01-Child Traffic Safety en
01-Child Traffic Safety en
01-Child Traffic Safety en
eu
Please refer to this document as: DaCoTA (2012) Children in road traffic, Deliverable 4.8c of
the EC FP7 project DaCoTA
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 1
www.dacota-project.eu
Contents
1 OVERVIEW ..................................................................................................................... 4
2 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................. 7
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 2
www.dacota-project.eu
REFERENCES .....................................................................................................................36
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 3
www.dacota-project.eu
1 Overview
Children: persons from 0 to 14 years
The road safety of children is Reduction of child fatalities in traffic but lower fertility rate and
influenced by several factors: decrease of independent mobility
- exposure through
increasing motorization
- restricted independent Development of physical and cognitive skills
mobility
- socio-economic
background Children have fewer opportunities to make experiences in
traffic as active road users
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 4
www.dacota-project.eu
While cars are becoming safer for children as car passengers, the highest risk of fatality for
children is still as car passenger followed as pedestrians and then as cyclists. Research
indicates that children from lower socio-economic backgrounds are more likely to be injured
in traffic incidents.
Due to diverging national definitions, traffic casualty data is not always easy to compare
across countries. The fact that child crash rates vary not only between the EU Member
States but also between national regions makes comparisons even more difficult. Data
about the exposure of children as road users is rare but the general increase in motorisation
increases the risk of traffic crashes such as collisions between cars pedestrians and cyclists
which are less likely to happen, per capita, if fewer people use a car and walk or cycle
instead.
Modal split
Children are very mobile; however, most of their travel is connected with journeys to school
or for leisure purposes. Children are the most vulnerable road users.
Little is known about the modal split of children’s travel but research shows that traffic density
and the distance travelled to school or childcare facilities are important factors influencing the
choice of transport mode for children. In the last 30 years parents have restricted the
independent mobility of children to an increasing extent because of traffic safety concerns
and the absence of suitable play spaces. More often than not, parents now drive their
children to a destination reducing their autonomy and opportunities to develop important
skills such as how to behave in road traffic and how to make use of public space or explore
their environment.
Differences in children’s physical and psychological skills depend on their age and other
influencing factors. In fact, greater levels of physical activity diminish the risk of falling or
other injuries related to motion.
Development of skills
Children who travel mainly by car are less successful in negotiating road traffic as
pedestrians, public transport users or cyclists. The behaviour of parents when driving or in
traffic becomes critical since their attitudes and interactions influence the behaviour of their
children.
The ability of children to develop risk awareness and, therefore, to perceive traffic situations
as dangerous is largely dependent on age. Inactive children are less healthy and their
psychomotor development and also their attentiveness are negatively affected.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 5
www.dacota-project.eu
Children between 8 and 14 are easily distracted and therefore are at higher risk around
traffic. Although the psychomotor skills of male teenagers are well developed, their tendency
towards risk-taking exposes them to the risk of being involved in road traffic crashes. This is
especially true of young cyclists.
Regardless of parental restrictions, or their stage of development, all young people have an
urge to be physically active which can lead to problems when trying to ensure that road
infrastructure is supporting safety (e.g. when crossing the street). This needs to be taken into
account when planning traffic infrastructure in areas used by numbers of children.
Many positive safety effects of infrastructure design affecting children are already known. For
example, the provision of separate facilities for pedestrians and cyclists, speed management
and traffic calming measures, such as speed humps and other physical self-enforcing
measures, and also the improvement of visibility conditions at junctions and/or the reduction
of parking spaces all ameliorate the risk of traffic crashes for children. In vehicles, driver
assistance systems such as ISA are of known benefit. In addition to these systems, children
as car passengers need to be protected through the use of child safety seats and safety
belts. However, some of the protective measures provided by airbags designed for adults in
a normal seating position pose a serious threat to children sitting in rearward facing child
seats. Research shows that the use of rearward facing restraints provides the best protection
and should be used up to as high an age as possible (although not used adjacent to frontal
passenger airbags). (See ERSO Vehicle Safety web text).
The most effective measure to protect children as pedestrians is to reduce the speed of
motor vehicles. For young cyclists, separated cycle lanes are a reasonably protective
measure and the use of bright helmets and light-reflecting clothing help to improve
conspicuity. Properly enforced laws and regulations need to be reinforced by continuing to
make young people aware of the potential dangers of the road environment thus avoiding a
false sense of security. (See also ERSO Roads, Speed and Speed Management web texts).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 6
www.dacota-project.eu
2 Introduction
Children in road traffic are defined here as persons aged from 0 to 14 years. The group aged
between 0 and 4 years are considered as babies and toddlers, between 5 and 10 years is
the pre-puberty phase and between 11 and 14 is the puberty or early adolescence phase.
Each phase within childhood is characterized, and consequently defined, by distinctive levels
of physical and psychological skill. Children as road users cannot, therefore, be considered
an homogenous group as their abilities or their mode of transport choice differ considerably.
Data about the mobility patterns of children as road users in general is scarce. In fact, little is
known about children under the age of six years because most of the statistical data starts
with children at this age and older. Children in different age groups are often merged and the
varying behaviours of children of different age groups neglected, e.g. longer distances
travelled by older children or their greater access to different modes of transport. There are
only few scientific projects which provide information about the mobility patterns of children.
However children, generally, are more vulnerable in road traffic than other road users
(Limbourg, 1976) making it very important to obtain good data about their participation in the
traffic system so that appropriate strategies can be implemented. The United Nations’
Universal Declaration of Human Rights specifically requires that children are entitled to
special care and assistance.
The UN Convention on the Rights of Children is of key relevance for road safety. Children
are vulnerable in road traffic for many reasons which can be categorised in three main
groups:
• The causes lie within the child due to a lack of necessary skills to interact safely in traffic.
• The causes lie within other road users (especially car drivers, due to a lack of special
care and consideration).
• The causes lie within traffic planning and traffic regulation due to a lack of child-friendly
infrastructures, regulations and assistance for children.
The American Academy of Pediatrics has also published a comprehensive policy statement
on Child Passenger safety (Committee on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention (2011)
Child passenger safety. Pediatrics;127(4):788-93).
The following chapters will give an overview about children’s’ needs in road traffic. The first
chapter gives an insight into some accident data. The seat belt usage and social aspects of
traffic safety are discussed as well as the question if a decrease in accident rates reflects an
increase in safety.
In chapter 3 an outline of children’s mobility behaviour will be given, about the number of
trips, trips purposes, choice of transport modes and how the preconditions of being
independently mobile has changed.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 7
www.dacota-project.eu
Chapter 4 deals with children’s development in relation to road safety and in chapter 5
measures and inventions how to improve children’s traffic safety are discussed.
The text concentrates mainly on children as active road users (see ERSO Vehicles web text
for detailed technical discussion on child occupant safety child restraints etc.). Much money
is put into research to improve children’s safety inside the car as passive road users. But the
aim of every traffic policy should also be to improve the traffic conditions for children as
active road users, in order to support the independent and sustainable mobility of children.
The number of children killed annually in road traffic crashes fell approximately by over a half
between 2000 and 2008 in EU countries. The number of child fatalities has decreased
gradually as a proportion of all fatalities. Although comparisons over long periods in some
countries show trends for child fatalities which are very similar to those of overall fatalities, in
the last five years, the decline in child fatalities has been greater than that of all fatalities,
which may indicate that safety is improving drastically. The falling birth rate across Europe is
another possible explanation (Eurostat).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 8
www.dacota-project.eu
Figure 1: Unnatural death per 100.000 inhabitants by cause, age and gender during the period 1999-
2008 in the Netherlands
Statistical data in the Netherlands indicate that child cyclists in the 12 to 14 age groups have
the highest mortality rate followed by the 10 to 11 age groups. Even though the mortality rate
in general increases with age it does not do so as rapidly as for children as pedestrians and
as car passengers. All fatal crashes involving children as active road users are collisions
with a motor vehicle. Collisions with obstacles occur only in fatal car crashes (SWOV, 2009,
Fs Road Safety of children in the Netherlands).
Road fatalities among older children and teenagers (aged between 10 and 17 years) in the
Netherlands, during the period 1999 to 2008) show that 26% died as car passengers, 27%
as moped riders and 35% as cyclists (SWOV, 2012, Fs Risky traffic behaviour among young
adolescents). When analysing European accident figures there are significant differences
between countries. In Austria and Portugal, for instance, about 60% of injured children under
the age of 15 years were hurt in cars while in all other European countries the percentage is
much lower (43%). In the Netherlands the number of injured children as cyclists is four times
higher than that of pedestrians while the pattern in Romania, Ireland or UK is the opposite.
The number and structure of crashes in the EU countries differs considerably due to
variations in use of transport modes, travel behaviour and traffic density.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 9
www.dacota-project.eu
The use of European and national level crash figures alone are not sufficient to fully assess
problems and recommend countermeasures. A survey by the German Automobile Club
(ADAC) showed that there are also significant differences in the nature of accidents at the
local level. Accident statistics in towns with more than 20.000 inhabitants, in one German
Land, show that worst performing towns are 10 times riskier for children compared with the
best performing ones (ADAC, 2012).
The safety gain from seat belt use and appropriate child restraint use is enormous.
Rearward-facing systems have been shown to reduce injuries between 90% and 95%, while
forward-facing systems have been shown to have an injury reducing effect of approximately
60%. The use of child safety seats has been shown to reduce infant deaths in cars by
approximately 71% and deaths to small children by 54%. See ERSO Vehicles web text.
According to a study, for children aged between 4 and 7 years, booster seats reduce injury
risk by 59% (Committee on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention, 2011).
In 2007, the Austrian Road Safety Board (KfV) observed approximately 2.000 children as
passengers in cars in urban areas of Austria in order to analyse the use of child or passenger
restraint systems. The survey showed that 20% of the children used neither a seat-belt (for
children with a height of more than 150 cm) nor a special safety-seat for smaller children. A
study by the ÖAMTC (Austrian Automobile and Touring Club) in 2010 showed that 40% of all
children were not appropriately secured and restrained when travelling as car passengers.
An observation in the Netherlands in 2008 showed a lower figure of 12% (SWOV, 2010 Fs
Seat belts and child restraint seats).
An Austrian study in 2007 showed that head injuries could be reduced by using safety seats
with a seat back. 44% of seriously injured children were more seriously injured than the
driver of the car and the severity of these injuries could be reduced by up to 30% using
appropriate child and passenger restraint systems. The study also pointed out that boys are
more often secured with the car seat belt, even if this is not appropriate for their age, than
girls (37 % boys and 27 % girls). Boys are also more likely to be protected with a booster
seat with seat back than girls (29 % boys: 16 % girls) (Spiter, 2007).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 10
www.dacota-project.eu
Analyses from Germany showed that the psychomotor development of children from migrant
backgrounds or poorer socio-economic background is worse than for children from better off
backgrounds (Limbourg, 2008). Female teenagers with low social status and migrant
background have the highest deficits in physical activities as the German child and youth
survey of 2008 shows ((Robert Koch-Institut, 2008). The situation is even worse in less
developed countries. Nantulay and Reich state that 96% of all children, who are killed in
traffic crashes living in less-developed countries (Pokriefke, 2011).
Fatality rates are widely used as an indicator of traffic density but without reference to
categorisation based on international standards. Many countries do not have or use
thoroughly assessed exposure data, specifically about walking, cycling or travel patterns
(Nilson, 1997).
Some countries have introduced travel surveys in order to estimate the average number of
kilometres travelled by different road user groups within national boundaries. These travel
surveys are carried out periodically (every 5-10 years), but do almost always exclude
children under 6 and sometimes between 6 and 15 years, as already mentioned in chapter 2.
At the same time, an assessment of the dangers children face from motor vehicles in public
spaces has to be derived from data which includes the number of registered cars or the total
number of motor vehicle kilometres in a country or region. In general, the motorisation rate
for cars is increasing in all European member states except Germany and the UK). In line
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 11
www.dacota-project.eu
with this the number of vehicle kilometres is also increasing (Eurostat). Increased car traffic
endangers children especially as non-motorised road users (Piagets).
In contrast, an increase in pedestrians and cyclists within the public space is likely to reduce
collisions between cars and cyclists (Jacobsen,2003)
As mentioned above, the number of children killed on the roads is decreasing but this
positive development can mostly be attributed to better car safety for children as passengers.
However, the consequences of better crash design for vehicle occupants for the safety of
other road users are not clear. The compatibility between vehicles of different sizes and
between vulnerable users and protected motor vehicile occupants is a critical issue. (See
ERSO Vehicles web text for discussion). For example, SUVs (Sport Utility Vehicles), are
considered as safe vehicles for drivers and passengers, but studies shows that they cause
severe safety risks for other road users which are increasing. The German Federal Highway
Research Institute found that crashes between SUVs and other road users have more
severe consequences than crashes involving smaller passenger cars (BAST, 2006). Data
from the USA shows that the probability of fatalities rises by 45%, and serious injuries by 11
% if a SUV hits a pedestrian or bicyclist (White, 2002). This will have an inevitable effect on
children.
Summary of key facts
• Decreasing number of child fatalities in traffic but also decreasing number of children
• The safety of vehicles has improved but there are compatibility issues for vehicles of
different masses and between vehicles and non motorised users.
• Highest percentage of child traffic fatalities is for children as car passengers, though
these bear a lower risk than child pedestrians or cyclists
• Children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds have a higher risk of becoming traffic
casualties
• Boys are more often involved in accident fatalities than girls
• Data about the exposure of children as road users is scarce
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 12
www.dacota-project.eu
younger than six years old. So at the beginning of this chapter it can be stated that there is a
need of good mobility data on the situation of 0-14 years old.
Austrian data show that children aged between 6 and 14 years undertake between 34% and
49% of all their journeys as pedestrians with 12% to 16% of all journeys as passengers in
cars (depending on the size of the community and public transport facilities). In urban areas
with good public transport, the percentage of public transport travel is high (45%), while in
smaller cities and rural areas the percentage varies from 32% to 37%. Overall, the
percentage of journeys by bicycle is between 5% and 7% (Bundesministerium für Verkehr
und Innovation, 2009).
There is some data available about the modal split of journeys to school and child care
facilities. In Belgium, children use active transportation modes (e.g. cycling and walking) on
their way to school (59%) more often than children in other European countries (29%). A
Belgian study showed that the distance to school is the main deciding factor between walking
or cycling. Children are more likely to walk to school if the facility is within a radius of 1.5
kilometres of their home. The same study showed that the distance criterion for cycling is up
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 13
www.dacota-project.eu
to 3 kilometres compared with adults at up to 8 kilometres. This shorter cycling distance for
children can be explained, in part, by the safety concerns of parents (D’Haese et al, 2011). In
Switzerland, cycling to school among 6 to 14 year olds has decreased by almost 20%
between 1994 and 2005 (European Cyclist Federation, 2012), while data from the UK shows
that the car use on the school run has doubled in the last 20 years. During the morning traffic
peak almost one in five cars is on the school run. This represents a considerable crash risk
as well as contributing to congestion and pollution. For children, travelling by bus is 7 times
safer than travelling by car (TIS, 2004).
A national study in Austria showed that less than half of parents in Vienna and Lower Austria
take their children to child day care facilities on foot (48%) with one third using the car to take
children to kindergarten (Ausserer et al, 2010). On the other hand, a German study indicated
that traffic density influences the choice of transport mode. In 2002, it was found that, in rural
areas with lower traffic density and in city centres with traffic calmed areas, 78% - 89% of
children walked to school unaccompanied by an adult. In areas with high traffic density the
figure was only 65% (Limbourg, 2008).
There is also little in-depth analysis of the mobility of younger children during leisure time. In
2002 in Austria, 22% of all journeys of persons over 6 years of age were for leisure although
variations in relation to location were noted. Children living in rural areas who have access to
public transport made more journeys in leisure time (roughly 21%) than children in areas with
poor access to public transport (roughly 17%). The highest percentage of children making
journeys during leisure time was found in Vienna and other urban areas with high quality
public transport system (roughly 24%) (Herry Consult, 2004). Further data of this type is
essential in order to assess the crash risk of children.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 14
www.dacota-project.eu
congestion) played outside, while 55% of the children who lived in safer and more attractive
environments played outside for two hours per day (Hüttenmoser, 1994). A more recent
study from Germany (2000) analysed whether children play or do sports (e.g. cycling, riding
a scooter) on the pavement or in the street. They found that, between the ages of 3 and 5
years, only 33% were active in this way. This increased to 55% for children between 6 and 7
years and rose to roughly 60% for older children (see Figure 1) (Funk & Fassmann, 2002). In
Austria, despite the scarcity of public play spaces, half of the children surveyed played in the
open several times per week. Although safer indoors, children’s development and knowledge
of their environment is restricted if not allowed to play outdoors (Limbourg, 2008).
Figure 2: Children’s play and sport in public spaces in the year 2000
70%
60% 59%
60%
55% 56%
50%
40%
33%
30%
20%
10%
0%
3-5 Jahre 6-7 Jahre 8-9 Jahre 10-11 Jahre 12-15 Jahre
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 15
www.dacota-project.eu
Figure 3: Unaccompanied school trips of 6 to 7 year old children between 1976 – 2000
The presence of adults also influences the radius of the child’s activity environment.
Children who are unaccompanied venture further. Depending on the distance between their
destination and where they live, children and their guardians use different transport modes.
Children who are driven in a car have a smaller activity radius than children who travel by
public transport, scooters, by bicycle or on foot (Daschütz, 2006).
Accompanying children on their daily journeys can have both positive and negative
consequences. An Austrian study shows that children, aged up to 8 years, living in urban
environment are often not allowed to cross even the least dangerous streets alone. The
consequence of this protectiveness is that they develop less independence, fewer motor-
sensory skills and are therefore restricted in their mobility. Furthermore they are at risk of not
developing appropriate road traffic behaviour (Daschütz, 2006).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 16
www.dacota-project.eu
physically possible and safe for them and they need enough space to practice and develop
skills without being restricted or endangered by traffic (Rümmele, 1993).
Activities such as riding a bicycle, using a scooter or walking support the development of
important, transferable skills. Several studies indicate that children’s ability to perform even
the simplest psychomotor activities is diminishing due to lack of opportunities for physical
activity (especially in larger cities). Further deficiencies are noted in psychomotor
development. 56% of 6 year olds in the Ruhr area of Germany, had coordination and motion
deficiencies when tested (Limbourg, 2008).
When an adult drives, the child’s role as car passenger is passive and opportunities for
learning are restricted by the adult’s need to concentrate on the road. It is also worth noting
that the increased use of cars potentially endangers children who are not transported to
school by car. A German study showed that drivers in these circumstances generally do not
adapt their traffic behaviour to the presence of other children (e.g. stop the car at a stopping
restriction zones). (Limbourg, 1997). Furthermore, studies of child day care centres in
Frankfurt showed that in kindergartens where children were physically active and supported
in psychomotor activities there was a significant decrease in the crash rate (50%) (Limbourg,
2008).
The ‘vicious’ circle of “protecting” children from becoming traffic casualties by transporting
them by car can be described as follows: increasing car traffic leads to a higher risk that
children along the road or crossing the road become involved in a traffic crash; walking or
cycling are considered increasingly risky activities, especially in less attractive areas with
high traffic volumes thus raising the safety concerns of parents who then take their children
by car, which causes more traffic and contributes to a higher risk of traffic crashes. Figure 3
illustrates this vicious circle (Rauh, 2001).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 17
www.dacota-project.eu
Traffic safety
decreases
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 18
www.dacota-project.eu
Children need certain abilities and skills in order to handle various traffic situations. They
must learn to recognise and anticipate dangerous situations and understand the principles of
road traffic. They must also judge the intentions of other road users and not be easily
distracted by multiple stimuli. These abilities and skills are acquired in a developmental
process starting in early years and progressing through childhood into teenage years.
In this section, the developmental processes relevant for safe traffic behaviour are discussed
briefly. The age categories used should only be considered as rough guidelines as each child
has its own “tempo” for developing skills. In addition, the developmental process depends
largely on experience. Children who are mostly transported by car are less able to negotiate
road traffic successfully than those with experience as pedestrians, cyclists or public
transport users (Limbourg, 1997).
The theories of Jean Piaget are often considered when trying to explain children’s behaviour
patterns in road traffic that differentiated between the following four levels of development
(Neuman-Opitz, 2008):
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 19
www.dacota-project.eu
In addition, the sensory functions of children are not fully differentiated at this point. They do
not distinguish between basic and negligible features. They are easily distracted and cannot
concentrate on two different tasks (e.g. playing with a ball and dealing with traffic when the
ball runs into the road). Their attention is often centred on one characteristic to the exclusion
of other features that are sometimes more important. For example, a child may be more
interested in the colour of an oncoming car rather than its speed or proximity. They are not
yet able to create logical links. An awareness of perspective (e.g. that the form and the
shape of an object depends on the perspective of the observer) is missing. They are not able
to combine two separated perceptions into one (e.g. size and distance indicating that an
oncoming car is getting bigger).
Difficulties arise when dealing with complex or combinatorial situations which require
simultaneous processing of more than one feature. A child will have problems determining
whether it is safe to cross the street between two parked cars (learned as dangerous) when
the cars are located on a crossing (learned as safe).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 20
www.dacota-project.eu
The peripheral perception of children is limited up to the age of eight (Safekids , 2004).
Safe Kids Walk This Way. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012 from: http://www.safekids.org/our-
work/programs/walk-this-way/ Detecting oncoming perils from the side is only partially
possible for children (Sandels, 1975).
The perception of depth of focus is fully developed at the age of nine. Younger children are
thus restricted when estimating distances (Zwahlen, 1975).
The ability to assess velocities is linked to the skill of thinking along a timescale. Only from 8
years and older are children able to relate distance covered to time needed. In general,
however, the ability to estimate velocities accurately comes between the ages of 10 and 12
years (Cross & Mehegan, 1988). For younger children, velocity can be connected with the
design of a car or the noise it makes. For example, they are more careful when a slower,
older and noisier car is approaching than a newer, faster, quieter car. In addition children up
to the age of 5 are often not able to differentiate between a moving and a stationary car
(Limbourg, 1976).
Heinrich & Langosch (1975) showed that nearly all children in primary school know that they
have first to look left and then right when crossing a road. Various experiments, however,
have indicated that children in primary school are not reliably able to distinguish between left
and right (Colborn, 1970). Even some adults have difficulties in determining left and right
spontaneously. Younger children often consider the concept of “left and right” to be static and
unrelated to themselves. The dangers here are obvious.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 21
www.dacota-project.eu
In a study of 5 to 10 year old children, Finlayson (1973) found that only children older than 8
years used their hearing in road traffic regularly. It is also that case that children often do not
hear noises in road traffic if they are concentrating on playing with their friends or are
distracted by other external stimuli. The integration of different senses increases in a linear
fashion up to the age of 11 (Limbourg, 2008).
In the first two years of life, psychomotor development concentrates on controlling sitting up,
locomotion and manipulation. Subsequently, children learn to jump, climb stairs, pedal etc.
Pre-school aged children have the psychomotor skills to participate as pedestrians in road
traffic. However, Arnberg et al (1978) record that the ability to cycle is less well developed.
Up to the age of approximately 7 years, children have problems in abruptly interrupting an
action which they have already started (e.g. to stop running at the kerb). However, a child’s
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 22
www.dacota-project.eu
urge to be active increases the probability of running or jumping in road traffic (Limbourg,
2008), This needs to be taken into account when planning traffic infrastructure in areas with
numbers of children to provide a Safe System environment. Between 6 and 12 years the
psychomotor skills of children rapidly increase in quality and quantity. With respect to safe
cycling behaviour a significant jump is made between the ages of 7 and 8 and a further big
jump between the ages of 13 and 14 (Arnberg et al, 1978). Despite having well-developed
psychomotor skills, 12 to 14 year old boys are particularly at risk in road traffic as cyclists due
to their higher propensity for risky behaviour (see section 3.2).
These changes in children’s mobility have had a negative impact on their health and their
psychomotor development. Bös (2003) analysed 54 studies from 1965 to 2002 and came to
the conclusion that the motor abilities of children have deteriorated by more than 10% during
a 25 year period. This trend has been confirmed in other studies (Dordel, 2000). A screening
of 950 six year old children in Germany showed that only 44% had the psychomotor skills
expected at their age. The majority of the children had deficiencies in motion and co-
ordination (Mannheim, 1999). Experience from cycle safety education indicates that in the 10
years from 1997 to 2007 children had increasing problems with (Günther & Degener, 2009):
- Turning e.g. children are less able to ride one-handed.
- Starting up and braking
- Maintaining balance
- Getting on and off the bike
- Keeping the bike on course
These psychomotor skills deficiencies increase the risk of traffic crashes as well as in other
areas of life ((Kunz, 1993) The growing numbers of children who display behavioural
problems such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorders (ADHD) need to be considered
when designing and implementing road infrastructure. The term hyperactive indicates that
those skills needed in traffic are impaired (Pieper, 2010). Various studies showed that
children with some kind of behavioural problem are more likely to be involved in a crash than
those without (Mannheimer, Mellinger, 1967).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 23
www.dacota-project.eu
Limbourg (1997) mentions that risk awareness in children develops in three stages:
• Acute risk awareness: “I perceive risk at the moment of danger” (around the age of 5). In
most cases it is too late to avoid a crash.
• Anticipation risk awareness: “I know that a certain situation is dangerous” (around the
age of 8) e.g. a child knows that cycling downhill might be dangerous and brakes
continuously when riding downhill
• Preventive risk awareness: “I take measures in advance to avoid risky situations” (around
the age of 10) e.g. a child makes a detour in order to be able to cross the road safely.
Limbourg underlines that these age categories provide only the roughest guideline and that
risk awareness depends largely on the individual child’s experience. In addition, several
studies indicate that there are different types of personality that support risky behaviour.
Children described as “lively fear naughts” (lively and without fear) are the most prone to
crash involvement; also, boys are more willing to take risks than girls (Ginsburg & Miller,
1982).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 24
www.dacota-project.eu
In the adolescent phase, (from around age 11 or 12 years) the awareness of risk is
particularly less pronounced. Even though behaviour is objectively risky, teenagers do not
describe it as such or do not relate it to themselves (“This cannot happen to me”). ‘Juvenile
egocentrism’ creates excessive feelings of self-importance. In addition the acquisition of an
important role within a group through unsafe conduct might boost self-esteem and thus lead
to more risky behaviour in order to gain approval (Limbourg , 2008).
Several studies indicate that lack of consideration by road users is generally thought of as a
safety problem (Ausserer et al, 2009). Children have no strategy to deal with this
phenomenon.
This task has two phases – pre-crossing and crossing. The pre-crossing phase includes
selection of the crossing site, detecting traffic, and choosing the moment to cross. Crossing
skills involve motor development and continuous feedback about decisions made. Data from
observing elementary school children crossing the street in the US indicates that less than
30% of American children stopped at the kerb when crossing, 20% stepped into danger, 20%
did not cross within the crossing area and 10% played in street when crossing (Grayson,
1975).
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 25
www.dacota-project.eu
Children’s behaviour when crossing the street differs from that of adults. The following
differences were indicated in various studies (Schieber. & Thompson, 1996).
• Adults base crossing decisions on the best time to cross; children use the best place to
cross (e.g. shortest crossing distance)
• Children cross the road more quickly than adults, often running
• Children wait for bigger gaps in the traffic flow
• Children use shorter crossing routes than adults (not diagonally)
• Children observe crossing rules more than adults (e.g. don’t cross adjacent to a crossing)
• Children less frequently orientate themselves before crossing than adults. Adults tend to
assess the traffic situation before reaching the kerb, children are only able to do it at the
kerb and then often only by looking left and right.
• Children tend to orientate themselves towards crossing by following people in front of
them (i.e blindly).
• Children crossing with those of the same age behave more riskily then with others.
• Children find it difficult between parked cars to cross. They stop at the kerb, even though
they have no view of the traffic situation and cross without stopping at the line of visibility.
• Children who mainly travel as car passengers are less able to negotiate traffic when
cycling, walking, etc
• Children aged between 8 and 14 are at higher risk in traffic
• The mobility behaviour of parents influences the behaviour of children
• Male teenagers are more at risk as cyclists due to their pre-disposition to risky behaviour
• Depending on age, the ability of children to perceive a traffic situation or be aware of
differs
• Children have an urge to be active; this needs to be taken into account in traffic planning
The main focus of the intervention programs cited here is on socio-demographic factors such
as income, location and children’s roles in road traffic. Unless human factors (psychology)
are taken into account in designing intervention packages, the resulting technologies may fail
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 26
www.dacota-project.eu
to adapt to human tolerances thresholds and needs. Children fulfil a variety of roles in
respect of their mobility as pedestrians, cyclists, public transport users or car passengers.
Children become pedestrians around the age of 5. In fact, between the ages of 5 and 14
years child pedestrian injuries are still the second most common cause of death or severe
injury (Safekids, 2004).
Other ERSO web texts set out the interventions which contribute to a Safe System which
have high relevance for child safety.
Improving traffic safety for children implies to have different kinds of approaches. Three E´s
are discussed in this chapter, areas where interventions can take place: Education, (traffic)
Environment and Equipment. Education means providing possibilities for children to improve
their skills of moving safely in traffic and at the same time to raise awareness for various
aspects concerning traffic. Environment includes the traffic infrastructure and legislation.
Equipment deals with appropriate kits for increasing traffic safety.
6.1 Education
The practical implications of developmental theories in both educational and domestic
settings play a prominent role in determining the success of educating children on traffic
concerns. The main theories which inform the way children interact and assimilate
information are those of Vygotski, Piaget, Bandura and Pavlov.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 27
www.dacota-project.eu
Therefore, new strategies or projects aimed at educating children on the dangers they face in
everyday life need to give more weight to the influence of parents, peers and teachers. There
is a fine line between what can be learned unaided and learning that requires guidance.
Zygotsky calls this the ‘Zone of Proximal Development’ and the relationship it may have with
traffic safety projects is that the education children receive at home and in school, although
significantly dissimilar, still determines much of the way in which a child perceives danger in
daily situations (Cherry, 2012b).
Due to difficulties in ensuring that education at home matches the levels of discipline of that
in school, schools and parents need to work together particularly on a subject that may be
seen as peripheral but which has life-saving potential. Pavlov’s Classical Conditioning theory
applies here and states that “development is considered a reaction to rewards, punishments,
stimuli and reinforcement.” If school and home life are well coordinated towards such
dynamics, children will learn better through practical stimuli and will associate certain
stimuli/situations with danger, which in turn will prevent them from acting irrationally and
underestimating how short term irrationality can lead to long term, severe injuries. By being
aware of the major child-learning theories, technology and economics can be applied in the
most accurate and holistic manner. This thinking can be incorporated into technologies such
as vehicle design and manufacture, urban planning and detecting effective stimuli to inhibit
speeding and non-use of restraint systems. For example, by introducing the points system
for car drivers for violating certain rules; (Keenan, 2008), perceptions of traffic safety were
shown to be internalised rather than externalised, thus avoiding the risk of turning
responsible behaviour in traffic into a remote concept.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 28
www.dacota-project.eu
The figure below (Figure 6) is based around a Cognitive Apprenticeship Model by Collins et
al. (1989). It comprises six phases which are outlined on the left and shows an intervention
program which requires the full collaboration of the 4 main bodies - friends, family, schools
and governments - to succeed. The reward and punishment system implemented in schools,
domestic settings and public settings encourages the public, in particular young people, to
treat traffic and mobility as issues which can lead to severe injury or, in some cases, death.
The first three phases (Modelling, Coaching and Scaffolding) can be best controlled and
monitored in educational and governmental settings and are essential steps to children
gaining an understanding of the dynamics of traffic and safe travel. The final three phases
(Articulation, Evaluation and Exploration) ensure that the child is supported in his/her
learning whilst being independent; reflecting Vygotsky’s social learning theory.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 29
www.dacota-project.eu
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 30
www.dacota-project.eu
Gehen geht” is a project by the Lower Austrian Environmental Consulting company (die
umwelt beratung). It aims at increasing the safety of children on their way to kindergarten or
school. The parents of pre-school children are encouraged, by various means, to use
environmental friendly modes for these journeys. The traffic safety of the children was
improved in two respects: Firstly, children become accustomed to the traffic environment
and learn how to behave safely in traffic and are thus prepared for walking to school on their
own. Secondly, traffic is reduced around kindergartens and around schools. In 2007, the
project was honoured with the UNESCO award „Commendable UN-Decadeproject“ and in
2011 with the Energy Globe Award NÖ
Parents received information materials at special events but were mainly addressed
indirectly. Children were encouraged to remind adults to fasten their seat belts, to buckle
them correctly and to make use of appropriate child restraint seats.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 31
www.dacota-project.eu
There are no results available on the impact of the campaign and the actual use and wearing
rates of child restraints seats and seat belts. An evaluation in Poland, however, showed that
the message had reached 42% of the children and 35% of parents. 90 % of the children said
that the campaign was very good. (EUCHIRES, 2006).
The campaign turned out to be a very effective way to reach the target group. Parents
supported the campaign and found the issues covered relevant to their concerns. Travel
habits were addressed directly and car drivers were engaged. The campaign had an impact
on the infrastructure in the residential area, too. Measures such as traffic calming around
schools and giving priority to bikes and pedestrians were implemented a consequence of the
project (Car Free Schools).
6.4 Environment
The objective is that children of school age do not encounter cars in their play areas or in
places where they habitually walk. In exceptional cases, vehicles travelling at a maximum
speed of walking pace are tolerated. Children from 7 to 12 years should not cross at
locations where vehicle speeds exceed 15-20 km/h. For older children, the same principles
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 32
www.dacota-project.eu
apply as for unprotected adult road users, i.e. they should not cross at locations where motor
vehicle speeds exceed 30 km/h. This applies to routes surrounding kindergartens, schools
and to social and other leisure activities.
Reducing the number of parking spaces on a given surface (car park or multi-storey garage)
is an important measure to improve the children’s visibility in traffic.
When crossings are signal-controlled there should be separate phases for pedestrians and
right or left turning traffic. If this is not possible, the pedestrian phase should start several
seconds earlier than the vehicle phase. Crossing distances should be short. Children should
be able to cross the whole road at green lights without stopping. Traffic islands at signalled
crossings should be avoided as children find it difficult to stop and wait at the traffic island.
The waiting time at signalled crossings should be short as children are generally not very
patient (Limbourg, 2008).
See ERSO Roads and Speed and Speed Management for comprehensive discussion.
6.5 Equipment
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 33
www.dacota-project.eu
Children as pedestrians
There are also more basic and integrative approaches to prevent children from being injured
in crashes as cyclists, car-passengers and pedestrians. The idea that children, as young as
the age of 5, can be seen as being semi-independent pedestrians is clearly erroneous. Their
cognitive, perceptual and visual systems are clearly underdeveloped for such a complex
task, such as crossing the street (Thomson et al, 1996) and they are physically vulnerable.
See ERSO texts on Pedestrian and Cyclists, Roads, Speed and Speed Management,
Vehicles.
The threats are not viewed as threats but rather as intriguing new information yet to be
discovered; which explains why so many children when they see their parents running after
them (to save them from car dangers) run even further as they see it as a fun game.
Particular measures such as lollipop women/men have proven to reduce such causalities;
however more needs to be offered to provide a safer system for use by children. The
implementation of speed humps force the driver to slow down and clear boards which
calculate the speed of each individual car. Stricter laws and regulation and introducing more
speed cameras will also help as will safer car fronts.
Children as cyclists
For children as cyclists there are some basic technologies which are known to reduce
fatalities and injuries. These are wearing of helmets and cycle lane dividers. The literature
suggests that children should also be instructed to wear yellow light-reflecting jackets and
brightly coloured helmets. Bicycles can be made more conspicuous by attaching a safety
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 34
www.dacota-project.eu
flag. Helmets – if properly fitted and correctly worn - cannot prevent crashes but might limit
the severity of the head and brain injury in case of an accident. According to a study in the
Netherlands one third of the cyclists who are admitted to hospital with serious injury after a
traffic crash are diagnosed with head and/or brain injury (SWOV 2012). However, the
compulsory use of helmets, on the other hand, could lead to a decline in the use of bicycles
(see e.g. Robinson 2006) In any case campaigns aimed at children could be launched which
promote a voluntary use of helmets. Besides it is very important to teach children and
parents how to wear a helmet correctly. Experiences of children bicycle trainers in Austria
had shown that more than 90% of all helmets were not correctly worn. See ERSO text on
Pedestrians and Cyclists.
• Intervention programs should take income level, location and children’s roles as road
users into account
• Measures are more likely to be effective if they take account of human factors
• Education: parents, peers and teachers have significant influence on the younger
generations
• Infrastructure: Traffic calming, speed reducing, visibility enhancing measures are most
effective
• Vehicles: Driver Assist systems and child safety seats though airbags can endanger
children
• Law: Strict laws, regulations and enforcement are needed.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 35
www.dacota-project.eu
References
ADAC: How To Address Accidents With Children - From European To Local Perspective; Dr.
N. Klassen; Allgemeiner Deutscher Automobilclub e.V., ADAC ; ©Association for European
Transport and contributors 2006 http://www.etcproceedings.org/paper/how-to-address-
accidents-with-children-from-european-to-local-perspective, called on June 28th 2012.
American Planning Association. (2012) What Is Planning?. [Website]. Retrieved on 3rd July
2012 from http://www.planning.org/aboutplanning/whatisplanning.htm.
ArchDaily.com
Arnberg, P., Ohlsson, E., Westerberg, A. & Östrom, C. (1978) The ability of preschool- and
schoochildren to manoeuvre their bicycles, VTI-Rapport Nr. 149 A, Statens väg- och
trafikinstitut, Linköging.
Atterton, J. (February, 2010). Imitation. [Website]. Reterieved on 3rd July 2012 from
http://www.learningandteaching.info/learning/imitation.htm.
Ausserer K., Röhsner U., Risser R. (2010) Zufußgehen beginnt im Kindesalter. Wege zum
und vom Kindergarten, finanzierte Studie des BMVIT im Rahmen der ways2go
Ausschreibung, Wien.
Ausserer K., Braguti I., Füssl E., Höfferer G., Risser A., Risser R. (2009) Bef(w)usst
unterwegs: Fußgängerstudie in Wien. Forschungsarbeiten aus dem Verkehrswesen, Band
191, BMVIT, Wien.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social Learning Theory. New York: General Learning Press.
Baumgart, F. (Hrsg. 1998. Entwicklungs- und Lerntheorien, Verlag Julius Klinkhardt Bad
Heilbrunn.
Bijur, P., Stewart-Brown, S. & Butler, N. (1986) Child behaviour and accident injury in 11,966
preschool children. American Journal of Diseases of Children 140, p. 487-492.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 36
www.dacota-project.eu
Bundesministerium für Gesundheit (2008) Strategie der Bundesregierung zur Förderung der
Kindergesundheit www.bmg.bund.de/cln_110/SharedDocs/Publikationen/DE/Praevention
/Strategie-Kindergesundheit,templateId=raw,property=publicationFile.pdf/Strategie-
Kindergesundheit.pdf (23.10.2008).
Centres for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Road Traffic Injuries: The Reality.
[Website]. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012 from: www.cdc.gov/SafeChild/Road_Traffic_Injuries/
Cherry, K. (2012c) What Is Sociocultural Theory?. [Website]. Retrieved on 3rd July, 2012
from http://psychology.about.com/od/developmentecourse/f/sociocultural-theory.htm.
Collins et al. (1989) Cognitive Apprenticeship Model. Retrieved on 3rd July, 2012
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/search/detailmini.jsp?_nfpb=true&_&ERICExtSearch_Sear
chValue_0=ED338729&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=no&accno=ED338729.
Committee on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention (2011) Child passenger safety.
Pediatrics;127(4):788-93.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 37
www.dacota-project.eu
Cross, R. & Mehegan, J. (1988) Young children`s conception of speed: possible implications
for pedestrians safety. International Journal of Science Education, 10, 3, 253-265.
David, S., Chapman, J., Foot, H. & Sheepy, N. (1986) Peripheral vision and child pedestrian
accidents. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 433-450.
Demetre J., Lee, D., Pticairn, T. Grieve, R., Thompson, J. & Ampofo-Boateng, K. (1992)
Errors in young children’s decisions about traffic gaps: Experiments with roadside
simulations. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 189-202.
[28] Department for Energy and Infrastructure [Website]. Retrieved 3rd July 2012 from
http://www.dpti.sa.gov.au/roadsafety/safer_vehicles/design_technology.
Euchires: www.krbrd.gov.pl/gambit/seminarium_gambit_2006/ENG/contents/PDF/44-
Maria%20Dabrowska-Loranc_Justyna%20Wacowska-Slezak_eng.pdf.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 38
www.dacota-project.eu
Finlayson, H. (1972) Children’s road behaviour and personality. Transport and Road
Research Laboratory, TN 739, Crowthorne.
Funk, W., & Fassmann, H. (2002) Beteiligung, Verhalten und Sicherheit von Kindern und
Jugendlichen im Straßenverkehr. Bericht der Bundesanstalt für Straßenwesen, Bergisch
Gladbach, Heft M 138.
Füssl, E. & Oberlader M. (2012) Jugendliche: Lebensqualität, Verkehr & Mobilität. FWF
Forschungsprojekt P 23194-G17 Endbericht.
Gil, T. &, S. L. Handy. (2008) Children’s Biking for Nonschool Purposes: Getting to soccer
Games in Davies, California. Transportation Research Record, Journal of the Transportation
Research Board 2074; p. 40-45.
Ginsburg, H. & Miller, S. (1982) Sex differences in children`s risk-taking behaviour. Child
Development, 53, 426 -428.
Grayson, GB. (1975) The Hampshire child pedestrian accident study, UK. Department of the
Environment Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Report 670, Crownthorne.
Heinrich, H.C. & Langosch, I. (1977) Einfluss von Informiertheit auf das Verhalten von
Kindern im Straßenverkehr. Bericht der Bundesanstalt für Straßenwesen, Erlebnis- und
Verhaltensformen von Kindern im Straßenverkehr. Köln: Reihe Unfall- und
Sicherheitsforschung Straßenverkehr, Heft 4.
Hübner, H. (1997) Mit Risiken umgehen können. In: H. Hübner & H. Hundeloh
(Hrsg.). (1997a). Kongress: Mehr Sicherheit im Schulsport – Bilanz und
Perspektiven. Dokumentation (S.17-46). Münster: LIT.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 39
www.dacota-project.eu
Hvoslev, H. (1994) Under-reporting of road traffic accidents recorded by the police at the
international level, IRTAD Special report.
Hüttenmoser, Marco (1994) Children and Their Living Surroundings. Empirical Investigations
into the Significance of the Living Surroundings for the Everyday Life and the Development of
Children. - Dokumentatonsstelle „Kind und Umwelt“ Zürich.
Jacobsen, P. (2003) Safety in numbers:more walkers and bicyclist, safer walking and
bicycling. Injury prevention 2003, 205-209.
Johansson, C., (2004) Safety and Mobility of Children Crossing Streets as Pedestrians and
Bicyclists. Luleå University of Technology, DOC 2004:27.
Keenan , S. (October, 2008) The Point System: Driving record affects insurance rates.
[Website]. Rertrieved on 3rd July 2012 from http://www.autoguide.com/buyers-guide/the-
point- system-884.html.
KFV: http://www.kfv.at/verkehr-mobilitaet/sicherheitsmassnahmen/kindersitz./
Limbourg, M. (1997) Kind und Verkehr – alles verkehrt? Kinderspezifische Mechanismen und
Verhaltensmuster als Auslöser für Unfälle im Verkehr, Bericht über die 3. Saarländische
Ökopädiatrie-Tagung „Wohin geht die Fahrt?, Saarbrücken.
Limbourg, M. (1976) Das Verhalten von 4- bis 9jährigen Kindern bei der
Straßenüberquerung. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 23, 666-
677.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 40
www.dacota-project.eu
McLeod, S. (2009) Jean Piaget. [Website]. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012 from
http://www.simplypsychology.org/piaget.html.
Neuman-Opitz, N. (2008). Radfahren in der ersten und zweiten Klasse. Eine empirische
Studie. Disseration zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors (Dr. phil.) der Bergischen
Universität Wuppertal, Bonn.
Nilson, G. (1997) Methods and necessity of exposure data in relation to accident and injury
statistics, IRTAD Special report.
ÖAMTC: www.netzwerk-verkehrserziehung.at/index.php?id=36.
Pease, K. & Preston, B. (1967) Road safety education for young children. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 33, 305-313.
Pieper, W. (1990). Entwicklung der Wahrnehmung. In: Hetzer H., Todt E., Seiffge-Krenke I. &
Arbinger R.: Angewandte Entwicklungspsychologie des Kindes- und Jugendalters, Quelle &
Meyer, Heidelberg, 19-46
Radworkshop. http://www.radworkshop.info/cms/index.php?option=com_content&view
=article&id=15&Itemid=196.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 41
www.dacota-project.eu
Rauh, Wolfgang (2001) Mobilitätsmanagement für Schulen – Wege zur Schule neu
organisieren. – VCÖ 1/2001.
Rauh, W., Fröhlich, M., Maierbrugger, G. (2001). Mobilitätsmanagement für Schulen – Wege
zur Schule neu organisieren, VCÖ Wissenschaft & Verkehr, Wien.
Roberts, I., Norton R. & Jackson, R. (1995) Driveway-related child pedestrian injuries: a
case-control study, Pediatrics 95.
Robinson, D.L. (2006). Do enforced bicycle helmet laws improve public health? No clear
evidence from countries that have enforced the wearing of helmets. In: British Medical
Jorunal, vol. 332, nr. 7543, p. 722-725.
Safekids (2004) Safe Kids Walk This Way. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012 from:
http://www.safekids.org/our-work/programs/walk-this-way/.
Schieber, R. & Thompson, N. (1996) Developmental risk factors for childhood pedestrian
injuries, Injury Prevention 1996,2, p. 228-236.
Spencer, C. & Wholly, H. (2001) Children and the city: a summary of recent environmental
psychology research. Child: Care, Health and Development, Volume 26, Issue 3, pages 181–
198, May 2000.
Sutherland, R. (2011) Perspective is everything [Video Podcast]. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012
from: http://www.ted.com/talks/rory_sutherland_perspective_is_everything.html.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 42
www.dacota-project.eu
SWOV (2012), Fact Sheet: Risky traffic behaviour among young adolescents,
Leidschendam;
http://www.swov.nl/UK/research/newsletter/articles/2012/201209_adolescents.pdf).
SWOV (2010) Fact sheet. Seat belts and child restraint seats, Leidschendam.
SWOV (2009) Fact sheet: Road safety of children in the Netherlands, Leidschendam.
Thomson, J. A., Tolmie, A., Foot, H. C., & McLaren, B. (1996) Child Development and the
Aims of Road Safety Education: A review and Analysis. [PDF]. Road Safety Research Report
No.1, Department of Transport, UK. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012 from:
http://www.worldbank.org/transport/roads/saf_docs/childdev.pdf
Transport, Planning, Infrastructure. (February, 2009) Road Safety: Design & technology.
White, Michelle J., The 'Arms Race' on American Roads: The Effect of Heavy Vehicles on
Traffic Safety and the Failure of Liability Rules (November 2002).
WHO: http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0010/73954/EN63.pdf.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 43
www.dacota-project.eu
World Health Organisation. (n.d.) Children and road traffic injury. Retrieved on 3rd July 2012
from: http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/child/injury/world_report/
Road_traffic_injuries_english.pdf.
Wright, J. & Vliestra, A. (1975) The development of selective attention: From perceptual
exploration to logical search. In: Rease, H. (Ed.). Advances in Child Development and
Behaviour. New York. Academic Press, Vol. 10, 196-236.
Wunsch D., Haindl G., Ausserer K. (2007) Gehen in der Donaustadt. Was das Gehen
attraktiv macht und was es erschwert: Kommunikation mit Bürgerinnen und Bürgern in
ausgewählten Gebieten des XXII. Wiener Gemeindebezirks, im Auftrag der MA 18
Stadtentwicklung und Stadtplanung, Wien.
Project co-financed by the European Commission Directorate General for Mobility and Transport
14/03/2013 Page 44