageal stimulations following 10 mg ml-1, Norpharma A/S, Hørsholm, Denmark) or
the same order for all stimulation series as illustrated in placebo in randomized order (all masked in colour by 5 ml
Figure 1. Subjects were blinded to all thresholds measured. orange juice concentrate and diluted with water to a total
This schedule should ensure sufficient testing throughout of 20 ml liquid). The drugs were given immediately after
the opioid absorption phase and the beginning of the baseline pain recordings. The colour of the mixture was
elimination phase without extending the test period too masked in the orange juice and the mixture was infused
much [32, 33]. The degrees of four known adverse effects, through a catheter inside the probe ending in the distal
nausea, dizziness, itching and sweating, were registered as oesophagus.The mixture was handled by a pharmacist not
(1) none, (2) slight, (3) heavy or (4) intolerable and specifi- otherwise involved in the study to ensure blinding of all
cally asked for after each pain test battery. Moreover, the other investigators.
degree of sedation was evaluated after each pain test The dose of morphine was twice that of oxycodone.The
battery by the finger tapping test. If the subject was rationale was that the greater oral bioavailability of imme-
sedated or otherwise feeling unwell during the study, diate release oxycodone compared with morphine, and
blood pressure and pulse were measured again to ensure more rapid onset of analgesia could result in superior
that everything was normal before the study continued. If effects of oxycodone [34]. Thus, we chose a relatively high
the subject felt dizzy, nauseated or otherwise indisposed at dose of morphine to be sure that differences in bioavail-
the end of the study they were observed in the laboratory ability did not favour oxycodone. Furthermore, the two
until the adverse effect ceased and they felt ready to leave. doses have previously showed comparable analgesic
The experiment was repeated three times with at least 1 potencies in experimental skin and muscle pain [21].
week ‘wash-out’ intervals. The randomized order should
deal with periodic effects e.g. learning effects, carry-over Skin stimulation
and interference effects. Hence bias from such effects The cutaneous heat pain tolerance threshold was deter-
would be evenly distributed in the three treatment periods. mined by a computer driven heat pain device (TSA-II Neu-
roSensory Analyzer, Medoc Ltd, Ramat Yishai, Israel). A
Medication thermode with a surface of 25 ¥ 50 mm was applied to the
Each subject was investigated in three separate periods volar surface of the left forearm at 10 cm from the elbow.
and received 30 mg of morphine (morphine oral liquid The temperature increased from 32°C to a maximum of
52°C at a rate of 1°C s-1 until the threshold was reached. nomic reactions leading to vomiting of the probe. All data
Three consecutive temperature measurements in °C were were displayed online (Openlab, GMC, Hornslet, Denmark)
noted and the average computed. The threshold was and stored for later analysis.
defined as the highest stimulus intensity the volunteer
could accept. The subjects were told to stop the stimula- Mechanical stimulation For mechanical stimulation the
tion by a click on a button when the pain tolerance thresh- oesophageal bag was distended at a constant infusion rate
old (PTT) was reached. of 15 ml min-1 until ‘moderate pain’ intensity ratings (VAS =
7) were reached. The volumes (ml) in the bag at ‘moderate
Muscle stimulation pain’ were used for further analysis.
Muscle stimulation was done by an electronic cuff algo-
meter consisting of a pneumatic tourniquet cuff, a com- Thermal stimulation A temperature probe (PR Electronics,
puter controlled air compressor, and an electronic 10 cm Rønde, Denmark) monitored the fluid temperature con-
visual analogue scale (VAS) (Aalborg University, Denmark). tinuously in the bag.The heat pain stimuli were performed
The cuff algometer has been described in detail previously by re-circulating water (60°C) in the bag.The infusion chan-
[35, 36].The 0 and 10 cm extremes on the VAS were defined nels in the probe were attached to a specially designed
as‘no pain’and as‘the worst pain imaginable’. The computer pump system. OpenLab software (GMC Aps, Hornslet,
continuously controlled the compression rate to ensure a Denmark) running on a computer was used to control an
linear increase in pressure. A tourniquet cuff was wrapped infusion/withdrawal syringe pump (PHD 2000, Harvard,
around the gastrocnemius muscle. In order to ensure reli- Massachusetts, USA). The pump held two 140 ml syringes
able pressure readings the cuff was fitted tightly around for infusion and withdrawal.The bag volume was kept con-
the limb before each measurement. All recordings were stant by withdrawing the same amount of fluid as infused
made on the right limb with the subject in a supine posi- using the two large lumens. This was done to obtain a
tion. In addition to the hand-held pressure release button linear temperature increase of 1.5°C min-1. The tempera-
the inflation could be terminated both mechanically and ture stimulus did not cause any mechanical stimulation
from the computer program. The cuff was automatically since the amount of fluid in the bag did not change. Previ-
inflated (compression rate 0.50 kPa s-1). The subject was ous to this recirculation 4 ml of water was applied to the
instructed to rate the pain intensity continuously on the bag to ensure sufficient mucosa contact. The subject was
VAS from the first sensation of pain being the pain detec- instructed to rate the intensity continuously on the VAS
tion threshold (PDT). The pressure continued increasing from the first vague perception to the pain detection
and the subject pressed the release button when the cuff threshold. The temperature of the water in the bag at pain
pressure pain tolerance threshold (PTT) was reached. detection threshold was used for analysis.
the subject (pain intensity higher than PDT for more than sixteen sedation, two itching and one sweating. After
1 min). This method has been described previously [41]. administration of oxycodone six volunteers experienced
nausea, sixteen sedation, six itching and four sweating.
Statistics After administration of placebo one volunteer experienced
To eliminate errors relating to differences in baseline pain nausea, two sedation, one itching and three sweating.
recordings, the change in stimulus intensity relative to None of the subjects reported any side effect as intoler-
baseline was used in the statistical analysis [19].The results able. There was no difference in the overall degree of side
are expressed as differences between the means of the effects reported after morphine and oxycodone (P > 0.05).
baseline corrected values and confidence intervals (95%) The finger tapping frequency increased over time in all
unless otherwise indicated. For multiple comparisons the three treatment arms. No difference was found in finger
overall effects were tested by two-way analysis of variance tapping frequency between placebo, morphine and oxyc-
(ANOVA) with the factors: 1) drug (with three levels: placebo, odone (F = 0.1, P = 0.9).
morphine and oxycodone) and 2) time (with three levels:
30, 60 and 90 min). This indicated whether there was a
Skin stimulation
difference in the data obtained in the three drug groups
Compared with placebo, administration of opioids
(placebo, morphine and oxycodone) or in the data
increased PTT to heat stimulation (F = 9.5; P < 0.001)
obtained at the three time points. When an overall signifi-
(Figure 2). In post hoc analysis oxycodone was found to be
cant difference was found, post hoc analyses were done
more effective than placebo (P < 0.001) with a difference
based on a generalized linear model. To evaluate differ-
between the means of 0.69°C (95% CI 1.12, 2.99) and more
ences in drug effects, a total score over the 90 min was
effective than morphine (P = 0.016) with a difference
used in the post hoc analysis. For comparisons of side
between the means of 0.39°C (95% CI 0.22, 2.09).The effect
effects, Friedmans test was used. P < 0.05 was considered
of morphine was not significantly different from placebo
significant. The software package Sigma Stat vs. 3.0 (Systat
(P > 0.05).
Software, San Jose, California, USA) was used for the ANOVA,
whereas the software package STATA version 10.1 (Stata-
Corp LP, College Station, Texas, USA) was used for the post Muscle stimulation
hoc analysis. There was a difference in the effect of opioids and placebo
regarding PDT (F = 3.8; P = 0.02). Post hoc analysis showed
that morphine had a greater analgesic effect than placebo
Results (P = 0.03) with a difference between the means of 9.32 kPa
(95% CI 1.1, 17.6) and oxycodone had a greater analgesic
All volunteers completed the study. The results are shown effect than placebo (P = 0.012) with the difference
in Table 1. The oesophageal perfusion with chemicals between the means of 10.39 kPa (95% CI 2.3, 18.5). No
caused nausea and sweating in all volunteers. After admin- difference was demonstrated between the effect of
istration of morphine two volunteers experienced nausea, morphine and oxycodone (P > 0.05).
Table 1
Results from all experimental pain stimulations given as mean ⫾ CI
Baseline Placebo 47.8 ⫾ 0.8 24.1 ⫾ 3.4 45.2 ⫾ 7.1 27.2 ⫾ 5.0 30.1 ⫾ 21.2 48.0 ⫾ 1.5 30.6 ⫾ 15.4 16.5 ⫾ 3.1 12.9 ⫾ 7.7
Morphine 47.4 ⫾ 0.8 23.3 ⫾ 4.9 43.9 ⫾ 7.3 29.6 ⫾ 5.1 38.8 ⫾ 27.9 50.1 ⫾ 1.7 27.4 ⫾ 11.5 16.7 ⫾ 3.8 9.9 ⫾ 8.5
Oxycodone 47.8 ⫾ 0.7 23.9 ⫾ 4.6 45.2 ⫾ 7.7 28.3 ⫾ 5.1 22.4 ⫾ 12.3 49.4 ⫾ 1.8 32.1 ⫾ 16.7 15.6 ⫾ 3.1 10.7 ⫾ 5.2
30 min. Placebo 47.1 ⫾ 0.9 23.8 ⫾ 4.0 44.9 ⫾ 6.9 26.9 ⫾ 4.6 34.1 ⫾ 23.3 48.1 ⫾ 1.2 37.3 ⫾ 19.1 12.0 ⫾ 1.4 16.2 ⫾ 8.9
Morphine 46.8 ⫾ 0.8 25.4 ⫾ 5.1 46.1 ⫾ 7.5 29.8 ⫾ 5.3 34.6 ⫾ 20.8 48.2 ⫾ 1.4 35.3 ⫾ 19.8 14.2 ⫾ 2.4 9.5 ⫾ 6.3
Oxycodone 47.6 ⫾ 0.7 29.0 ⫾ 6.3 51.8 ⫾ 7.1 30.1 ⫾ 5.0 24.8 ⫾ 16.1 49.2 ⫾ 1.4 21.4 ⫾ 9.1 15.7 ⫾ 2.5 10.7 ⫾ 5.5
60 min. Placebo 47.0 ⫾ 0.7 25.9 ⫾ 4.4 46.1 ⫾ 7.1 29.0 ⫾ 4.7 37.1 ⫾ 21.4 48.3 ⫾ 1.3 45.8 ⫾ 23.7 12.0 ⫾ 1.6 15.4 ⫾ 7.9
Morphine 46.8 ⫾ 0.8 28.6 ⫾ 6.9 48.8 ⫾ 7.7 31.7 ⫾ 5.8 38.4 ⫾ 18.7 49.2 ⫾ 1.7 30.4 ⫾ 11.2 14.1 ⫾ 2.1 11.1 ⫾ 6.7
Oxycodone 47.9 ⫾ 0.7 28.9 ⫾ 5.9 53.2 ⫾ 7.6 29.8 ⫾ 6.1 25.5 ⫾ 11.5 48.7 ⫾ 1.6 24.2 ⫾ 9.5 14.9 ⫾ 2.1 11.7 ⫾ 5.5
90 min. Placebo 47.0 ⫾ 0.9 27.2 ⫾ 4.1 47.8 ⫾ 7.0 30.9 ⫾ 4.9 43.8 ⫾ 25.2 49.3 ⫾ 1.1 43.5 ⫾ 22.3 12.1 ⫾ 1.8 16.1 ⫾ 9.1
Morphine 46.7 ⫾ 0.9 29.1 ⫾ 7.0 49.3 ⫾ 7.7 33.0 ⫾ 6.8 36.3 ⫾ 18.3 49.9 ⫾ 1.1 30.3 ⫾ 10.1 14.0 ⫾ 2.1 12.6 ⫾ 6.1
Oxycodone 47.6 ⫾ 0.8 28.4 ⫾ 6.0 53.9 ⫾ 7.4 30.4 ⫾ 6.0 24.9 ⫾ 9.6 48.2 ⫾ 1.4 24.1 ⫾ 10.1 14.9 ⫾ 2.6 10.6 ⫾ 5.1
Statistical results O>M = P O = M>P O>M>P No difference No difference No difference O>M = P O>M = P No difference
Data were obtained as pain detection thresholds (PDT), pain tolerance thresholds (PTT) or referred pain areas (RPA). The statistical results are summarized as better analgesic effect
(>), equal effect (=) of placebo (P), morphine (M) and oxycodone (O).
sensitization
48.5
Temperature (ºC)
48 oxycodone
47.5
placebo
47
1 2
morphine
Baseline 30 60 90
Time (min)
Figure 2
Results from painful skin stimulation. Stimulus intensities evoking skin
pain tolerance threshold to heat stimulation at baseline, 30, 60 and 90 min
after drug administration. Compared with placebo (white) and morphine
(30 mg, grey) administration of oxycodone (15 mg, black) resulted in
higher temperatures. The error bars represent SEM. To eliminate errors
relating to differences in baseline pain recordings (illustrated by a lower
3 4
baseline value for morphine in the figure), the change in stimulus inten-
sity relative to baseline was used in the statistical analysis
Figure 4
Illustration of referred pain areas to painful oesophageal stimulations.The
58 mean of all reported referred pain areas is illustrated at (1) baseline; (2)
56 after perfusion of the oesophagus with acid+capsaicin; (3) after perfusion
oxycodone of the oesophagus with acid + capsaicin and administration of morphine
Pressure (kPa)
54 sensitization and (4) after perfusion of the oesophagus with acid + capsaicin and
52 administration of oxycodone. The referred pain area was drawn immedi-
50 morphine ately after the stimulation. The area reported after electrical stimulation
(white) was often smaller than area reported after mechanical (grey) or
48 thermal stimulation (hatched)
46 placebo
44
phine (P < 0.001) with a difference between the means of
Baseline 30 60 90 11.93 kPa (95% CI 5.4, 18.5). Moreover, there was an overall
Time (min) effect of time on muscle PTT (F = 4.2; P = 0.016).
morphine
30 Discussion
oxycodone
20 The effects of morphine, oxycodone and placebo were
Baseline 30 60 90 compared in an experimental pain study after hyperalge-
Time (min) sia was evoked in healthy volunteers using acid and cap-
saicin perfusion of the oesophagus. At the doses studied
differences in analgesic potencies were found in various
Figure 5 tissues. Oxycodone attenuated pain to heat stimulation of
Results from painful heat stimulation of the oesophagus. The referred
skin. Both opioids attenuated muscle pain, but oxycodone
pain areas corresponding to pain detection thresholds to heat stimula-
tion of the oesophagus at baseline, 30, 60 and 90 min after drug admin- showed a greater analgesic effect than morphine. Oxyc-
istration are shown. Morphine (30 mg, grey), oxycodone (15 mg, black) odone also showed greater analgesic effect compared
and placebo (white). The effect of oxycodone, but not morphine, was with both morphine and placebo on pain from heat and
better than placebo. The error bars represent SEM. To eliminate errors electrical stimulation of the oesophagus.
relating to differences in baseline pain recordings, the change in stimulus
intensity relative to baseline was used in the statistical analysis
Methodological considerations
A period effect could affect the results, but the randomiza-
sensitization tion of the study ensured that any such effect was equally
19 distributed in all treatment arms. Moreover, previous
studies have demonstrated stable and reproducible pain
recordings in multi-modal, multi-tissue experimental pain
Intensity (mA)
17
studies both within and between days [19, 41].Therefore, a
15
oxycodone period effect was not believed to influence the analysis
significantly in the present study. Another factor, sedation,
morphine could also potentially influence study outcome. Quante
13
et al. investigated this issue after morphine administration
placebo in an experimental pain study in healthy volunteers and
Baseline 30 60 90 found no effect on sedation variables as the experimental
Time (min) pain was likely to increase arousal level thus counteracting
morphine-induced sedation [42]. This could apply to the
present study involving a rather intensive stimulation
Figure 6 paradigm and the increased arousal was supported by an
Results from painful electrical stimulation of the oesophagus. The stimu-
increase in finger tapping frequency in all three treatment
lus intensities in pain detection thresholds to electrical stimulation of the
oesophagus at baseline, 30, 60 and 90 min after drug administration are arms.Thus, it is not likely that sedation influenced the study
shown. Morphine (30 mg, grey), oxycodone (15 mg, black) and placebo outcome.
(white). The effect of oxycodone, but not morphine, was better than In a previous study the current model of oesophageal
placebo. The error bars represent SEM. To eliminate errors relating to sensitization was evaluated and evoked consistent hyper-
differences in baseline pain recordings (illustrated by a slightly lower
algesia in healthy volunteers [41]. Therefore, further inves-
baseline value for oxycodone in the figure), the change in stimulus inten-
sity relative to baseline was used in the statistical analysis tigation of the hyperalgesia was not done in the present
study. Hyperalgesia in chronic pain is often seen as a state
of hypersensitivity of the central nervous system (CNS) that
15.37, 61.71) (Figure 5). Morphine had no significant effect amplifies nociceptive input arising from damaged tissues
compared with placebo (P = 0.056). [4]. Changes in the CNS after peripheral tissue injury have
been shown in animal [43] and human studies [39, 44–47].
Electrical stimulation There was a significant effect of In healthy volunteers experimentally induced peripheral
opioids on the stimulation intensity at PDT (F = 9.7; P < sensitization in muscle and viscera have also evoked both
0.001) (Figure 6). Post hoc analysis showed that oxycodone peripheral and central sensitization [26–28, 30, 48, 49].
attenuated the pain response more than placebo (P < Thus, shorter chemical perfusion of the oesophagus can
0.001) with a difference in the means of 12.10 mA (95% CI induce generalized hyperalgesia with increased sensitivity
6.68, 17.53) and more than morphine (P = 0.016) with a in remote organs and other tissues and act as a transla-
difference in the means of 6.69 mA (95% CI 1.23, 12.13), tional bridge to the clinical situation.
When studying analgesic effects on experimental pain do not require any correction for multiplicity [62]. The
it is essential to choose the right dose, dosing regime and unpleasantness of pain is associated with the limbic struc-
time point for testing the analgesia as the kinetic profile tures in the brain, an area where opioids traditionally are
could affect the results [20]. A previous study demon- known to modulate the pain response. As deep pain is
strated similar pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic rela- considered more unpleasant than skin type of pain, opioid
tionships for somatic analgesia for morphine and analgesia is more robust in deep pain [20]. Therefore, the
oxycodone [50]. tmax is also comparable for the two opioids results from the deeper structures were considered most
as for both morphine and oxycodone it was previously important. However, results from skin stimulation sup-
found to be 1 h [51, 52]. Moreover, after 30 min both mor- ported results from deeper structures and comparable
phine and oxycodone should be present at the CNS effect results were demonstrated in all tissues which strengthen
sites [50].Therefore, it is not likely that the pharmacokinetic the conclusion.
differences can fully explain the different analgesic effects
between the opioids. A total score over 90 min was found
to be the most appropriate method for the post hoc analy- Different effects of morphine vs. oxycodone
sis as in pain treatment the analgesic effect is also evalu- Skin The skin heat stimulation had a narrow dynamic
ated over time and not at a specific time point. range of a few °C and might not be clinically relevant.
The analgesic potency ratio of oral morphine : oxyc- However, it supports the other results and indicates
odone has been widely discussed [34, 53]. However, it has different anti-hyperalgesic potencies of morphine and
been estimated and found to range from 1:1 to 2.2:1 oxycodone as an induced generalized hyperalgesia
[54–59]. The consensus in the pain management commu- (demonstrated in the placebo arm) was blocked by oxyc-
nity is that oral oxycodone is 1.5 to 2 times as potent as odone, but not significantly by morphine. As the P value for
oral morphine regarding analgesia [60, 61]. In the present the effect of morphine approached significance it could be
study a high dose of morphine was chosen to secure that hypothesized that an increased sample size would be nec-
differences in bioavailability did not favour oxycodone essary to demonstrate an effect of morphine in this experi-
analgesic efficacy. Furthermore, in a previous study the mental pain model of hyperalgesia. Previously, Staahl et al.
doses have showed comparable analgesic potencies in demonstrated equal analgesic potencies of the same
human experimental skin and muscle pain [21]. However, doses of morphine and oxycodone in the skin heat pain
the analgesic potency ratio of oral morphine : oxycodone model [21]. These conflicting results could be due to the
can be different from the non-analgesic pharmacody- effect of the induced generalized hyperalgesia, as this was
namic ratio. Some clinical studies have suggested that believed to affect both the pain system and the opioid
oxycodone produces less severe side effects than mor- system.
phine whereas a study in healthy volunteers suggested
that oral morphine and oral oxycodone have a different Muscle An overall increase in tolerated pressure was
relative potency (closer to 3:1) regarding non-analgesic found over time, most likely due to habituation to the tonic
pharmacodynamics, with oxycodone producing more side stimulation. Thus, no sensitization was found for this
effects [61]. No significant difference in adverse event pro- modality after the induction of generalized hyperalgesia.
files was found in the present study. However there was a This could be due to the fact that nerves from the gastroc-
trend towards more reports of side effects after oxyc- nemius muscle terminate in the lumbar and sacral part of
odone than after morphine. the spinal cord, whereas nerves from the oesophagus and
Opioid side effects could complicate blinding of the the arm are believed to terminate predominantly in the
placebo administration. An inert placebo substance was cervical and thoracic part of the spinal cord. Therefore,
included as the aim was to demonstrate differential effect spinal sensitization caused by acid and capsaicin perfusion
of opioids and not to reveal analgesic efficacy. However, of the oesophagus will not necessarily affect incoming
the inert placebo was to some degree masked by nausea pain signals from the leg. However, the analgesic effect of
and sweating caused by the chemical perfusion of morphine and oxycodone was demonstrated and oxyc-
oesophagus. odone had greater analgesic effect than morphine (and
Numerous endpoints could limit the impact on inter- placebo) in attenuating PTT. Opioids mainly attenuate pain
pretation of the findings by the multiple testing problem. intensities above the PDT [20]. This could explain why a
The two-way ANOVA was chosen for statistical analysis as it differential effect was only found regarding PTT. Staahl
decreases the probability of type 1 errors in multiple et al. also demonstrated differential analgesic effects on
testing. However, the aim was not to investigate whether experimental muscle stimulation in which oxycodone had
all trials were positive. The exploratory endpoints are a greater effect in patients with chronic pancreatitis [23].
thought to provide potentially worthwhile information Conversely, they could not demonstrate differential effects
about the treatment that could serve to generate hypoth- of morphine and oxycodone on pressure evoked muscle
eses for future studies. For this reason, endpoints that are pain in healthy volunteers, when no sensitization was
prespecified in the design of a clinical trial as exploratory induced [21]. A significant effect of morphine was only
demonstrated on this deep muscle pressure, which could pressure, hyperalgesia might nevertheless have affected
be due to a higher sensitivity and reproducibility of this the variation and a larger sample size would have been
stimulation method. more appropriate to demonstrate the effect of morphine.
However, differential analgesic potencies of morphine and
Oesophagus An analgesic effect of oxycodone using heat oxycodone were demonstrated in all tissues confirming
stimulation of the oesophagus was previously shown in that the sample size was sufficient for the main outcome of
healthy volunteers [21]. In the current study a new method the study.
ensuring linear rise in temperature was used, but no anal- It has been demonstrated in animal studies that oxyc-
gesic effect was demonstrated.This could be related to the odone may interact, at least in part, with a different popu-
faster change in temperature, which could cause uncer- lation of opioid receptors or modulate m-opioid receptor
tainty in pain assessments as it takes some time to rate the signalling in hyperalgesia in a subtly different way from
intensity. As individual differences in reaction time could other opioids [14, 65]. Moreover, the generalized hyperal-
not be excluded this could affect the accuracy of rating. gesia could lead to changes in the opioid system [66, 67].
Therefore, in future studies a slower temperature increase For example a more pronounced up-regulation of
should be recommended. However, the heat evoked k-receptors in inflammation has been speculated [8]. Fur-
referred pain areas were smaller after oxycodone com- thermore, animal studies have shown that binding to
pared with placebo and morphine. This likely reflects the k-receptor sites in the spinal cord are increased to a greater
effects of the drug on spinal or supraspinal changes in the degree than binding to m-receptors during peripheral
pain system after induction of hyperalgesia. The low base- inflammation [12]. It has been proposed from rodent
line value for morphine on referred pain areas from heat experiments that oxycodone is a partial k-agonist [14–18].
stimulation of the oesophagus could not be the reason for Therefore, it could be hypothesized that the differential
lack of significance of morphine effect, as all results were analgesic potencies are caused by different pharmacologi-
baseline corrected for the same reason before analysis. cal profiles of the opioids. Whether the present results are
However, as for the effect of morphine on skin heat pain, due to pharmacology and caused by different affinities for
the P value for the effect of morphine on referred pain area k-opioid receptors cannot be concluded from this or any
from oesophageal heat stimulation approached signifi- other human study as it would require a selective
cance, indicating an effect of morphine. However, in paral- k-antagonist suitable for human administration.
lel with other results the analgesic effect of oxycodone was
greater. In a previous study the opioids attenuated the Translational pain research
response to oesophageal mechanical stimulation in Animal models may provide pharmacodynamic informa-
healthy volunteers [21]. However, no effect was found in tion during drug development, but have limitations mim-
the present study. Acid perfusion causes oesophageal icking human pain conditions. Hence, they are based on
motility changes seen as increasing number and sizes of motor reflexes or behavioural responses, whereas human
contractions [63]. This could lead to squeezing of the pain is a net result of complex sensory, affective and cog-
balloon as well as traction force exerted in the pharynx and nitive processing [68]. Research with patients offers a
therefore the data must be carefully interpreted. means to explore the actual pain states of interest. On the
Electrical stimulation is often the most reliable visceral other hand, they suffer from difficulties in assessment of
stimulus as it is not affected by motility and has a high the pain, due to many confounding factors such as general
dynamic range [64]. Once more, the effect of morphine on malaise, fever, nausea, psychological status, etc [68]. There-
electrically induced pain approached significance, indicat- fore, experimental pain models in healthy volunteers, con-
ing an effect which could be demonstrated with an trolling the pain stimulus and assessment, can act as a
increased sample size. Nevertheless, oxycodone had a translational bridge from studies in animals to humans
greater effect than morphine in attenuating electrically [24]. Traditional models are short lasting and have many
induced pain by blocking the induced hyperalgesia. As the limitations compared with the complex clinical pain con-
electrical stimulation bypasses the peripheral nerves the ditions. However, experimental pain models can include
difference could be related to spinal and supraspinal hyperalgesia and be more consistent with phenomena
changes in the pain system [5–7]. The referred pain area to observed in patients.They may reasonably explain many of
electrical stimulation of the oesophagus increased after the abnormal pain responses typical of chronic pain. This
placebo indicating hyperalgesia. This hyperalgesia was was demonstrated in the current translational study as the
also blocked by the opioids, but non-significantly. The lack differential analgesic potency of morphine and oxycodone
of significance could be due to the high variance in how was comparable with the data previously found in our
subjects reported the referred pain area after electrical group on experimental pain stimulation in patients with
stimulation. chronic visceral pain [23]. Therefore, human experimental
The effect of morphine has previously been proved pain models of hyperalgesia may help to predict analgesic
detectable in 24 healthy volunteers [21]. As the results only potency in a sensitized pain system. Models evoking con-
demonstrated a significant effect of morphine on muscle trolled hyperalgesia have the advantages of experimental
pain and reflect the clinical situation to a higher degree 10 Pol O, Palacio JR, Puig MM. The expression of delta- and
than acute models.Therefore, they may speed up develop- kappa-opioid receptor is enhanced during intestinal
ment programmes within the industry and provide new inflammation in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2003; 306:
455–62.
fundamental knowledge on analgesics and pain mecha-
nisms [24]. 11 Stein C. Peripheral mechanisms of opioid analgesia. Anesth
It is difficult to predict whether the demonstrated dif- Analg 1993; 76: 182–91.
ferential analgesic potencies have a significant impact on 12 Iadarola MJ, Douglass J, Civelli O, Naranjo JR. Differential
the clinical efficacy of the opioids. However, subtle differ- activation of spinal cord dynorphin and enkephalin neurons
ences in analgesic potencies as found in the present study during hyperalgesia: evidence using cDNA hybridization.
are hard to elucidate in the clinical setting, but the results Brain Res 1988; 455: 205–12.
support the theory and practical experience about oxyc- 13 Riley J, Eisenberg E, Muller-Schwefe G, Drewes AM,
odone being different from morphine regarding analgesia Arendt-Nielsen L. Oxycodone: a review of its use in the
[1, 69, 70]. management of pain. Curr Med Res Opin 2008; 24: 175–92.
14 Nielsen CK, Ross FB, Lotfipour S, Saini KS, Edwards SR,
Smith MT. Oxycodone and morphine have distinctly
Competing interests different pharmacological profiles: radioligand binding and
behavioural studies in two rat models of neuropathic pain.
C. S. has been employed by Grünenthal after completion of Pain 2007; 132: 289–300.
the study and manuscript. There are no other competing
15 Ross FB, Smith MT. The intrinsic antinociceptive effects of
interests to declare.
oxycodone appear to be kappa-opioid receptor mediated.
The study was supported by Norpharma (Mundipharma), Pain 1997; 73: 151–7.
the Research Initiative, Aarhus University Hospital, ‘Det
Obelske Familie Fond’ and the Spar Nord Foundation. 16 Nozaki C, Saitoh A, Kamei J. Characterization of the
antinociceptive effects of oxycodone in diabetic mice. Eur J
Pharmacol 2006; 535: 145–51.
17 Nozaki C, Saitoh A, Tamura N, Kamei J. Antinociceptive effect
REFERENCES of oxycodone in diabetic mice. Eur J Pharmacol 2005; 524:
75–9.
1 Riley J, Ross JR, Rutter D, Wells AU, Goller K, du Bois R,
18 Holtman JR, Wala EP. Characterization of the antinociceptive
Welsh K. No pain relief from morphine? Individual variation
effect of oxycodone in male and female rats. Pharmacol
in sensitivity to morphine and the need to switch to an
Biochem Behav 2006; 83: 100–8.
alternative opioid in cancer patients. Support Care Cancer
2006; 14: 56–64. 19 Staahl C, Reddy H, Andersen SD, Arendt-Nielsen L,
Drewes AM. Multi-modal and tissue-differentiated
2 Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher D.
experimental pain assessment: reproducibility of a new
Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on
concept for assessment of analgesics. Basic Clin Pharmacol
daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain 2006; 10: 287–333.
Toxicol 2006; 98: 201–11.
3 De Schepper HU, Cremonini F, Park MI, Camilleri M. Opioids 20 Staahl C, Olesen AE, Andresen T, Arendt-Nielsen L,
and the gut: pharmacology and current clinical experience. Drewes AM. Assessing analgesic actions of opioids by
Neurogastroenterol Motil 2004; 16: 383–94. experimental pain models in healthy volunteers – an
4 Curatolo M, Arendt-Nielsen L, Petersen-Felix S. Central updated review. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2009; 68: 149–68.
hypersensitivity in chronic pain: mechanisms and clinical 21 Staahl C, Christrup LL, Andersen SD, Arendt-Nielsen L,
implications. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 2006; 17: 287–302. Drewes AM. A comparative study of oxycodone and
5 Cervero F. Visceral pain-central sensitisation. Gut 2000; 47 morphine in a multi-modal, tissue-differentiated
(Suppl. 4): iv56–7. experimental pain model. Pain 2006; 123: 28–36.
6 Anand P, Aziz Q, Willert R, van Oudenhove L. Peripheral and 22 Drewes AM, Krarup AL, Detlefsen S, Malmstrom ML,
central mechanisms of visceral sensitization in man. Dimcevski G, Funch-Jensen P. Pain in chronic pancreatitis:
Neurogastroenterol Motil 2007; 19: 29–46. the role of neuropathic pain mechanisms. Gut 2008; 57:
1616–27.
7 Dimcevski G, Sami SA, Funch-Jensen P, Le Pera D,
Valeriani M, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM. Pain in chronic 23 Staahl C, Dimcevski G, Andersen SD, Thorsgaard N,
pancreatitis: the role of reorganization in the central nervous Christrup LL, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM. Differential
system. Gastroenterology 2007; 132: 1546–56. effect of opioids in patients with chronic pancreatitis: an
experimental pain study. Scand J Gastroenterol 2007; 42:
8 Sengupta JN, Snider A, Su X, Gebhart GF. Effects of kappa 383–90.
opioids in the inflamed rat colon. Pain 1999; 79: 175–85.
24 Arendt-Nielsen L, Curatolo M, Drewes A. Human
9 Pol O, Alameda F, Puig MM. Inflammation enhances experimental pain models in drug development:
mu-opioid receptor transcription and expression in mice translational pain research. Curr Opin Investig Drugs 2007;
intestine. Mol Pharmacol 2001; 60: 894–9. 8: 41–53.
25 Hammer J, Vogelsang H. Characterization of sensations sensitization in patients with non-cardiac chest pain: a
induced by capsaicin in the upper gastrointestinal tract. clinical experimental study. Scand J Gastroenterol 2006; 41:
Neurogastroenterol Motil 2007; 19: 279–87. 640–9.
26 Drewes AM, Schipper KP, Dimcevski G, Petersen P, 40 Drewes AM, Reddy H, Pedersen J, Funch-Jensen P,
Andersen OK, Gregersen H, Arendt-Nielsen L. Multi-modal Gregersen H, Arendt-Nielsen L. Multimodal pain stimulations
induction and assessment of allodynia and hyperalgesia in in patients with grade B oesophagitis. Gut 2006; 55: 926–32.
the human oesophagus. Eur J Pain 2003; 7: 539–49.
41 Olesen AE, Staahl C, Brock C, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM.
27 Willert RP, Delaney C, Kelly K, Sharma A, Aziz Q, Hobson AR. Evoked human oesophageal hyperalgesia: a potential tool
Exploring the neurophysiological basis of chest wall for analgesic evaluation? Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 2009;
allodynia induced by experimental oesophageal 105: 126–36.
acidification – evidence of central sensitization.
Neurogastroenterol Motil 2007; 19: 270–8. 42 Quante M, Scharein E, Zimmermann R, Langer-Brauburger B,
Bromm B. Dissociation of morphine analgesia and sedation
28 Sarkar S, Woolf CJ, Hobson AR, Thompson DG, Aziz Q. evaluated by EEG measures in healthy volunteers.
Perceptual wind-up in the human oesophagus is enhanced Arzneimittelforschung 2004; 54: 143–51.
by central sensitisation. Gut 2006; 55: 920–5.
43 Roza C, Laird JM, Cervero F. Spinal mechanisms underlying
29 Frokjaer JB, Andersen SD, Gale J, Arendt-Nielsen L, persistent pain and referred hyperalgesia in rats with an
Gregersen H, Drewes AM. An experimental study of experimental ureteric stone. J Neurophysiol 1998; 79:
viscero-visceral hyperalgesia using an ultrasound-based 1603–12.
multimodal sensory testing approach. Pain 2005; 119:
191–200. 44 Wilder-Smith CH, Robert-Yap J. Abnormal endogenous pain
modulation and somatic and visceral hypersensitivity in
30 Sami SA, Rossel P, Dimcevski G, Nielsen KD, Funch-Jensen P, female patients with irritable bowel syndrome. World J
Valeriani M, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM. Cortical changes Gastroenterol 2007; 13: 3699–704.
to experimental sensitization of the human esophagus.
Neuroscience 2006; 140: 269–79. 45 Azpiroz F, Bouin M, Camilleri M, Mayer EA, Poitras P, Serra J,
Spiller RC. Mechanisms of hypersensitivity in IBS and
31 Drewes AM, Schipper KP, Dimcevski G, Petersen P, functional disorders. Neurogastroenterol Motil 2007; 19:
Andersen OK, Gregersen H, Arendt-Nielsen L. Multimodal 62–88.
assessment of pain in the esophagus: a new experimental
model. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 2002; 283: 46 Verne GN, Robinson ME, Price DD. Hypersensitivity to visceral
G95–103. and cutaneous pain in the irritable bowel syndrome. Pain
2001; 93: 7–14.
32 Kalso E. Oxycodone. J Pain Symptom Manage 2005; 29:
S47–56. 47 Chang L, Mayer EA, Johnson T, FitzGerald LZ, Naliboff B.
Differences in somatic perception in female patients with
33 Christrup LL, Sjogren P, Jensen NH, Banning AM, Elbaek K, irritable bowel syndrome with and without fibromyalgia.
Ersboll AK. Steady-state kinetics and dynamics of morphine Pain 2000; 84: 297–307.
in cancer patients: is sedation related to the absorption rate
of morphine? J Pain Symptom Manage 1999; 18: 164–73. 48 Arendt-Nielsen L, Svensson P. Referred muscle pain: basic
and clinical findings. Clin J Pain 2001; 17: 11–9.
34 Kalso E. How different is oxycodone from morphine? Pain
2007; 132: 227–8. 49 Brock C, Andresen T, Frokjaer JB, Gale J, Olesen AE,
Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM. Central pain mechanisms
35 Jespersen A, Dreyer L, Kendall S, Graven-Nielsen T, following combined acid and capsaicin perfusion of the
Arendt-Nielsen L, Bliddal H, Danneskiold-Samsoe B. human oesophagus. Eur J Pain 2009.
Computerized cuff pressure algometry: a new method to
assess deep-tissue hypersensitivity in fibromyalgia. Pain 50 Staahl C, Upton R, Foster DJ, Christrup LL, Kristensen K,
2007; 131: 57–62. Hansen SH, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM.
Pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modeling of morphine
36 Polianskis R, Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L. and oxycodone concentrations and analgesic effect in a
Computer-controlled pneumatic pressure algometry – a multimodal experimental pain model. J Clin Pharmacol
new technique for quantitative sensory testing. Eur J Pain 2008; 48: 619–31.
2001; 5: 267–77.
51 Collins SL, Faura CC, Moore RA, McQuay HJ. Peak plasma
37 Drewes AM, Gregersen H, Arendt-Nielsen L. Experimental concentrations after oral morphine: a systematic review. J
pain in gastroenterology: a reappraisal of human studies. Pain Symptom Manage 1998; 16: 388–402.
Scand J Gastroenterol 2003; 38: 1115–30.
52 Poyhia R, Seppala T, Olkkola KT, Kalso E. The
38 Frokjaer JB, Andersen SD, Ejskaer N, Funch-Jensen P, pharmacokinetics and metabolism of oxycodone after
Arendt-Nielsen L, Gregersen H, Drewes AM. Gut sensations in intramuscular and oral administration to healthy subjects. Br
diabetic autonomic neuropathy. Pain 2007; 131: 320–9. J Clin Pharmacol 1992; 33: 617–21.
39 Drewes AM, Pedersen J, Reddy H, Rasmussen K, 53 Bostrom E, Hammarlund-Udenaes M, Simonsson US.
Funch-Jensen P, Arendt-Nielsen L, Gregersen H. Central Blood-brain barrier transport helps to explain discrepancies
in in vivo potency between oxycodone and morphine. McQuay HJ, Quessy S, Rappaport BA, Revicki DA,
Anesthesiology 2008; 108: 495–505. Rothman M, Stauffer JW, Svensson O, White RE, Witter J.
Analyzing multiple endpoints in clinical trials of pain
54 Glare PA, Walsh TD. Dose-ranging study of oxycodone for
treatments: IMMPACT recommendations. Initiative on
chronic pain in advanced cancer. J Clin Oncol 1993; 11:
Methods, Measurement, and Pain Assessment in Clinical
973–8.
Trials. Pain 2008; 139: 485–93.
55 Kalso E, Vainio A. Morphine and oxycodone hydrochloride in 63 Drewes AM, Reddy H, Staahl C, Funch-Jensen P,
the management of cancer pain. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1990; Arendt-Nielsen L, Gregersen H, Lundbye-Christensen S.
47: 639–46. Statistical modeling of the response characteristics of
56 Bruera E, Belzile M, Pituskin E, Fainsinger R, Darke A, mechanosensitive stimuli in the human esophagus. J Pain
Harsanyi Z, Babul N, Ford I. Randomized, double-blind, 2005; 6: 455–62.
cross-over trial comparing safety and efficacy of oral 64 Staahl C, Drewes AM. Experimental human pain models: a
controlled-release oxycodone with controlled-release review of standardised methods for preclinical testing of
morphine in patients with cancer pain. J Clin Oncol 1998; 16: analgesics. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 2004; 95: 97–111.
3222–9.
65 Virk MS, Williams JT. Agonist-specific regulation of
57 Heiskanen TE, Ruismaki PM, Seppala TA, Kalso EA. Morphine mu-opioid receptor desensitization and recovery from
or oxycodone in cancer pain? Acta Oncol 2000; 39: 941–7. desensitization. Mol Pharmacol 2008; 73: 1301–8.
58 Curtis GB, Johnson GH, Clark P, Taylor R, Brown J, 66 Stanfa L, Dickenson A. Spinal opioid systems in
O’Callaghan R, Shi M, Lacouture PG. Relative potency of inflammation. Inflamm Res 1995; 44: 231–41.
controlled-release oxycodone and controlled-release
morphine in a postoperative pain model. Eur J Clin 67 Riviere PJ. Peripheral kappa-opioid agonists for visceral pain.
Pharmacol 1999; 55: 425–9. Br J Pharmacol 2004; 141: 1331–4.
59 Foley KM. The treatment of cancer pain. N Engl J Med 1985; 68 Langley CK, Aziz Q, Bountra C, Gordon N, Hawkins P, Jones A,
313: 84–95. Langley G, Nurmikko T, Tracey I. Volunteer studies in pain
research – opportunities and challenges to replace animal
60 Hanks GW, Conno F, Cherny N, Hanna M, Kalso E, McQuay HJ, experiments: the report and recommendations of a Focus
Mercadante S, Meynadier J, Poulain P, Ripamonti C, on Alternatives workshop. Neuroimage 2008; 42: 467–73.
Radbruch L, Casas JR, Sawe J, Twycross RG, Ventafridda V.
Morphine and alternative opioids in cancer pain: the EAPC 69 Lenz H, Sandvik L, Qvigstad E, Bjerkelund CE, Raeder J. A
recommendations. Br J Cancer 2001; 84: 587–93. comparison of intravenous oxycodone and intravenous
morphine in patient-controlled postoperative analgesia
61 Zacny JP, Lichtor SA. Within-subject comparison of the after laparoscopic hysterectomy. Anesth Analg 2009; 109:
psychopharmacological profiles of oral oxycodone and oral 1279–83.
morphine in non-drug-abusing volunteers.
70 Narabayashi M, Saijo Y, Takenoshita S, Chida M,
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008; 196: 105–16.
Shimoyama N, Miura T, Tani K, Nishimura K, Onozawa Y,
62 Turk DC, Dworkin RH, McDermott MP, Bellamy N, Burke LB, Hosokawa T, Kamoto T, Tsushima T. Opioid rotation from
Chandler JM, Cleeland CS, Cowan P, Dimitrova R, Farrar JT, oral morphine to oral oxycodone in cancer patients with
Hertz S, Heyse JF, Iyengar S, Jadad AR, Jay GW, Jermano JA, intolerable adverse effects: an open-label trial. Jpn J Clin
Katz NP, Manning DC, Martin S, Max MB, McGrath P, Oncol 2008; 38: 296–304.