Scrupulosity in OCD
Scrupulosity in OCD
net/publication/7253056
CITATIONS READS
72 836
4 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Stephen P. H. Whiteside on 28 June 2018.
Abstract
* Correspondence to: Mayo Clinic, 200 First Street SW, Rochester, MN 55905, USA.
Tel.: +1 507 284 4431; fax: +1 507 284 4158.
E-mail address: [email protected] (J.S. Abramowitz).
0887-6185/$ – see front matter # 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.02.001
1072 E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086
1
Scrupulosity, with a focus on morality, is also mentioned in DSM-IV-TR as a symptom of
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD). However, whereas the thoughts and doubts
pertaining to morality are experienced as unwanted and unwelcome (i.e., ‘‘ego-dystonic’’) in
OCD, they are experienced as consistent with the person’s world view (i.e., ‘‘ego-syntonic’’) in
OCPD. More specifically, scrupulous thoughts, ideas, and images (i.e., obsessions) in OCD are
associated with (a) anxiety and fear, (b) subjective resistance (i.e., they elicit rituals and neutralizing
responses), and (c) other types of obsessions (e.g., sexual, violent; McKay et al., 2004). In contrast, the
scrupulous ideation in OCPD (a) does not evoke anxiety or fear, (b) is not subjectively resisted, and (c)
is not associated with violent and sexual obsessions.
E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086 1073
Tek and Ulug (2001) compared groups of OCD patients with and without
religious obsessions, finding no between group differences in global OCD
symptom severity. However, patients with religious symptoms endorsed a greater
number of obsession categories on the symptom checklist of the Yale-Brown
Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS; Goodman et al., 1989a, 1989b) relative to
patients without religious OCD symptoms. Consistent with this result, a growing
number of factor and cluster analytic studies on the structure of obsessive–
compulsive symptoms as assessed by the Y-BOCS symptom checklist have
reported that religious obsessions load on a factor with sexual obsessions, and
often with aggressive and somatic obsessions (e.g., Mataix-Cols, Rosario-
Campos, & Leckman, 2005; McKay et al., 2004). Less often, religious obsessions
were found to load with checking and mental compulsions. Taken together, these
findings suggest that religious OCD symptoms do not necessarily represent a
more severe presentation of OCD per se, but do appear to be most strongly
associated with other obsessional phenomena, as opposed to with compulsive
rituals (with the possible exception of checking and mental neutralizing).
Contemporary cognitive-behavioral models of OCD implicate specific
cognitive phenomena in the development and maintenance of the disorder.
These cognitions include: (a) overestimation of threat (the belief that negative
events are especially likely and would be especially awful); (b) inflated
responsibility (the belief that one has the power to cause, and/or the duty to
prevent, negative outcomes); (c) overimportance of intrusive thoughts (the belief
that the mere presence of a thought indicates that the thought is significant); (d)
the need to control intrusive thoughts (the belief that complete control over one’s
mental processes is both necessary and possible); (e) perfectionism (the belief that
mistakes and imperfection are intolerable); and (f) intolerance of uncertainty (the
idea that it is important to be 100% certain that negative outcomes will not occur
(Frost & Steketee, 2002). The essential tenet of cognitive-behavioral models (e.g.,
Rachman, 1998; Salkovskis, 1999) is that OCD develops when unpleasant, yet
harmless, intrusive thoughts, doubts, impulses, and images are misinterpreted
along the lines of the cognitive factors described above. This misappraisal evokes
anxiety and motivates efforts to reduce this anxiety via neutralizing behavior (e.g.,
rituals) which is reinforced by the immediate (albeit temporary) reduction in
distress it engenders (for further description, see Rachman, 1997).
Studies with nonclinical samples have found that certain OCD-relevant
cognitive styles are related to religiosity (i.e., strength of religious devotion). For
example, greater religiosity was associated with inflated responsibility,
perfectionism, and dysfunctional beliefs about the importance of, and need to
control, intrusive thoughts among Catholics in Italy (Sica et al., 2002) and among
Protestants in the U.S. (Abramowitz et al., 2004). Rassin and Koster (2003) found
that religiosity was positively correlated with the belief that thoughts (even
involuntary ones) are the moral equivalent of actions (i.e., moral thought–action
fusion [TAF]), but not the belief that thinking of a negative event increases the
probability that the event will occur (i.e., likelihood TAF). Collectively, these
1074 E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086
1. Method
1.1. Participants
1.2. Measures
The following measures of OCD and related symptom severity, and related
cognitive distortions, were completed for the study:
E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086 1075
attending religious worship services (Abramowitz et al., 2002). The PIOS has
adequate psychometric properties in nonclinical samples (Abramowitz et al.,
2002) but has not been studied in clinical groups.
Thought–Action Fusion Scale (TAFS; Shafran, Thordarson, & Rachman,
1996). This is a 19-item self-report measure of the tendency to believe that
thoughts are equivalent to actions. Twelve items assess moral TAF, which is the
belief that thoughts are the moral equivalent of actions (e.g., ‘‘Having a
blasphemous thought is almost as sinful to me as a blasphemous action’’); three
assess likelihood-self TAF, which is the belief that merely thinking about harm
coming to oneself increases the likelihood of being harmed (e.g., ‘‘If I think of
myself being in a car accident this increases the risk that I will have a car
accident’’); and the remaining four items assess likelihood-other TAF, which is
the belief that thinking about harm coming to someone else increases the
likelihood of that person being harmed (e.g., ‘‘If I think of a relative/friend losing
their job, this increases the risk that they will lose their job). Agreement with each
item is rated on a scale from 0 (disagree strongly) to 4 (agree strongly). The
instrument’s psychometric properties are good and have been described by
Shafran et al. (1996). We combined the likelihood self- and likelihood-other
scales in the following study.
Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory (III; Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions
Working Group [OCCWG], 2003). The III is a 31-item semi-idiographic
questionnaire that assesses appraisals or interpretations of unwanted, distressing
intrusive thoughts, images or impulses. After reading a definition of unwanted
mental intrusions (which includes several examples), respondents identify two
intrusive thoughts, images, or impulses they have recently experienced. They then
rate the extent to which they believe in each of the 31 statements as related to the
identified intrusive thoughts (e.g., ‘‘Thinking this thought could make it
happen’’). Strength of belief is rated from 0 (‘‘I did not believe this idea at all’’) to
100 (‘‘I was completely convinced this idea was true’’). The 31 items form three
subscales: (a) importance of thoughts, (b) responsibility, and (c) control of
thoughts. As suggested by the scale’s developers (OCCWG, 2003), to ease
interpretation we transformed the 100-point scale by dividing by 10.
Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS; Freeston, Rhéaume, Letarte, Dugas, &
Ladouceur, 1994). The IUS is a 27-item self-report measure of the need for
assurance (sample items: ‘‘Uncertainty makes life intolerable;’’ ‘‘I always want to
know what the future has in store for me’’). The scale has adequate psychometric
properties and validity (Freeston et al., 1994). Items are rated on a scale from 1
(not at all characteristic of me) to 5 (entirely characteristic of me), and summed to
produce a total score ranging from 27 to 135.
1.3. Procedure
their completed packets to their assessor at their first visit. Diagnostic evaluations
took place in an anxiety disorders specialty clinic housed within a large academic
medical center. Each patient received our standard 1.5- to 2-h diagnostic
assessment performed by a trained psychologist who administered the anxiety and
mood disorders sections of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview
(MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998), a structured diagnostic interview that has
demonstrated good reliability (Sheehan et al., 1998). Axis II psychopathology
was assessed by means of a semi-structured interview that was based on DSM
criteria. On the basis of the primary symptoms identified by the Y-BOCS
checklist, clinicians classified each of these patients according to the five cluster
model of OCD symptom presentation identified by Abramowitz, Franklin,
Schwartz, and Furr (2003). Thus, OCD patients were classified as belonging to
one of the following five groups: harming (n = 20), contamination (n = 15),
hoarding (n = 2), unacceptable obsessional thoughts with religious, violent, and
sexual content (n = 22), or symmetry (n = 9). Clinicians had been instructed in
how to classify patients in this way by observing while others conducted similar
interviews, and by conducting such interviews themselves under direct
supervision. All classifications (and supporting data) were subsequently reviewed
with the clinic supervisor (the second author), and only patients for whom
consensus was reached (100% agreement) were included in the study. This
resulted in the exclusion of three patients for whom consensus was not reached.
2. Results
As initial analyses indicated that the two subscales of the PIOS were highly
correlated (r = .85, P < .01), and that both the Fear of God and Fear of Sin
subscales correlated significantly with the total score (r’s = .94 and .98,
p’s < .01), all analyses reported below were conducted using the PIOS total
score. The ranges, means, and standard deviations for all study measures are
displayed in Table 1. Examination of the shape of the distributions indicated that
the range of scores was reasonably broad and, in the majority of variables,
normally distributed. The distributions of the remaining variables were mildly
positively skewed. A series of one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) indicated
that scores on the study measures were not related to gender (all p’s NS).
Similarly, correlational analyses indicated that scores were not related to age (all
p’s NS). Measures of clinical severity demonstrated acceptable internal
consistency (Cronbach’s alpha): alpha coefficients ranged from .71 (Y-BOCS)
to .91 (BDI). Other self-report measures also evidenced good internal consistency,
with alpha coefficients ranging from of .91 (IUS) to .96 (PIOS).
We examined whether PIOS scores differed across patients with different
OCD symptom presentations. Because there were only two patients with primary
1078 E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086
Table 1
Range of scores, means, and standard deviations on measures of clinical severity, cognitions, and
scrupulosity for 71 patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder
Domain and measure Range M S.D.
Clinical severity
Y-BOCS total 14.00–36.00 24.17 4.87
Y-BOCS obsession 6.00–18.00 11.76 2.71
Y-BOCS compulsion 7.00–18.00 12.41 2.87
OCI-R total 7.00–66.00 28.14 12.96
OCI-R washing 0.00–12.00 4.91 4.41
OCI-R checking 0.00–12.00 5.30 3.79
OCI-R neutralizing 0.00–12.00 3.00 3.57
OCI-R obsessing 0.00–12.00 6.80 3.91
OCI-R ordering 0.00–12.00 5.25 4.14
OCI-R hoarding 0.00–12.00 2.87 3.53
BDI 2.00–49.00 17.39 10.89
STAI-T 36.00–78.00 55.96 10.33
Cognition
IUS 31.00–137.00 75.86 23.38
TAFS moral 0.00–44.00 19.70 11.61
TAFS likelihood 0.00–22.00 5.93 5.97
III importance of thoughts 0.00–90.00 36.59 23.22
III control of thoughts 3.00–100.00 58.65 24.44
III responsibility 0.00–99.00 54.67 26.43
Scrupulosity
PIOS 0.00–70.00 26.96 18.88
Note. Y-BOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale; OCI-R = Obsessive–Compulsive Inven-
tory—Revised; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; STAI-T = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Trait
Version; IUS = Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; TAFS = Thought Action Fusion Scale; III = Inter-
pretation of Intrusions Inventory; PIOS = Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity.
hoarding symptoms, we excluded this group from this analysis. Table 2 displays
the means and standard deviations on the PIOS across groups. A oneway
analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated significant between-group differences,
F(3, 62) = 3.95, P < .05. Post hoc Tukey HSD tests revealed that patients whose
main OCD symptoms were unacceptable obsessional thoughts had higher scores
than patients with primarily contamination symptoms (P < .05).
Table 2
Means and standard deviations on the PIOS for different OCD symptom presentations
Symptom presentation n M S.D.
Contamination 15 15.13 11.41
Harming 20 27.05 21.40
Symmetry 9 23.89 20.62
Unacceptable thoughts 22 35.55 16.88
E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086 1079
Given that previous research with nonclinical samples found that scores on
the PIOS were associated with religious affiliation and strength of religious
devotion (Abramowitz et al., 2002), we examined these relationships in the
present clinical population. This sample contained 19 Catholics, 32 Protestants, 3
Jews, 3 individuals of other religious affiliations, and 14 individuals who
identified themselves as not religiously affiliated. To examine the association
between scrupulosity and religious affiliation, we compared PIOS scores across
religious groups, excluding the Jewish and ‘‘other religion’’ groups on the basis
of insufficient power. A one-way ANOVA revealed significant between group
differences, F(2, 62) 4.32, P < .05. Post hoc comparisons using Tukey HSD tests
revealed that Protestant patients (M = 33.47, S.D. = 18.12) scored significantly
higher (P < .05) on the PIOS than did patients with no religious affiliation
(M = 19.07, S.D. = 17.46), but not significantly higher (P > .05) than did
Catholic patients (M = 20.74, S.D. = 19.79). Correlational analyses, excluding
the 14 non-religious patients, indicated that PIOS scores were not significantly
related to the patient’s strength of religious devotion, r(57) = .24, NS. When we
computed similar correlations for Catholic and Protestant patients separately,
we found no significant relationships between PIOS score and strength of
religious devotion in either of these groups.
2.4. Prediction of the PIOS total score through measures of clinical severity
and cognition
Multiple regression analysis was conducted to predict the total PIOS score. As
we thought that a tendency toward obsessional thinking would predict
scrupulosity, the OCI-R obsessing subscale was included in Step 1. Given the
relationship between OCD and general distress, Step 2 incorporated the BDI and
1080 E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086
Table 3
Pearson correlation coefficients between the PIOS and measures of clinical severity and cognition for
71 OCD patients
Domain and measure r with PIOS total score
Clinical severity
Y-BOCS total .01
Y-BOCS obsession .08
Y-BOCS compulsion .06
OCI-R total .01
OCI-R washing .04
OCI-R checking .04
OCI-R neutralizing .12
OCI-R obsessing .40*
OCI-R ordering .22
OCI-R hoarding .06
BDI .28
STAI-T .32
Cognition
IUS .30
TAFS moral .44*
TAFS likelihood .04
III importance of thoughts .44*
III control of thoughts .60*
III responsibility .44*
Note. PIOS = Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity; Y-BOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale;
OCO-R = Obsessive–Compulsive Inventory—Revised; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; STAI-
T = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Trait Version; IUS = Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale;
TAFS = Thought Action Fusion Scale; III = Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory.
*
P < .003.
3. Discussion
The present study is the first to address the phenomenon of scrupulosity and
its affective and cognitive correlates in a clinical sample of OCD patients. We
E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086 1081
Table 4
Hierarchical regression analysis for variables predicting the PIOS total score
Predictor B SEB b R2 DR2 P
Step 1 .16 .001
OCI-R – obsessions 1.94 0.53 .40 .001
Step 2 .20 .04 .002
BDI 0.16 0.33 .09 NS
STAI-T 0.24 0.35 .13 NS
Step 3 .45 .025 .000
TAFS-moral 0.47 0.17 .29 .009
III-importance 0.02 0.02 .29 NS
III-responsibility 0.01 0.01 .11 NS
III-control 0.04 0.01 .55 .001
Note. OCI-R = Obsessive–Compulsive Inventory—Revised; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory;
STAI-T = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Trait Version; TAFS-M = Thought Action Fusion Scale,
moral subscale; III = Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory.
bad thoughts) co-occurred with sexual, harming, and religious obsessions. Our
failure to find a relationship between scrupulosity and neutralizing deserves
further comment. One possible explanation for this null finding is that all of the
items on the neutralizing subscale of the OCI-R involve numbers and counting
(e.g., I feel I have to repeat certain numbers). Whereas some patients with OCD
use numbers and counting to neutralize obsessional fear, clinical observations
indicate that patients with religious obsessions typically use other neutralizing
strategies (e.g., thought suppression, mental phrases or prayers) to neutralize
these kinds of obsessions. Similarly, the lack of a relationship between
scrupulosity and checking symptoms requires further interpretating since some
factor analytic studies report that religious obsessions load with checking rituals
(e.g., Leckman, Grice, Boardman, & Zhang, 1997). However, items on the OCI-R
checking subscale mainly assesses the checking of household items such as
appliances, doors locks, and drawers, as opposed to checking with religious
authorities or checking for harm or mistakes, which are more likely to be present
among individuals with scrupulosity. Thus, although scrupulosity may be related
to some types of neutralizing and checking symptoms, the items on the OCI-R
may not be sensitive to the specific sorts of neutralizing and checking rituals
displayed by individuals with scrupulosity.
Consistent with previous findings (e.g., Tek & Ulug, 2001), we found that
scrupulosity was unrelated to global OCD symptom severity (e.g., time spent
with symptoms, functional interference) as assessed by the interviewer
administered Y-BOCS. This supports the view that compared to other types
of obsessions and compulsions, religious symptoms do not represent a generally
more severe variant of OCD. Although the correlation coefficients between
scrupulosity and depressive (.28) and anxiety symptoms (.32) were significant at
the P < .05 level, they were somewhat weak and not significant at the more
conservative Bonferroni-corrected significance level of P < .003. Thus, even if
religious obsessions are not associated with especially high frequency,
interference in functioning, difficulty with resistance or control (i.e., the
symptom parameters assessed by the Y-BOCS), these phenomena might
represent a particularly distressing presentation of OCD. Although additional
research is needed on this topic, the fact that scrupulosity involves the perception
of sin, violation of one’s moral standards, and fear of punishment (e.g., from
God), it is not surprising that religious obsessions are experienced as highly
depressing and anxiety-evoking.
In support of our second hypothesis, scrupulosity was moderately associated
with multiple cognitive biases believed to underlie the development of
obsessional symptoms, including moral TAF, overestimates of the importance
of and need to control intrusive thoughts, and inflated perceptions of
responsibility. These findings are consistent with previous research suggesting
that among religious individuals, an overly stringent moral code coupled with the
tendency to catastrophically misinterpret the significance of intrusive unwanted
thoughts set the stage for the development of obsessional problems (e.g.,
E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086 1083
Abramowitz et al., 2004; Sica et al., 2002). Our regression analysis indicated that
scrupulosity was best accounted for by the triumvirate of (a) obsessional
symptoms, (b) beliefs that unwanted thoughts are the moral equivalent of
unacceptable behavior (moral TAF), and (c) maladaptive beliefs about the
necessity of controlling unwanted intrusive thoughts. Abundant research indicates
that it is quite difficult to control or suppress intrusive thoughts (e.g., Abramowitz,
Tolin, & Street, 2001). Accordingly, the clinical manifestation of scrupulosity
may arise from the fear of negative religious consequences (e.g., punishment from
God, eternal damnation) resulting from inability to control intrusive thoughts
(e.g., sexual, sacrilegious) that are perceived as sinful and morally unacceptable
(i.e., equivalent to sinful behavior). In an effort to reduce obsessional distress,
individuals engage in compulsive (neutralizing) behaviors such as excessive
prayer, confession, and checking for reassurance from religious authorities,
among other strategies.
Whereas scrupulosity was not associated with likelihood TAF, it was related
with moral TAF. This is consistent with results reported by Rassin and Koster
(2003) and in line with the idea that certain aspects of religious doctrine foster the
belief that thoughts should be treated as the moral equivalent of actions. For
example, that thinking about harming someone is the same as committing harm.
Indeed, religious doctrine explicitly states that certain thoughts are sinful. For
example, in the Sermon on the Mount, Jesus cautions, ‘‘You have heard that it was
said ‘you shall not commit adultery’; but I say to you, that everyone who looks on
a woman to lust for her has committed adultery with her already in his heart’’
(Matthew 5:27–28; New American Standard Version). Research indicates that
many strongly religious Christians incorporate this doctrine into their belief
system (e.g., Cohen & Rozin, 2001). Future studies should examine the ways in
which individuals with religious OCD symptoms (regardless of their level of
religiosity) acquire such beliefs. The relationship between scrupulosity and moral
TAF also raises the question of how much the PIOS and TAF-moral subscale
overlap in what they purport to measure; indeed, both assess to some degree the
tendency to regard thoughts and behaviors as equivalent.
An important aim of psychopathology research is to inform clinical practice,
and the present findings have implications for the treatment of religious OCD
symptoms. Research indicates that exposure and response prevention (ERP) is the
most effective treatment for OCD (Kozak & Coles, 2005a), although many OCD
patients with scrupulosity have difficulty accepting and adhering to ERP because
it involves directly confronting situations and thoughts that are perceived to be
sinful. Some authors (e.g., Kozak & Coles, 2005b) have suggested using cognitive
therapy (CT) techniques to increase adherence to ERP, although to date, this
suggestion has not been studied empirically. Nevertheless, we speculate that some
CT techniques have relevance for facilitating ERP in cases of scrupulosity. For
instance, patients could be taught that everyone sometimes experiences unwanted
(morally repugnant) thoughts. The therapist could also arrange a meeting between
the patient and a clergy member to disconfirm the idea that the occurrence of
1084 E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086
Acknowledgment
This study was partially supported by grants from the Obsessive Compulsive
Foundation.
References
Abramowitz, J. (2001). Treatment of scrupulous obsessions and compulsions using exposure and
response prevention: a case report. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8, 79–85.
Abramowitz, J. S., Deacon, B. J., Woods, C. M., & Tolin, D. F. (2004). Association between protestant
religiosity and obsessive-compulsive symptoms and cognitions. Depression and Anxiety, 20, 70–76.
Abramowitz, J. S., Franklin, M., Schwartz, S. A., & Furr, J. M. (2003). Symptom presentation and
outcome of cognitive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 71(6), 1049–1057.
Abramowitz, J. S., Huppert, J. D., Cohen, A. B., Tolin, D. F., & Cahill, S. P. (2002). Religious
obsessions and compulsions in a non-clinical sample: the Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity (PIOS).
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 825–838.
Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: a
meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(5), 683–703.
Akhtar, S., Wig, N. N., Varma, V. K., Pershad, D., & Verma, S. K. (1975). A phenomenological analysis
of symptoms in obsessive-compulsive neurosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 127, 342–348.
E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086 1085
Antony, Downie, & Swinson, (1998). Diagnostic issues and epidemiology in obsessive-compulsive
disorder. In: R. P. Swinson, & A. M. Martin (Eds.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Theory,
research, and treatment (pp. 3–32). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression
Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77–100.
Beck, A., Ward, C., Medelsohn, M., Mock, J., & Erlbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring
depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Clark, D. A. (2004). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for OCD. New York: Guilford.
Cohen, A. B., & Rozin, P. (2001). Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 81, 697–710.
Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langer, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., et al. (2002). The Obsessive-
Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment,
14(4), 485–495.
Foa, E. B., Kozak, M. J., Salkovskis, P. M., Coles, M. E., & Amir, N. (1998). The validation of a new
obsessive-compulsive disorder scale: the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory. Psychological Assess-
ment, 10, 206–214.
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1995). DSM-IV field trial: obsessive-compulsive disorder. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 90–96.
Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., Thibodeau, N., Rheume, J., Letarte, H., et al. (1997).
Cognitive-behavioral treatment of obsessive thoughts: a controlled study. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 405–413.
Freeston, M. H., Rhéaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladouceur, R. (1994). Why do people
worry? Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 791–802.
Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (Eds.). (2002). Cognitive approaches to obsessions and compulsions:
Theory, assessment, and treatment. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Pergamon/Elsevier Science Inc.
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al.
(1989a). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. Development, use, and reliability.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1011.
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Delgado, P., Heninger, G. R., et al.
(1989b). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. II. Validity. Archives of General Psy-
chiatry, 46, 1012–1016.
Greenberg, D. (1987). The behavioral treatment of religious compulsions. Psychology and Judaism,
11, 41–47.
Khanna, S., & Channabasavanna, S. M. (1988). Phenomenology of obsessions in obsessive-compul-
sive neurosis. Psychopathology, 21(1), 12–18.
Kozak, M., & Coles, M. (2005a). Treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder: unleashing the power
of exposure. In: J. S. Abramowitz, & A. C. Houts (Eds.), Concepts and controversies in obsessive-
compulsive disorder. New York: Springer.
Kozak, M., & Coles, M. (2005b). Reply to Fama and Wilhelm. Cognitive therapy and exposure
treatment for OCD: contrast and rapprochement. In: J. S. Abramowitz, & A. C. Houts (Eds.),
Concepts and controversies in obsessive-compulsive disorder. New York: Springer.
Leckman, J. F., Grice, D. E., Boardman, J., & Zhang, H. (1997). Symptoms of obsessive-compulsive
disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(7), 911–917.
Mataix-Cols, D., Rosario-Campos, M. C., & Leckman, J. F. (2005). A multidimensional model of
obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 228–238.
McKay, D., Abramowtiz, J. S., Calamari, J. E., Kyrios, M., Radomsky, A., Sookman, D., et al. (2004).
A critical evaluation of obsessive-compulsive disorder subtypes: symptoms versus mechanisms.
Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 283–313.
Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group (OCCWG). (2003). Psychometric validation of the
Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire and the Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory: Part 1. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 41, 863–878.
Rachman. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793–802.
1086 E.A. Nelson et al. / Anxiety Disorders 20 (2006) 1071–1086
Rachman. (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions: elaborations. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
36, 385–401.
Rassin, E., & Koster, E. (2003). The correlation between thought-action fusion and religiosity in a
normal sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 361–368.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1999). Understanding and treating obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 37, S29–S52.
Shafran, R., Thordarson, D. S., & Rachman, S. (1996). Thought-action fusion in obsessive-compulsive
disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10, 379–391.
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini-
International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I): the development and validation of a structured
diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 9(Suppl.
20), 22–23 quiz 34–57.
Sica, C., Novara, C., & Sanavio, E. (2002). Religiousness and obsessive-compulsive cognitions and
symptoms in an Italian population. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 813–823.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. E., Vaag, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the
state-trait anxiety inventory (Form Y) (Self-Evaluation Questionnaire). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Steketee, G. S., Frost, R. O., Wincze, J., Greene, K. A. I., & Douglass, H. (2000). Group and individual
treatment of compulsive hoarding: a pilot study. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 28,
259–268.
Tek, C., & Ulug, B. (2001). Religiosity and religious obsessions in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Psychiatry Research, 104, 99–108.
Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Kozak, M. J., & Foa, E. B. (2001). Fixity of belief, perceptual
aberration, and magical ideation in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 15, 501–510.