The Origins of Ethics: Mythical Accounts Introduction of Moral Codes
The Origins of Ethics: Mythical Accounts Introduction of Moral Codes
The Origins of Ethics: Mythical Accounts Introduction of Moral Codes
Mythical accounts
Nonhuman behaviour
Because, for obvious reasons, there is no historical record of a human society in the period before it had
any standards of right and wrong, history cannot reveal the origins of morality. Nor is anthropology of
any help, because all the human societies that have been studied so far had their own forms of morality
(except perhaps in the most extreme circumstances). Fortunately, another mode of inquiry is available.
Because living in social groups is a characteristic that humans share with many other animal species—
including their closest relatives, the apes—presumably the common ancestor of humans and apes also
lived in social groups. Here, then, in the social behaviour of nonhuman animals and in the theory of
evolution that explains such behaviour may be found the origins of human morality.
Social life, even for nonhuman animals, requires constraints on behaviour. No group can stay together if
its members make frequent, unrestrained attacks on each other. With some exceptions, social animals
generally either refrain altogether from attacking other members of the social group or, if an attack does
take place, do not make the ensuing struggle a fight to the death—it is over when the weaker animal
shows submissive behaviour. It is not difficult to see analogies here with human moral codes. The
parallels, however, go much further than this. Like humans, social animals may behave in ways
that benefit other members of the group at some cost or risk to themselves. Male baboons threaten
predators and cover the rear as the troop retreats. Wolves and wild dogs take meat back to members of
the pack not present at the kill. Gibbons and chimpanzees with food will, in response to a gesture, share
their food with other members of the group. Dolphins support other sick or injured dolphins, swimming
under them for hours at a time and pushing them to the surface so they can breathe.
It may be thought that the existence of such apparently altruistic behaviour is odd,
for evolutionary theory states that those who do not struggle to survive and reproduce will be eliminated
through natural selection. Research in evolutionary theory applied to social behaviour, however, has
shown that evolution need not be so ruthless. Some of this altruistic behaviour is explained
by kin selection. The most obvious examples are those in which parents make sacrifices for their
offspring. If wolves help their cubs to survive, it is more likely that genetic characteristics, including the
characteristic of helping their own cubs, will spread through further generations of wolves.