Article PDF
Article PDF
Article PDF
Abstract
________________
This qualitative-quantitative study is conducted as a classroom research. Focusing on two
question types, display and referential, it explored recurring patterns of questioning
behavior and their interactive effects were observed through non-participant observation.
Forty reading comprehension classes in Tehran universities were observed by the
investigator. The findings indicated that display questions were used by teachers more
frequently than referential questions. Also, it was concluded that NOT all referential
questions could create enough interaction. Further elaboration on the results may be found
in the study report.
________________
Introduction
Inadequacy of quantitative approaches to research led to the development of a separate
field known as the qualitative approach to research. Such methods attempted to present the
data from the perspective of the subjects on the observed groups so that the cultural and
intellectual biases of the researcher would not distort the collection, presentation and
analysis of the data (Alwright, 1988). It has increasingly been integrated into SLA research
in recent years, its ultimate goal being to discover and to understand those phenomena from
the perspective of participants in the activity. Long (1983) introduced non-participant
observation (in contrast with participant observation introduced by Bailey, 1983) in which
the investigator observes and records or takes notes of the observed activity, but without the
control or guidance of a questionnaire or other instruments.
After the failure of several important methods comparison studies in the 1960's (e.g.
Scherer and Werthimer, 1964; Smith, 1970) the influence of interaction analysis stimulated
interest in foreign language classroom processes. A few years later, more careful
observational studies gradually revealed which process variables were of interest.
Recently, there has been much research on teacher talk (e.g. Long and Sato, 1983).
Issues such as the amount and type of teacher talk, speech modifications made by teachers,
instructions and explanations, error correction and questions have been more or less the
center of attention.
Questions
The Longman Dictionary of English language provides the following definition for a
question: a command or interrogative expression used to elicit information or a response, or
to test knowledge. Lynch (1991), however, criticizes the last aspect of it, i.e. to test
knowledge.
In non-educational settings, people seldom ask questions to which they have already an
answer. Although, there are a number of exceptions such as jokes (e.g. what's the difference
between – and -?), quizzes (e.g. Which country will host the next Olympics?) and
courtroom discourse (e.g. And what did the defendant say to you then?). Lynch
characterizes a question as an utterance with a particular illocutionary force; and Quirk et al.
97
(1970 and 1985) define a question as a semantic class used to seek information on a specific
subject (Lynch, 1991).
Long and Sato (1983) and Brock (1986) have investigated the role of questions in second
language learning in the classroom environment. They have worked on the role of teacher's
question types (especially display and referential questions) and their facilitating the
learning. Van Lier (1988) believes that classroom questions of whatever sort are designed to
get the learners to produce language. Brock contends that referential questions increase the
amount of learner output; therefore, an increased use of referential questions by teachers
may create discourse which can produce a flow of information from students to the teacher,
and may create a more near-normal speech. However, it is believed that display questions
require short or even one-word answers and hence are less likely to get learners to produce
large amounts of speech.
Lynch quotes a number of researchers who investigated the balance between referential
questions and display questions in the foreign language classroom. Long and Sato (1983)
compared the questions occurring in informal NS-NNS conversation, and teacher-learner
interaction in the second language classroom. Lynch (1991) summarizes their findings as
follows:
Referential (information-seeking) questions which predominate in NS-NNS conversation
outside classrooms (76% of all questions asked) made up a 14% of questions asked by
teachers. This result suggests that, contrary to the recommendations of many writers on
second language teaching methodologies, communicative use of the target language makes
up only a minor part of typical classroom activities. "Is the clock on the wall?" and “Are
you a student?" are still the staple diet, at least for beginners.
Further qualitative distinctions were made by Long and Sato (1983) who suggest that
learner responses would differ not only quantitatively but also qualitatively, depending on
the type of questions. Referential questions, which seek information unknown to the
speaker, were thought more likely to elicit longer, more authentic responses than display
questions, for which responses are predetermined by lesson content. This hypothesized
effect of a process variable was tested both in a simulated classroom interaction (Brock,
1986) and in a natural classroom experiment (Long, 1983). The results suggested that
referential questions elicited slightly longer and more student utterances.
The study reported here consists of two parts; in the first part which is more or
less qualitative in nature, no predetermined line of search is followed; rather, the obtained
data is investigated for recurring patterns of EFL teachers' questioning behavior. This aspect
of the study will reveal generalizable patterns of teachers' use of questions in the observed
EFL classrooms. However, the second part, which is of a quantitative nature, is concerned
with clarifying two issues. The first issue concerns EFL teacher's questioning behavior, and
the second concerns with the amount of interaction caused by teacher's questions.
Method
Focusing on the distribution of display and referential questions in EFL
classrooms and their interactive effects, two hypotheses were shaped at the outset.
Hypothesis 1: There is no difference between the distribution of teachers' use of display and
referential questions.
Hypothesis 2: Referential questions create more interaction in the classroom than display
questions do.
The qualitative side needed no hypothesis to be made beforehand; it helped a
number of patterns (of teacher's questioning behavior and EFL classroom interaction)
emerge out of the study data, which will be presented later in this report.
Referential questions are those questions for which the answer is not already known by the
98
teacher. Such questions may require interpretations and judgments on the part of the
"answerer". Display questions refer to those questions for which the questioner knows the
answer beforehand; such questions are usually asked for comprehension checks,
confirmation check, or clarification requests. Interaction is used in a general sense in this
study, referring to any sort of interaction, student-student or teacher-student discussions,
group discussions, and any type of classroom participation (Long and Sato, 1983).
The main assumption behind this study is that interaction in language
classrooms will lead the learners to better learning, and will activate their competence
(Malamah-Thomas, 1987). Brock (l986) also maintains that an increase in the amount of
classroom interaction will help (foreign) language learners learn the target language easily
and quickly. She believes that increased language output will improve language learning.
None of the already-invented and used schemes (e.g. Moskowitz, Flint system, Flanders'
interaction Analysis Categories, etc.) completely suited the purpose of the study. Therefore,
it was decided to carry out the observation in a way similar to partial ethnography.
Three EFL instructors from Allamah Tabatabaee University and two from Tehran
University, Iran, were randomly selected for observation. The classes were observed as
carefully as possible during a two-mouth period. The researcher carried out the observation
personally, sitting in the classes from beginning to the end of each session, taking notes of
teachers’ questions, their number and functions (e.g. comprehension check, talk initiation,
etc), listening to the discussions of the students, writing down the amount of student-student
or teacher-student interactions, length of the learner's responses to different types of
questions, and other noticeable patterns.
After data collection, the observed patterns were analyzed with reference to earlier studies,
first, to find and reveal noticeable generalizations and patterns in teachers’ questioning
behavior and EFL classroom interaction and second, to verify the hypotheses. Descriptive
and inferential statistics were used for data analysis.
There were no experimental subjects in the sense that we have in experimental
research, neither control nor treatment groups. The subjects of the study were five non-
native speaking (NNS) English instructors at Allamah Tabatabaee University and Tehran
University, Tehran; three holding Mater’s and two PhDs ranging from 30 to 52 years of age.
All, having experience in teaching EFL courses for several years, taught Reading
Comprehension at upper-intermediate level with the same text-books (cited in the
bibliography). Their only behavior to be observed was their use of questions. None of them
knew about the focus of the study. However, they were informed about it after the data were
collected.
Other participants in the study were Iranian students of English (Language and
Literature or Translation) attending Reading Comprehension II and III at the above
mentioned universities. The focus of the observation was not on a certain student; rather,
their participations in teacher-fronted or group discussions were observed.
The only way to collect useful data was non-participant observation, the focus being
on the teacher's use of questions and its effect on classroom interaction. The number of
display and referential questions and their interactive results were all recorded. However,
teachers and students were observed with no reference to their age, psychological traits or
another sort of bias.
Findings
A. Quantitative Results
In order to find a distribution balance for teacher’s display and referential
questions, their corresponding numbers in each session were added up. It was found that the
99
observed teachers have used display questions 4.4 times more than the number of referential
questions. In other words, out of a total of 1628 questions, 1335 have been display (about
82%) and only 293 referential (about 18%).
Also, using chi-square (p<0.05, df=1), the first hypothesis was rejected. That is, the
observed chi-square was far greater than the corresponding critical chi-value (3.841).
Statistically speaking, the number of DQs used by EFL teachers was significantly higher
than the number of RQs they asked. In other words, the distribution of the two question
types was absolutely different.
As for the second hypothesis, two groups of questions were randomly selected from
among the obtained data for statistical analysis. The amount of time each question resulted
in classroom interaction was measured (in minutes); then, they were added up; and a mean
was calculated for each type (DQ-mean=0.625, RQ-mean= 2.833 minutes). Then using a t-
test (p<0.05, df= 24), the observed t-value (3.82) was noticed to be greater than the t-critical
(1.71) (See Table 1).
Comparing the two means, it was seen that the observed results were statistically
significant. In other words, the second hypothesis of this study is to be confirmed. That is,
the amount of classroom interaction caused by RQs is much greater than that caused by
DQs; RQs cause more interaction than DQs.
The second hypothesis was statistically confirmed. However, the qualitative data,
gathered through observation, implied that it is to be modified. An exemplary referential
question from the study data is, "Have you ever been close to death? How did you survive
then?" Such questions were found to cause more learner speech than display questions -
such as "What is the opposite of "near"? - did. It is reasonable to accept that learners tend to
speak and participate more when the expected answer is longer. While display questions are
usually asked for comprehension checks, confirmation checks or clarification requests
(Long and Sato 1983; Brock, 1986), referential questions are usually used to fill ill the
information gaps. Therefore, motivation and interest causes the interaction to be more
lifelike (Long, l981).
However, there can be found a number of referential questions which required a shorter
answer and after them there was a period of silence or topic-change. Questions like the
following did not help initiate a well-formed interaction:
Where do you live?
What's your father's job or name?
What's the combination of your briefcase lock?
What page is it?
Do we have any exercises left?
B. Qualitative Findings
The study was not confined to answering the two research questions; rather, during the data
collection procedure, a number of patterns of classroom interaction and questioning were
explored, which will be presented now.
1. Teachers ask a number of questions - mostly referential - before they start to work
directly on the Reading. These questions usually help the students warm up for the task, and
get familiar with the topic of the reading. It is usually in this phase that some of the
vocabulary (and idioms) are activated in the learner's mind and they experience their use in
real contexts. This is in fact what is already known as advance organizers.
2. Teachers used a number of general questions which were not directly addressed to any of
the students present in the classroom. Such questions as "What’s a generation gap?”, “Has
anybody ever been to Japan?”, “Any questions so far?” and so on, are of this kind. The
interesting aspect of these questions is that learners responded to these questions in two
contrasting ways: they either kept silent or gave a chora1 response. Examples:
a) Teacher: Does anybody know about the American Immigration Permit?
Class: (silence)
101
Discussion
The study focused on what is going on in the classroom, specifically on teachers'
questioning behavior - what kind of questions they ask, for what purposes, and so on.
Through observation, it was found that display questions outnumbered referential ones. It
was further observed that referential questions produced more classroom interaction. Seliger
and Long (1083) quote Swain (1983), who argues that output may be an important factor in
successful second language acquisition. She suggests that output creates the necessity for
the learner to perform a syntactic analysis of the language. She further notes that
comprehending the input or getting the message is possible without such an analysis.
Producing one's own messages, on the other hand, may force the learner to pay attention to
the means of expression to successfully convey his/her intended meaning. If it is true that
such questions increase the amount of learner output, and if output leads to better learning,
then questions can be an important tool in the language classroom, especially in those EFL
contexts where the classroom provides the only opportunity to produce the target language.
There are a number of factors leading to the reduced amount of interaction. Repeated
questions, low language proficiency, and limiting the class to the textbook were among the
observed factors leading to the reduction of interaction. When a certain question is asked
several times, students lose their interest in it. It becomes boring, and discourages any
motivation to continue. Also, when teachers ask a low-proficiency learner a talk-initiating
question, and s/he fails to respond, communication stops and the teacher is disappointed,
turning to another learner in the hope of achieving communication. Furthermore, when the
class-work is limited to a textbook, it was observed that negotiation of meaning is reduced
to zero when doing the structural-formalistic exercises of the book. However, when there
was, say, warm-up discussion at the outset of sessions, learners were more inclined to talk
and initiate.
However, there are a number of factors enhancing the amount of interaction:
interesting topics, teacher's attention, misunderstanding, information gap and humor. It was
observed that there was more student participation when the topic interests them (e.g. topics
such as marriage, religion, etc.). Also, in one interesting case it was observed that one of the
students had got a wrong idea of the meaning of the word "food-shopper". He thought that
the word equaled shopkeeper. There was real interaction for about several minutes. Then
the student understood the meaning after a real challenge of his conception. This can also be
considered as an example of interaction resulting from a considerable information gap.
Moreover, teachers can increase the amount of interaction in their classes by applying two
factors: humor and interest (attention). A learner will be more inclined to talk when s/he
finds the teacher paying attention and interested in the answer. Also, when the teacher
incorporates a piece of humor into the atmosphere of the classroom, students are
encouraged to participate more than when it is a gloomy atmosphere with the teacher as the
sole speaker.
It was inferred from the obtained data that display questions requiring short answers
contained small pieces of information- e.g. on part of speech, word stress, intonation,
antonyms and synonyms, word pronunciation and meaning, comprehension checks, etc.
Brock (1986) and Long and Sato (1983) have reported that classroom interaction was
characterized by the use of display questions. However, it seems that the use of display
questions can encourage language learners, especially beginners, to get interested. It may
also help teachers provide comprehensible input for learners. Referential questions, typical
of content classrooms and high proficiency language classrooms, and usually requiring long
103
and syntactically complex answers contain, in fact, important points, e.g. interpretation,
elaboration, giving opinions, etc
However, it would be dangerous to generalize that referential questions are more
useful for language learning or display ones are useless. Each context requires an
appropriate strategy for itself. Allwright and Bailey (1990) maintain that it is a dangerous
oversimplification to suggest that verbal interaction in the classroom is just a case of "the
more, the merrier". They add that "there may be times when teacher's desire to get students
to interact verbally can be counterproductive. Rather, it is important for teachers to adjust
their teaching style to learners' strategies".
Finally, it is suggested to study the findings if this study in an experimental research so that
the results are scientifically verified.
References
Allwright, D. and K.M. Bailey. (1990). Focus on the Language Classroom research for
Language teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge University press.
Brock, C.A. (1986). The Effects of Referential Questions on ESL Classroom Discourse.
TESOL Quarterly 20:47 – 59.
Long, M.H. (1983). Does second Language Instruction Make a Difference? A review of
research. TESOL Quarterly 17: 359 – 382.
Long, M.H. and sato C.J. (1983). Classroom foreigner talk Discourse: forms and functions of
teacher’s questions.
Seliger and M.H. Long (eds). Classroom – oriented research in second language acquisition.
(PP.268 – 285) Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Lynch, T.(1991). Questioning Roles in the classroom. ELT Journal 45 (3): 201 – 210.
Markstein, L. and L. Hirasawa. (1982). Expanding Reading skills (advanced) Mass. Newbury
House (Coursebook for Reading comprehension III)
Pica, T. and C. Doughty. (1985). The Role of Groupwork in classroom Second Language
Acquisition. Studies in second Language Acquisition 7: 233 – 248
Richards, J.C. and D. Nunan (eds). (1990). Second Language Teacher Education New York:
Cambridge University press.
Seliger, H.W. and M.H. Long (eds). (1983). Classroom – oriented Research in secon
Language Research Acquisition. Rowley. Rowley. Mass. Newbury House.
Van Lier, L. 1988. The classroom and the Language Learner. London: Longman.
104
Acknowledgements: I would like to express my thanks to Dr. J. B. Sadeghian and Dr. M. Nowroozi
Khiabani who supervised this study; also, my special thanks go to Dr. H. Marefat for her invaluable
help in data analysis. Also, I feel indebted to the instructors whose classroom performance is
reported here.