Ideasthesia
Ideasthesia
Ideasthesia
Contents
Examples and evidence
In normal perception
Implications for development of synesthesia
Implications for art theory
Neurophysiology of ideasthesia
See also
References
External links
For lexical-gustatory synesthesia evidence also points towards ideasthesia: In lexical-gustatory synesthesia, verbalisation of the
stimulus is not necessary for the experience of concurrents. Instead, it is sufficient to activate the concept.[4]
Another case of synesthesia is swimming-style synesthesia in which each swimming style is associated with a vivid experience of
a color.[5][10] These synesthetes do not need to perform the actual movements of a corresponding swimming style. To activate the
concurrent experiences, it is sufficient to activate the concept of a swimming style (e.g., by presenting a photograph of a swimmer
or simply talking about swimming).[11]
It has been argued that grapheme-color synesthesia for geminate consonants also provides evidence for ideasthesia.[12]
In pitch-color synesthesia, the same tone will be associated with different colors depending on how it has been named; do-sharp
(i.e. di) will have colors similar to do (e.g., a reddish color) and re-flat (i.e. ra) will have color similar to that of re (e.g.,
yellowish), although the two classes refer to the same tone.[13] Similar semantic associations have been found between the
acoustic characteristics of vowels and the notion of size.[14]
One-shot synesthesia: There are synesthetic experiences that can occur just once in a lifetime, and are thus dubbed one-shot
synesthesia. Investigation of such cases has indicated that such unique experiences typically occur when a synesthete is involved
in an intensive mental and emotional activity such as making important plans for one's future or reflecting on one's life. It has
been thus concluded that this is also a form of ideasthesia.[15]
In normal perception
Recently, it has been suggested that the Bouba/Kiki phenomenon is a case of ideasthesia.[16][17][18] Most people will agree that
the star-shaped object on the left is named Kiki and the round one on the right Bouba.[19][20] It has been assumed that these
associations come from direct connections between visual and auditory cortices.[20] For example, according to that hypothesis,
representations of sharp inflections in the star-shaped object would be physically connected to the representations of sharp
inflection in the sound of Kiki. However, Gomez et al.[16][21] have shown that Kiki/Bouba associations are much richer as either
word and either image is associated semantically to a number of concepts such as white or black color, feminine vs. masculine,
cold vs. hot, and others. These sound-shape associations seem to be related through a large overlap between semantic networks of
Kiki and star-shape on one hand, and Bouba and round-shape on the other hand. For example, both Kiki and star-shape are clever,
small, thin and nervous. This indicates that behind Kiki-Bouba effect lies a rich semantic network. In other words, our sensory
experience is largely determined by the meaning that we assign to stimuli. Food description and wine tasting is another domain in
which ideasthetic association between flavor and other modalities such as shape
may play an important role.[22] These semantic-like relations play a role in
successful marketing; the name of a product should match its other
characteristics.[23]
Ideasthesia theory of art may be used for psychological studies of aesthetics. It may also help explain classificatory disputes about
art as its main tenet is that experience of art can only be individual, depending on person's unique knowledge, experiences and
history.[30] There could exist no general classification of art satisfactorily applicable to each and all individuals.
Neurophysiology of ideasthesia
Ideasthesia is congruent with the theory of brain functioning known as practopoiesis.[31]. According to that theory, concepts are
not an emergent property of highly developed, specialized neuronal networks in the brain, as is usually assumed; rather, concepts
are proposed to be fundamental to the very adaptive principles by which living systems and the brain operate.[32]
See also
Aesthetics
Charles Bonnet syndrome
Classificatory disputes about art
Consciousness
Explanatory gap
Mind–body problem
New Mysterianism
Perception
Phantom eye syndrome
Phantom limb
Philosophical zombie
Practopoiesis
Qualia
Sentience
Synesthesia
References
1. Nikolić, D. (2009) Is synaesthesia actually ideaesthesia? An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon.
Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Synaesthesia, Science & Art, Granada, Spain, April 26–29,
2009.
2. Dixon, M.J., Smilek, D., Duffy, P.L., Zanna, P. M., Merikle, P. M. (2006) The Role of Meaning in Grapheme-Colour
Synaesthesia, Cortex 42: 243-252.
3. Mroczko A., T. Metzinger, W. Singer, D. Nikolić (2009) Immediate transfer of synesthesia to a novel inducer.
Journal of Vision, 9: 2521-2528.
4. Simner, J.; Ward, J. (2006) The taste of words on the tip of the tongue, Nature 444: 438.
5. Nikolić, D., U.M. Jürgens, N. Rothen, B. Meier, A. Mroczko (2011) Swimming-style synesthesia. Cortex,
47(7):874-879.
6. Chiou, R., Rich N.A. (2014) The role of conceptual knowledge in understanding synaesthesia: Evaluating
contemporary findings from a ‘hub-and-spoke’perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5: 105.
7. Curwen, C. (2018). Music-colour synaesthesia: Concept, context and qualia. Consciousness and Cognition, 61,
94-106.
8. Mroczko-Wąsowicz, A., Nikolić D. (2014) Semantic mechanisms may be responsible for developing synesthesia.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:509. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00509
9. Jürgens U.M., Nikolić D. (2012) Ideaesthesia: Conceptual processes assign similar colours to similar shapes.
Translational Neuroscience, 3(1): 22-27.
10. Mroczko-Wąsowicz, Aleksandra, and Markus Werning. Synesthesia, sensory-motor contingency, and semantic
emulation: how swimming style-color synesthesia challenges the traditional view of synesthesia. Frontiers in
Psychology 3 (2012).
11. Jarrett, C. (2014). Great myths of the brain. John Wiley & Sons.
12. Weaver, D.F., Hawco C.L.A. (2015) Geminate consonant grapheme-colour synaesthesia (ideaesthesia). BMC
Neurology, 15:112.
13. Itoh, K., Sakata, H., Kwee, I. L., & Nakada, T. (2017). Musical pitch classes have rainbow hues in pitch class-
color synesthesia. Scientific reports, 7(1), 17781.
14. Hoshi, H., Kwon, N., Akita, K., & Auracher, J. (2019). Semantic Associations Dominate Over Perceptual
Associations in Vowel–Size Iconicity. i-Perception, 10(4), 2041669519861981.
15. Kirschner, A., & Nikolić, D. (2017). One-shot synesthesia. Translational Neuroscience, 8(1), 167-175.
16. Gómez Milán, E., Iborra, O., de Córdoba, M.J., Juárez-Ramos V., Rodríguez Artacho, M.A., Rubio, J.L. (2013)
The Kiki-Bouba effect: A case of personification and ideaesthesia. The Journal of Consciousness Studies. 20(1-
2): pp. 84-102.
17. Shukla, A. (2016). The Kiki-Bouba paradigm: Where senses meet and greet. Indian Journal of Mental Health,
3(3), 240-252.
18. De Carolis, L., Marsico, E., Arnaud, V., & Coupé, C. (2018). Assessing sound symbolism: Investigating phonetic
forms, visual shapes and letter fonts in an implicit bouba-kiki experimental paradigm. PloS one, 13(12),
e0208874.https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0208874
19. Köhler, W (1929). Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liveright.
20. Ramachandran, VS & Hubbard, EM (2001) Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language.
Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(12): 3–34.
21. Milán, Emilio Gómez, Oscar Iborra Martínez, and María José de Córdoba Serrano. El Universo Kiki-Bouba:
Ideaestesia, Empatía y Neuromárketing. Fundación Internacional artecittà, 2014.
22. Spence, Charles, and Ophelia Deroy. On the shapes of flavours: A review of four hypotheses. Theoria et Historia
Scientiarum 10 (2014): 207-238.
23. Bridger, D. (2015). Decoding the irrational consumer: How to commission, run and generate insights from
neuromarketing research. Kogan Page Publishers.
24. Rapp, B., & Caramazza, A. (1997). From graphemes to abstract letter shapes: levels of representation in written
spelling. Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, 23(4), 1130.
25. Gsöllpointner, Katharina. "DIE KUNST DER SINNE–DIE SINNE DER KUNST." Exploring Cybernetics: Kybernetik
im interdisziplinären Diskurs (2015): 137-165.
26. Albertazzi, Liliana, et al. "The hue of angles—was Kandinsky right?." Art & Perception 3.1 (2015): 81-92.
27. Mărginaş, Raluca. "The Spurious Case of Synesthesia in the Popular Arts." Ekphrasis 1 (2012): 144-151.
28. Johnson, A. (2017). Hendrick ter Brugghen's Musicians and the Engagement of the Viewer. Temple University.
29. Prendergast, J. (2018). Grinding the moor–ideasthesia and narrative. New Writing, 1-17.
30. Nikolić D. (2016) Ideasthesia and art. In: Gsöllpointner, Katharina, et al. (eds.). 2016. Digital Synesthesia. A
Model for the Aesthetics of Digital Art. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter (http://ieet.org/index.php/IEET/print/11666)
31. van Leeuwen, T. M., Singer, W., & Nikolić, D. (2015) The merit of synesthesia for consciousness research.
Frontiers in psychology, 6, 1850.
32. Nikolić, D. (2015). Practopoiesis: Or how life fosters a mind. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 373, 40-61.
External links
TED Ed video (http://ed.ted.com/lessons/ideasthesia-how-do-ideas-feel-danko-nikolic) explaining ideasthesia
Danko Nikolić's website on Ideasthesia (http://www.danko-nikolic.com/synesthesia-ideasthesia/)
Swimming-style synesthesia (https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn20182-ideas-conjure-up-colour-for-swimmin
g-synaesthete.html) described by New Scientist
Audio Criticism & Ideasthesia (https://depaul.digication.com/the_future_of_museums/Michelle_Mustin_Super_Un
titled_Audio_Criticism_and)
A YouTube video (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=k0hHSa3Lpxw) of a lecture on ideasthesia at
SoundThinking conference
Ideasthesia (http://www.somarts.org/ideasthesia/): A Multisensory Installation
Totem And Token (https://medium.com/@TotemAndToken/ideasthesia-art-genius-insanity-and-semiotics-7fff1896
34e2) on ideasthesia
Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. By using
this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia
Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.