Review of Related Literature
Review of Related Literature
2.0 Introduction
This chapter provides theories and reviews the literature related to error analysis.
and the differences between errors and mistakes are describes. To understand the errors
produced by the learners, interlanguage and sources of errors are explained with their 13
subtypes. In addition, vocabulary acquisition is also given. Besides, the error correction in
second language teaching and learning is discussed with its advantages and disadvantages.
At the end of this chapter, studies on lexical errors are also given.
Ellis (1995) sees the process of second language acquisition (SLA) as a complex
one, involving many interrelated factors. It indicates the learning of an additional language
after acquiring the first language. SLA is the product of many factors relating to the learner
on the one hand and the learning situation on the other. It is not a uniform and predictable
phenomenon. In addition, there is not only one way that learners acquire knowledge of a
Besides, second language acquisition refers to all aspects of language that the language
learners need to acquire and master. With regard to the acquisition of lexis, there have been
relatively few studies in this area. The focus has been on how L2 learners acquire
grammatical sub-systems.
15
SLA is not contrasted with foreign language acquisition. SLA is brought into use
as a general term that involved both untutored (or ‘naturalistic’) acquisition and tutored (or
with second language learning on the assumption that these are different processes.
Krashen (1982: 10) proposes “the acquisition-learning distinction” hypothesis for second
language acquisition. He states that “adults have two distinct and independent ways of
process similar, if not identical, to the way children develop ability in their first language”.
It is said to be a subconscious process. Learners do not consciously know that they are
acquiring language. They pick up a language in everyday interactions and are not
consciously aware of the rules of grammar. According to this way of acquiring language
competence, the atmosphere is usually “relaxed and natural” (Ong, 2007: 15). Learners
“have a feel for correctness. Grammatical sentences sound right, or feel right, and errors
feel wrong, even they do not consciously know what rules were violated” (Krashen, 1982:
10). The second way of developing competence in a second language is language learning.
In this process, learners know the rules, are consciously aware of and are able to talk about
them. The learning process usually takes place in the classroom in a formal situation.
Error analysis (EA) is a method used in the study of errors in the 1970s, there was
a need to look for sources of errors other than the mother tongue. Teachers and researchers
provided evidence that “a great number of student errors cannot possibly be traced to their
native language” (Dulay, Burt and Krashen, 1982: 140). “There were many kinds of errors
beside those due to interlingual interference that could neither be predicted nor explained
explain errors. It probably had a long tradition. Sridhar (1980: 221) describes traditional
error analysis in the following manner: “until recently, a typical error analysis went little
into categories (mistakes of agreement, omission of articles, etc)”. The particular goal of
traditional error analysis was an attempt to deal with the practical needs of the classroom
teacher.
EA was inspired by the generative linguistics movement of the 60s which focused
attempt to account for learner errors that could not be explained or predicted by CA or the
behaviorist theory. EA looks upon errors as evidence of the processes and strategies of
language acquisition. It tries to account for learner performance in terms of the cognitive
processes learners make use of in reorganizing the input they receive from the target
language (TL). The main focus of error analysis is on the evidence that learners’ errors
From the linguistic point of view, EA attempts to account for the developmental
errors the learner makes in learning a second language. Richards (1971) acknowledges that
within the TL itself. These errors reflect the learner’s competence at a particular stage of
language learning. Errors are no longer considered as undesirable forms; rather, they are
seen as a necessary part of the language learning process (Ellis, 1995). In other words,
errors can be considered as beneficials for the learner because he/she can use them as
three arguments: (1) error analysis does not suffer from the inherent limitations of CA—
restriction to errors caused by transfer from the mother tongue; EA brings to light many
other types of errors frequently made by learners, (2) error analysis, unlike contrastive
analysis, provides data on actual, attested problems, and not hypothetical problems and
therefore forms a more efficient and economical basis for designing pedagogical strategies,
and (3) error analysis is not confronted with the complex theoretical problems encountered
166), four weaknesses of errors are revealed: (1) too much attention to learners’ errors, (2)
overstressing of production data, (3) fails to account for the strategy of avoidance, and (4)
keeps too closely focused on specific languages rather than viewing universal aspects of
language. In the same token, Dulay, Burt, and Krashen (1982: 141-144) provide three
different weaknesses that are different from Brown’s. They propose that there are three
major conceptual weaknesses that seem to have impeded the potential contributions it
might have made to the EA field. They are: (1) the confusion of error description with error
explanation (the process and product of error analysis), (2) the lack of precision and
specificity in the definition of error categories, and (3) simplistic categorization of the
causes of learners’ errors. Thus, in relation to these weaknesses, the errors which a learner
18
2.3 Errors and Mistakes
a distinction between mistakes and errors. According to (Brown, 1994: 205), these two
error that is either a random guess or a slip”, while an error refers to “idiosyncrasies in the
interlanguage of the learner that are direct manifestation of a system within which a learner
is operating at that time… Put in another way, an error is a noticeable deviation from the
adult grammar of a native speaker, reflecting the interlanguage competence of the learner”.
For example, “if a learner of English asks, “Does John can sing?” he is probably reflecting
a competence level in which all verbs require a pre-posed do auxiliary for question
formation. He has committed an error, most likely not a mistake, rather, it reveals a portion
Corder also points out that mistakes are deviations due to performance factors such
as memory limitations, spelling, pronunciation, fatigue and emotional strain. Errors, on the
other hand, are systematic consistent deviances characteristic of the learner’s linguistic
system at a given stage of learning. ‘The key point’ he states, “is that the learner is using a
the second language….the learner’s errors are evidence of this system and are themselves
Keshavarz (1993: 49) points out that errors may be seen as “rule-governed and
systematic in nature and as such indicative of the learner’s linguistic system at a given stage
of language learning and systematic errors reveal something about the learner’s underlying
knowledge of the TL to date, i.e. his transitional competence”. Norrish (1983: 7) says that
19
“errors are systematic deviation when a learner has not learned something and consistently
gets it wrong, whereas mistakes are inconsistent deviations”. It means that sometimes the
learner gets it right, but sometimes he or she makes a mistake and uses the wrong form.
Ngara (1983: 35) looks at errors and mistakes as competence errors and performance errors.
He explains that errors can be seen as “the limit of the learner’s competence in using the
TL.” In contrast, mistakes are performance errors which “can be easily eliminated by
emphasis on accuracy and carefulness”. This is because “the learner makes this error not
Dušková (1969) states that many recurrent errors of a systematic character which
might be defined as errors in competence, do not reflect a real defect in knowledge, because
the learners are aware of the relevant rule and are able to apply it. However, this application
can not work automatically, it probably need mechanism. In addition, Lado (1957)
maintains that the mistakes the learner made do not reflect the knowledge of their language.
The learner could predict the knowledge of their language and what they still had to learn
by studying their errors. James (1998: 83) differentiates errors and mistakes in the way that
the former “cannot be self-corrected until the further relevant (to that error) input (implicit
or explicit) has been provided…errors require further relevant learning to take place before
they can be self-corrected”. The latter, he explains, “can only be corrected by their agent if
20
2.4 Interlanguage
There are many similarities between the terms “idiosyncratic dialect” (Coder,
1972). All these terms derive from the belief that it is valuable to view the learner’s set of
hypotheses about interlanguage as a language in its own right. Although this language may
According to Selinker (1972), there are psychological structures latent in the brain
which is activated when one attempts to learn a second language. In other words, the
attempts of the learner to produce meaningful utterances in the TL has resulted the
set of utterances produced by the learner will probably contain some elements of structure
which parallel those of the learner’s native language and some which parallel those of the
target or second language. The learner is most likely not aware of where his meaningful
performance does not parallel that of the target language, though he is in the process of
“successsful second language learning, for most learners, is the reorganization of linguistic
element of the interlanguage becoming frozen for a given learner, never to be replaced by
the corresponding target language. “Fossilizable linguistic phenomena are linguistic items,
rules, and subsystems which speakers of a particular NL will tend to keep in their IL
relative to a particular TL, no matter what the age of the learner or amount of explanation
it as “the deviant linguistic system actually employed by the learner attempting to utilize
the target language” (Nemser, 1971: 55). He gives a threefold assumption that relates to the
interlanguage theory: (a) learner speech at a given time is the patterned product of a
linguistic system; the approximative system is distinct from both the MT and the TL, (b)
approximative system at successive stages of learning form the developing stages when a
learner first attempts to use the TL, and (c) in a given contact situation, the approximative
Corder (1971) uses the term “idiosyncratic dialects” for the interlanguage of the
learners of a second language. He explains that “it is a dialect whose rules share
A number of scholars have done valuable work on the sources of errors as a basis
for researchers to trace, discuss, and classify errors according to their sources. Brown
(1980: 173-181) gives four types of errors with respect to their sources: (a) interlingual
transfer; the transfer from the native language at the beginning stages of learning a second
language, (b) intralingual transfer; the negative transfer of items or the incorrect
generalization of rules within the TL, (c) context of learning; a misleading explanation
from the teacher, faulty presentation of a structure or word in a textbook or a pattern that
was rotely memorized in a drill but not properly contextualized, and (d) communication
authority and language switch. In addition, Selinker (1972: 48) distinguishes five sources of
22
errors: (a) language transfer, (b) transfer of training, (c) strategies of second language
material.
Dulay and Burt (1974: 115) categorize sources of errors which they prefer to call
“goofs” into four types: (a) interference-like goofs (errors which reflect native language
structure), (b) L1 developmental goofs (those that do not reflect native language structure),
(c) ambiguous goofs (those that can be classified as either ‘interference-like goofs’ or ‘L1
developmental goofs’), and (d) unique goofs (those do not reflect L1 structure, and also not
Larsen-Freeman and Long (1991: 58-59) divides errors into six types: (a)
redundancy reduction, (e) communication-based errors, and (f) induced errors. In the same
token, but slightly different, James (1998: 179-189), proposes three major categories of
errors: (a) interlingual errors or mother-tongue influence, (b) intralingual errors or target
language causes which is further divided into two sub-categories, that is, (i) learning
strategy-based errors (which occurs due to circumstances like false analogy, misanalysis,
strategy-based errors (when learners apply either holistic strategies or analytic strategies),
and (c) induced errors which result more from the classroom situation than from
23
Norrish (1983: 21-34) lists six sources of errors: (a) carelessness which is often
closely related to ‘lack of motivation’, (b) first language interference, (c) translation
learner’s first language), (d) overgeneralization (which covers the instance when learners
conduct a deviant structure on the basis of his experience of other structures in the TL), (e)
incomplete application of rules (the occurrence of structures when deviancy represents the
development degree of rules in the TL), and (f) materials-induced errors; (i) false concept
(building false concepts and faulty comprehension of distinction in the TL), and (ii)
ignorance of rule restrictions (applying rules to contexts to which they do not apply).
Ngara (1983: 36-40) states that errors can possibly happen according to many
sources of errors:
the second language learner produces forms that are either identical
(c) Incomplete learning. This refers to those errors which occur because
the second language speaker has only half-learned any features of the
occur not because the learner has half-learned a linguistic form, not
context.
Lexical errors found in the data have been classified into two general categories
according to the sources of errors, that is, interlingual and intralingual errors. Interlingual
The researcher adopted the models related to lexical errors propounded by several
linguists. Together, they form the conceptual framework which forms the basis for the
researcher’s data analysis. For the category of interlingual errors, the researcher adopted
two of Woon’s (2003), categories, namely (1) direct translation, and (2) use of native words.
The last error type for this category is misodering. This type of error was taken from Dulay,
Burt and Krashen (1982). For the intralingual errors, three subcategories were adopted from
James’ (1998) classification: (1) confusion of sense relations, (2) collocational errors, and
(3) distortions. James examined distortions especially at the morphological level. However,
regarding the analysis of this type of error, the researcher has also included distortion errors
at the morphosyntactic level. Another two error types from Dulay, Burt and Krashen (1982)
were adopted for this categories as well: (1) omission, and (2) additions. Three more error
types were taken from Woon (2003): (1) confusion of derivatives, (2) redundancy, and (3)
25
paraphrase. Confusibles is an error type taken from Laufer’s (1992) taxonomy, while
confusion of binary terms is from Zughoul’s (1991). The reason that the researcher
adopted types of errors from several linguists is because in the process of evaluation
students’ lexical errors, the researcher is able to cover all lexical errors found in the
composition.
of the learner’s mother tongue” (Richard, 1971: 173). Errors occur when the automatic
structures are different. Interlingual transfer or interference from the native language
significantly occurs at the beginning stages of learning the L2. “Before the system of the
second language is familiar, the native language is the only linguistic system in previous
experience upon which the learners can draw… These errors are attributable to negative
interlingual transfer” (Brown, 1994: 213-124). There are two subcategories of interlingual
This type of errors occurs when the learners translate directly words
from the L1 to the L2, but does not convey the meaning intended in the target language.
Sentences produced with this kind of error sound odd and funny to proficient speakers of
English or native speakers, but they are understood among the Thais even though they are
not acceptable.
drawing near.)
- *I saw many people had happy and enjoyment with their family.
(I saw many people were happy and enjoyable with their family.)
2.6.1.2 Misordering
morpheme or group of morphemes in an utterance” (Dulay, Burt and Krashen, 1982: 162).
They observe that misordering frequently occur when the learners produce written or
This type of error can be grouped into two categories: (i) the use of
L2 loan words in the L1 and (ii) the use of L2 words. In his analysis of lexical errors made
27
by Polish learners of English, Arabski (1979; cited in Zughoul, 1991: 56) uses the term
“lexical shift” which is the use of words or lexical items of the L1 in the L2. Arabski
further states that the most obvious influence of the L1 is seen in the use of L1 lexical items.
However, this kind of error found in one interlanguage may not be as serious
in another due to the differences in the lexicon. They are attributed to “perceived language
distance” (Kellerman, 1977 & Ringbom, 1982, cited in Zughoul, 1991: 56). For instance,
Malay is perceived as “closer” to English compared to Thai where the use of technical
vocabulary is concerned. Certain lexical items in Malay are loan words from English and
they look and sound similar, for examples, teknologi, operasi, klinik, etc. These words in
Thai are absolutely different. There is a great distance between the MT (Thai) and the TL
(English).
Intralingual errors within the target language itself are another major
category of error found in second language learning. Intralingual errors occur because of
the difficulty of the TL or due to partial exposure to the TL. According to Richards (1971:
174-178), intralingual errors produced by the learners involve four systematic sources of
errors: (1) overgeneralization (when the learner creates a deviant structure on the basis of
other structures in the TL), (2) ignorance of rules restrictions (the application of rules to
contexts where they do not apply), (3) incomplete application of rules (a failure to fully
develop a structure), and (4) false concepts hypothesized (a failure to comprehend fully a
distinction in the TL). Many researchers, especially, Taylor (1975, cited in Brown, 1994:
214) have found that after the learners have familiarized with the system of the second
language, “more and more intralingual” errors occur. At this stage, “negative intralingual
28
transfer or overgeneralization” takes place. The researcher has identified eleven
subcategories in the analysis of errors found in the study. There are: confusion of sense
This kind of error is one of the semantic errors found in the use of L2
vocabulary. In this category, errors occur when the learners select inappropriate words to
convey the intended meaning in the TL. The learners have not understood the different
meanings of an English word and its usage in different contexts. Students need to learn that
in English, there are word sets such as synonyms, superonyms, hyponyms, and so on. The
semantic field theory claims that “the vocabulary of language is structured, just as the
grammar and phonology of a language are structured – the words of a language can be
classified into sets which are related to conceptual fields” (Lehrer, 1974: 15) Thus, the
learners cannot generalize these word sets by assuming that they can be used
interchangeably. The problem of this kind of error can be seen clearly, especially among
foreign language learners who are encouraged to learn synonyms and rely heavily on
monolingual or bilingual dictionaries. They tend to assume that synonyms of a word have
the same meaning and can be used in all contexts. As stated by Zughoul (1991: 47-48) that
English is “very rich in synonyms because of the French, Latin, and Greek influences on
the language and because of the vast number of borrowings from different languages”.
However, it has been generally argued in the semantic field theory that “there are no real
synonyms in language and that no two words or two sentences have exactly the same
meaning”. Palmer (1976, cited in Zughoul, 1991: 48) argues that “it seems unlikely that
two words with exactly the same meaning would both survive in a language. Words that are
29
considered synonyms, especially those “exploited” in dictionaries are in fact different in
meaning in some respect”. With regard to this, James (1998: 151) provides four major
types of errors:
(i) Using a more general term where a more specific one is needed
• *On the beach which full of the white beach as far as the eyes can see
• *It is easier to think you understand when you just have surface
knowledge. (superficial)
• *It (work) is not all of life, so let’s give yourself time. (ourselves)
Collocations are “the other words any particular word normally keeps company with”
(James, 1998: 152). Cowie (1992, cited in Yang & Xu, 2001: 54) defines collocations as “a
composite unit that permits the substitutability of items for at least one of its constituents.”
As Firt aptly puts it, “You shall know a word by the company it keeps” (cited in Zughoul,
1991: 51). Thus, collocations rely heavily on word-association knowledge. The wrong
30
understood by the native speakers of English because it makes sense, but is not normally
“It is right to say crooked stick but not crooked year because in
the example of the word ‘army’ with the collocations ‘big losses’
attempt” and “have a try” but we cannot “make a try” and “have
• *She rushed to the beach for touching the fresh air. (to breathe
their aim)
31
2.6.2.3 Distortions
of form created without recourse to L1 resources”. The outcomes are forms non-existent in
the TL. They are the result of the misapplication of one or more of the following processes:
room)
The following are examples of distortions from the data except blending
(i) omission
ambassdor. (ambassador)
(ii) overinclusion
(iii) misselection
(iv) misordering
32
2.6.2.4 Omissions
an item that must appear in a well-formed utterance” (Dulay, Burt and Krashen, 1982: 154).
The cause of this error is probably difficulties in finding the correct word to convey the
intended meaning. When the learners face such a problem, they tend to leave a blank
because they do not know the correct word to use. Zahira (2003: 95) argued that “when the
learners have limited vocabulary, they prefer to leave out the lexical item because they
cannot find suitable lexical items for their sentences”. As a result, the sentences make very
little or no sense at all. This state of ignorance is a sign of incompetence and the learners
are not yet able to make corrections themselves. On the other hand, if the cause of the
omissions is due to errors of slips of the tongue or lapses, the learners would be able to
correct themselves if they were given the opportunity. Slips of the tongue can be considered
1998: 87).
ending. (experience)
33
2.6.2.5 Additions
Dulay, Burt and Krashen (1982: 156) define addition errors as “the
opposite of omissions. They are characterized by the presence of an item which must not
appear in the well-formed utterance”. Addition errors result from the all-too-faithful use of
(i) double marking e.g. *I didn’t went; *He doesn’t knows my name.
(ii) regularization
clear.
adverbs in the TL itself are complex and interrelated. When learners produce written or
spoken utterances, they are confused by these different forms of the TL. This is due to the
difficulties in the TL. A rule that exists in the L2 is not found in the L1. Thus, such errors
occur when the learners fail to identify the correct word class, for example the learner uses
34
adjectives where nouns are required. Woon (2003: 65) divides confusion of derivatives into
4 groups:
subcategories:
• *When I was on the beach I feel freedom, happy and relax. (free,
relaxed)
35
• *Everybody couldn’t deny that English has no important to them.
(importance)
2.6.2.7 Redundancy
repeat the meanings of other words or phrases in the same sentence. “The needless
• *Secondly, for getting sun bed some people they are really crazy
thing.
2.6.2.8 Paraphrasing
paraphrasing. When the learners have learned an idea in the L1 but do not know or recall
the appropriate word to express such an idea in the L2, they may use more words or phrases
than necessary to convey their intended meaning. Woon (2003: 81) describes paraphrase as
36
a state of “simplification strategy which the learners employ to replace lexical item that
2.6.2.9 Confusibles
(1979) refers to them as ‘confusibles’; and Phythian (1989) uses the term ‘confusables’
(cited in James, 1998: 145). They are pairs of words that look and sound similar:
37
Laufer groups confusibles into four main types:
• *How is lucky for anybody who leave near wonderful place like
“relational opposites” of lexical items. In other words, binary terms refers to two lexical
items that are rationally opposite to each other such as come-go, here-there, give-take, etc,.
These words generally exhibit the relationship between items rather than “oppositeness in
meaning”. Erdmenger (1985, cited in Zughoul, 1991: 55) also includes antonymy relations
patient and directional relations as in come-go. The confusion of binary terms occurs when
the learners are confused by the “rational opposites” and use inappropriate terms.
38
• *People all around the world go to take diving courses here. (come)
• *It was very fantastic for us and very excited because we never came
one of them is vocabulary. Even though traditionally vocabulary has given less attention in
second language pedagogy than any other aspects, particularly grammar, vocabulary is the
most important component in L2 ability. Wilkins (1972: 111) states the importance of
vocabulary that “while without grammar very little can be conveyed, without vocabulary
nothing can be conveyed”. Thus, for the second language learners they need vocabulary
knowledge to function well in that language. Allen (1983) observes the relevance of
interfere with communication. When people do not use the right words, they fail to
communicate..
The quality of vocabulary use in the compositions and the lexical errors which
appear in the written work, to some extent, are fundamentally a result of vocabulary
acquisition. The learning of vocabulary is much more than knowing ‘a list of words’, There
are in fact many different kinds of vocabulary items or “words” for second language
learners to acquire. Besides a set of single words, they also need to know “set phrases,
variable phrases, phrasal verbs and idioms” (Folse, 2004: 2). In addition, in order to learn a
new word, learners need to “find out what it means and consider how to use it in sentences
before incorporating it into their natural vocabulary” (Hilton and Hyder, 2003: 10).
39
Laufer (1997: 141) points out several strategies on how to learn a new word. They
are: (a) knowing the form of the words including knowing how the word is pronounced and
spelt, (b) knowing the word structure and its root that go with it, affixes, and inflection such
as plural forms, (c) knowing the syntactic pattern of the word such as knowledge of the part
of speech, (d) knowing the meaning of the word, e.g. the referential, metaphorical, and
affective and pragmatic meanings, (e) knowing the lexical relation of the word e.g.
synonyms and antonyms, and (f) knowing common collocations and the rules on how the
word can be used together. Oxford and Scarcella (1994: 231-233) asserts that “knowing a
word involves not only an ability to recognize it when it is heard or seen, but also the
Gu (2003, cited in Ong, 2007: 34) indicates three important stages to learn a new
word: (a) identifying and handling a new word, (b) committing a word to memory, and (c)
attempting to use the learned word. In the same way, Nation (1999, cited in Ong, 2007: 34)
points out three important methods and routes for SLA students to learn vocabulary. They
acquisition. He claims that second language learners acquire vocabulary through the same
processes that children go through when learning their native language: “explicit learning
through the focused study of words and incidental learning through exposure when the
learner focuses on the use of language”, but their learning context is usually different.
When learners acquire vocabulary through explicit learning, their attention will directly
focus on the information to be learned, which gives the greatest chance for its acquisition.
40
However, the process of this type of acquisition is time-consuming and it is difficult to
learn an adequately sized vocabulary. Incidental learning, on the other hand, can occur
when the learners use the language for communicative purposes. In this process, the
students will get a double benefit for time expended but it is slower and more gradual.
points out that “vocabulary acquisition is incremental in nature”. The component types of
word knowledge cannot be completely learned simultaneously. For example, the learners
cannot have full collocational competence before they know the basic meaning of a word.
In the same token, Henricksen (1999, cited in Schmitt, 2000: 118) gives a description of the
dimensions of knowledge which can be acquired to various degrees. They are: (a) learners
can have knowledge ranging from zero to partial to precise; all word knowledge ranges on
a continuum, rather than being known and unknown, (b) the depth of knowledge requires
mastery of a number of lexical aspects, and (c) receptive and productive mastery; words are
is advantageous for both teachers and learners. Teachers are able to know what the students
have learned, and what they have failed to learn. For the students, error correction is
beneficial to them because they are able to recognize their own weaknesses in language use.
Eskey (1983, cited in Ferris, 2002: 4) states that “the ability to correct errors is crucial in
many settings and that students’ accuracy will not magically improve all by itself”.
Moreover, because L2 students are in the process of acquiring the L2 lexicon and the
41
morphological and syntactic system, they need distinct involvement from the teacher to
help them develop strategies for finding, correcting, and avoiding errors (Ferris, 2002).
However, error correction has disadvantages as well. If teachers pay too much
attention to the errors and correct every error, the students may lose confidence in their
language use and try to avoid committing errors. According to the results of avoidance,
teachers cannot know the true ability of the learners’ language competence. Holley and
King (1971, cited in Hendrickson, 1982: 115) state that “teachers need to be aware of how
they correct student errors and to avoid using correction strategies that might embarrass or
frustrate students”.
71-75) gives three approaches in correcting written errors. (1) Checking work in group or
pairs. This approach saves the teacher’s time and encourages communication among the
students. It has even more advantages if the correction work is conducted in English.
possibilities. (2) Integrated skills activities. This is a practical approach which can help
students learn a second language effectively. When the teacher feels that corrective work is
necessary, the treatment will be much more successful if the written exercise involves the
learner in activities that use all the language skills. (3) Using a correcting code. By using
this approach, the teacher writes a code of indication in margins or over the error. It will
lead the learners, if they are given adequate time, to work out for themselves what is wrong,
and try to correct it. It is more profitable if the teacher concentrates only on errors which
42
Byrne (1991: 124-126) provides four approaches to error correction: (1) Correct
all the mistakes. This is the traditional approach to the correction of written work which is
time-consuming and can be discouraging to the students if they get their work back covered
with red ink. He suggests that it is better if the teacher can correct the students’ errors in the
class while they are still engaged in writing and everything is still fresh in their mind. (2)
Correct mistakes selectively. This approach is more positive than total correction. The
teacher attempts to correct only the mistakes in certain areas. (3) Indicate mistakes so that
the students can correct them. Normally, it is done by underlining the mistakes and using
some kind of symbol to focus the attention of the students on the kind of mistake they have
made. The teachers can get the students, individually, in pairs, or in small groups, to
identify most of the mistakes for themselves. In addition, this approach makes the students
more aware of the kind of mistakes and is likely to result in something being learned. (4)
Let the students identify and correct their own mistakes. This approach is not a procedure
that the teachers are likely to be able to follow all the time. However, the teachers should
occasionally be prepared to hand over the whole business of correction to the students.
syntax and phonology to the neglect of the lexicon as evidenced by many researchers such
as Ramsey (1981); Zoghoul (1991); and Dušková, (1969). Ramsey (1981, cited in Zughoul,
1991: 45) claims that “teachers and syllabus designers have been under the influence of the
phonology and syntax”. In addition, the complexity inherent in the area of lexis does not
lend itself as easily as phonology and syntax to quantification and scientific analysis
43
(Zughoul, 1991). Dušková (1969) has acknowledged that certain lexical errors are difficult
Ong’s (2007) study of lexical errors in the English written work of pre-university
science students reveals that the main lexical errors found in the study were intralingual
errors (75.77%), caused by the complexity and difficulty in the learners’ TL. The error type
while lapses had the least frequency of occurrence (1.5%) among the lexical errors
identified. In most studies conducted on lexical errors, for examples, Woon (2003), Zahira
(2003), and Tan (1994), the percentage of intralingual errors is greater than that of
interlingual errors.
According to Woon (2003), the lexical errors in the written work of form four
Chinese educated ESL students of SMK Taman Connaught, Kuala Lumpur were examined.
Out of the total number of lexical errors, 84.2% were accounted for intralingual errors
whilst only 15.8% were accounted for interlingual errors. She concluded in her study that
MT interference was not the main factor of lexical errors made by Chinese ESL students.
Similar to Zahira (2003) and Tan (1994), their study revealed that Malay students generally
committed intralingual errors than interlingual errors. Zahira (2003) conducted the study of
lexical errors of upper secondary Malay learners of English. It is revealed that 66.13% were
accounted for intralingual errors while 33.87% were accounted for interlingual errors. In
Tan’s (1994) study, the result revealed that intralingual errors constituted 88% of the total
number of errors and interlingual errors constituted only 11%. It is implied that in the ESL
44
On the other hand, the research conducted with the EFL students gave the different
result. Cha (1996) who conducted a study on the lexical errors of Korean EFL students
found that interlingual errors are the main source of errors committed by Korean EFL
students. The highest lexical errors found in her study were the “literal translations” error
type (32.2%). This study shows that the MT interference has a great influence on the
secondary factor after grammar. Khaourai (2002) conducted a study on errors in the
her analysis, she grouped errors into three categories: grammatical errors, syntactic errors,
and lexical errors. The results revealed that grammatical errors occurred most frequently
(69.65%), while lexical errors came in third place (10.31%). Kemthong (1981) conducted
research on the errors of students at Ramkhamhaeng University. The results showed that
the most frequent error that committed was in the use of punctuation (59.93%) and in
second place came the use of lexical items and expression (54.73%).
He found that the main cause of errors was incomplete application of rules, followed by
errors due to translation from Thai into English. Lexical errors both in meaning and
function were also found. Lukanavanich (1988) carried out an analysis of the written errors
of first-year English students at Bangkok University. The results showed that the errors
were mainly grammatical or structural errors. Lexical errors and stylistic errors were also
found.
45
Cheojarnchin (1990) conducted an error analysis of the written errors of Mattayom
Suksa 4 students at Udonpittayanukul School. The results revealed that the errors found in
the written work of the students were structural errors, lexical errors, spelling errors, and
punctuation errors, respectively. The most frequent errors were structural errors. Srinon
(1999) studied an error analysis of the free compositions written by the first-year students
at Mahamakut Buddhist University, Ayutthaya Province. The results showed that a total of
47 types of errors in the sample which could be ordered into top ten frequencies from the
most to the least: (1) errors in the use of tenses, (2) errors in the use of determiners, (3)
errors in the use of prepositions, (4) errors in the use of verb forms, (5) punctuation errors,
(6) literal translation from L1 (Thai) to L2 (English), (7) errors in the use of adverbs, (8)
wrong choice of words, (9) errors in the use of nouns, and (10) errors in the use of adverbs.
Regarding the sources of errors analyzed, 32.10% resulted from the mother tongue
ignorance of rule restrictions, 10.29% from false concept hypothesized, and 9.88% from
incomplete application of rules. The most important finding of this study is that most of the
students in the sample group used first language structures to write the English
2.10 Summary
The literature related to error analysis provided in this chapter is the conceptual
framework that the researcher uses to analyze and explain the errors of this study. They are
the framework of analyzing used for all the discussions and explanations of the causes of
errors.
46