University of Groningen: Jin, Yinxing
University of Groningen: Jin, Yinxing
IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to
cite from it. Please check the document version below.
Document Version
Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record
Publication date:
2016
Copyright
Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the
author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).
Take-down policy
If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately
and investigate your claim.
Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the
number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.
This chapter reports a study that investigates and compares the effects of foreign language
proficiency, social status of a learner’s family, self-esteem, and competitiveness on foreign
language anxiety. Chinese university students (N=146), who were learning Japanese and
English, participated in this study. Social status data were collected once with the Social
Status Scale. Other variables were measured twice over a two-month interval, using the
Competitiveness Index, the Self-esteem Scale, the English/Japanese Classroom Anxiety Scale,
and the English/Japanese Proficiency Scale. Results showed that foreign language proficiency,
competitiveness, and self-esteem all significantly and negatively predicted foreign language
anxiety levels. Foreign language proficiency was the best predictor, followed by self-esteem,
then competitiveness. Social status was not related to foreign language anxiety, either directly
or indirectly.
1
This chapter is based on Jin, Y. X., De Bot, K., & Keijzer, M. (2015b). Factors associated
with foreign language anxiety: A study of Chinese university learners of Japanese and
English. Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics, 4, 67-85.
24
2.1. Introduction
Since the mid-1980s, there has been a burgeoning interest in foreign language (FL) anxiety
among second language researchers. It is now widely accepted that FL anxiety impairs FL
learning (e.g., Horwitz, 2001; MacIntyre & Gardner, 1994b; Woodrow, 2006). The
consequences of FL anxiety even extend beyond the classroom, affecting the continuous use
of a second language after leaving school (Dewaele, 2007b). Hence, measures should be
taken to lessen learners’ anxiety and the identification of the factors underlying FL anxiety
FL anxiety has been found to be linked to FL proficiency (e.g., Hewitt & Stephenson,
2012; Zhang, 2013). Other variables which have been associated with FL anxiety include
learners’ personality traits (Dewaele, 2013). Among them, low self-esteem has been
established as a source of FL anxiety (e.g., MacIntyre, 1999; Zare & Riasati, 2012). In
contrast, much less well understood is the role that another personality trait, i.e.,
competitiveness, plays in FL anxiety, though competitiveness has been singled out by Young
(1991) as a crucial factor underlying FL anxiety (the other is low self-esteem), as past studies
have produced mixed results. The contribution of competitiveness to FL anxiety thus needs to
be further examined. Similarly, a learner’s family’s social status has been mentioned as a
predictor of FL anxiety, but not investigated in detail. In short, this factor too necessitates
further studies, also in line with Yan and Horwitz’s (2008) suggestion that FL anxiety studies
In this study, we examined the roles of the family’s social status and competitiveness
in FL anxiety in a sample of 146 Chinese university students learning English and Japanese,
also introduced the two much more elaborately assessed factors of FL proficiency and
has not been attempted before, but is vital, because it helps to deepen our understanding of
those indicating a stronger predictive power. Besides, previous research has typically looked
at anxiety-provoking factors at a single time point. In this study, the 146 participants were
tested at two different time points in both English and Japanese learning contexts. Thus with
multiple data points from two learning contexts, more accurate conclusions in relation to the
In the mid-1980s, a unique type of anxiety specific to learning and/or using a foreign or
second language was identified and subsequently labeled foreign language (FL) anxiety
(Horwitz, Horwitz, & Cope, 1986). Earlier, psychologists had distinguished three types of
anxiety, i.e., trait anxiety, state anxiety, and situation-specific anxiety. Trait anxiety is "a more
event, such as public speaking (MacIntyre & Gardner, 1994a). FL anxiety is thus a situation-
specific anxiety (Dewaele, 2007b; Horwitz, 2001). Several researchers have attempted to
define FL anxiety (Horwitz et al., 1986; MacIntyre, 1999; Williams, 1991). The most
extensively quoted definition is the one formulated by Horwitz et al. (1986), who conceive of
FL anxiety as "a distinct complex of self-perceptions, beliefs, feelings, and behaviors related
to classroom language learning arising from the uniqueness of the language learning process"
(p. 128). Horwitz et al. (1986) also developed the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety
Scale (FLCAS) to measure the degree of FL anxiety. Horwitz et al.’s definition and
26
subsequent construction of the FLCAS has been a milestone in anxiety studies. Not only does
it help to understand the nature of anxiety related to FL learning and distinguish this type of
anxiety from personality anxiety and other academic anxiety forms, but it can also help to get
a firm grip on past work in this area, as FL anxiety studies can now be better grouped and
compared (Horwitz, 2001). The FLCAS as a standard instrument makes the identification of
anxious learners more feasible, irrespective of whether these learners show explicit clinical
anxiety symptoms.
To date, studies on FL anxiety have spanned diverse topics within the realm of second
language studies, such as the effects of FL anxiety on FL learning, the sources and stability of
FL anxiety, and the relationships of FL anxiety to other learner variables. The identification
of the sources of anxiety in particular has been viewed as one of the key issues (Ellis, 2008).
actually tested or elicited via self-reports (e.g., Hewitt & Stephenson, 2012; Liu & Jackson,
2008; Zhang, 2013), suggesting the likely reciprocal effects between FL anxiety and
proficiency variables.
claims that low self-esteem is a significant anxiety source. FL specialists like Krashen,
Hadley, Terrell, and Rardin have also agreed with the role of this personal characteristic in FL
anxiety (Young, 1992), which has been endorsed by quantitative studies. Liu and Zhang’s
(2008) study of 934 first-year non-English majors from three Chinese universities, for one,
found that three FLCAS factors, namely fear of negative evaluation, communication
apprehension, and test anxiety, showed a significantly negative correlation with self-esteem
as measured by Rosenberg’s (1965) Self-esteem Scale (SES). The entire FLCAS scores were
also significantly negatively related to the SES scores. In Zare and Riasati (2012), self-esteem
was strongly linked with the FLCAS scores, r(106)=-.74, p<.001. However, the relationship
27
between self-worth and FL anxiety was weak in Onwuegbuzie, Bailey, and Daley (1999),
variation in FL anxiety. Onwuegbuzie et al. (1999) thus concluded that “both self-esteem and
self-concept play a role in determining levels of foreign language anxiety” (p. 229).
Another personality trait Young (1991) has singled out as one crucial source of FL
anxiety is competitiveness. Ellis (2008) and Tóth (2007) have also suggested that learners’
competitive nature can lead to their FL anxiety. However, the function of competitiveness in
FL anxiety is rather vague, despite these claims, which largely draw on Bailey's (1983) work.
Retrospecting her own diary entries and others', Bailey (1983) found anxiety to arise when
premature to draw a definite conclusion. This is because a competitive nature may not be the
sole cause of competitive comparison. Other factors likely include a competitive classroom
environment. There are explicit clues about the existence of classroom competition in the
from environment-related behavior, or constitutes the interaction of both. The conclusion that
competitiveness that indeed refers to competitive comparison in Bailey (1983), rather than a
needed. Diary studies are ideally suited for a "hypothesis-generating, not hypothesis-testing,
undertaking" (Long, 1980, p. 27). Unfortunately, few quantitative studies have addressed this
issue and the studies that are available lead to additional confusion. For example,
relationship in Tóth (2007) was significant and positive. Competitiveness was established as
Furthermore, FL anxiety has been shown to be related to the family’s social status as a
societal variable, rather than just to FL proficiency and personality traits. In Dewaele’s (2002)
study of 100 pupils (98 native speakers of Dutch) in their last year of secondary school, the
family’s social class -indexed by the highest educational level attained by the parents- was
in French. Noteworthy is that Dewaele’s (2002) study has -to our knowledge- been the only
one to investigate the relationship between FL anxiety and the family’s social status. The
great lack of empirical studies endorses the necessity to further look at this societal factor
before affirming its role in FL anxiety. Yan and Horwitz (2008) also suggested that studies of
In sum, more research is needed into the sources of FL anxiety, as the relationships
between FL anxiety and a number of potential anxiety contributors, like the family’s social
status and competitiveness, remain to be further clarified. Studies that compare the effects of
different variables on FL anxiety should continue. This is because FL anxiety arises from a
complex of factors (Rodríguez & Abreu, 2003; Young, 1991). Teachers are thus faced with
great difficulties in dealing with all anxiety-provoking factors. Hence, it is practical and
effective to focus on the more prominent factors that can be established by studies comparing
variables in terms of their effects on FL anxiety. In addition, our understanding of the nature
of FL anxiety can be enhanced by identifying the more prominent factors. In the current study
29
involving 146 Chinese university students, we aim to contribute to the field of FL anxiety by
investigating and comparing the effects of four factors: FL proficiency, the family’s social
status, self-esteem, and competitiveness on FL anxiety. Such a comparison has not been done
2.3. Methodology
In this study, Chinese students learning English and Japanese were surveyed twice, using
anxiety within English and Japanese learning contexts and at two time points. It should be
noted that two self-report instruments were used to elicit the participants’ English and
Japanese proficiency, rather than objective proficiency tests. This approach was chosen
because in the project to which this study belongs, we administered many questionnaires
containing quite a number of items to the participants at each time, with a view to answering
several FL anxiety-related questions. If proficiency had been assessed with objective tests,
the students may have been greatly discouraged due to the heavy workload and may not have
cooperated fully in the data collection phase. The details of participants, instruments, and data
2.3.2. Participants
Participants constituted 146 Japanese majors recruited from six Japanese classes at three
Chinese universities (125 females and 21 males), who were also learning English. Their ages
ranged from 17 to 23 (M=19.57, SD=1.00). The students were all native speakers of Chinese.
They were attending more than one Japanese course and Japanese teachers were not identical
for different classes within the same university. However, the students from each university
were taking a compulsory English course as a group, taught by one and the same teacher. In
fact, the 146 students came from three English classes. They had learned English for 4.5 to
13.5 years up to Time 1 (M=9.05, SD=1.88), but virtually none of the students (n=145) had
2.3.3. Instruments
Seven questionnaires were administered: the Demographic Information Index (DIQ), the
Social Status Scale (SSS), the Competitiveness Index (CI) (Houston, Harris, McIntire, &
Francis, 2002), the Self-esteem Scale (SES) (Rosenberg, 1965), the Foreign Language
Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS) (Horwitz et al., 1986), the English Proficiency Scale
(EPS), and the Japanese Proficiency Scale (JPS). The DIQ, the SSS, the EPS, and the JPS
were developed in Chinese by the current researchers. The CI, the SES, and the FLCAS that
are originally in English were translated into Chinese, so that the participants could
understand all items well. The translation procedure was as follows: the Chinese researcher in
this study translated the questionnaires into Chinese (Guo and Wu [2008] was referred to in
the process of translating the FLCAS. In addition, the online translation was also referred to
cross-checked by a senior university student enrolled in a Chinese program, who had a good
command of English. The two translators discussed minor discrepancies until these were
resolved. The resulting Chinese translations were pretested as part of a pilot procedure for
2.3.3.1. The Demographic Information Questionnaire and the Social Status Scale
The DIQ has eight items pertaining to each participant’s name, age, gender, residential
location, the duration of Japanese and English learning, and parental education. Those for
residential location (one item) and parental education (two items for mother and father,
respectively) constitute the SSS. Both residential location and parental education have four
school, 2=junior school, 3=senior school, 4=college. As response options to the SSS items
are not identical, the participants’ scores on each item were standardized into z-scores when
estimating the internal reliability of the SSS. The aggregated z-scores on the three items index
The CI has two subscales: the Enjoyment of Competition and the Contentiousness subscales.
In total, the CI contains 14 items, all following a 5-point Likert format (1=strongly disagree,
2=disagree, 3=neither agree nor disagree, 4=agree, 5=strongly agree). The minimum
obtainable score on the CI is 14 and the maximum is 70. Higher scores represent more
intense competitiveness. Sample items include I like competition and I will do almost
The original SES consists of 10 items, all using a 4-point Likert format. In this study, the
4-point Likert format was altered to a 5-point Likert format (1=strongly disagree, 2=
disagree, 3=neither agree nor disagree, 4=agree, 5=strongly agree) by inserting a neutral
option, to be consistent with the CI and to increase the sensitivity of the scale. The minimum
score on the resulting scale is 10 and the maximum is 50. High scores suggest higher levels of
self-esteem. Two sample items include: I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an
equal basis with others and All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure.
The FLCAS contains 33 items, all based on five Likert-type responses (1=strongly disagree,
2=disagree, 3=neither agree nor disagree, 4=agree, 5=strongly agree). The score range spans
from 33 to 165, with higher scores indicating higher levels of FL anxiety. In this study, the
FLCAS was used to measure anxiety in the English and Japanese classroom, so foreign
language in the FLCAS was replaced with Japanese and English. The scales for the English
and Japanese classroom were respectively labeled ECAS (English Classroom Anxiety Scale)
and JCAS (Japanese Classroom Anxiety Scale). Two exemplar items from the ECAS and the
JCAS are: I always feel that the other students speak English better than I do and I get
nervous when my Japanese teacher asks questions which I haven’t prepared in advance.
2.3.3.5. The English Proficiency Scale and the Japanese Proficiency Scale
The EPS and the JPS are self-assessments of English and Japanese proficiency, and consist of
four subscales for listening, speaking, reading, and writing proficiency. The two FL
proficiency scales comprise 20 items (5 items for each subscale), all following a 4-point
Likert format (1=almost impossible, 2=difficult, 3=a bit difficult, 4=easy). The scores range
33
from 20 to 80. Higher scores represent higher English or Japanese proficiency. Sample items
from the EPS and the JPS include: I can deliver a 2-minute speech in Japanese on a familiar
topic after a short preparation and I can understand different viewpoints and attitudes in the
English comments on current affairs. The EPS was constructed based on the Curriculum
Standard for Senior High School English (Experimental) (MOE, 2003) and furthermore is a
test of intermediate level of English. The JPS was developed according to the Curriculum
Standard for Japanese Majors at Elementary Level in Higher Education (MOE, 2001) and is a
measure of elementary level of Japanese (two items were adapted from Xu [2010]). The two
scales were validated against teachers’ ratings of students’ proficiency on a 5-point Likert
scale (1=poor, 2=fair, 3=good, 4=very good, 5=excellent), as introduced in the Procedures
section. The score range for teachers’ ratings is between 4 and 20. The results of this
validation are reported below together with the reliability levels of the EPS and the JPS.
2.3.4. Procedures
Data collection proceeded in three steps. Step 1 was a pilot study conducted during regular
class hours, which itself comprised two parts. In Part 1, two intact classes containing 41
first-year students of Japanese at a university in West China’s Shaan’xi Province were tested.
The students were also taking a compulsory English course, similar to those in the full
surveys. A few Chinese words in the scales, except the SSS, were adjusted after being tested
in Class 1, and the resultant scales were then retested in Class 2. It should be noted that no
student in Class 2 reported difficulties in understanding the items in the scales. In addition, as
the JCAS and the ECAS differ only in the wording of Japanese or English, only the JCAS
was tested in the pilot study. In Part 2 of the pilot study, the two proficiency scales that had
shown satisfactory internal consistency in the test for Class 2 were administered to 27
more closely resembles -in terms of level- the universities from which the 146 participants
were recruited. Obtaining valid results in this context would thus better indicate the effective
use of the scales in the full surveys. Teachers’ ratings of the students’ Japanese and English
listening, speaking, reading, and writing proficiency were also collected (one teacher for each
language). There were 26 valid self-evaluations for the JPS and the EPS that hence were
validated by correlating these 26 valid self-reports and the teacher’s ratings. In short, the
result of pilot study (checking internal reliability) of the SSS was based on the samples in
Class 1 in Part 1; those of the JCAS, the CI, the SES, the EPS, and the JPS were based on
Class 2.
Step 2 of this study’s design involved two full surveys, administered to the 146
participants at two time points across two months in an out-of-class session with no teacher
present, using the same scales and following the same procedures. At each time, the
participants from the same university completed the full set of questionnaires in a classroom
at the same time, with the questionnaires following a set order: the DIQ (the SSS included),
the CI, the SES, the JPS, the EPS, the JCAS, and the ECAS (the DIQ was excluded at Time
2). Questionnaires were immediately checked after being collected at each time for
unanswered items. When missing items were found, those subjects would be traced to obtain
their answers. Step 3 of this study’s design was data registration during which the
2.4. Results
Table 2.1 shows the reliability levels of the various scales used in this study.
Table 2.1 shows that the scales used in this study achieved satisfactory reliability
levels. As the participants’ sociodemographic backgrounds were stable over the two-month
interval, the SSS was only administered at Time 1. As a result, a test-retest coefficient is not
available. To further check the convergence of two social status indexes, i.e., residential
location and parental education, a simple correlation analysis was performed after observing
scatterplots. Results indicated that the two indexes were highly correlated, r(144)=.59,
p<.001.
Furthermore, the results for the CI, SES, and the FLCAS based on the current samples
were in line with those obtained in prior studies, particularly their internal reliability. For
instance, the CI attained an internal reliability coefficient of .87 in Houston et al. (2002). Its
test-retest reliability was .85 as reported by Harris and Houston (2010) in which the test
interval ranged from 18 to 34 days and 91% of the participants returned to retest after 4-5
36
weeks. Fleming and Courtney (1984) found that internal reliability was .88 and that the
test-retest reliability was .82 over a 1-week interval for the SES. The internal reliability of the
FLCAS has typically been high in previous studies, such as .94 in Aida (1994), and Park and
French (2013). As for its test-retest reliability, Horwitz (1986) obtained a coefficient of .83 on
In addition, the results of the EPS and the JPS validation by correlating students’ self-
reports and teachers’ ratings were also satisfactory. Correlation was .71 for Japanese
proficiency and .63 for English proficiency, suggesting that the two scales can effectively
Table 2.2 presents the descriptive results of measurements, including mean scores and
standard deviations.
As can be seen, the participants -on average- came from less developed areas (home
location: 1=village, 2=township, 3=county, 4=prefecture city or above). Their parents had not
received much education (parental education: 1=primary school, 2=junior school, 3=senior
school, 4=college), though fathers overall had a higher educational background than mothers.
Generally speaking, these students came from families of relatively low social status.
37
In addition, the samples showed moderate competitiveness and self-esteem at the two
time points (maximum score: 70 for the CI and 50 for the SES), as well as a moderate level of
English and Japanese anxiety (165 for the FLCAS). These students generally rated their
English and Japanese proficiency highly (80 for the EPS and JPS).
Tables 2.3 and 2.4). The variable of the family’s social status is not included in the tables,
because it clearly showed no relation with all the other variables investigated in this study, as
suggested by the correlation analyses which were carried out after observing scatterplots. As
a result, social status was excluded from the ensuing regression analyses as well.
As can be seen from Tables 2.3 and 2.4, self-esteem and competitiveness were
positively associated, which in their turn were positively correlated with English and
38
Japanese proficiency, but negatively with English and Japanese anxiety. FL proficiency and
FL anxiety were negatively related to each other, indicating a stronger relationship than that
anxiety was investigated, using a standard multiple regression procedure1. Following the
regression analysis, a check was done to see whether there were cases that had standardized
residual values that fell out of the range -3 to 3, but no outliers were identified. The
violated in any of the computations. Multicollinearity was not attested either. The regression
As can be seen in Table 2.5, the model including English proficiency, competitiveness,
and self-esteem was highly significant and explained 38.1% of the English anxiety variance.
predicted English anxiety. In other words, students with a high level of English proficiency or
with a strong self-esteem showed a lower level of English anxiety. Competitive learners
tended to experience less English anxiety than their non-competitive counterparts. English
proficiency was shown to be the most prominent predictor, followed by self-esteem and,
lastly, competitiveness.
39
Table 2.6 shows that the regression of English proficiency, self-esteem, and
competitiveness at Time 2 was significant and 34.5% of the English anxiety variance was
explained by the overall model. English proficiency and self-esteem were found to be
significant and negative predictors of English anxiety. English proficiency contributed more
to English anxiety than self-esteem. Competitiveness did not significantly predict English
anxiety anymore.
The predictive power of self-esteem, competitiveness, and Japanese proficiency for Japanese
anxiety was also investigated using standard regression analyses. For the regression analysis,
no outliers were identified on the basis of the same method as reported for English anxiety: it
was checked whether some cases had standardized residual values above 3 or below -3.
residuals were not found to be violated. Multicollinearity was not found either. The results are
Table 2.7 shows that the regression of Japanese proficiency, self-esteem, and
competitiveness at Time 1 was highly significant and the model explained 38.5% of the
Japanese anxiety variance. Japanese proficiency, competitiveness, and self-esteem were all
40
significant and negative predictors of Japanese anxiety. Among them, Japanese proficiency
was the most prominent predictor. Contrary to the findings for English anxiety,
In Table 2.8, the results of the regression analysis for Japanese anxiety at Time 2 are
presented. As shown, 39.2% of the variance in Japanese anxiety was explained by the model
including Japanese proficiency, self-esteem, and competitiveness, which all significantly and
no longer a better predictor than self-esteem. As in all the previous tests, FL proficiency was
This study aimed to explore the roles that competitiveness and the family’s social status play
in FL anxiety and compare the contributions of FL proficiency, the family’s social status,
with what Houston, Harris, Moore, and Brummett (2005) reported on the basis of 61 Chinese
individualism-collectivism continuum and the Chinese are often labeled collectivists. One of
(Grimm, Church, Katigbak, & Reyes, 1999). However, the Chinese students in this study tend
to compete, rather than maintain a social harmony, with others2. This finding bears out Green,
The mean EPS scores showed little fluctuation over time, but the JPS mean indicated
a striking increase. Japanese proficiency thus showed a clear development among students,
but English proficiency did not3. The participants were Japanese majors who devoted many
more hours to Japanese than to English learning. Being more fossilized learners of English
due to a prolonged exposure to English learning, they needed to log considerably more hours
to advance to the next stage of English proficiency. The unbalanced time and energy devoted
to Japanese and English learning can partly explain the different development in English and
Japanese proficiency.
anxiety in each regression analysis, further substantiating that FL anxiety is closely related to
FL learning achievement. Social status was not found to relate to FL anxiety, either directly or
indirectly. Nonetheless, it does not automatically follow that the family’s social status does
not contribute to FL anxiety at all. The participants had studied in their universities for around
six months when data collection took place. They had adjusted relatively well to campus life
and, more importantly, FL classes. If the surveys had been administered earlier, perhaps a
closer relationship would have been found between the societal and affective variables.
Moreover, the family’s social status may be interconnected with other psychological or
situational factors through which learners’ levels of anxiety are affected. As it is, research into
the relationship between the family’s social status and FL anxiety is only in its infancy. More
studies are needed to clarify the effects of parents’ social status on learners’ anxiety reactions.
self-esteem should not be interpreted as parents’ social status not affecting their children’s
personality. The family’s social status as an element of societal context may have already
contributed to the personality formation at an early age, of the now adult learners. As Dörnyei
42
(2005) put forward, “It is evident that the potential determinants of an adult’s personality
include both environmental factors related to the nature of the home in which the person was
raised as a child, and biological factors related to hereditary factors associated with the
FL anxiety in Tóth (2007). Young (1991), Ellis (2008), and Tóth’s (2007) assumption that
competitiveness is a source of FL anxiety was thus not supported by this study. Rather than
across the two time points and learning contexts. Nevertheless, it should be pointed out that
this study -to our knowledge- is the first to use Houston et al.’s (2002) Competitiveness Index
Hence, further studies are needed to corroborate its findings. In addition, it is possible that the
differences in study design led to the contradictory results for the competitiveness-anxiety
relationship between Onwuegbuzie et al. (1999), Tóth (2007), and our study, for instance
differences in the background of participants and sample size. The other explanation is that
not measured, for instance classroom variables, and consequently indicated different
students who value themselves more highly were shown to experience less FL anxiety. The
findings for self-esteem endorsed those reported by Liu and Zhang (2008), and Zare and
Riasati (2012). Of the two personality attributes addressed in the current study, self-esteem
of the possible interplay between these variables. Thus, it means that each of the three
variables may impose an influence on learners’ anxiety levels through the other two, although
a direct effect also existed. The interconnections further point to the complexity of FL
anxiety-related factors.
The findings of this study have some pedagogical implications. We call on teachers to
degree of self-esteem and competitiveness. This study found self-esteem and competitiveness
to negatively predict FL anxiety, suggesting that learners with low competitiveness and
self-esteem are the likeliest candidates to exhibit a higher level of FL anxiety. FL anxiety has
been widely accepted as an interfering variable in FL learning, which has made it crucial for
teachers to identify the self-deprecatory or less competitive learners and take measures to
Notes
1. Before we carried out regression analyses, the ECAS and the JCAS scores were compared
across classes at two times. Comparisons were performed among six classes for Japanese
anxiety, but three classes for English anxiety, as the participants in six Japanese classes were
actually from three English classes, as mentioned the Methodology section. A Kruskal-Wallis
test was conducted for Japanese anxiety, but a one-way ANOVA was used for English anxiety,
after checking the assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances. Results showed
that Japanese anxiety did not significantly differ across six classes at the two moments in
time: x2(5, n=146)=4.12, p=.52 at Time 1 and x2(5, n=146) =3.89, p=.57 at Time 2. Similar
44
results were rendered for English anxiety: F(2, 143)=.30, p=.74 at Time 1 and F(2, 143)=.41,
p=.66 at Time 2. As a consequence of these results, the class variable was not entered into the
2. The scores of the CI range from 14 to 70. At the two time points, there were only
18 (Time 1) and 21 (Time 2) students obtaining scores below 35, further supporting the
conclusion elicited by the mean scores that these Chinese students tend to compete.
3. There were 86, 12, and 48 students who respectively indicated increase, no change,
and decrease in Japanese proficiency across two time points. The numbers were 71, 15, and
60 respectively for English proficiency. The findings suggest a clear pattern of increase in
Japanese proficiency among the samples, but English proficiency did not show a similar
improvement. Moreover, inferential analysis showed that the increase in Japanese proficiency
was significant, t(145)=-3.39, p<.005, d=.28, but no significance level was found in English