An Integrated Comparison of Captive-Bred and Wild Atlantic Salmon (Salmo Salar) : Implications For Supportive Breeding Programs
An Integrated Comparison of Captive-Bred and Wild Atlantic Salmon (Salmo Salar) : Implications For Supportive Breeding Programs
An Integrated Comparison of Captive-Bred and Wild Atlantic Salmon (Salmo Salar) : Implications For Supportive Breeding Programs
available at www.sciencedirect.com
A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T
Article history: Supportive breeding is a strategy consisting in maintaining a pool of locally-adapted wild
Received 26 October 2007 genitors in captivity whose offspring are released in the wild at an early developmental
Received in revised form stage. In this study, we tested the utility of this strategy in preventing phenotypic and
21 May 2008 genetic divergences between captive-bred and wild animals that could be detrimental for
Accepted 25 May 2008 wild populations. Combining microsatellite analyses, morphological measurements and
Available online 10 July 2008 behavioural trials in the laboratory, we compared the progeny of Atlantic salmon (Salmo
salar) born in captivity with individuals born in the wild. At all these levels, we found sig-
Keywords: nificant differences between the progeny of the two groups. Specifically, allelic frequencies
Captive breeding significantly differed between groups, with captive-bred fish tending to be less variable
Salmonids conservation with lower heterozygosity and allelic richness values. The shape of wild-born fish was also
Population restoration different from that of the captive-group, particularly in the depth of the head and the
Microsatellite length of the pectoral fins. Finally, captive-bred individuals were, on average, more aggres-
Behaviour sive than wild-born fish. We demonstrated that this difference was strongly dependent
Morphometry upon the environment as captive-bred fish were more aggressive only when together with
their wild conspecifics or with an exotic competitor, the rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus
mykiss). Overall, our results showed that both phenotypic and genetic changes can arise
even if genitors share a common brood-stock and after only a few months of rearing in a
controlled environment. We conclude that the progeny produced in such supportive breed-
ing programs does not meet the criteria necessary to ensure preserving the genetic and
ecological integrity of wild populations.
2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction eral authors have argued that captive breeding suffers impor-
tant limitations (reviewed in Snyder et al., 1996). Particularly,
Captive breeding is a widely used management practice that captive breeding often leads to genetic, morphological and
aims to restore, conserve and/or enhance wild populations. behavioural differences between captive-bred and wild popu-
In contrast to the potential benefits for species recovery, sev- lations which may pose major difficulties when attempting to
* Corresponding author: Address: Laboratoire Evolution et Diversité Biologique, U.M.R 5174, C.N.R.S – University Paul Sabatier, 118 route
de Narbonne, F-31062, Toulouse cedex 4, France. Tel.: +33 5 61 55 85 81; fax: +33 5 61 55 67 28.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (S. Blanchet), [email protected] (D.J. Páez), [email protected] (L. Bernatchez),
[email protected] (J.J. Dodson).
0006-3207/$ - see front matter 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2008.05.014
1990 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9
preserve wild populations (Snyder et al., 1996; Price, 1999; and allelic richness) are important for populations to face
Heath et al., 2003). For instance, such deficiencies may have environmental changes (Frankham, 2008), and traits such as
harmful effects on wild populations (through ecological or ge- body size and competitive ability determine an individual’s
netic interactions) and may limit the settlement success of ability to exploit and survive in natural habitats (e.g. Håkans-
captive-bred animals in nature (Wang and Ryman, 2001; Ford, son and Jensen, 2005; Hill et al., 2006; Kraaijeveld-Smit et al.,
2002; McGinnity et al., 2003; Theodorou and Couvet, 2004; 2006). Although many studies have only considered one or
Håkansson and Jensen, 2005; Mathews et al., 2005; Frankham, two of these components (e.g. McPhee, 2004; Hill et al., 2006;
2008). Thus, researchers have attempted to develop captive Kraaijeveld-Smit et al., 2006), the information contained in
breeding strategies that aim to preserve both the genetic all of these components is needed to accurately forecast the
and phenotypic integrity of the target population (e.g. Duch- success of captive-bred animals in the wild and the effects
esne and Bernatchez, 2002; Fiumera et al., 2004; Theodorou they may have on wild populations (Kraaijeveld-Smit et al.,
and Couvet, 2004). 2006).
Recently, an alternative strategy to traditional captive Recent studies (Mathews et al., 2005; Kelley et al., 2006)
breeding has been developed to solve these potential prob- have highlighted the importance of considering several envi-
lems. This strategy (called ‘‘supportive breeding’’; Wang and ronments when attempting to compare the behaviour of cap-
Ryman, 2001) consists in maintaining a pool of locally- tive-bred and wild animals. For example, as density is
adapted wild genitors (i.e. genitors originating from the target generally higher in captivity and behaviour may vary accord-
population) in captivity whose offspring are released at an ing to density (Price, 1999; Blanchet et al., 2006; Kelley et al.,
early developmental stage. In theory, such a strategy may of- 2006), one can hypothesize that captive-bred animals behave
fer several advantages. First, the genetic integrity of the pop- differently according to the density they encounter in the
ulation is preserved through the use of randomly caught wild environment. Similarly, wild animals may also coexist with
breeders at each breeding season in combination with an ade- interspecific competitors that have been recently introduced
quate breeding design (e.g. Fiumera et al., 2000, 2004). Second, in their habitat (i.e., exotic species). Thus, if captive-bred ani-
the phenotypic and genotypic differences between captive- mals are unable to perform well (relative to their wild conspe-
bred and wild animals is limited by releasing first-generation cifics) in the presence of an exotic competitor, one can
captive animals at an early stage of development, thus limit- hypothesize that the exotic species may limit the settlement
ing exposure to the selective pressures imposed by captivity of the captive-bred animals.
(Salonen and Peuhkuri, 2006; Kraaijeveld-Smit et al., 2006). Currently, the enhancement of wild stocks of salmonid
Although this would be context-dependent and vary accord- fishes using hatchery-born fish to restore or supplement wild
ing to the census size of wild populations to be restocked, a populations is practiced worldwide, although the benefits re-
moderately high number of breeders (>20) should theoreti- main uncertain (Dodson et al., 1998). The question of the util-
cally be maintained in captivity to avoid undesirable genetic ity of supportive breeding strategies is therefore particularly
consequences that may negatively affect fitness of wild pop- relevant for this group of animals. For instance, Atlantic sal-
ulations (e.g. Tenhumberg et al., 2004; Theodorou and Couvet, mon (Salmo salar) is a cultural and economically important
2004; Favé et al., 2008). Typically, such a number is not always species. Atlantic salmon stocks are severely declining
obtainable due to logistical constraints or to the scarcity of throughout the species distribution, with some populations
the target population or species (Aho et al., 2006; Ramirez being considered endangered or nearly extinct (Klemetsen
et al., 2006). et al., 2003). Atlantic salmon naturally inhabit rivers of the
Evaluating the utility of captive breeding strategies is a North-Atlantic coastlines, where they spend their first two
prerequisite for future management decisions, and can be to five years of life. They then migrate to the ocean to feed
achieved through several approaches. A conservative ap- and grow for one year or more and then return to their natal
proach is to test for genetic and/or phenotypic differences be- rivers to spawn (see Klemetsen et al., 2003 for more informa-
fore releasing captive-bred animals into the wild (i.e., tests in tion). In this species, supportive breeding consists in produc-
the laboratory and/or in a controlled environment). To date, ing fish (from locally-adapted genitors) in a hatchery and
most studies that aim to compare captive-bred with wild ani- releasing them at the juvenile stage (usually within the first
mals consider captive populations that (i) were founded by year after hatching) into the wild (Tessier et al., 1997; Dodson
genitors from non-target wild populations and/or (ii) re- et al., 1998). Juvenile Atlantic salmon are territorial predators
mained captive for up to two generations (e.g. Mathews interacting through interference competition to defend profit-
et al., 2005; Kelley et al., 2006; Salonen and Peuhkuri, 2006). able territories that are rich in food and provide refuges
Therefore, the assumption that supportive breeding designs against predators (Klemetsen et al., 2003). In some rivers,
are useful for limiting genetic and phenotypic divergences juvenile salmon must also confront exotic competitors such
has, to our knowledge, rarely been tested. as the rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss), an anadromous
Evaluating the success of captive-bred animals in nature salmonid from the north-western coast of North America,
and their impacts on wild populations involves assessing representing an additional risk to declining populations
the genetic risks (e.g. inbreeding depression, change in allelic (Blanchet et al., 2007). The ability of captive-bred fish to adapt
frequencies and/or the introduction of deleterious alleles in to these competitive and unpredictable environments is cru-
wild populations, Ryman et al., 1995; Ford, 2002) as well as cial to forecast the benefits and also the genetic and ecologi-
the morphological, physiological and behavioural capabilities cal consequences of supportive breeding programs.
of captive-bred fish to survive in the wild. For instance, The main objective of this study was to assess the utility of
parameters linked to genetic diversity (e.g. heterozygosity a supportive breeding strategy in preventing potential diver-
B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9 1991
gences between captive-bred and wild-born animals that are commercial fish food pellets before the beginning of the
usually considered detrimental for the target population. To experiments. Behavioural experiments were conducted dur-
do so, we compared the phenotypic and genetic characteris- ing the following winter (i.e., five months after fish capture).
tics of the progeny of wild breeders produced in captivity Mortality in the holding tank was relatively low (i.e. 10–20 fish
and in the wild. We posit the null hypothesis that no genetic per group) suggesting that no strong artificial selection (in-
or morphological changes occurred between captive-born duced by laboratory conditions) occurred during this period.
and wild-born Atlantic salmon sharing the same brood-stock.
We also compared the behaviour of captive-born and wild- 2.2. Genetic analyses
born Atlantic salmon in different competitive environments
to test the null hypotheses that (i) no behavioural changes oc- We selected 35 fish from each experimental group (n = 70). To-
curred between captive-born and wild-born Atlantic salmon tal DNA was extracted from muscle tissue using a salt extrac-
(ii) the behaviour of captive-born animals is not density- tion method described in Aljanabi and Martinez (1997). Ten
dependent and (iii) captive-born and wild-born conspecifics nuclear microsatellite loci were amplified using polymerase
behave similarly in the presence of an exotic competitor. chain reaction (PCR) (Table 1). PCR products were run on an
ABI 3100 automated capillary sequencer (Applied Biosys-
TM
The two groups of young-of-the-year (YOY) fish we compared For the morphometric analysis, we selected 30 fish from each
were produced from adult Atlantic salmon from the river Mal- experimental group (n = 60). Fish were euthanized with an ex-
baie (Québec, Canada, 47 67 0 N; 70 16 0 W). The Atlantic sal- cess of the sedative eugenol and each was positioned on their
mon population in this river was relatively small until a left side on a measuring board with the lower jaw closed and
restoration program began in 1992. Presently, about 400–500 caudal fin extended. Each individual was photographed using
adults enter this river annually to spawn. A pool of genitors an Olympus digital camera. From each image, 16 morpholog-
(i.e., 10 anadromous males and 10 females) is maintained at ical traits (Fig. 1) were measured to the nearest 0.001 mm
the provincial hatchery of Tadoussac. Each year, this pool is using the free software IMAGE J (U.S. National Institute of
renewed by haphazardly capturing approximately 10 wild Health, http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/). We focused on these traits
genitors (five anadromous males and five females) in a fish because they have been associated with swimming perfor-
ladder installed on an insurmountable dam during the sum- mance and habitat selection (Letcher, 2003; Páez et al., 2008).
mer spawning migration. An effective breeder’s pool of 20
genitors (with an equal sex-ratio) renewed at 50% each gener- 2.4. Behavioural analyses
ation falls within the range theoretically predicted for pre-
serving wild populations for a population with such census Behavioural tests were conducted using 12 artificial channels
size (Duchesne and Bernatchez, 2002; Favé et al., 2008) and fitted with a re-circulating water system. Each channel mea-
in the range used in other programs (Aho et al., 2006; Ramirez sured 1.90 m long, 0.30 m wide and 0.30 m deep. One pool/rif-
et al., 2006). At the hatchery, the genitors are mated and the fle succession was simulated using small pebbles (see
progeny is reared in tanks. Fish densities at this hatchery Blanchet et al., 2006 for more details). Water depth and water
are relatively low (500 YOYs m3 compared to velocity were consistent with natural habitat conditions
3
2000 YOYs m in commercial hatcheries, see McDonald experienced by juvenile Atlantic salmon (Klemetsen et al.,
et al., 1998), and water temperature is free to fluctuate natu- 2003; Blanchet et al., 2007). Two large pebbles (7–10 cm diam-
rally as the hatchery receives its water supply from a neigh- eter) were added in each riffle and each pool as refuges. Pho-
bouring lake. Fish are reared until the age of about four toperiod was controlled with two 60-W lights above each
months before being released in September. Thus, the salmon channel at 80% and 7% of the available intensity during day,
population in the river Malbaie consists of wild-born and dawn, dusk, and night, respectively. Light-to-dark cycle was
hatchery-born fish. In the downstream part of this river (i.e., 9:14 h plus 30 min of dawn and of dusk. Light intensity and
below the fish ladder) a self-sustaining population of exotic photoperiod were automatically set with a photoperiod mon-
rainbow trout cohabits with the Atlantic salmon. itor (SunMatch, Aquabiotech Inc.). We maintained water tem-
During August 2005, before hatchery-born fish were re- perature constant at 14 ± 1 C. Fish were fed with commercial
leased in the river, we sampled (by electrofishing) 250 YOY food pellets. Daily food ration (4% of the total wet biomass per
Atlantic salmon at two locations above the dam (these fish channel and per day) was dispensed at the upstream end of
are hereafter referred to as the ‘‘wild group’’). In addition, the channel.
we sampled 100 YOY rainbow trout in the downstream part The behavioural experiment involved seven competitive
of the river. During the same period, we haphazardly sampled treatments (Table 2) designed to separate the effect of adding
25 YOYs from each of 10 families produced in 2005 for the a given competitor from a simple density effect (see Weber
supportive breeding program. These 250 fish are hereafter re- and Fausch, 2003). Firstly, we tested each group indepen-
ferred to as the ‘‘hatchery group’’. dently, only varying their intraspecific densities (i.e. in treat-
We transferred all fish groups to the laboratory and raised ments 1 and 3, we observed three fish per channel and in
them in separate holding tanks. They were fed ad-libitum with treatments 2 and 4 we observed six fish per channel).
1992 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9
comprised 10 mM Tris–HCl [pH 9.0], 1.5 mM MgCl2, 0.1% Triton X-100, 50 mM KCl. The concentration of dNTPs was 10 mM each. References denoted with * correspond to loci that were directly
Description of the ten microsatellites markers used to compare genetic parameters between captive-born and wild-born Atlantic salmon. Number of alleles (A) is presented for each locus. The buffer
Poteaux et al. (1999)
Cairney et al. (2000)
Cairney et al. (2000)
groups were in contact together (i.e. in treatment 5 we ob-
O’Reilly et al.*
Slettan et al.*
we assessed behaviour when the two salmon groups were
in contact with the rainbow trout (i.e. we independently ob-
served three fish of each group together with three rainbow
trout per channel in treatments 6 and 7, Table 2). Each treat-
ment lasted five days, including a three-day acclimatisation
period followed by two days of observation. Each treatment
5 min at 94, 34 · (45 s at 94, 45 s at 60, 120 s at 68), 10 min at 68
fish used for each replicate (i.e. no fish was ever used twice).
During the experiment, fish body weight was 3.22 ± 0.92 g
Cycle (temperature in Celsius degrees)
plied to account for multiple comparisons) and for all the loci
10
10
10
10
10
10
10
10
SSSOSl 417
CA 054978
CA 054565
SSAD 71
SsaD 85
Ssa 197
Fig. 1 – Landmarks used for morphological measurements. 1–2 Fork length (FL), 3–4 maximum body depth (BD), 1–7 head
length (HL), 5–6 head depth (HD), 8–9 orbital length (OL), 10–11 orbital depth (OD), 1–5 distance between tip of the snout and
highest head height (SHL), 12–13 pectoral fin length (PCL), 1–3 predorsal length (PDL), 1–14 prepelvic length (PPL), 3–15 distance
between origins of the dorsal and anal fins (ODAFL), 3–16 distance between the origins of the dorsal and adipose fins
(ODADFL), 17–18 distance between insertions of the adipose and anal fins (OAAFL), 18–19 caudal peduncle length (DPL), 20–21
caudal peduncle depth (CPD), 22–23 minimum caudal fin height (MinCFH), 24–25 maximum caudal fin height (MaxCFH).
Table 2 – Design of the experiment used to compare the using a Bonferroni procedure (so that the acceptable signifi-
behaviours of wild-born versus hatchery-born Atlantic cance level was reduced to a < 0.05/16 = 0.003).
salmon
Wild-born Hatchery-born Rainbow 2.5.3. Behavioural analyses
salmon salmon trout A two-way MANCOVA was computed to test whether the gen-
eral behaviour of the fish changed according to origin (wild or
Low intraspecific W 3 – –
High intraspecific W 6 – –
hatchery) and competitive treatments (see Table 2). We used
Low intraspecific H – 3 – the averaged body length of Atlantic salmon in each replicate
High intraspecific H – 6 – and for each group as the covariate. All two-term interactions
Mixed intraspecific 3 3 – were considered in this analysis. If the MANCOVA was signif-
Interspecific W 3 – 3 icant, we performed three independent ANCOVAs to compare
Interspecific H – 3 3
(i) the aggression rate, (ii) the time spent in the pool and (iii)
Experimental design used to evaluate the effects of intraspecific the time spent being active. The aggression rate was log(x + 1)
and interspecific competition on the behaviour of hatchery-born transformed and the two other behaviours were arcsine
and wild-born Atlantic salmon. The number of fish introduced in
transformed to meet the assumption of normality and homo-
each treatment (seven treatments) is indicated. ‘‘W’’ means wild-
scedasticity. Initially, a ‘‘trial’’ factor was included in the mod-
born fish and ‘‘H’’ means hatchery-born fish. Each treatment was
replicated four times (n = 4) and new fish were used for each el to account for possible temporal effects. As it was not
replicate. significant, this term was excluded from the final models.
All statistical analyses were performed using R version
should be larger than the heterozygosity expected from the 2.2.1 (R Development Core Team, 2005).
observed allele number at mutation-drift equilibrium (Cornu-
et and Luikart, 1996). We tested this assumption using the 3. Results
BOTTLENECK software (Cornuet and Luikart, 1996). This soft-
ware used sign test to compare the observed number of loci 3.1. Genetic analyses
with heterozygosity excess to the number expected under
the mutation-drift equilibrium (Cornuet and Luikart, 1996). When all loci were pooled, allele frequencies significantly var-
ied between the two groups (Fisher exact test, v2 = +1,
2.5.2. Morphometric analyses p = 0.000, Table 3A). Seven out of the ten loci we compared
We used ANCOVAs (analyses of covariance) to asses shape showed different frequencies between the two groups after
differences between the two groups of fish while controlling the Bonferroni correction (Table 3A). As illustrated in Fig. 2,
for the allometric effect of body size on each morphological some of these loci showed marked differences, with each
trait. We first computed a multivariate ANCOVA (MANCOVA) group being characterized by specific patterns of allelic fre-
with the total body length of each fish as the covariate and quencies and sometimes by private alleles with high frequen-
the origin of the fish as categorical predictor. The resulting cies. The allele frequencies for all the ten loci used in this
two-term interaction was also included to test for allometric experiment are available upon request to the corresponding
differences between the two groups of fish. If the MANCOVA author. We also found that these frequency differences trans-
was significant, we then used a univariate ANCOVA to assess lated into a trend whereby both individual heterozygosity and
how each trait independently differed between each group. allelic richness tended to be lower in the hatchery than in the
Significant p-values were corrected for multiple comparison wild group (Table 3B). These differences, however, were
1994 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9
Alleles size(bp)
Results of several tests used to compare genetic parameters
between hatchery-born (‘‘Hatchery fish’’) and wild-born (‘‘Wild
210
fish’’) Atlantic salmon. Table (A) – results (p-value ± SD) of Fisher
exact tests used to compare the allelic frequencies, for each mar- 205
ker independently and for all the loci combined, between the
hatchery and wild groups. Table (B) – genetic diversity (mean ± S.E. 200
for allelic richness and multilocus heterozygosity respectively) for
both groups of salmon. Bold p-values are significant after Bon- 195
feronni corrections.
190
Results of ANCOVAs used to compare each morphological trait (see Fig. 1 for a description of the traits) independently between hatchery-born
(‘‘Hatchery fish’’) and wild-born (‘‘Wild fish’’) Atlantic salmon. The means corrected for fish body length (±SE) for each trait and each group are
given for comparison. Bold p-values are significant after Bonferoni corrections. A star (*) indicated trait(s) for which a significant interaction
term between fish body length and origin of the fish was detected.
Table 5 – Statistical comparison of the behaviours of wild-born versus hatchery-born Atlantic salmon
Response variables
Time spent in the pool Time spent being active Aggression rate
Multivariate (A) and univariate (B) analyses of variance used to compare the behavioural responses (time spent in the pool, time spent being
active and aggressive rate) between hatchery-bred and wild Atlantic salmon under different competitive treatments. Analysis of variance
included the fixed effects of groups and competitive treatments. Significant p-values (p < 0.05) are in bold.
1996 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9
1.4 tion indicated that hatchery fish differed from wild fish but
a Wild fish only in some of the treatments (Table 5B, Fig. 3c). Particularly,
Arcsine (time spent in a pool)
1.2 Hatchery fish hatchery fish were more aggressive than wild fish only in
presence of their wild conspecifics or with the rainbow trout
1.0 (Fig. 3c).
0.8
0.6 4. Discussion
0.8
Hatchery fish sults from a bottleneck inherent in the low number of geni-
tors used in captivity (Ryman et al., 1995; Tessier et al., 1997;
Kraaijeveld-Smit et al., 2006). It is worth noting that before
0.6 the restoration program began in river Malbaie, the popula-
tion size was relatively small, as for most populations that
are endangered or exploited (Smith and Bernatchez, 2008).
0.4 However, it did not seem that this low population size was
sufficient to induce a detectable bottleneck in the wild-born
0.2 group. These results suggest that the number of breeders (or
refreshment rate) used in supportive design should be care-
fully estimated to avoid undesirable genetic effects.
0.0 Similarly, significant morphological and allometric differ-
Low intra High intra Mixed Interspe. ences were detected between captive-bred and wild-born sal-
density density intra mon, with the strongest changes observed for fin lengths and
other aspects of body shape, that are functionally important
Fig. 3 – Behavioural repertory of wild-born versus hatchery- for swimming. These results concur with those obtained for
born Atlantic salmon. Patterns of behavioural responses [(a) other animal species (e.g. Håkansson and Jensen, 2005) and
time spent in a pool, (b) time spent being active and (c) particularly with those obtained for salmonids (Kostow,
aggression rate] of hatchery-born (‘‘Hatchery fish’’, white 2004; Dahl et al., 2006, reviewed in Weber and Fausch, 2003).
bars) and wild-born (‘‘Wild fish’’, black bars) Atlantic salmon As demonstrated by Kostow (2004) in rainbow trout, the mor-
when reared under four different competitive treatments phological differences seen in captive-bred individuals were
see Table 2 for a description of the treatments. Data are the directly associated with a higher mortality rate in the wild.
mean (±S.E.). Each treatment was replicated four times This link has been interpreted as a consequence of the re-
(n = 4) and new fish were used for each replicate. duced ability of captive animals to occupy favourable habitats
(Weber and Fausch, 2003). Although we failed to detect signif-
icant differences in habitat use by captive-bred and wild-born
body length (Table 5B) with bigger fish being on average more animals in our laboratory setting, an association between
aggressive (results not shown). Irrespectively of individual morphology and habitat use seems highly plausible in the
body length, we found a significant interaction between wild. Indeed, morphological traits such as long paired fins
groups and competitive treatments (Table 5B). This interac- and a large caudal peduncle and caudal fin facilitate the
B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9 1997
exploitation of habitats such as high current velocity zones, at environments and when they interbred with wild conspecif-
lower energetic costs (Arnold et al., 1991; Letcher, 2003; Páez ics. This implies that further studies should be specifically de-
et al., 2008). signed to elucidate the relative importance of genetic versus
Changes in behavioural characteristics such as activity or environmental factors (and their interaction) on the pheno-
aggression rate have also been associated with poor survival typic differentiation of captive-bred and wild animals during
rate of captive fish in the wild (i.e. a high predation rate, see Biro early ontogeny.
et al., 2004). Here, in accordance with many other studies, we The number of breeding events in captivity separating our
found that captive-bred animals were on average more aggres- two groups of fish was difficult to evaluate. Indeed, the wild-
sive than wild-born conspecifics. This suggests that the proto- born fish we used here might have originated from captive-
col used to raise fish under hatchery conditions induces reared fish previously released in the River Malbaie. Similarly,
behaviours that may be maladapted for surviving in the wild the genitors that were used by the hatchery to produce the
or that may be detrimental for wild populations (Biro et al., captive-bred fish might themselves have originated from cap-
2004; McPhee, 2004; Mathews et al., 2005; Kelley et al., 2006). tive-bred fish, signifying in this case that more than a single
We also demonstrated that such a behavioural difference breeding event could have separated purely-wild fish from
between groups was highly dependent on the social environ- captive-bred fish. Araki et al. (2007b) recently demonstrated
ment (i.e. density and type of competitors). Indeed, captive- that the effect of captivity on fitness was cumulative with a
bred fish were more aggressive only when together with their fitness decline of 37.5% per captive-reared generation. Thus,
wild-born conspecifics or when together with rainbow trout. the phenotypic and genetic differences we report might
It is worth noting that this difference was due to a concomi- directly reflect this cumulative effect of captivity. These
tant decrease and increase in aggression of the wild and differences are expected to increase if measures are not taken
hatchery group, respectively. This also indicates that wild- to ensure that genitors used in captivity have not been them-
born salmon strongly modify their natural behaviour (i.e. in selves bred in captivity.
comparison to the purely intraspecific treatments) when fac-
ing captive-bred salmon and/or rainbow trout. This behav-
ioural plasticity has several important implications for 5. Conclusions and implications
supportive breeding programs. First, by being more aggressive
when together with wild-born salmon, captive-raised ani- We showed that using genitors from a ‘‘local’’ brood-stock and
mals could exert a new selective pressure on wild-born indi- a limited rearing period in captivity did not prevent the mor-
viduals, as suggested by Blanchet et al. (2007) in the case of phological, behavioural and genetic divergences that have
competition between exotic and native salmonids. Second, commonly been identified as detrimental for target popula-
the behavioural response of Atlantic salmon differed between tions. Therefore, the progeny produced in such supportive
groups when facing an exotic competitor. This result suggests breeding programs does not meet the criteria necessary to en-
that the captive environment can change behavioural charac- sure preserving the genetic and ecological integrity of wild
teristics that are relevant to biological interactions such as populations. In view of these results and following the pre-
interspecific competition. cautionary principle, we argue that such supportive breeding
Taken as a whole, our results provide insight into the programs cannot be considered as acceptable conservation
mechanisms underlying the different phenotypic changes strategies, at least in their present form. However, such pro-
we documented. In the absence of genetic data, many authors grams could easily be improved. For instance, the first aim
have hypothesized that because of a common brood-stock of these programs should be maintaining the genetic diversity
(hence a common genetic background) phenotypic plasticity and allelic frequencies of the native population rather than
predominantly determines phenotypic differences between maximizing production. To do so, the genetic structure of
captive-bred and wild animals (Kostow, 2004; Dahl et al., the native population must be well known, the census size
2006). This hypothesis is particularly attractive when consid- of the captive breeders must be adequately calculated and
ering animals that phenotypically diverge within a single frequently refreshed, and factorial breeding designs should
generation of captivity. In our case, phenotypic plasticity be favoured (Fiumera et al., 2000, 2004; Duchesne and Bernat-
probably contributes in part to the phenotypic differentiation chez, 2002; Wedekind, 2002). Also, it has recently been dem-
we observed between groups. However, since we observed ge- onstrated that equalizing milt volume of males reduces the
netic differences between both groups, we can also suggest loss of genetic variation in a captive population (Wedekind
that these phenotypic differences also result from the pheno- et al., 2007). Efforts should also be devoted to avoid using gen-
typic expression of functional alleles present in the wild-born itors that are themselves issued from captive brood-stock and
group that are not expressed in captivity (or vice versa) hence to limit the cumulative effects of captivity (Araki et al.,
(Wedekind, 2002; Heath et al., 2003). Thus, if genetic factors 2007b). Second, selection and/or plasticity acting during cap-
contribute to the differentiation of captive-bred and wild- tivity must be reduced to avoid phenotypic differentiation.
born animals, one can expect potential detrimental effects This can be achieved by reducing the captive period. Metcalfe
for wild populations in the case of interbreeding between cap- et al. (2003) proposed to limit the period of captivity to egg
tive-bred and wild-born animals (Araki et al., 2007b; Roberge production with release at the egg stage. Another possibility,
et al., 2008). For instance, Araki et al. (2007b) used pedigree albeit more costly, would be to rear captive juveniles in more
analyses to demonstrate that in the rainbow trout the genetic natural conditions by increasing the physical heterogeneity of
effects of domestication strongly reduced subsequent repro- the captive habitat or by building artificial nursery habitat
ductive capabilities when captive fish were moved to natural (see Berejikian et al., 2000). Once this has been achieved,
1998 B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9
short-term and long-term assessment of the costs (both eco- recruit at higher rates than hacked falcons in a common
logical and economical) and benefits of supportive programs environment. Biological Conservation 131, 453–458.
can be performed in the wild through the use of parentage Cairney, M., Taggart, J.B., Hoyheim, B., 2000. Characterization of
microsatellite and minisatellite loci in Atlantic salmon (Salmo
assignment analyses, capture-mark-recapture programs,
salar L.) and cross-species amplification in other salmonids.
large-scale experiments and/or meta-analyses (e.g. Tessier Molecular Ecology 9, 2175–2178.
et al., 1997; Hansen, 2002; Brown et al., 2006; Adamski and Cornuet, J.M., Luikart, G., 1996. Description and power analysis of
Witkowski, 2007; Araki et al., 2007a, b). two tests for detecting recent population bottlenecks from
allele frequency data. Genetics 144, 2001–2014.
Dahl, J., Pettersson, E., Dannewitz, J., Jarvi, T., Lof, A.C., 2006. No
Acknowledgments
difference in survival, growth and morphology between
offspring of wild-born, hatchery and hybrid brown trout (Salmo
We are grateful to the LARSA team (Québec, Canada) for rear- trutta). Ecology of Freshwater Fish 15, 388–397.
ing fish. We also want to thank Prof. Helga Guderley and Or- Dodson, J.J., Gibson, R.J., Cunjak, R.A., Friedland, K.D., Garcia de
lane Rossignol who helped on several aspects of the work. Leaniz, C., Gross, M.R., Newbury, R., Nielsen, J.L., Power, M.E.,
We also thank Dimitar Serebezov and Anne-Marie Gale for Roy, S., 1998. Elements in the development of conservation
their help with the optimization of several loci. We thank plans for Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar). Canadian Journal of
Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 55, 312–323.
two anonymous referees for their fruitful comments on an
Duchesne, P., Bernatchez, L., 2002. An analytical investigation of
earlier version of the draft. This research was financially sup- the dynamics of inbreeding in multi-generation supportive
ported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering research Coun- breeding. Conservation Genetics 3, 47–60.
cil of Canada grant (strategic program) to J.J.D. and L.B. The Favé, M.J., Duchesne, P., Turgeon, J., 2008. Inbreeding dynamics in
experiments conducted comply with current Canadian laws reintroduced, age-structured populations of highly fecund
(License No. 2004-140). species. Conservation Genetics 9, 39–48.
Fiumera, A.C., Parker, P.G., Fuerst, P.A., 2000. Effective population
size and maintenance of genetic diversity in captive-bred
populations of a Lake Victoria Cichlid. Conservation Biology
R E F E R E N C E S
14, 886–892.
Fiumera, A.C., Porter, B.A., Looney, G., Asmussen, M.A., Avise, J.C.,
2004. Maximizing offspring production while maintaining
Adamski, P., Witkowski, Z.J., 2007. Effectiveness of population genetic diversity in supplemental breeding programs of
recovery projects based on captive breeding. Biological highly fecund managed species. Conservation Biology 18, 94–
Conservation 140, 1–7. 101.
Aho, T., Ronn, J., Piironen, J., Bjorklund, M., 2006. Impacts of Ford, M.J., 2002. Selection in captivity during supportive breeding
effective population size on genetic diversity in hatchery may reduce fitness in the wild. Conservation Biology 16, 815–
reared Brown trout (Salmo trutta L.) populations. Aquaculture 825.
253, 244–248. Frankham, R., 2008. Genetic adaptation to captivity in species
Aljanabi, S., Martinez, I., 1997. Universal and rapid salt-extraction conservation programs. Molecular Ecology 17, 325–333.
of high quality genomic DNA for PCR-based techniques. Goudet, J., 1995. FSTAT (Version 1.2): A computer program to
Nucleic Acids Research 25, 4692–4693. calculate F-statistics. Journal of Heredity 86, 485–486.
Araki, H., Ardren, W.R., Olsen, E., Cooper, B., Blouin, M.S., 2007a. Håkansson, J., Jensen, P., 2005. Behavioural and morphological
Reproductive success of captive-bred steelhead trout in the variation between captive populations of red junglefowl
wild: Evaluation of three hatchery programs in the Hood River. (Gallus gallus) – possible implications for conservation.
Conservation Biology 21, 181–190. Biological Conservation 122, 431–439.
Araki, H., Cooper, B., Blouin, M.S., 2007b. Genetic effects of captive Hansen, M.M., 2002. Estimating the long-term effects of stocking
breeding cause a rapid, cumulative fitness decline in the wild. domesticated trout into wild brown trout (Salmo trutta)
Science 318, 100–103. populations: an approach using microsatellite DNA analysis of
Arnold, G.P., Webb, P.W., Holford, B.H., 1991. The role of the historical and contemporary samples. Molecular Ecology 11,
pectoral fins in station-holding of Atlantic Salmon Parr (Salmo 1003–1015.
salar L). Journal of Experimental Biology 156, 625–629. Heath, D.D., Heath, J.W., Bryden, C.A., Johnson, R.M., Fox, C.W.,
Berejikian, B.A., Tezak, E.P., Flagg, T.A., LaRae, A.L., Kummerow, E., 2003. Rapid evolution of egg size in captive salmon. Science
Mahnken, C.V.W., 2000. Social dominance, growth, and habitat 299, 1738–1740.
use of age-0 steelhead (Oncorhynchus mykiss) grown in enriched Hill, M.S., Zydlewski, G.B., Gale, W.L., 2006. Comparisons between
and conventional hatchery rearing environments. Canadian hatchery and wild steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss)
Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 57, 628–636. smolts: physiology and habitat use. Canadian Journal of
Biro, P.A., Abrahams, M.V., Post, J.R., Parkinson, E.A., 2004. Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 63, 1627–1638.
Predators select against high growth rates and risk-taking Kelley, J.L., Magurran, A.E., Garcia, C.M., 2006. Captive breeding
behaviour in domestic trout populations. Proceedings of the promotes aggression in an endangered Mexican fish.
Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271, 2233– Biological Conservation 133, 169–177.
2237. King, T.L., Eackles, M.S., Letcher, B.H., 2005. Microsatellite DNA
Blanchet, S., Dodson, J.J., Brosse, S., 2006. Influence of habitat markers for the study of Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) kinship,
structure and fish density on Atlantic salmon Salmo salar L population structure, and mixed-fishery analyses. Molecular
territorial behaviour. Journal of Fish Biology 68, 951–957. Ecology Notes 5, 130–132.
Blanchet, S., Loot, G., Bernatchez, L., Dodson, J.J., 2007. The Klemetsen, A., Amundsen, P.A., Dempson, J.B., Jonsson, B.,
disruption of dominance hierarchies by a non-native species: Jonsson, N., Mortensen, E., 2003. Atlantic salmon Salmo salar
an individual-based analysis. Oecologia 152, 569–581. L., brown trout Salmo trutta L. and Artic charr Salvelinus alpinus
Brown, J.L., Collopy, M.W., Gott, E.J., Juergens, P.W., Montoya, A.B., (L.): a review of aspects of their life histories. Ecology of
Hunt, W.G., 2006. Wild-reared aplomado falcons survive and Freshwater Fish 12, 1–59.
B I O L O G I C A L C O N S E RVAT I O N 1 4 1 ( 2 0 0 8 ) 1 9 8 9 –1 9 9 9 1999
Kostow, K.E., 2004. Differences in juvenile phenotypes and signatus captive breeding program. Conservation Genetics 7,
survival between hatchery stocks and a natural population 861–878.
provide evidence for modified selection due to captive Raymond, M., Rousset, F., 1995a. Genepop (Version-1.2) –
breeding. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences Population-genetics software for exact tests and
61, 577–589. ecumenicism. Journal of Heredity 86, 248–249.
Kraaijeveld-Smit, F.J.L., Griffiths, R.A., Moore, R.D., Beebee, T.J.C., Raymond, M., Rousset, F., 1995b. An exact test for population
2006. Captive breeding and the fitness of reintroduced species: differentiation. Evolution 49, 1280–1283.
a test of the responses to predators in a threatened Rise, M.L., von Schalburg, K.R., Brown, G.D., et al, 2004.
amphibian. Journal of Applied Ecology 43, 360–365. Development and application of a salmonid EST database and
Letcher, B.H., 2003. Life history dependent morphometric cDNA microarray: Data mining and interspecific hybridization
variation in stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon. Oecologia 137, characteristics. Genome Research 14, 478–490.
533–540. Roberge, C., Normandeau, E., Einum, S., Guderley, H., Bernatchez,
Mathews, F., Orros, M., McLaren, G., Gelling, M., Foster, R., 2005. L., 2008. Genetic consequences of interbreeding between
Keeping fit on the ark: assessing the suitability of captive-bred farmed and wild Atlantic salmon: insights from the
animals for release. Biological Conservation 121, 569–577. transcriptome. Molecular Ecology 17, 314–324.
McDonald, D.G., Milligan, C.L., McFarlane, W.J., Croke, S., Currie, Ryman, N., Jorde, P.E., Laikre, L., 1995. Supportive breeding and
S., Hooke, B., Angus, R.B., Tufts, B.L., Davidson, K., 1998. variance effective population size. Conservation Biology 9,
Condition and performance of juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo 1619–1628.
salar): effects of rearing practices on hatchery fish and Salonen, A., Peuhkuri, N., 2006. The effect of captive breeding on
comparison with wild fish. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and aggressive behaviour of European grayling, Thymallus
Aquatic Sciences 55, 1208–1219. thymallus, in different contexts. Animal Behaviour 72, 819–
McGinnity, P., Prodohl, P., Ferguson, K., Hynes, R., O’Maoileidigh, 825.
N., Baker, N., Cotter, D., O’Hea, B., Cooke, D., Rogan, G., Taggart, Smith, T.B., Bernatchez, L., 2008. Evolutionary change in human-
J., Cross, T., 2003. Fitness reduction and potential extinction of altered environments. Molecular Ecology 17, 1–8.
wild populations of Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, as a result of Snyder, N.F.R., Derrickson, S.R., Beissinger, S.R., Wiley, J.W., Smith,
interactions with escaped farm salmon. Proceedings of the T.B., Toone, W.D., Miller, B., 1996. Limitations of captive
Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270, 2443– breeding in endangered species recovery. Conservation
2450. Biology 10, 338–348.
McPhee, M.E., 2004. Generations in captivity increases behavioral Tenhumberg, B., Tyre, A.J., Shea, K., Possingham, H.P., 2004.
variance: considerations for captive breeding and Linking wild and captive populations to maximize species
reintroduction programs. Biological Conservation 115, 71–77. persistence: Optimal translocation strategies. Conservation
Metcalfe, N.B., Valdimarsson, S.K., Morgan, I.J., 2003. The relative Biology 18, 1304–1314.
roles of domestication, rearing environment, prior residence Tessier, N., Bernatchez, L., Wright, J.M., 1997. Population structure
and body size in deciding territorial contests between and impact of supportive breeding inferred from
hatchery and wild juvenile salmon. Journal of Applied Ecology mitochondrial and microsatellite DNA analyses in land-locked
40, 535–544. Atlantic salmon Salmo salar L. Molecular Ecology 6, 735–750.
Páez, D.J., Hedger, R., Bernatchez, L., Dodson, J.J., 2008. The Theodorou, K., Couvet, D., 2004. Introduction of captive breeders
morphological plastic response to water current velocity to the wild: harmful or beneficial. Conservation Genetics 5, 1–
varies with age and sexual state in juvenile Atlantic salmon, 12.
Salmo salar. Freshwater Biology, in press, doi:10.1111/j.1365- Wang, J.L., Ryman, N., 2001. Genetic effects of multiple
2427.2008.01989.x. generations of supportive breeding. Conservation Biology 15,
Poteaux, C., Bonhomme, F., Berrebi, P., 1999. Microsatellite 1619–1631.
polymorphism and genetic impact of restocking in Weber, E.D., Fausch, D.K., 2003. Interactions between hatchery
Mediterranean brown trout (Salmo trutta L). Heredity 82, 645– and wild salmonids in streams: differences in biology and
653. evidence for competition. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and
Price, E.O., 1999. Behavioral development in animals undergoing Aquatic Sciences 60, 1018–1036.
domestication. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 65, 245–271. Wedekind, C., 2002. Sexual selection and life-history decisions:
R Development Core Team, 2005. R: A language and environment Implications for supportive breeding and the management of
for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical captive populations. Conservation Biology 16, 1204–1211.
Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http:// Wedekind, C., Rudolfsen, G., Jacob, A., Urbach, D., Muller, R., 2007.
www.R-project.org. The genetic consequences of hatchery-induced sperm
Ramirez, O., Altet, L., Ensenat, C., Vila, C., Sanchez, A., Ruiz, A., competition in a salmonid. Biological Conservation 137, 180–
2006. Genetic assessment of the Iberian wolf Canis lupus 188.