Parenting Ecologica
Parenting Ecologica
Parenting Ecologica
An Ecological Perspective
Second Edition
MONOGRAPHS IN PARENTING
Marc H. Bornstein, Series Editor
For more information on LEA titles, please contact Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates, Publishers, at www.erlbaum.com.
PARENTING
An Ecological Perspective
Second Edition
Edited by
Tom Luster
Michigan State University
Lynn Okagaki
Purdue University
HQ755.8.P379123 2005
649′.1—dc22 2004065021
CIP
Introduction xi
Tom Luster and Lynn Okagaki
vii
viii CONTENTS
Parenting is fundamental to the survival and success of the human race. Ev-
eryone who has ever lived has had parents, and most adults in the world be-
come parents. Opinions about parenting abound, but surprisingly little
solid scientific information or considered reflection exists about parenting.
Monographs in Parenting intends to redress this imbalance. The chief aim of
this series is to provide a forum for extended and integrated treatments of
fundamental and challenging contemporary topics in parenting. Each vol-
ume treats a different perspective on parenting and is self-contained, yet
the series as a whole endeavors to enhance and interrelate studies in
parenting by bringing shared perspectives to bear on a variety of concerns
prominent in parenting theory, research, and application. As a conse-
quence of its structure and scope, Monographs in Parenting will appeal, indi-
vidually or as a group, to scientists, professionals, and parents alike. Re-
flecting the nature and intent of this series, contributing authors are drawn
from a broad spectrum of the humanities and sciences—anthropology to
zoology—with representational emphasis placed on authorities actively
contributing to the contemporary literature in parenting.
Parenting is a job whose primary object of attention and action is the
child—children do not and cannot grow up as solitary individuals—but
parenting is also a status in the life course with consequences for parents
themselves. In this forum, parenting is defined by all of children’s principal
caregivers and their many modes of caregiving. Monographs in Parenting en-
compass these central themes in parenting.
ix
x SERIES FOREWORD
WHO PARENTS?
—Marc H. Bornstein
Series Editor
Introduction
Tom Luster
Lynn Okagaki
xi
xii INTRODUCTION
parenting as children manage to provide very supportive care for their chil-
dren while others with similar backgrounds struggle in their parenting
role? Chapter 3 focuses on adolescent mothers and their children. Al-
though intuitively it seems reasonable that age and maturity are important
influences on parental behavior, Luster and Haddow conclude that other
factors are likely to be more important influences on how they parent than
their age; these factors include who becomes an adolescent parent and the
context in which adolescent mothers care for their children. A fourth char-
acteristic of the parent that is likely to influence behavior is the parent’s
gender. To date, much of the research on parenting has focused on mater-
nal behavior. However, in recent decades there has been growing interest
in the parenting behavior of fathers. In Chapter 4, Parke, Dennis, Flyr, Mor-
ris, Leidy, and Schofield review this research on fathers.
The second part of the book examines characteristics of the children.
Karraker and Coleman provide a comprehensive overview of research on
how children and parents influence each other’s behavior over time. How
parents respond to their children is likely to depend on the children’s age,
temperament, gender, and other factors. Some children have special needs
which require parents to adjust their practices to accommodate these
needs. Hodapp and Ly summarize research on parents with special needs
children in Chapter 6.
The third part of the book focuses on contextual influences on the par-
ent–child relationships. In Chapter 7, Fincham and Hall focus on the mari-
tal relationship. This is followed by Cochran and Walker’s review of re-
search on social networks. Chapter 9 explores the world of work; Crouter
and McHale discuss links between parents’ experiences in the workplace
and their behavior in the home. Garbarino, Bradshaw and Kostelny con-
sider the many ways in which neighborhoods influence children directly
and indirectly via their parents; risks and assets vary by neighborhood and
parents adjust their parenting strategies based on their perceptions of their
neighborhood characteristics. In Chapter 11, Leyendecker, Harwood,
Comparini and Yalç2nkaya consider the unique and combined influences
of socioeconomic status and ethnicity. In many communities, programs
have been designed to influence parenting behavior and ultimately the de-
velopment of their children. Powell reviews the literature on these interven-
tion programs in Chapter 12, noting both the effects of these programs and
changes in the questions that have been addressed by evaluators of these
programs.
The first edition of this book was published more than a decade ago.
Since that time, there has been a plethora of research on parenting prac-
tices, and these new findings are incorporated into each of the chapters of
the second edition of the book. In addition, new chapters were added to
each of the three parts of the book. Part I was expanded to include a chap-
xiv INTRODUCTION
ter on fathers. A chapter on special needs children was added to Part II.
Part III includes new chapters on socioeconomic status and ethnicity, and
parenting education programs. These additional chapters make the book a
more comprehensive resource for scholars and students interested in why
parents do what they do.
The final chapter differs from the earlier chapters in that it considers the
extent to which parents influence the development of their children. During
the past decade, several scholars have raised questions about much influence
parents have on their children’s development; they have also questioned the
methods that have been used to examine parental influences on children’s
development. Parenting research has been criticized by researchers who em-
ploy behavioral genetics designs to study human development (Scarr, 1992;
Rowe, 1994). These critics point out that studies of parents and their biologi-
cal children make it difficult to disentangle the effects of nature and nurture.
Another critic, Harris (1998), proposed that over the long run, peers have a
more enduring effect on children’s personality development than parents.
In the final chapter, we discuss the concerns and the evidence of those who
have been critical of socialization researchers. We also summarize some of
the evidence and counter-arguments put forth by parenting researchers in
response to these criticisms. The last chapter was written primarily for stu-
dents who are not familiar with these debates. It is intended to help them un-
derstand why scholars have reached differing conclusions about the role that
parents play in children’s development.
Having been influenced by Bronfenbrenner’s work, we recognize that
children’s development is influenced by myriad factors including genetic
endowment, peers, and parents. Proximal processes that occur in the home
are among the most important influences on children’s development. For
this reason we believe that it is essential to understand factors influencing
experiences in the home. Which factors influence the way parents interact
with their children, structure the home environment, and help their chil-
dren understand the world beyond the home? This book summarizes the
latest research on parenting, with each chapter providing a look at one im-
portant influence and the linkages among the various factors. An ecologi-
cal perspective draws attention to the fact that the lives of parents and chil-
dren are intertwined, and that understanding influences on parents is
important for understanding the experiences of children.
REFERENCES
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55,
83–96.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
INTRODUCTION xv
CHARACTERISTICS
OF THE PARENTS
This page intentionally left blank
1
Lynn Okagaki
Purdue University
Gary E. Bingham
Washington State University
INTRODUCTION
3
4 OKAGAKI AND BINGHAM
consider the evidence that supports these claims. In the final section, we
conclude with our perspectives on developments in research on parental
social cognition that have the potential to move the field forward.
Parental Beliefs
phasis on training and teaching children. Being a good mother meant that
one started training the child as soon as the child was ready to learn. Chi-
nese immigrant mothers expressed the belief that the primary way in which
mothers express their love to their child is by helping the child succeed, es-
pecially in school. The ideas expressed by the Chinese immigrant mothers
reflect what Chao (1994, 1996, 2000) described as the Chinese concept of
training a child (chiao shun). Similar to the Western concept of parenting
style, training is comprised of beliefs about parental demandingness and re-
sponsiveness to the child. The demandingness dimension of training em-
phasizes “a continuous monitoring and guidance of children” (Chao, 2000,
p. 234). Unlike parenting style, however, the demandingness dimension in
training a child does not include restricting or dominating the child. The
responsiveness dimension of training emphasizes parental involvement and
support, but does not include overt demonstrations of the parent’s affec-
tion for the child (e.g., praising or hugging the child). Thus, the concept of
training a child is similar to the Western understanding of parenting style,
but distinct in important ways and reflects a Chinese conceptualization of
parenting.
Another view of what it means to be a parent comes from Native Ameri-
can communities. Among many Native American nations, the role of the
parent is defined in ways that are distinct from Western models of parent-
ing. For example, in some Native American nations, caring and nurturing
children is a shared responsibility that extends beyond the biological par-
ents to members of the extended family and members of the community.
Primary responsibility for the discipline of the child may be given to grand-
parents, aunts, or uncles (Machamer & Gruber, 1998). Tribal elders may
have a formal role on matters regarding the care of the child ( Joe &
Malach, 1992). To be a parent does not mean that one has sole authority
for making decisions about the child.
The majority of research on ethnic and cultural variation in parenting in
the United States has relied on Western psychological traditions and has
not situated parenting by specific ethnic groups within their own cultural
traditions (e.g., Dornbusch, Ritter, Leiderman, Roberts, & Faleigh, 1987;
Okagaki & Sternberg, 1991; Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992; Stein-
berg, Mounts, Lamborn, & Dornbusch, 1991). Chao’s work (1994, 2000)
on examining immigrant Chinese mothers’ beliefs about parenting from
the Chinese concept of training illustrates development in research on cul-
tural variation in parenting—understanding parenting within the psycho-
logical context of the target culture, rather than using Western psychologi-
cal theories as the framework. More research in this vein—connecting
parents’ social cognitions to broader cultural values within specific cultural
contexts—is needed to increase our understanding of the role of culture in
shaping parental cognitions.
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 7
Parental Attitudes
Parental Perceptions
dren were more confident than fathers in their ability to use strategies that
involved reasoning or talking to the child. In contrast, fathers reported
greater confidence than mothers in their ability to use strategies that used
directives (e.g., “Stop fighting!”) or the threat of force.
Parental Attributions
son, Pine, Chan, & Buriel, 1990). Differences in these general cultural values
or expectations for members of communities are associated with differences
in the socialization goals and strategies which parents adopt for their chil-
dren (Harwood, 1992; Ogbu, 1981). As LeVine (1988) observed, differences
in parental goals and expectations arise, in part, because societies have differ-
ent expectations for the adult members of their communities.
Harwood’s (1992) comparison of Puerto Rican and European American
mothers’ goals and expectations for their young children illustrates cul-
tural differences in expectations. As compared to Puerto Rican mothers
from lower-class families, European American mothers from both lower-
and middle-class families were more likely to focus on socialization goals
that related to personal development (e.g., self-confidence, independence,
development of talents and abilities) and self-control (e.g., restraining one-
self from being greedy, aggressive or selfish). For the middle-class Euro-
pean American mothers, 35% of the responses emphasized some aspect of
personal development. In contrast, the Puerto Rican mothers were more
likely to talk about characteristics that focused on being respectful (e.g., be
polite, obedient) and loving (e.g., be friendly, get along with others).
Nearly 40% of the characteristics spontaneously mentioned by the Puerto
Rican mothers were related to respectfulness. In contrast, less than 3% of
the traits mentioned by the middle-class European American mothers fell
into this category. Thus, mothers’ long-term goals for their children dif-
fered across cultural groups, and these differences appear to reflect general
cultural orientations toward individuals and relationships. Although Har-
wood did not measure general cultural values in her study, her data are
consistent with the notion that general cultural values are translated by par-
ents into specific socialization goals for their children.
Substantial attention in the parenting literature has also focused on vari-
ation in parents’ developmental expectations that are associated with dif-
ferences in social class or socioeconomic status (SES). Early research on the
relation between social class and childrearing identified variation in par-
ents’ beliefs about the role of parents and in their expectations and goals
for their children as a function of social class differences (e.g., Kohn, 1963,
1969, 1979; Kohn & Schooler, 1983; Miller, Schooler, Kohn, & Hiller,
1979). Kohn hypothesized that social class differences in paternal goals and
expectations for children were related to differences in the requirements
and expectations fathers needed to meet to succeed in their jobs (Kohn &
Schooler, 1983).
Research by Crouter (1984) supports Kohn’s hypothesis. Through ex-
tensive interviews Crouter found that parents who worked in more partici-
pative environments (e.g., where team meetings are used to discuss issues
and to problem solve) emphasized the importance of cooperation among
family members at home and even adopted some of the strategies used at
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 11
Among the filial precepts are: obeying and honoring one’s parents, providing
for the material and mental well-being of one’s aged parents, performing the
ceremonial duties of ancestral worship, taking care to avoid harm to one’s
body, ensuring the continuity of the family line, and in general conducting
oneself so as to bring honor and not disgrace to the family name. (p. 287)
much education they ideally wanted their child to attain; how much educa-
tion they expected their child to receive; and the minimum amount of edu-
cation they would allow their child to receive before the child could termi-
nate his or her education. In all three conditions, Asian American parents
had higher expectations than Latino and European American parents.
Asian American parents ideally wanted their child to earn a graduate or
professional degree. They expected their child to receive a college degree,
and the minimum education that would satisfy the parents was a college de-
gree. In contrast, European American parents ideally wanted their child to
obtain a college degree, expected their child to receive some college educa-
tion, and minimally required their child to graduate from high school. Par-
ents’ expectations for the grades the child received followed a similar pat-
tern. Although all parents indicated that they would be pleased if their
child received an A, Asian American parents were less satisfied if their child
received a B or a C than Latino or European American parents were. In a
related study of high-achieving and low-achieving (as determined by school
achievement test scores) children of Mexican descent (Okagaki, Frensch, &
Gordon, 1995), parents of both high-achieving and low-achieving children
valued education, wanted their children to obtain good educations, and
held similar expectations for the amount of schooling they expected their
children to obtain. Parents of high-achieving and low-achieving children,
however, differed in the minimum educational attainment level that they
set for their children and in their responses to grades of Cs and Ds. Parents
of high achievers set a higher minimum boundary for their children’s
grades and educational attainment. Although about half of the parents in
each group wanted their child at least to complete college, of those who
would be satisfied with less than a college degree, the parents of high
achievers were more likely to want their child to complete at least some col-
lege or vocational training after high school. Parents of high achievers were
also less satisfied with Cs and Ds than were parents of low achievers. Again,
difference in parents’ responses to grades may reflect children’s prior per-
formance. However, when only the responses of parents who reported that
their children normally received As and Bs were examined, parents of low
achievers were still more likely to be satisfied with Cs and Ds than parents of
high achievers. In short, parents’ expectations are multifaceted. They are,
at least, comprised of parents’ ideals, parents’ realistic assessments of what
might happen, and what Goodnow (2002) has called the “bottom line”—
that is, the minimum acceptable performance.
Summary
back and reinforcement, and (4) children learn through negative feedback
and punishment. Parents were also asked to describe how they would help
their child learn or solve the problems presented in the vignettes, and these
responses were coded in a similar fashion. Finally, parents were observed
helping their child learn how to tie a knot. Their behaviors were coded to re-
flect the type of teaching strategy they used by indicating the degree to which
the parent required the child to think through the problem himself. Behav-
iors were coded as (a) high cognitive demands (i.e., requiring the child to
think abstractly and consider alternatives), (b) low demands (i.e., requiring
the child to answer focused, concrete questions that require minimal repre-
sentational thinking), or (c) structuring (i.e., facilitating or defining the task
for the child to help move the task along).
In Sigel’s study (1992), parents’ beliefs about how children learn were
related to their descriptions of what they would do if they were helping
their child solve the same problems posed in the vignettes. As expected,
mothers who believed that children learn by thinking through problems
themselves were more likely to report strategies that involved asking ques-
tions that required children to think abstractly and consider alternative so-
lutions and were less likely to report giving children explicit instructions for
solving the problem (r = .72 and r = −.60, respectively). On the other hand,
mothers who believed that children learn through direct instruction were
less likely to report that they would ask questions to help their child think
through the problem and more likely to report giving explicit instructions
(r = −.60 and r = .56, respectively). The degree to which fathers believed
that children learn by thinking through problems on their own was posi-
tively correlated with fathers’ reports of asking questions (r = .77); but the
degree to which fathers believed that children learn through direct instruc-
tion was not related to fathers’ reported use of this strategy. Meanwhile, fa-
thers’ beliefs about direct instruction were negatively related to their re-
ported use of distancing strategies (r = −.37). In general, the correlations
between beliefs and self-reported behaviors were moderately strong.
Examinations of the relation between parents’ reported beliefs and
their observed parenting behaviors revealed weaker associations. First, the
degrees to which mothers and fathers believed that children learn by
thinking through problems were unrelated to the frequency with which
they asked their child questions during the knot-tying task. However, the
degree to which fathers believed that children learn through direct in-
struction was negatively correlated with the degree to which fathers asked
questions that required their child to think through the problem (r =
–.34). The degree to which mothers believed that children learn through
direct instruction was positively related to how they talked to their child
about the steps that were necessary to complete the task (i.e., structured
the task for the child; r = .27).
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 17
Much of the research on the relations between parents’ attitudes and their
behaviors has focused either on parenting style (e.g., Daggett, O’Brien,
Zanolli, & Peyton, 2000; Gorman, 1998; Holden & Miller, 1999) or the qual-
ity of the home environment (e.g., Benasich & Brooks-Gunn, 1996; Iverson
& Segal, 1992; Kochanska, Kuczynski, & Radke-Yarrow, 1989). For example,
Benasich and Brooks-Gunn (1996) found that maternal childrearing atti-
tudes were positively related to the quality of the home environment, as
measured by the Home Observation Measurement of the Environment.
In a study of Puerto Rican mothers and their preschool-age child, moth-
ers’ attitudes toward authoritative parenting behaviors (e.g., encouraging
child to talk about feelings) and authoritarian parenting behaviors (e.g.,
parents should control parenting) were correlated with mothers’ behaviors
during a mother–child teaching task (Vargas & Busch-Rossnagel, 2003).
Mothers were asked to teach their child how to build two Tinkertoy models.
18 OKAGAKI AND BINGHAM
The frequencies with which mothers asked questions, gave direct instruc-
tions, praised, scolded or showed verbal disapproval, modeled or demon-
strated what to do, were physically affectionate, and used physical control
were coded. Significant, but weak, correlations were obtained between
mothers’ childrearing attitudes and their teaching behaviors. The degree
to which mothers endorsed authoritative childrearing beliefs was positively
correlated with asking questions and praising their child during the teach-
ing task. However, there was no correlation between authoritarian
childrearing attitudes and parenting behaviors.
Although researchers for decades have recognized that both parents and
their children are active agents in the socialization process, only recently
have examinations of parenting behavior begun to seriously consider po-
tential influences of the child on parents’ behaviors (Grusec, Goodnow, &
Kuczynski, 2000). One mechanism by which children may influence their
parents’ behaviors is through parents’ perceptions of children’s character-
istics and behaviors. For example, previous research has linked parents’
perceptions of difficult temperaments (Dadds, 1987), discipline problems
(Bates, Bayles, Bennet, Ridge, & Brown, 1991; Nix et al., 1999), and inatten-
tiveness or impulsivity (Russell, 1997) to childrearing behavior and the
quality of the parent–child relationship.
Parents appear to adapt their childrearing strategies to account for their
perceptions of their child’s abilities or special needs. In families with typi-
cally developing children, researchers have examined the ways in which
childrearing changes according to the developmental needs of the child.
For example, Cicognani and Zani (1998) found that parents of adolescents
often adopt a more flexible parenting style in order to cope with their per-
ceptions of their child’s changing developmental needs. Research on chil-
dren with special needs has linked parents’ perceptions of children’s ability
to family functioning (Blacher, Nihira, & Meyers, 1987) and childrearing
behaviors (Marks & Dollahite, 2001).
One limitation of the research examining the relations between parents’
perceptions of their child to their childrearing behavior is that most of it is
correlational and limited to data collected at one point in time. Few studies
have examined whether parents’ perceptions of children’s abilities or char-
acteristics are related to subsequent parental behavior. A notable exception
is a study of the relation between parents’ perceptions of their child’s shy-
ness and their subsequent parenting style (Rubin, Nelson, Hastings, &
Asendorpf, 1999). At 2 years of age, children’s behavioral inhibition was ob-
served in a laboratory setting and mothers’ and fathers’ ratings of child shy-
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 19
ness and their own parenting style were collected via self-report. At 4 years
of age, mothers and fathers completed additional self-report measures of
child shyness and parenting style.
Mothers’ and fathers’ perceptions of their child’s early shyness at age 2
directly predicted their reports of parenting behaviors that encouraged
child independence 2 years later. Mothers and fathers who perceived their
child as being shy at age 2 were less likely to encourage the child’s inde-
pendent behavior at age 4. However, the relation between early shyness and
self-reported parenting practices was not replicated when observers’ ratings
of child shyness at age 2 were substituted for parents’ reports of shyness in
the analysis. These data are consistent with the hypothesis that it appears to
be parents’ perceptions of their young child’s temperament, rather than
the child’s observed level of inhibition, that directly influences parents’
self-reported behaviors over time (Rubin et al., 1999).
Because parents’ perceptions of child temperament and their child-
rearing behaviors were both assessed using self-report measures, the ob-
tained relations may be a methodological artifact (i.e., shared method vari-
ance). The researchers, however, argued that by using structural equation
modeling, they were able to control for shared method variance better than
with other types of analyses and therefore have stronger support for par-
ents’ perceptions mediating the relation between child’s characteristics
and parent’s behaviors.
Perception of parental efficacy has also been posited to influence parent-
ing behaviors (e.g., Coleman & Karraker, 1998). For example, mothers and
fathers who were more confident about their ability to use specific parent-
ing strategies have been found to be more likely to use those strategies
when managing conflict between their children (Perozynski & Kramer,
1999). Mothers who felt more confident in their ability to perform control
strategies (e.g., threatening to punish children) were more likely to use
these strategies. Mothers were more likely to use a passive nonintervention
approach (e.g., ignored the conflict, let the children work out conflict)
when they believed that this was an effective strategy for managing sibling
conflicts and when they believed that they could effectively use this tech-
nique. The results for fathers were not as straightforward. Fathers’ percep-
tions of their ability to use control strategies or passive nonintervention
strategies were not related to their use of these strategies. Rather, the only
relation that emerged was between fathers’ use of control strategies and
their perception of their inability to implement child-centered strategies.
Fathers who viewed child-centered strategies as an effective way to resolve
sibling conflict but who doubted their ability to use such strategies were
more likely to use control strategies, rather than child-centered strategies,
during the observed sibling conflicts.
20 OKAGAKI AND BINGHAM
Intuitively one would expect parents’ goals and expectations for their child
to be related to parents’ childrearing behaviors. Relatively little research,
however, has directly assessed the relation between parents’ goals and ex-
pectations and their behaviors. In a series of studies, Hastings and Grusec
(1998) tested the hypothesis that parents’ goals are related to their self-
reported disciplinary strategies. In one of these studies, mothers and fa-
thers were presented with a series of vignettes describing parent–child
interactions (e.g., behaving badly in a grocery store, wanting to watch televi-
sion before chores are completed) and were asked what they would do in
each situation. Parents were then given a pregenerated list of goals and
were asked to rate how important each goal was in the context of each vi-
gnette. The goals reflected the degree to which parents (a) wanted their
child to obey and comply with their requests within a specific situation, (b)
wanted to teach their child important values, and (c) wanted to develop
and maintain a close parent–child relationship. As hypothesized, parents
who focused on obedience and compliance were more likely to report us-
22 OKAGAKI AND BINGHAM
Summary
ing behaviors. More research that seeks to causally connect parenting be-
liefs and behaviors in this manner is necessary next step.
In the past decade, researchers have begun to take a more complex view of
parents’ social cognitions and use more sophisticated approaches to exam-
ine the relations between parents’ social cognitions and their childrearing
behaviors. In this final section, we discuss new research developments and
highlight the types of changes that we believe will move the field forward.
child, with the success or failure of childrearing strategies, and with unan-
ticipated parenting situations, they may refine their initial beliefs and ac-
quire new ideas and attitudes related to childrearing. Goodnow (2002) has
hypothesized that it is during transitions in parenthood (e.g., when one
first becomes a parent, when the child becomes an adolescent, when the ad-
olescent becomes a young adult and leaves the home) that parents are most
open to new ideas about parenting and to advice from others.
In a study of first-time mothers-to-be, Scott and Hill (2001) examined
mothers’ beliefs prior to and 6 months after the birth of the infant. Among
other findings, after 6 months of parenting, the mothers were less likely to
believe that infants’ behavior should be controlled by parents and more
likely to believe that nurturing behaviors (e.g., showing love and affection
toward child) were important. In addition, mothers’ attitudes toward
breastfeeding were less positive after the birth than prior to the birth. This
finding suggests mothers may have preferred to breastfeed their infant, but
that interaction with their infant or the experience of breastfeeding itself
contributed to a change in these attitudes. It may simply have been that
mothers’ initial conceptions of breastfeeding their infant were unrealistic
and romanticized and that after the experience of breastfeeding their in-
fant, their attitudes toward breastfeeding were based on a more realistic un-
derstanding of what it entails.
Another illustration of change and development in parents’ social cog-
nitions comes from a study of mothers’ attitudes toward corporal punish-
ment (Holden et al., 1997). Researchers asked mothers of a 3-year-old child
questions about their attitudes toward corporal punishment and if their at-
titude was the same as it had been before they were parents. Because the
data were retrospective, the evidence for change is weaker than in the previ-
ous study, but nonetheless interesting. About two thirds of the mothers re-
ported that their attitudes towards spanking had changed since they had
become a parent. About half of these mothers indicated that they went
from having a positive attitude toward spanking to being against spanking.
The other half changed from being against spanking to becoming more
positive toward it. Almost all of the mothers (87%) who reported develop-
ing a more positive attitude toward spanking indicated that experiences
with their child had led to the change in attitude. For example, mothers re-
ported they began to use spanking as a disciplinary strategy because they
needed to use a stronger response to get their child’s attention or they per-
ceived their child as being very independent and determined such that the
child needed a strong disciplinary strategy in order for the parent to gain
some control over the child. Among those mothers who became less in fa-
vor of spanking, the majority (65%) reported that something about the
child led them to change their attitude. For example, mothers indicated
that they did not think that spanking worked with their child or the child
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 25
Methodological Developments
processing of anomalous data are also consistent with an implicit social cog-
nition framework. Greenwald and Banaji suggest that social cognitions may
generally be expressed in behavior through an implicit mode. Research on
implicit social cognitions and parenting behaviors may result in stronger
and more consistent relations between the two.
CONCLUSION
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank Tom Luster and Marc Bornstein for their helpful
editorial comments with previous drafts of this manuscript.
REFERENCES
Alexander, K. L., & Entwisle, D. R. (1988). Achievement in the first two years of school: Pat-
terns and processes. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 53(Serial No.
218).
Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white
men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experi-
mental Social Psychology, 35, 29–46.
Bartkowski, J. P., & Ellison, C. G. (1995). Divergent models of childrearing in popular manu-
als: Conservative Protestants vs. the mainstream experts. Sociology of Religion, 56(1), 21–34.
Bates, J., Bayles, K., Bennet, D. S., Ridge, B., & Brown, M. (1991). Origins of externalizing be-
havior problems at eight years of age. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development
and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 93–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates.
Benasich, A. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Maternal attitudes and knowledge of child-rearing:
Associations with family and child outcomes. Child Development, 67, 1186–1205.
Blacher, J., Nihira, K., & Meyers, C. E. (1987). Characteristics of home environment of families
with mentally retarded children: Comparisons across levels of retardation. American Journal
of Mental Deficiency, 91, 313–320.
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Azuma, H., Galperin, C., Maital, S., Ogino, M., et al. (1998).
A cross-national study of self-evaluations and attributions in parenting: Argentina, Bel-
gium, France, Israel, Italy, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology, 34,
662–676.
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Pascual, L., Haynes, O. M., Painter, L. M., Galperin,
C. Z., et al. (1996). Ideas about parenting in Argentina, Belgium, France, and the United
States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 347–367.
Brody, G. H., Flor, F. L., & Gibson, N. M. (1999). Linking maternal efficacy beliefs, develop-
mental goals, parenting practices, and child competence in rural single-parent African
American families. Child Development, 70, 1197–1208.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 29
Bugental, D. B., & Happaney, K. (2002). Parental attributions. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Hand-
book of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 509–535). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understand-
ing Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65,
1111–1119.
Chao, R. K. (1996). Chinese and European American mothers’ beliefs about the role of
parenting in children’s school success. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 403–423.
Chao, R. K. (2000). The parenting of immigrant Chinese and European American mothers:
Relations between parenting styles, socialization goals, and parental practices. Journal of Ap-
plied Developmental Psychology, 21, 233–248.
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-
rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-
cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677–686.
Chen, Z. Y., & Uttal, D. H. (1988). Cultural values, parents’ beliefs, and children’s achieve-
ment in the United States and China. Human Development, 31, 351–358.
Chiang, T., Barrett, K. C., & Nunez, N. N. (2000). Maternal attributions of Taiwanese and
American toddlers’ misdeeds and accomplishments. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31,
349–368.
Cicognani, E., & Zani, B. (1998). Parents’ educational style and adolescent autonomy. Euro-
pean Journal of Psychology of Education, 13, 485–502.
Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (1998). Self-efficacy and parenting quality: Findings and fu-
ture applications. Developmental Review, 18, 47–85.
Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (2003). Maternal self-efficacy beliefs, competence in
parenting, and toddlers’ behavior and developmental status. Infant Mental Health Journal,
24, 126–148.
Coplan, R. J., Hastings, P. D., Lagace-Seguin, D. G., & Moulton, C. E. (2002). Authoritative and
authoritarian mothers’ parenting goals, attributions, an emotions across different
childrearing contexts. Parenting: Science & Practice, 2, 1–26.
Cote, L. R., & Bornstein, M. H. (2003). Cultural and parenting cognitions in acculturating cul-
tures: 1. Cultural comparisons and developmental continuity and stability. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 34, 323–349.
Crouter, A. C. (1984). Participative work as an influence on human development. Human De-
velopment, 5, 71–90.
Dadds, M. R. (1987). Families and the origins of child behavior problems. Family Process, 26,
341–357.
Dadds, M. R., Mullins, M. J., McAllister, R. A., & Atkinson, E. (2003). Attributions, affect, and
behavior in abuse-risk mothers: A laboratory study. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27, 21–45.
Daggett, J., O’Brien, M., Zanolli, K., & Peyton, V. (2000). Parents’ attitudes about children: As-
sociations with parental life histories and child-rearing quality. Journal of Family Psychology,
14, 187–199.
Danso, H., Hunsberger, B., & Pratt, M. (1997). The role of parental religious fundamentalism
and right-wing authoritarianism in child-rearing goals and practices. Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion, 36, 496–511.
DeBaryshe, B. D. (1995). Maternal belief systems: Linchpin in home reading process. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 1–20.
Dix, T. (1992). Parenting on behalf of the child: Empathic goals in the regulation of respon-
sive parenting. In I. E. Sigel, A. V. McGillicuddy-De Lisi, & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental be-
lief systems: The psychological consequences for children (2nd ed., pp. 319–346). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Donahue, M. L., Pearl, R., & Herzog, A. (1997). Referential communication with preschool-
ers: Effects of children’s syntax and mothers’ beliefs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 18, 133–147.
30 OKAGAKI AND BINGHAM
Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Roberts, D. F., & Faleigh, M. J. (1987). The
relation of parenting style to adolescent school performance. Child Development, 58,
1244–1257.
Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is
good, but . . . : A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype.
Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109–128.
Ellison, C. G., & Sherkat, D. E. (1993). Obedience and autonomy: Religion and parental values
reconsidered. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 32(4), 313–329.
Entwisle, D. R., & Hayduk, L. A. (1978). Too great expectations: The academic outlook of young chil-
dren. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Fazio, R. H., Ledbetter, J. E., & Towles-Schwen, T. (2000). On the costs of accessible attitudes:
Detecting that the attitude object has changed. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
197–210.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gershoff, E. T., Miller, P. C., & Holden, G. W. (1999). Parenting influences from the pulpit:
Religious affiliation as a determinant of parental corporal punishment. Journal of Family
Psychology, 13, 307–320.
Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parents’ ideas, actions, and feelings: Models and method from develop-
mental and social psychology. Child Development, 59, 286–320.
Goodnow, J. J. (1995). Parent’s knowledge and expectations. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Hand-
book of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 305–332). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Goodnow, J. J. (2002). Parents’ knowledge and expectations: Using what we know. In M. H.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (pp. 439–460).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources, and
consequences of parents’ ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gorman, J. C. (1998). Parenting attitudes and practices of immigrant Chinese mothers of ado-
lescents. Family Relations, 47, 73–80.
Greenberger, E., O’Neil, R., & Nagel, S. K. (1994). Linking workplace and homeplace: Rela-
tions between the nature of adults’ work and their parenting behaviors. Developmental Psy-
chology, 30, 990–1002.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognitions: Attitudes, self-esteem, and
stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.
Grusec, J. E. Goodnow, J. J., & Kuczynski, L. (2000). New directions in analyses of parenting
contributions to children’s acquisition of values. Child Development, 71, 205–211.
Haight, W. L, Parke, R. D., & Black, J. E. (1997). Mothers’ and fathers’ beliefs about and spon-
taneous participation in their toddlers’ pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 271–290.
Hao, L., & Bornstead-Bruns, M. (1998). Parent–child differences in educational expectations
and the academic achievement of immigrant and native students. Sociology of Education, 71,
175–198.
Harkness, S., & Super, C. (Eds.). (1996). Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions,
and consequences. New York: Guilford.
Harrison, A. O., Wilson, M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan, S. Q., & Buriel, R. (1990). Family ecologies of
ethnic minority children. Child Development, 61, 347–362.
Harwood, R. L. (1992). The influence of culturally derived values on Anglo and Puerto Rican
mothers’ perceptions of attachment behavior. Child Development, 63, 822–839.
Harwood, R. L., Handwerker, W. P., Schoelmerich, A., & Leyendecker, B. (2001). Ethnic cate-
gory labels, parental beliefs, and the contextualized individual: An exploration of the indi-
vidualism–sociocentrism debate. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1, 217–236.
Harwood, R. L., Schoelmerich, A., & Schulze, P. A. (2000). Homogeneity and heterogeneity in
cultural belief systems. In S. Harkness & C. Raeff (Eds.), Variability in social construction of the
child (pp. 41–57). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
1. PARENTS’ SOCIAL COGNITIONS 31
Hastings, P. D., & Grusec, J. E. (1998). Parenting goals as organizers of responses to par-
ent–child disagreement. Developmental Psychology, 34, 465–479.
Hess, R. D., Chih-Mei, C., & McDevitt, T. M. (1987). Cultural variations in family beliefs about
children’s performance in mathematics: Comparisons among People’s Republic of Chi-
nese, Chinese American, and Caucasian American families. Journal of Educational Psychology,
79, 179–188.
Ho, D. Y. F. (1994). Cognitive socialization in Confucian heritage cultures. In P. M. Greenfield
& R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 285–313).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic status and parenting. In M. H.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 231–252).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Holden, G. W., & Buck, M. J. (2002). Parental attitudes toward childrearing. In M. H.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (pp. 537–562).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Holden, G. W., & Edwards, J. (1989). Parental attitudes toward child rearing: Instruments, is-
sues, and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 223–254.
Holden, G. W., & Miller, P. C. (1999). Enduring and different: A meta-analysis of the similarity
in parents’ child rearing. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 223–254.
Holden, G. W., Thompson, E. E., Zambarano, R. J., & Marshall, L. A. (1997). Child effects as a
source of change in maternal attitudes toward corporal punishment. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 14, 481–490.
Iverson, T. J., & Segal, M. (1992). Social behavior of maltreated children: Exploring links to
parent behavior and beliefs. In I. E. Sigel, A. McGillicuddy-De Lisi, & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.),
Parental beliefs systems: The psychological consequences for children (2nd ed., pp. 267–289).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Jambunathan, S., Burts, D. C., & Pierce, S. (2000). Comparisons of parenting attitudes among
five ethnic groups in the United States. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 31, 395–406.
Joe, J., & Malach, R. S. (1992). Families with Native American roots. In E. W. Lynch & M. J.
Hanson (Eds.), Developing cross-cultural competence: A guide for working with young children and
their families (pp. 89–119). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
Kinlaw, C. R., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Goldman-Fraser, J. (2001). Mothers’ achievement beliefs and
behaviors and their children’s school readiness: A cultural comparison. Applied Developmen-
tal Psychology, 22, 493–506.
Kochanska, G., Kuczynski, L., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1989). Correspondence between mothers’
self-reported and observed child-rearing practices. Child Development, 60, 56–63.
Kohn, M. L. (1963). Social class and parent–child relationships: An interpretation. American
Journal of Sociology, 18, 471–480.
Kohn, M. L. (1969). Class and conformity: A study in values. Homewood, IL: The Dorsey Press.
Kohn, M. L. (1979). The effects of social class on parental values and practices. In D. Reiss &
H. Hoffman (Eds.), The American family: Dying or developing (pp. 45–68). New York: Plenum.
Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality: An inquiry into the impact of social strati-
fication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
LeVine, R. A. (1988). Human parental care: Universal goals, cultural strategies, and individual
behavior. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. A. Levine, P. M. Miller, & M. M. West (Vol. Eds.),
New directions for child development: Parental behavior in diverse societies (Vol. 40, pp. 3–11). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Machamer, A. M., & Gruber, E. (1998). Secondary school, family, and educational risk: Com-
paring American Indian adolescents and their peers. Journal of Educational Research, 91,
357–369.
Mansbach, I. V., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1999). Developmental maturity expectations of Israeli
fathers and mothers: Effects of education, ethnic origin, and religiosity. International Jour-
nal of Behavioral Development, 23, 771–797.
32 OKAGAKI AND BINGHAM
Developmental Origins
of Parenting: Personality
and Relationship Factors
Joan Vondra
University of Pittsburgh
Jay Belsky
Birkbeck University of London
INTRODUCTION
The premise and starting point for this, and probably any, discussion of
parenting is a simple one. Individual differences in parenting depend on
who the parent is, first, as an individual psychological agent and, second, as a
partner in close relationships. All the intrinsic qualities that shape an individ-
ual’s psychological functioning—his or her personality and temperament,
physical and mental health, intelligence, maturity, gender, physical attrac-
tiveness and so on—will also shape what kind of partner he or she is in close
relationships. These same qualities will also influence what kind of partner
he or she chooses for close relationships, as well as the quality of the relation-
ship the two co-create. Every close relationship, in turn, has important effects
on the Psychological well-being and functioning of each partner.
These points have several implications for a discussion of the develop-
mental origins of parenting. First, there is an inherent confound in any ex
post facto investigation of parenting: Psychological functioning and rela-
tionship functioning are inextricably interconnected. One cannot take the
“person” out of the parent any more than one can separate the “person”
from the relationship in which he or she is a partner. This is as true of a par-
ent in a parent–child relationship as it is of a parent in his or her adult inti-
35
36 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
very organized, somewhat careless, and not especially planful. Once again,
it is not exactly clear how this trait should relate to parental behavior, as it
seems possible that, however attractive high conscientious may appear—es-
pecially to an employer—it could prove too demanding to a child. At the
same time, disorder and chaos, in contrast to organization, are typically not
in children’s best interests, so one could imagine low levels of conscien-
tiousness also predicting parental behavior that might not be especially sup-
portive of children’s functioning. The aforementioned study by Losoya et
al. (1997) that examined this trait in relation to the childrearing attitudes
and practices of mothers and fathers with children under 8 years of age
found conscientiousness to be positively related to supportive parenting
and inversely related to negative, controlling parenting. Clark and associ-
ates (2000) chronicled similar relations when looking at mothers of tod-
dlers, finding that more conscientious mothers are more responsive and
less power assertive than less conscientious mothers. However limited, the
evidence to date suggests that conscientiousness and positive parenting go
together.
across all levels of functioning (Gotlib, Lewinsohn, & Seeley, 1998; Sarason,
Sarason, & Gurung, 1997).
What is very much unclear in these correlational data is the direction of
effects. Do anxious, depressed, defended, and angry individuals select part-
ners and create relationships that reflect these personality characteristics,
or do distressed, conflicted, and abusive relationships create less agreeable
and more troubled partners? We feel comfortable in arguing that the proc-
ess of influence undoubtedly works in both directions. Long-range longitu-
dinal data suggest that this is the case, at least among clinical populations.
Back in 1966, Robins (see also Robins, 1986) reported that children with
greater antisocial behavior in childhood were more likely in adulthood to
be unemployed, lack social support, experience rejection by friends and
have repeated marital break-ups. Similar findings were documented by
Champion, Goodall, and Rutter in 1995. Bardone and her colleagues
(1996) found that adolescent girls with conduct disorder and, to a lesser ex-
tent, those with depression left school earlier, were more likely to be cohab-
iting with a partner, to have cohabited with multiple partners, to have be-
come pregnant, and to report violence in their relationship by age 21 than
comparison girls without mental illness (and presumably the family circum-
stances that accompany it). Comparable findings have been reported by
Kessler, David, and Kendler (1997) and by Gotlib and colleagues (1998),
who reported earlier and less satisfying marriages among individuals with a
history of adolescent depression. Quality of close relationships in adult-
hood, in other words, is predicted by psychological adjustment in child-
hood and adolescence.
Perhaps even more compelling for the Personality→Relationship path is
the association between childhood problems and selection of a teenage or
adult partner with various mental health problems. Quinton, Rutter, and
their colleagues (Quinton, Pickles, Maughan, & Rutter, 1993; Rutter,
Quinton, & Hill, 1990) found that children removed from their homes as a
result of problems in parenting were more likely than their peers to have a
partner in adulthood with mental health problems or a criminal history.
Referring specifically to depression, Petersen et al. (1993, p. 161) observed
that, “once in a depressed trajectory in development, an individual be-
comes more likely to stay on this course because of the tendency to both
alienate and withdraw from the very social supports that can minimize neg-
ative effects.” Rutter (2000) argued that this observation is applicable in
general to individuals with psychopathology.
However, when an individual with problems or at high risk of problems
during youth manages to select a better-adjusted partner and co-create a
more supportive relationship, he or she demonstrates better than expected
psychosocial functioning in adulthood, suggesting the opposite causal
path: Relationships→Personality and Personal Adjustment. Laub and col-
2. DEVELOPMENTAL ORIGINS OF PARENTING 47
leagues (Laub, Nagin, & Sampson, 1998) found this true of adults with a
criminal history, and Quinton and Rutter (Quinton & Rutter, 1988; Rutter,
Quinton, & Hill, 1990) documented such life course changes in their sam-
ple of urban, low-income women and men who had been institutionalized
as children due to parenting breakdown in their families. As Rutter (2000)
pointed out, “experiences that people bring about through their own ac-
tions can nevertheless constitute a major risk or protective influence on
their subsequent behavior” (p. 391). The extent to which these actions can
be attributed to genetic inheritance should not, however, be underesti-
mated.
Even studies of parenting interventions testify to a relationship self-selection
effect. In reviewing social support interventions for families in need,
Thompson and Ontai (2000; see also Gomby, Culross, & Behrman, 1999;
Reichman & McLanahan, 2001) discussed the pervasive problem of lack of
participation, lack of engagement, and attrition in all kinds of interven-
tions—including home visitation—when parents are stressed, depressed,
defended, addicted, or otherwise troubled (e.g., DePanfilis & Zuravin,
2002; Gaudin, Wodarski, Arkinson, & Avery, 1990–1991). A parent who has
problematic relationships with children, partners, parents, and/or friends
is especially likely to have problems developing a relationship—therapeutic
or otherwise—with service providers. Without engagement and alliance,
the intervention is unlikely even to take place, let alone be effective (Frai-
berg, 1980; Krupnick et al., 1996).
Conversely, intervention efficacy appears to be concentrated among
those parents who, to a large extent, self-select themselves into the group of
better attending, more involved, and more responsive participants (Gomby
et al., 1999; Thompson & Ontai, 2000) and/or when the intervention in-
volves frequent home visiting sustained for at least a year or more (i.e., with
those parents who have chosen not to drop out), using highly trained home
visitors with low turnover rates (who are presumably more effective in
building therapeutic relationships with less adjusted parents; Brooks-Gunn,
2001). In other words, the link between service intensity and client im-
provement (e.g., Erickson Warfield et al., 2000; Reichman & McLanahan,
2001) may be as much or more a product of client adjustment and motiva-
tion (choosing or being able to participate more fully in the intervention)
than of service efficacy. Better adjusted adults tend to provide better quality
parenting and to make better use of parenting interventions.
Among nonclinical populations, well-adjusted adults tend to have and
perceive supportive relationships both within and outside their homes
(Blum & Mehrabian, 1999; Contreras et al., 1999). But supportive relation-
ships may also foster greater adjustment in those who experience them
(Collins, Dunkel-Schetter, Lobel, & Scrimshaw, 1993; Sarason et al., 1997).
Bost and her colleagues (2002) interviewed rural, working class, European-
48 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
tics and functioning in the next generation of adults and, more specifically,
parents. A consistent theme in this research is that parent personality and
quality of relationships may play key roles as mechanisms of transmission.
We conclude this chapter with discussion about how this may transpire.
In the remainder of the chapter we elaborate and extend the empirical and
conceptual ideas raised about the developmental origins of parenting, with
an emphasis on understanding: (a) the processes by which developmental
history shapes both personality and patterns of relationship—and thereby
parenting—and (b) the conditions under which both continuity and dis-
continuity are predicted and documented. To do this, we consider two im-
portant research directions: first, studies about the development of psycho-
logical and interpersonal functioning in childhood, with an emphasis on
the notion of an internal working model of attachment, and second, studies
about continuity and discontinuity of functioning into adulthood.
Continuity, change, and the conditions under which each is likely to prevail
have received considerable attention within the attachment arena. An in-
ternal working model, after all, is a working model that should not neces-
sarily demonstrate stability over time, but should come to reflect new expe-
riences, particularly when those experiences involve strong emotion and/
or are repeated. Bowlby (1973) argued that working models would evolve
and respond to experience. Although much of the research on this topic in-
volves a considerable degree of inference, recent work helps articulate how
and when early working models might demonstrate continuity over time
and thus correlate with later patterns of functioning, including personality
and parenting.
Stability of Attachment
Summary
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
Bardone, A. M., Moffitt, T. W., Caspi, D., Dickson, N., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Adult mental
health and social outcomes of adolescent girls with depression and conduct disorder. Devel-
opment and Psychopathology, 8, 811–829.
Beach, S. R., & O’Leary, K. D. (1993). Marital discord and dysphoria: For whom does the mari-
tal relationship predict depressive symptomatology? Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 10(3), 405–420.
Beckwith, L., Howard, J., Espinosa, M., & Tyler, R. (1999). Psychopathology, mother–child in-
teraction, and infant development: Substance-abusing mothers and their offspring. Devel-
opment and Psychopathology, 11, 715–725.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55,
83–96.
Belsky, J., Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., & Moore, G. (1996). Instability of infant–parent attach-
ment security. Developmental Psychology, 32, 921–924.
Belsky, J., Crnic, K., & Woodworth, S. (1995). Personality and parenting: Exploring the medi-
ating role of transient mood and daily hassles. Journal of Personality, 63, 905–931.
Belsky, J., Spritz, B., & Crnic, K. (1996). Infant attachment security and affective–cognitive in-
formation processing at age 3. Psychological Science, 7, 111–114.
62 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
Belsky, J., & Vondra, J. (1985). Characteristics, consequences, and determinants of parenting.
In L. L’Abate (Ed.), Handbook of family psychology and therapy (pp. 523–536). Homewood, IL:
The Dorsey Press.
Bierman, K. L., & Wargo, J. B. (1995). Predicting the longitudinal course associated with ag-
gressive–rejected, aggressive (nonrejected), and rejected (nonaggressive) status. Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 7, 669–682.
Blum, J. S., & Mehrabian, A. (1999). Personality and temperament correlates of marital satis-
faction. Journal of Personality, 67(1), 93–125.
Bost, K. K., Cox, M. J., Burchinal, M. R., & Payne, C. (2002). Structural and supportive changes
in couples’ family and friendship networks across the transition to parenthood. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 64(2), 517–531.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss: Sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books.
Bradbury, T., & Fincham, F. (1990). Attributions in marriage. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 3–33.
Brody, G., Murry, V., Kim, S., & Brown, A. (2002). Longitudinal pathways to competence and
psychological adjustment among African American children living in rural single-parent
households. Child Development, 73, 1505–1516.
Brooks-Gunn, J. (2001). What are the components of successful early childhood programs?
(Society for Research in Child Development). Social Policy Report, 15(2), 9.
Bugental, D., & Shennum, W. (1984). Difficult children as elicitors and targets of adult com-
munication patterns. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49(1, Serial
No. 205).
Burchinal, M., Follmer, A., & Bryant, D. (1996). The relations of maternal social support and
family structure with maternal responsiveness and child outcomes among African Ameri-
can families. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1073–1083.
Burns, K., Chethik, L., Burns, W. J., & Clark, R. (1997). The early relationship of drug-abusing
mothers and their infants. An assessment at eight to twelve months of age. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 53, 279–287.
Cadoret, R. J., Winokur, G., Langbehn, D., & Troughton, E. (1996). Depression spectrum dis-
ease I: The role of gene–environment interaction. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
892–899.
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stewart, M. A. (1995). Adoption
study demonstrating two genetic pathways to drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52,
42–52.
Campbell, S. B., Pierce, E. W., Moore, G., Marakovitz, S., & Newby, K. (1996). Boys’
externalizing problems at elementary school age: Pathways from early behavioral prob-
lems, maternal control, and family stress. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 701–719.
Campbell, S. B., Shaw, D. S., & Gilliom, M. (2000). Early externalizing behavior problems:
Toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later maladjustment. Development and Psychopathology,
12(3), 467–488.
Cardemil, E. V., Reivich, K. J., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002, May 8). The prevention of depres-
sive symptoms in low-income, minority middle-school students. Prevention and Treatment, 5.
Retrieved January 4, 2005, from http://gateway1.ma.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi.
Carter, A., Garrity-Roukous, F., Chazan-Cohen, R., Little, C., & Briggs-Gowan, M. (2001). Ma-
ternal depression and comorbidity: Predicting early parenting, attachment security, and
toddler social–emotional problems and competencies. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 18–26.
Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) &
R. B. Cattell (Vol. Ed.). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38, 476–506.
2. DEVELOPMENTAL ORIGINS OF PARENTING 63
Caspi, A., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1988). Emergent family patterns: The intergenerational con-
struction of problem behavior and relationships. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde
(Eds.), Relationships within families: Mutual influences (pp. 218–240). Oxford, England: Clar-
endon.
Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. J. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of attachment: Theory and
research. Child Development, 65, 971–991.
Cassidy, J., Kirsh, S. J., Scolton, K. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Attachment and representations of
peer relationships. Developmental Psychology, 32, 892–904.
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. In J.
Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300–323). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38, 476–506.
Cattell, R. B. (1945). The principal trait clusters for describing personality. Psychological Bulle-
tin, 42, 129–161.
Champion, L. A., Goodall, G. M., & Rutter, M. (1995). Behavioural problems in childhood
and stressors in early adult life: A 20-year follow-up of London school children. Psychological
Medicine, 25, 231–246.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). Child maltreatment and attachment organization: Implica-
tions for intervention. In S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, de-
velopmental, and clinical perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Rogosch, F. A. (1999). The efficacy of toddler–parent psychother-
apy to increase attachment security in offspring of depressed mothers. Attachment and Hu-
man Development, 1, 34–66.
Clark, L. A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers’ personality and its interaction with
child temperament as predictors of parenting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 79, 274–285.
Clarke, G. N., Hawkins, W., Murphy, M., Sheeber, L. B., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R.
(1995). Targeted prevention of unipolar depressive disorder in an at-risk sample of high
school adolescents: A randomized trial of a group cognitive intervention. Journal of The
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(3), 312–321.
Cloninger, C. R., Bohman, M., & Sigvardsson, S. (1981). Inheritance of alcohol abuse: Cross-
fostering analysis of adopted men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 861–868.
Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., & Bohman, M. (1996). Type I and type II alcoholism: An up-
date. Alcohol Health and Research World, 20, 18–23.
Cohen, M. M. (1999). A meta-analysis of the predictive ability of infant attachment: Is the emperor wear-
ing any clothes? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.
Coie, J. D. (2004). The impact of negative social experiences on the development of antisocial
behavior. In J. B. Kupersmidt & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), Children’s peer relations: From development
to intervention. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Collins, N. L., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Lobel, M., & Scrimshaw, S. (1993). Social support in preg-
nancy: Psychosocial correlates of birth outcomes and postpartum depression. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 65, 1243–1258.
Conger, R., Conger, K., Elder, G., Lorenz, F., Simons, R., & Whitbeck, L. (1992). A family proc-
ess model of economic hardship and adjustment of early adolescent boys. Child Develop-
ment, 63(3), 526–541.
Conger, R., Conger, K., Elder, G., Lorenz, F., Simons, R., & Whitbeck, L. (1993). Family eco-
nomic stress and adjustment of early adolescent girls. Developmental Psychology, 29, 206–219.
Conger, R., Lorenz, F., Elder, G., Melby, J., Simons, R., & Conger, K. (1991). A process model
of family economic pressure and early adolescent alcohol use. Journal of Early Adolescence,
11(4), 430–449.
64 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
Conger, R., McCarthy, J., Yang, R., Lahey, B., & Kropp, J. (1984). Perception of child,
childrearing values, and emotional distress as mediating links between environmental
stressors and observed maternal behavior. Child Development, 54, 2234–2247.
Conger, R., Patterson, G., & Ge, X. (1995). It takes two to replicate: A mediational model for
the impact of parents’ stress on adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 66, 80–97.
Contreras, J. M., Lopez, I. R., Rivera-Mosquera, E. T., Raymond-Smith, L., & Rothstein, K.
(1999). Social support and adjustment among Puerto Rican adolescent mothers: The mod-
erating effect of acculturation. Journal of Family Psychology, 13(2), 228–243.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Cox, M. J., & Owen, M. T. (1993, March). Marital conflict and conflict negotiation: Effects on in-
fant–mother and infant–father relationships. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the
Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
Crick, N. C., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information process-
ing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.
Crittenden, P. M. (1999). Danger & development: The organization of self-protective strate-
gies. In J. I. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds.), Atypical patterns of attachment in infancy and
early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Serial No. 258).
Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Support for adolescent mothers during the postnatal period. In C.
Boukydis (Ed.), Research on support for parents and infants in the postnatal period (pp. 3–24).
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2002). Effects of marital discord on children: Recent ad-
vances and emerging themes in process-oriented research. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 43, 31–63.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Plomin, R. (1999). An adoption study of the etiology of teacher and par-
ent reports of externalizing behavior problems in middle childhood. Child Development,
70(1), 144–154.
DePanfilis, D., & Zuravin, S. J. (2002). The effect of services on the recurrence of child mal-
treatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26(2), 187–205.
Diener, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Contrerae, J., Hazelwood, L., & Rhodes, J. (1995, March). Corre-
lates of parenting competence among Latina adolescent mothers. Paper presented at the Biennial
Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.
Dix, T., Ruble, D., & Zambarano, R. (1989). Mothers’ implicit theories of discipline. Child De-
velopment, 60, 1373–1391.
Dodge, K. A. (1985). Attributional bias in aggressive children. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances
in cognitive–behavioral research and therapy (pp. 73–110). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children.
In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77–125).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Fontaine, R., & Price, J. M.
(2003). Peer rejection and social information-processing factors in the development of ag-
gressive behavior problems in children. Child Development, 74(2), 374–393.
Dozier, M., Albus, K., Fisher, P. A., & Sepulveda, S. (2002). Interventions for foster parents: Im-
plications for developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 14(4), 843–860.
Dozier, M., Stovall, K. C., Albus, K. E., & Bates, B. (2001). Attachment for infants in foster care:
The role of caregiver state of mind. Child Development, 72, 1467–1477.
Dunn, M. G., Mezzich, A., Janiszewski, S., Kirisci, L., & Tarter, R. (2001). Transmission of ne-
glect in substance abuse families: The role of child dysregulation and parental SUD. Child
and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 10(4), 126–134.
Eaves, L. J., Silberg, J. L., Meyer, J. M., Maes, H. H., Simonof, E., Pickles, A., Rutter, M., Neale,
M. C., Reynolds, C. A., Erikson, M. T., Heath, A. C., Loeber, R., Truett, K. R., & Hewitt, J. K.
2. DEVELOPMENTAL ORIGINS OF PARENTING 65
(1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The main effects of genes and
environment on behavioral problems in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral
Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 965–980.
Edelbrock, C., Rende, R., Plomin, R., & Thompson, L. A. (1995). A twin study of competence
and problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 36, 775–785.
Egeland, B., & Farber, E. (1984). Infant–mother attachment: Factors related to its develop-
ment and changes over time. Child Development, 55, 753–771.
Egeland, B., Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1988). Breaking the cycle of abuse: Relationship
predictors. Child Development, 59, 1080–1088.
Eiden, R. D., Chavez, F., & Leonard, K. E. (1999). Parent–infant interactions among families
with alcoholic fathers. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 745–762.
Elder, G. H., Jr., Caspi, A., & Downey, G. (1986). Problem behavior and family relationships:
Life course and intergenerational themes. In A. Sorensen, F. Weinert, & L. Sherrod (Eds.),
Human development and the life course (pp. 293–340). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates.
Elder, G. H., Jr., Conger, R. D., Foster, E. M., & Ardelt, M. (1992). Families under economic
pressure. Journal of Family Issues, 13(1), 5–37.
Elder, G. H., Jr., Liker, J. K., & Cross, C. E. (1984). Parent–child behavior in the Great Depres-
sion: Life course and intergenerational influences. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.),
Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 109–158). New York: Academic Press.
Elder, G. H., Jr., Nguyen, T. V., & Caspi, A. (1985). Linking family hardship to children’s lives.
Child Development, 56(2), 361–375.
Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Stevenson, J. (1999). Sex differences in the etiology of aggres-
sive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior: Results from two twin studies. Child Development,
70(1), 155–168.
Engfer, A., & Schneewind, K. (1982). Causes and consequences of harsh parental punishment.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 6, 129–139.
Erickson Warfield, M., Hauser-Cram, P., Wyngaarden Krauss, M., Shonkoff, J. P., & Upshur,
C. C. (2000). The effect of early intervention services on maternal well-being. Early Educa-
tion and Development, 11, 499–518.
Fraiberg, S. (1980). Clinical studies in infant mental health. New York: Basic Books.
Frommer, E., & O’Shea, G. (1973). The importance of childhood experiences in relation to
problems of marriage and family building. British Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 157–160.
Gaudin, J. M., Wodarski, J. S., Arkinson, M. K., & Avery, L. S. (1990–1991). Remedying child
neglect: Effectiveness of social network interventions. Journal of Applied Social Sciences, 15,
97–123.
Gillham, J., Reivich, K., Jaycox, L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). Prevention of depressive symp-
toms in schoolchildren: Two-year follow-up. Psychological Science, 6, 343–351.
Goldstein, L., Diener, M., & Mangelsdorf, S. (1996). Maternal characteristics and social sup-
port across the transition to motherhood: Associations with maternal behavior. Journal of
Family Psychology, 10, 60–71.
Gomby, D. S., Culross, P. L., & Behrman, R. E. (1999). Home visiting: Recent program evalua-
tions—Analysis and recommendations. The Future of Children, 9, 4–26.
Gondoli, D., & Silverberg, S. (1997). Maternal emotional distress and diminished responsive-
ness. Developmental Psychology, 33, 861–868.
Gotlib, I. H., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1998). Consequences of depression during ado-
lescence: Marital status and marital functioning in early adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
chology, 107, 686–690.
Hamilton, C. E. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through ado-
lescence. Child Development, 71(3), 690–694.
66 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
Hanson, R. F., Lipovsky, J. A., & Saunders, B. E. (1994). Characteristics of fathers in incest fam-
ilies. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9(2), 155–169.
Harris, T., Brown, G. W., & Bifulco, A. (1986). Loss of parent in childhood and adult psychiat-
ric disorder: The role of lack of adequate parental care. Psychological Medicine, 16, 641–659.
Harris, T., Brown, G. W., & Bifulco, A. (1990). Loss of parent in childhood and adult psychiat-
ric disorder: A tentative overall model. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 311–328.
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (1999). Second generation effects of unresolved trauma in non-
maltreating parents: Dissociated, frightened, and threatening parental behavior. In D. Dia-
mond & S. J. Blatt (Eds.), Psychoanalytic theory and attachment research I: Theoretical
considerations. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 19, 481–540.
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2000). Disorganized infant, child and adult attachment: Collapse in be-
havioral and attentional strategies. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48(4),
1097–1127.
Hipwell, A. E., Goossens, F. A., Melhuish, E. C., & Kumar, R. (2000). Severe maternal
psychopathology and infant–mother attachment. Development and Psychopathology, 12(2),
157–175.
Jacklin, C. N. (1989). Female and male: Issues of gender. American Psychologist, 44, 127–133.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory
and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
Johnston, C., & Patenaude, R. (1994). Parent attributions for inattentive–overeactive and
oppositional–defiant child behaviors. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 261–275.
Kaufman, J., & Zigler, E. (1993). The intergenerational transmission of abuse is overstated. In
R. J. Gelles & D. R. Loseke (Eds.), Current controversies on family violence. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Kessler, R. C., David, C. G., & Kendler, K. S. (1997). Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric
disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine, 27, 1101–1119.
Kim, H. K., & Mckenry, P. C. (2002). The relationship between marriage and psychological
well-being: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 23(8), 885–911.
Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Lorenz, F. O. (2003). Reciprocal influences be-
tween stressful life events and adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. Child
Development, 74(1), 127–143.
Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2004). Genetic and environmental proc-
esses in young children’s resilience and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation. Child
Development, 75(3), 651–668.
Kobak, R. (1999). The emotional dynamics of disruptions in attachment relationships. In J.
Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical application
(pp. 21–43). New York: Guilford.
Kochanska, G., Clark, L., & Goldman, M. (1997). Implications of mothers’ personality for
parenting and their young children’s developmental outcomes. Journal of Personality, 65,
389–420.
Krupnick, J. L., Sotsky, S. M., Simmens, D., Moyer, J., Elkin, I., Witkins, J., & Pilkonis, P. A.
(1996). The role of the therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy out-
come: Findings in the National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Col-
laborative Research Program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 532–539.
Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional understanding in pre-
school children. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1038–1045.
Lakatos, K., Toth, I., Nemoda, Z., Ney, K., Sasvari-Szekely, M., & Gervai, J. (2000). Dopamine
D4 receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism is associated with attachment disorganization in
infants. Molecular Psychiatry, 5(6), 633–637.
Laub, J. H., Nagin, D. S., & Sampson, R. J. (1998). Trajectories of change in criminal offend-
ing: Good marriages and the desistance process. American Sociological Review, 63, 225–238.
2. DEVELOPMENTAL ORIGINS OF PARENTING 67
Levy-Shiff, R., & Israelashvilli, R. (1988). Antecedents of fathering. Developmental Psychology, 24,
434–441.
Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time. Child Development, 71(3),
707–720.
Lieberman, A. F., & Pawl, J. H. (1988). Clinical applications of attachment theory. In J. Belsky
& T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 325–351). Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Lieberman, A. F., Silverman, R., & Pawl, J. (2000). Infant–parent psychotherapy: Core con-
cepts and current approaches. In C. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd
ed., pp. 472–484). New York: Guilford Press.
Lieberman, A. F., & Zeanah, C. H. (1999). Contributions of attachment theory to infant–par-
ent psychotherapy and other interventions with infants and young children. In J. Cassidy &
P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp.
555–574). New York: Guilford Press.
Longfellow, C., Zelkowitz, P., & Saunders, E. (1982). The quality of mother–child relation-
ships. In D. Belle (Ed.), Lives in stress (pp. 163–176). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Losoya, S., Callor, S., Rowe, D., & Goldsmith, H. (1997). Origins of familial similarity in
parenting. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1012–1023.
Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (1999). Attachment disorganization: Unresolved loss, rela-
tional violence, and lapses in behavioral and attentional strategies. In J. Cassidy & P. R.
Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp.
520–554). New York: Guilford Press.
MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Castellino, D., Brody, G., & Fincham, F. D. (2001). A longitudinal exam-
ination of the associations between fathers’ and children’s attributions and negative inter-
actions. Social Development, 10, 473–487.
MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Lamb, M. E., Arbuckle, B., Baradaran, L. P., & Volling, B. L. (1992). The
relationship between biased maternal and filial attributions and the aggressiveness of their
interactions. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 403–415.
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant
disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the
linking mechanism? In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attach-
ment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 161–182). Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Kestenbaum, R., Lang, S., & Andreas, D. (1990). Infant prone-
ness-to-distress temperament, maternal personality, and mother–infant attachment: Asso-
ciations and goodness of fit. Child Development, 61, 820–831.
McGroder, S. M. (2000). Parenting among low-income, African American single mothers with
preschool-age children: Patterns, predictors, and developmental correlates. Child Develop-
ment, 71(3), 752–771.
Milner, J. S., & Chilamkurti, C. (1991). Physical child abuse perpetrator characteristics: A re-
view of the literature. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6, 345–366.
Muller, R. T., Hunter, J. E., & Stollak, G. (1995). The intergenerational transmission of corpo-
ral punishment: A comparison of social learning and temperament models. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 19(11), 1323–1335.
Muñoz, R. F., Ying, Y., Bernal, G., Perez-Stable, E. J., Sorenson, J. L., Hargreaves, W. A.,
Miranda, J., & Miller, L. S. (1995). Prevention of depression with primary care adults: A
randomized control trial. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 199–222.
Murray, L., & Cooper, P. J. (Eds.). (1997). Postpartum depression and child development. New York:
Guilford Press.
National Research Council. (1993). Understanding child abuse and neglect. Washington, DC: Na-
tional Academy Press.
68 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive symp-
toms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months: Results from the NICHD
Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1297–1310.
Nix, R. L., Pinderhughes, E. E., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & McFadyen-Ketchum,
S. A. (1999). The relation between mothers’ hostile attribution tendencies and children’s
externalizing behavior problems: The mediating role of mothers’ harsh discipline prac-
tices. Child Development, 70(4), 986–909.
O’Leary, D. A., Christian, J. L., & Mendell, N. R. (1994). A closer look at the link between mari-
tal discord and depressive symptomatology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13,
33–41.
Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H. J. (2002).
Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development,
73(3), 916–934.
Patterson, G. R., & Yoerger, K. (1997). A developmental model for late-onset delinquency. Ne-
braska Symposium on Motivation, 44, 119–177.
Peterson, A. C., Compas, B. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., Stemmler, M., Ey, S., & Grant, K. E. (1993).
Depression in adolescence. American Psychologist, 48, 155–186.
Quinton, D., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. (1993). Partners, peers, and pathways:
Assortative pairing and continuities in conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 5,
763–783.
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting breakdown: The making and breaking of intergen-
erational links. Aldershot, UK: Avebury.
Radke-Yarrow, M. (1998). Children of depressed mothers. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rauh, H., Ziegenhain, U., Muller, B., & Wijnroks, L. (2000). Stability and change in in-
fant–mother attachment in the second year of life: Relations to parenting quality and vary-
ing degrees of daycare experience. In P. M. Crittenden & A. H. Claussen (Eds.), The organi-
zation of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Reichman, N. E., & McLanahan, S. S. (2001). Self-sufficiency programs and parenting inter-
ventions: Lessons from New Chance and the Teenage Parent Demonstration. Society for
Research in Child Development. Social Policy Report, 15(2).
Reiss, D., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2000). The interplay of genetic influences and social processes
in developmental theory: Specific mechanisms are coming into view. Development and
Psychopathology, 12(3), 357–374.
Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. (2000). The relationship code: De-
ciphering genetic and social patterns in adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grow up. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Robins, L. N. (1986). The consequence of conduct disorder in girls. In D. Olweus, J. Block, &
M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior: Research, theories, and
issues (pp. 385–414). Orlando, FL: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Rutter, M. (1999). Psychosocial adversity and child psychopathology. British Journal of Psychia-
try, 174, 480–493.
Rutter, M. (2000). Psychosocial influences: Critiques, findings, and research needs. Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 12, 375–405.
Rutter, M., & Quinton, D. (1984). Long-term follow-up of women institutionalized in child-
hood: Factors promoting good functioning in adult life. British Journal of Developmental Psy-
chology, 2, 191–204.
Rutter, M., Quinton, D., & Hill, J. (1990). Adult outcome of institution-reared children: Males
and females compared. In L. Robins & M. Rutter (Eds.), Straight and deviant pathways from
childhood to adulthood (pp. 135–157). New York: Cambridge University Press.
2. DEVELOPMENTAL ORIGINS OF PARENTING 69
Sarason, B. R., Sarason, I. G., & Gurung, R. A. (1997). Close personal relationships and health
outcomes: A key to the role of social support. In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal rela-
tionships: Theory, research and interventions (2nd ed., pp. 547–573). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child–parent attachment and children’s
peer relations: A quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 37(1), 86–100.
Seligman, M. E. P., Schulman, P., DeRubeis, R. J., & Hollon, S. D. (1999). The prevention of
depression and anxiety. Prevention and Treatment, 2. Retrieved on January 5, 2005, from
http://gateway1.ma.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi.
Silberg, J., Rutter, M., Neale, M., & Eaves, L. (2001). Genetic moderation of environmental
risk for depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 116–121.
Simons, R., Beaman, J., Conger, R., & Chao, W. (1993). Childhood experience, conceptions of
parenting, and attitudes of spouse as determinants of parental behavior. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 55, 91–106.
Skinner, M. L., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Conger, R. D. (1992). Linking economic hardship to adoles-
cent aggression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21(3), 259–276.
Skodak, M., & Skeels, H. M. (1949). A final follow-up study of one hundred adopted children.
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 75, 85–125.
Slep, A., & O’Leary, S. (1998). The effects of maternal attributions on parenting. Journal of
Family Psychology, 12, 234–243.
Spitz, R. A. (1945). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early
childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1, 53–74.
Spitz, R. A. (1946). Anaclitic depression: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions
in early childhood, II. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2, 313–324.
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development,
71(4), 1072–1085.
Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (1996). Antisocial development: A holistic approach. Development
and Psychopathology, 8(4), 617–645.
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton.
Suomi, S. J. (1999). Attachment in Rhesus monkeys. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Hand-
book of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 181–197). New York: Guilford
Press.
Tellegen, A., Watson, D., & Clark, L. (1999). On the dimensional and hierarchichal structure
of affect. Psychological Science, 10, 297–303.
Terman, L. (1938). Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Teti, D. M., Saken, J. W., Kucera, E., Corns, K. M., & Eiden, R. D. (1996). And baby makes four:
Predictors of attachment security among preschool-age firstborns during the transition to
siblinghood. Child Development, 67, 579–596.
Thompson, R. A. (1996). Early sociopersonality development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N.
Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality de-
velopment (5th ed.). New York: Wiley.
Thompson, R. A. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver
(Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 265–286). New
York: Guilford Press.
Thompson, R. A., & Ontai, L. (2000). Striving to do well what comes naturally: Social support,
developmental psychopathology, and social policy. Development and Psychopathology, 12,
657–675.
Tienari, P., Sorri, A., & Lahti, I. (1985). Interaction of genetic and psychosocial factors in
schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatric Scandinavica, 71, 19–30.
Tienari, P., Wynne, L. C., Moring, J., Lahti, I., Maarala, M., Sorri, A., Wahlberg, K. E., Saarento,
O., Seitamaa, M., Kaleva, M., & Laksy, K. (1994). The Finnish Adoption Family Study of
Schizophrenia: Implications for family research. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 20–26.
70 VONDRA, SYSKO, BELSKY
Toth, S. L., Maughan, A., Manly, J. T., Spagnola, M., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). The relative effi-
cacy of two interventions in altering maltreated preschool children’s representational
models: Implications for attachment theory. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 877–908.
Touris, M., Kromelow, S., & Harding, C. (1995). Mother–firstborn attachment and the birth of
a sibling. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 293–297.
Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. C. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings (Tech.
Rep.). Lackland Air Force Base, TX: USAF.
van Bakel, H. J. A., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2002). Parenting and development of one-year-
olds: Links with parental, contextual, and child characteristics. Child Development, 73(1),
256–273.
Van den Oord, E. J., Boomsma, I., & Verhulst, F. C. (1994). A study of problem behavior in 10-
to 15-year-old biologically related and unrelated international adoptees. Behavior Genetics,
24, 193–205.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., & Duyvesteyn, M. G. C. (1995). Breaking the intergen-
erational cycle of insecure attachment: A review of the effects of attachment-based inter-
ventions on maternal sensitivity and infant security. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
36, 225–248.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Moran, G., Belsky, J., Pederson, D., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., &
Kneppers, K. (2000). The similarity of siblings’ attachments to their mother. Child Develop-
ment, 71(4), 1086–1098.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized
attachment in early childhood: Meta-analyses of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae.
Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225–249.
Vaughn, B., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1979). Individual differences in in-
fant–mother attachment: Stability and change in families under stress. Child Development,
50, 971–975.
Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working model of the self, at-
tachment, and competence in five-year-olds. Child Development, 67, 2493–2511.
Vondra, J., & Belsky, J. (1993). Developmental origins of parenting: Personality and relation-
ship factors. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (1st ed., pp.
1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Vondra, J. I., Hommerding, K. D., & Shaw, D. S. (1999). Stability and change in infant attach-
ment in a low-income sample. In J. I. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds.), Atypical patterns of at-
tachment in infancy and early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
opment (Serial No. 258, pp. 119–144). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Vondra, J. I., Shaw, D. S., Swearingen, L., Cohen, M., & Owens, E. B. (2001). Attachment stabil-
ity and emotional and behavioral regulation from infancy to preschool age. Development
and Psychopathology, 13, 13–33.
Voydanoff, P., & Donnelly, B. (1998). Parents’ risk and protective factors as predictors of pa-
rental well-being and behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 344–355.
Wartner, U. G., Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., & Suess, G. (1994). Attachment patterns
at age six in South Germany: Predictability from infancy and implications for preschool be-
havior. Child Development, 65, 1014–1027.
Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment security
in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 71(3),
684–689.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. (1984). Negative affectivity. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 465–490.
Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from infancy to early adult-
hood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates. Child Develop-
ment, 71(3), 695–702.
Whipple, E. E., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zucker, R. A. (1995). Parent–child interactions in alcoholic
and non-alcoholic families. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 153–159.
2. DEVELOPMENTAL ORIGINS OF PARENTING 71
Youngblade, L. M., & Belsky, J. (1990). The social and emotional consequences of child mal-
treatment. In R. Ammerman & M. Hersen (Eds.), Children at risk: An evaluation of factors con-
tributing to child abuse and neglect (pp. 109–146). New York: Plenum Press.
Zaslow, M., Pedersen, F., Cain, R., Suwalsky, J., & Kramer, E. (1985). Depressed mood in new
fathers. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 111, 133–150.
Zelkowitz, P. (1982). Parenting philosophies and practices. In D. Belle (Ed.), Lives in stress (pp.
154–162). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Zimmerman, P., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Spangler, G., & Grossman, K. E. (1997). Attachment in
adolescence: A longitudinal perspective. In W. Koops, J. B. Hoeksema, & D. C. van den
Boom (Eds.), Development of interaction and attachment: Traditional and non traditional ap-
proaches (pp. 281–291). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Zucker, R. A., & Gomberg, E. S. L. (1986). Etiology of alcoholism reconsidered: The case for a
biopsychosocial process. American Psychologist, 41, 783–793.
This page intentionally left blank
3
Adolescent Mothers
and Their Children:
An Ecological Perspective
Tom Luster
Michigan State University
INTRODUCTION
During the 1990s, there was a decline in the birthrate among adolescent fe-
males in the United States. The number of births per 1,000 females ages 15
to 19 was 42.9 in 2002 compared to 62.1 per 1,000 in 1991 (Child Trends,
2003). A number of factors contributed to this decline in the birthrate. The
percentage of high school students who were sexually active decreased
(Centers for Disease Control, 2001), and the use of contraception generally
increased (Terry & Manlove, 2000). In addition, longer-term forms of birth
control, such as Depo Provera and the birth control patch, became avail-
able.
Although fewer teens are becoming parents, there were still 431,988
births to adolescents in the U.S. in 2002 (Child Trends, 2003). Moreover,
the birthrate to adolescents in the U.S. continues to be much higher than it
is in most other developed countries (Child Trends, 2001). Countries with
very low birthrates (e.g., Japan, Italy, Spain) have fewer than 10 births per
1,000 teenage females. Teens in the U.S. are less likely to use contraception
consistently than teens in most other developed countries, and that is the
primary reason why other developed countries have lower birthrates
among teens than the U.S. (Alan Guttmacher Institute, 2002).
During the past several decades, numerous studies have been conducted
to determine the antecedents of early childbearing and the effects of early
childbearing on the adolescents and their children. Other studies focused
73
74 LUSTER AND HADDOW
on factors that influence the way in which adolescents parent their chil-
dren. In this chapter, we summarize the results of some of the key studies in
these areas. The chapter is divided into four parts. In the first section, we
examine factors associated with early sexual activity and childbearing. In
what ways do adolescents who become mothers differ from adolescents who
do not? It is important to consider selection factors when discussing the
outcomes of adolescent mothers and their children, and when examining
the parenting practices of adolescent mothers. The second section focuses
on the outcomes of adolescent mothers in areas such as education and in-
come. In this section, we review research that has addressed two related
questions: (a) How do adolescent mothers differ from peers who delay
childbearing on these outcomes? and (b) To what extent are these out-
comes due to early childbearing, and to what extent can they be explained
by selection factors? In the third section, we consider the consequences of
early childbearing for the children. The questions of interest are: (a) How
do the children of adolescent mothers fare compared to children born to
older mothers? and (b) What factors contribute to successful outcomes
among children born to adolescent mothers? The fourth section examines
research concerned with individual differences in parenting practices
among adolescents mothers. Some adolescent mothers provide very sup-
portive environments for their children, while other mothers provide less
optimal environments. Based on Belsky’s (1984) model of the determi-
nants of parenting, the influences on parenting of interest to us include
characteristics of the young mothers, contextual sources of stress and sup-
port, and characteristics of their children. We are especially interested in
adolescent mothers who are functioning well in their role as parents.
Knowledge about why they adapt successfully to the challenges of early par-
enthood should be useful to those who design programs for adolescent
mothers and their children.
Adolescent mothers are diverse. They come from all racial, ethnic, and in-
come groups. There are adolescent mothers who are National Merit Schol-
ars and others who are in special education classes. Some adolescent moth-
ers are well adjusted, and others are troubled or depressed. Because of this
diversity, it is important not to stereotype adolescent mothers. On the other
hand, we must also recognize that adolescent mothers are not representa-
tive of adolescent females in the population. For example, birthrates differ
by race. The birthrate in the United States in 2001 was highest for Latina
adolescents (84 per 1,000), followed by African American (74 per 1,000)
and Non-Hispanic Whites (30 per 1,000) (Child Trends, 2003). In this sec-
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 75
Given what is known about selection factors that are associated with becom-
ing a teenage parent, it is not surprising that adolescent mothers tend to fare
less well than their peers on a variety of life-course outcomes. For example,
adolescent mothers are less likely than women who delay having children to
finish high school or obtain a GED (Hotz et al., 1997; Klepinger, Lundberg,
& Plotnick, 1995). By age 30, 61% of women who had their first child by age
18 had a high school diploma or GED compared with 91% of women who
did not have a child during their teen years (Hotz et al., 1997). Researchers
have been trying to determine if early childbearing is the reason why adoles-
cent mothers obtain lower level of education or if the lower level of educa-
tion is due to characteristics of the young mothers (e.g., low academic apti-
tude) or family background characteristics (e.g., low parental education,
poverty). Many approaches have been used to tease apart the influence of
early childbearing and selection factors on educational attainment.
One approach is to control for characteristics of the women and their
families in the analysis when examining the relation between early child-
bearing and educational attainment. Klepinger and his colleagues (1995)
used this approach and found that having a child before age 20 reduced
the level of education attained by almost three years for Whites, Blacks, and
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 77
Hispanics at age 25. The variables they controlled included such things as
parental education, parental divorce, and attending religious services.
Some researchers have argued that it is difficult to control statistically for
all relevant background characteristics and have used other creative ap-
proaches to address this issue. Hotz and his colleagues (1997) compared fe-
males who had their first child before age 18 with females who became
pregnant before age 18 but had a miscarriage and, therefore did not have
their first child until later; on average, those who had a miscarriage had
their first children three to four years later than the teenage mothers.
Given that both groups had become pregnant as teenagers, the researchers
concluded that females who had miscarriages as teenagers provided an
ideal comparison group for teenage mothers. The two groups were not
likely to differ markedly in terms of family background characteristics and
other factors associated with becoming pregnant at an early age. Hotz
found that teen mothers were much less likely than those who had miscar-
riages to receive a standard high school diploma (41% vs. 61%), but they
were more likely to obtain a GED (23% vs. 2%). Overall there was no effect
of early childbearing on obtaining a high school level education if high
school diplomas and GEDs are treated as equivalent.
By age 30, the annual earnings of women who had their first children be-
fore age 18 is only 58% of the annual earnings of all other women who de-
layed having children (Hotz et al., 1997). The earnings gap is due both to
the lower wages teenage mothers earn and the fewer hours that they work
per year. Once again researchers have tried to determine if the earnings
gap is due to early childbearing per se or selection factors associated with
early childbearing. Hotz and colleagues compared the annual earnings of
teenage mothers and women who had miscarriages prior to age 18. The an-
nual earnings of both groups were low, but those who had their first births
before age 18 earned significantly more from their mid 20s through mid
30s than those who had miscarriages. Hotz and his colleagues explained
this surprising finding by noting that both groups of women ended up in
jobs that did not require special educational credentials; job skills were ac-
quired on the job and higher compensation was obtained through the ac-
quisition of these skills and through continuity on the job. Women who
completed their childbearing in their early 20s had an advantage over
women from similar backgrounds who had children later; the women who
had miscarriages delayed their entry into the labor force or moved in and
out of jobs as they had their children during their 20s.
Geronimus and Korenman (1992) also used a creative approach to dis-
entangle the effects of early childbearing and selection factors on socioeco-
nomic status outcomes. Utilizing three national data sets, they compared
women who had their first children during their teenage years with their
sisters who had their children after their teen years; the sisters were about
78 LUSTER AND HADDOW
five years older, on average, than the teenage mothers when they had their
first child. Presumably the sisters have similar family background character-
istics and exposure to other risk factors such as substandard schools or
neighborhoods with high concentrations of impoverished families. As ex-
pected, differences in family income, family income-to-needs ratio, and
poverty rates were less pronounced when the sisters were compared than
when a cross section of early childbearers and women who delayed child-
bearing were compared and family background characteristics were statisti-
cally controlled. (This was true for two out of three national data sets; for
the Panel Study of Income Dynamics, differences between the sisters were
comparable to differences between early and later childbearers in the cross-
sectional sample.) What differences there were between the sisters in their
family income-to-needs ratio could be explained by the higher level of edu-
cation obtained by the sister who delayed childbearing and by the fact that
the later childbearers were more likely to be married at the time that family
income was measured.
The studies by Hotz et al. (1997) and Geronimus and Korenman (1992)
indicate that simple comparisons (i.e., those that do not control for selec-
tion factors) of teenage mothers and females who delay childbearing until
their 20s tend to overestimate the effects of early childbearing per se on
outcomes such as educational attainment, income, and poverty status. Nev-
ertheless, whatever the cause of the disadvantage, the fact remains that chil-
dren of teenage mothers are more likely than other children to be raised by
parents with low educational levels and low levels of income.
Another outcome of interest is the marital status of the young mothers.
Approximately four out of five teen births are nonmarital (Child Trends,
2003); among women of all ages in the US, about one third of births are
nonmarital. Thus, children of teenage mothers are more likely than other
children to live with a never-married mother for at least part of their lives.
Hotz et al. (1997) compared teenage mothers and women who had miscar-
riages during their teen years on the percentage of time spent as a single
mothers from age 14 to 30. Teenage mothers spent more time as single
mothers during those years than those who had miscarriages as teens and
had their children three to four years later on average. Part of the concern
about teenage parenthood results from the knowledge that a dispropor-
tionate number of their children grow up in homes without their fathers.
Moreover, among adolescent mothers who marry the fathers, divorce rates
tend to be high (Furstenberg, Brooks-Gunn, & Morgan, 1987).
In addition to interest in the marital status of adolescent mothers, there
is growing interest in the quality of relationships they have with their male
partners or spouses. Leadbeater and Way (2001) conducted one of the few
studies of domestic violence among adolescent mothers in their sample
from New York City. They interviewed the mothers six years after their chil-
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 79
dren were born and asked about physical abuse, sexual abuse, and control-
ling behavior by male partners (e.g., telling them when they could leave the
house). Of the 93 young mothers in the sample, 41% had experienced
abuse by a male partner. Another 31% had experienced or were currently
involved in relationships with male partners that were viewed primarily as
negative and conflicted. Only 28% described their former or current rela-
tionships as positive or neutral. Mothers who were abused by their partners
experienced more depressive symptoms and were more likely to be self-
critical than mothers involved in positive relationships with their partners.
They also tended to change residences more frequently than other mothers
over the six years of the study. Maternal depressive symptoms and frequent
changes in residence were associated with more problem behaviors in chil-
dren at age 6.
Some studies have compared adolescent mothers and older mothers
(i.e., 20 or older) on measures of depression or other indicators of psycho-
logical well-being. Several studies have shown that adolescent mothers ex-
hibit more depressive symptoms than mothers who are older, but often
these comparison are made without controlling for possible confounding
variables (Osofsky, Hann, & Peebles, 1993). Whitman, Borkowski, Keogh,
and Weed (2001) assessed depression levels in their sample at three and
five years after the children were born. Approximately 10% of their sample
exhibited moderate to severe levels of depression at each assessment and
another quarter of the sample experienced mild depression levels. The ad-
olescent mothers also had self-esteem scores that were low relative to ado-
lescents in the general population. Given the troubled histories of some ad-
olescent mothers and the challenges of their current circumstances, it is
not surprising that some adolescent mothers exhibit signs of psychological
distress.
However, the young age of the mother may be less important for predict-
ing depression in later years than her marital status and background char-
acteristics. Using data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
(NLSY), Kalil and Kunz (2002) found that women who had their first chil-
dren as teenagers had higher depression scores at ages 27 to 28 than
women who had their first child at age 20 or later. However, the effect of
age at first birth was reduced to nonsignificance when marital status at the
time of the birth was controlled. Mothers who were unmarried had more
depressive symptoms in young adulthood than mothers who were married.
Further analyses suggested that prebirth individual and family characteris-
tics (i.e., selection factors) were stronger predictors of depressive symptoms
in young adulthood than either marital status or age at first birth. Women
with more depressive symptoms were more likely to have lived with a stepfa-
ther at age 14 and to have scored low on the Armed Forces Qualification
Test (AFQT), a measure of educational aptitude.
80 LUSTER AND HADDOW
Developmental Assessments
Although most infants born to adolescent mothers are healthy at birth, de-
velopmental delays become notable shortly thereafter for a disproportion-
ate number of these children (Whitman et al., 2001). At 1 year of age, chil-
dren born to adolescent mothers made fewer vocalizations than infants of
adult mothers (Culp, Osofsky, & O’Brien, 1996). Children of adolescent
mothers also had lower scores on receptive language assessments than their
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 81
peers born to adult mothers in the Notre Dame longitudinal study (Whit-
man et al., 2001). At 3 years of age, nearly three fourths of the children
born to adolescent mothers in the Notre Dame study experienced delays in
one domain of development, and nearly one half of the children had delays
in two domains of development.
Academic Difficulties
Investigators have found that these developmental delays extend past the
earliest years of life. It has been noted that children born to adolescent
mothers are at risk for low school readiness (Furstenberg et al., 1987;
Luster et al., 2000). This is a troublesome finding because school readiness
is predictive of academic outcomes in elementary school, as well as educa-
tional attainment in adulthood (Luster & McAdoo, 1996).
Academic difficulties continue for many children born to adolescent
mothers throughout their school years (Furstenberg et al., 1987; Whitman
et al., 2001). Furthermore, high rates of special education placement have
been documented for these students (Furstenberg et al., 1987; Whitman et
al., 2001). By adolescence, nearly fifty percent of children born to adoles-
cent mothers who participated in the Baltimore Longitudinal Project had
repeated a school grade (Furstenberg et al., 1987). Not surprisingly, low ed-
ucational aspirations have also been found in these youth (Furstenberg et
al., 1987). Thus, school achievement is an area of concern for children
born to adolescent mothers.
There are both selection factors and environmental factors that predis-
pose the children born to adolescent mothers to do less well in school than
their peers born to adult mothers ( Jaffee et al., 2001). Levine, Pollack, and
Comfort (2001) found that most of the relation between early childbearing
and academic and behavioral outcomes of children born to adolescent
mothers can be explained by individual and family background factors of
the adolescent mother. Adolescent mothers who participated in Whitman
and colleagues’ (2001) Notre Dame study had an average full scale IQ of
87, well below 100, the average IQ score for the population. Low intellec-
tual ability on the part of the mothers was predictive of poor school perform-
ance by the children.
It has also been documented that many of children born to adolescent
mothers did not grow up in enriched environments in which they were be-
ing stimulated by adult interaction and had developmentally appropriate
toys and activities (Dubow & Luster, 1990; Moore et al., 1997; Whitman et
al., 2001). In addition, Whitman and his colleagues (2001) found that chil-
dren born to adolescent mothers were more likely than their peers born to
adult mothers to exhibit insecure attachment, which may reduce their
problem-solving ability by undermining their ability to self-regulate (Bor-
82 LUSTER AND HADDOW
kowski & Dukewich, 1996). Low maternal intellectual ability, the quantity
and quality of their interaction with adults, rates of insecure attachment,
and unstimulating environments may be some of the underlying reasons
why the children born to adolescent mothers have more academic prob-
lems than children born to adult mothers.
Behavior Problems
and disciplined homes, had family members who valued education and
who were positive role models. Many successful adolescent mothers ac-
knowledged that support by family members had been provided condition-
ally, with an understanding that the parents had clear expectations for the
young mothers. Frequently, these mothers’ internal characteristics in-
cluded an optimistic attitude, autonomy, tenacity, and a strong desire to
succeed. Leadbeater and Way also found that these young women often
had access to and support for post-secondary education.
The psychological well-being of the mother has a profound effect on the
parenting of the child (Belsky, 1984). Maternal depression has been associ-
ated with problematic child behaviors and continued dependence on wel-
fare (Almgren, Yamashiro, & Ferguson, 2002). Crockenberg (1988) found
that higher levels of maternal social support increased both feelings of ma-
ternal self-esteem and positive parenting of her child. Furthermore, high
maternal self-esteem has been found to be predictive of children’s social
competence (Hubbs-Tait, Osofsky, Hann, & Culp, 1994). Similarly, it has
been reported that adolescent mother’s parenting skills are related to her
self-esteem (Hurlbut, Culp, & Jambunathan, 1997). In sum, the evidence
suggests that greater maternal psychological well-being contributes to posi-
tive outcomes for her child.
Academically, the young women had more education than any of the other
subgroups and were more likely to be at grade level. This group of adoles-
cent mothers was also characterized as being socially competent and high
in self-esteem.
Resilient mothers with vulnerable children accounted for nearly 20% of
the Notre Dame Project sample. In general, this subgroup of mothers had
high academic attainment and good social competence. However, this sub-
group differed from the previous subgroup in their ability to transition to
the role of mother, and the level of emotional support that they received
from their partners. The investigators reported that the mothers of this sub-
group were either less able or less willing to perform the role of mother.
Many were unaware of the developmental needs of their children and were
not child-centered in their focus. Most adolescent mothers in this group
also had conflicted or nonexistent relationships with the child’s father.
Vulnerable mothers with resilient children comprised nearly 14% of the
overall sample. Vulnerable mothers tended to be younger, to have com-
pleted less education, and were less likely to be at grade level. Many of these
mothers also had difficulty accepting their maternal role and often had lit-
tle child development knowledge. The vulnerable mothers who had resil-
ient children often reported positive relationships with the child’s father.
Interestingly, the children of this subgroup spent the greatest amount of
time in their grandparents’ care; these resilient children had surrogates to
provide for them.
The remaining 50% of the mother–child dyads of the Notre Dame Proj-
ect were categorized as vulnerable mothers and vulnerable children.
were rated as less supportive caregivers by home visitors (Luster, 1998). Un-
fortunately, we did not have a measure of IQ , so we could not determine if
educational attainment was predictive of parenting when intellectual ability
was controlled.
Zuravin and DiBlasio (1996) found that adolescent mothers who ne-
glected their children achieved lower levels of education than adolescents
mothers who did not neglect their children. However educational attain-
ment was unrelated to whether or not the child had been physically abused.
The lack of association between maternal education and physical abuse
may be due to the fact that the mothers were often not the perpetrators of
the abuse. The perpetrators were sometimes fathers, boyfriends, baby-
sitters, or unidentified people.
Whitman and colleagues (2001) assessed the intelligence of the young
mothers in their study with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised
(WAIS–R). The mothers who scored higher on the vocabulary and block
design subtests of the WAIS–R received more favorable scores on video-
taped interactions with their children. Similarly, in a sample of African
American mothers, most of whom were adolescents when their children
were born, mothers with low scores on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
(PPVT) were more likely to exhibit problematic parenting while working
with their preschoolers on puzzles (Wakschlag, Chase-Lansdale, & Brooks-
Gunn, 1996). However, mothers’ educational attainment was not a signifi-
cant predictor of parenting when PPVT scores and other background char-
acteristics were controlled.
Looking across studies, the findings are consistent with the view that ma-
ternal cognitive competence is associated with more supportive caregiving
practices. However, more research is needed to determine the processes in-
volved, if indeed there is a causal relation. Maternal intellectual ability
could have a direct effect on parenting given that parenting involves a fair
amount of problem solving, especially with uncommunicative infants.
Brighter or better educated mothers may also value education more and
work harder to promote the intellectual growth of their children than
mothers who struggled in school. Alternatively intellectual ability could af-
fect parenting or the home environment indirectly via family income or
poverty status.
support this hypothesis is limited. The child characteristics that have been
examined most include difficult temperament and low birth weight.
Some studies have found that irritable or difficult babies elicit less sup-
portive caregiving than their easier peers (e.g., Luster, 1998), but other
studies report no relation between the two variables (e.g., Hess, Papas, &
Black, 2002). Inconsistent results have also been found in studies of older
mothers (Crockenberg, 1986). As Crockenberg explained, the effect of in-
fant temperament on parenting may depend on characteristics of the
mother (e.g., patience, her interpretation of the behavior) or contextual
factors (e.g., level of support the mother receives or other stressors she is
dealing with). Thus, simple correlations between temperament and parent-
ing measures may not be very informative. We are likely to learn more by
examining the goodness-of-fit between the infant and parent characteristics
(Lerner, 1993) while considering the influence of other contextual factors,
such as other stressors and the mother’s level of social support.
Low birth weight and the health of the infant have been the focus of re-
searchers’ attention even less often than temperament. Two early studies
reported that preterm and low birth weight infants were at greater risk for
poor parenting by adolescent mothers during the newborn period (Field,
1980; Wise & Grossman, 1980). In our longitudinal study, we found that low
birth weight was associated with lower HOME scores when the infants were
12 months old, but was unrelated to subsequent measures of the home envi-
ronment. We speculated that low birth weight infants may be more difficult
to care for than their peers early in life, but differences in behavior may
largely disappear if there are no lingering medical complications.
Dubow, 1990) and neglect (Zuravin & DiBlasio, 1996). A precarious finan-
cial situation is also associated with frequent changes of residence which
can be problematic for children, especially when they reach school age
(Bates, Luster, & Vandenbelt, 2003). Most adolescents have difficulty mak-
ing ends meet if they must depend on income from their typically low-
paying jobs. Therefore, much depends on the financial support they re-
ceive from their parents or partners. In many cases, the father of the baby
or their current partner have low incomes and provide limited financial
support.
Social Support. How well adolescents deal with the challenges of parent-
ing and the other stressors they face is likely to depend on their personal
strengths and the amount of support that they receive from others. Several
94 LUSTER AND HADDOW
studies have examined the relation between the social support young moth-
ers receive from their families and the parenting they provide. These stud-
ies have produced mixed results with some studies showing that mothers
with higher levels of support providing more supportive care for their chil-
dren and other studies showing no relation between perceived support and
caregiving. In some cases, the lack of relationship between social support
and parenting may be due to the fact that the mothers who are having the
greatest difficulty adjusting to parenting are receiving the most support
from their families; the support is being offered by family members in re-
sponse to a perceived need on the part of the mother (Barratt, Roach, Mor-
gan, & Colbert, 1996). In these instances, social support may be helpful to
the mothers receiving it, but they would not necessarily receive high marks
on measures of parenting when compared to other adolescent mothers.
Moreover, in some cases support from family members has a direct effect
on the child rather than being mediated by the caregiving of the adolescent
mothers. Some of the children in our study who were doing very well in first
grade had been cared for primarily by their grandparents for most of their
lives (Luster et al., 2004).
The other key person in the young mother’s social network is likely to be
the father of the child or current partner. Potentially the male partner
could influence the mother and child by providing financial support for
the family, emotional support for the mother, and care for the child. How-
ever, there is surprisingly limited research on the relation between level of
support from the male partner and maternal caregiving or child outcomes.
The studies that have been done have produced mixed results with some
showing level of partner support having a positive effect on maternal
parenting at least some of the time (Unger & Wandersman, 1985) and oth-
ers reporting no relation (Voight, Hans, & Bernstein, 1996). The impor-
tance of support from the male partner may depend on other factors such
as how much support the mother is receiving from her family or on her own
developmental history (Crockenberg, 1987b; Quinton & Rutter, 1988).
Developmental History
who selected someone else or no one at all (Luster, 1998). We also asked
them about their relationship history with men. Our interest in this topic was
due to the advocates’ report that many of these young mothers had been in
abusive, controlling, or exploitive relations with men since childhood. Young
mothers who reported having more positive relationships with men also
tended to receive higher parenting ratings from the home visitors. Although
these questions did not focus solely on the period before the focal child was
born, the findings suggest that the relationship history of adolescent mothers
is relevant for understanding their approach to parenting.
CONCLUSIONS
This chapter is divided into four parts. The first section is concerned with
factors related to becoming an adolescent mother. The research shows
that early parenthood occurs in all demographic groups in the U.S., but
higher birth rates are found among teens who struggle in school, come
from low-income or multi-problem families, live in neighborhoods with
high concentrations of poverty, and are exposed to other risk factors.
Teens are less likely to become parents if their vision of their future in-
cludes going to school beyond high school and establishing a career be-
fore having children.
In the second section, we examined the impact of early childbearing on
the life course of the mother. Adolescent mothers are more likely than
peers who delay having children to: achieve low levels of education, live in
poverty, exhibit depressive symptoms, and rear their children as single par-
ents. At one time it was thought that these outcomes were largely the conse-
quence of early childbearing, and the research still suggests that early child-
bearing affects the life course of the mothers and their children. However,
recent research has considered the effect of selection factors on these out-
comes, and has contributed to a more balanced view about the combined
influence of selection factors and the effects of early childbearing on the
life-course outcomes of the mother.
In the third section, we focus on the children of adolescent mothers.
Given selection factors and the context in which many of these children are
being reared, it is not surprising that children of adolescents fare less well
than other children in various developmental domains. However, many
children of adolescent mothers are competent and well-adjusted. Typically
these children have received supportive care from their mothers and/or
other caregivers such as fathers and grandparents that set them on a posi-
tive developmental trajectory during the early years of life. Many of their
mothers are intelligent, child-centered individuals who are determined to
create a better life for their children.
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 97
Adolescent mothers, like older mothers, differ markedly in the way that
they parent their children. In the final section, we examined factors related
to individual differences in the caregiving practice of young mothers.
Correlational studies show that differences in caregiving are related to
characteristics of the young mothers, characteristics of the children, and
the context in which the relationship is evolving. The experiences of the
young mothers prior to the time that they have children also influence pa-
rental behavior.
The research that we have reviewed shows that much has been learned
about adolescent mothers and their children since the first edition of this
book was published a decade ago (Luster & Okagaki, 1993). Given the con-
tinuing interest in this topic, we expect the next decade of research to add
considerably to our understanding of these families. Insights gained from
these efforts are likely to be useful to those who work directly with young
mothers and their children, program planners, and policymakers.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Alan Guttmacher Institute. (2002). Facts in brief. Teenager’s sexual and reproductive health. Re-
trieved December 8, 2004, from http://www.agi-usa.org
Almgren, G., Yamashiro, G., & Ferguson, M. (2002). Beyond welfare or work: Teen mothers,
household subsistence strategies, and child development outcomes. Journal of Sociology and
Social Welfare, 29(3), 125–149.
Barratt, M. S., Roach, M. A., Morgan, K. M., & Colbert, K. K. (1996). Adjustment to mother-
hood by single adolescents. Family Relations, 45, 209–215.
Bates, L. V., Luster, T., & Vandenbelt, M. (2003). Factors related to social competence in ele-
mentary school among children of adolescent mothers. Social Development, 12(1), 107–124.
Bavolek, S. (1984). The Adult–Adolescent Parenting Inventory. Schaumburg, IL: Family Develop-
ment Resources.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55,
83–96.
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 2–62). New York: Academic Press.
Bolton, F. (1990). The risk of child maltreatment in adolescent parenting. Advances in Adoles-
cent Mental Health, 4, 223–237.
Borkowski, J., & Dukewich, T. (1996). Environmental covariations and intelligence: How at-
tachment influences self-regulation. In D. Determan (Ed.), Current topics in human intelli-
gence (Vol. 5, pp. 3–15). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
98 LUSTER AND HADDOW
Boyer, D., & Fine, D. (1992). Sexual abuse as a factor in adolescent pregnancy and child mal-
treatment. Family Planning Perspectives, 24, 4–11.
Brewster, K. (1994). Race differences in sexual activity among adolescent women: The role of
neighborhood characteristics. American Sociological Review, 59, 408–424.
Butler, J., & Burton, L. (1990). Rethinking teenage childbearing: Is sexual abuse a missing
link? Family Relations, 39, 73–80.
Cassidy, B., Zoccolillo, M., & Hughes, S. (1996). Psychopathology in adolescent mothers and
its effects on mother–infant interactions: A pilot study. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 41,
379–384.
Centers for Disease Control. (2001). Youth risk behavior surveillance system. Retrieved June 2003,
from http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dash/yrbs/
Chen, F. (1997). Factors related to adolescent mothers’ caregiving practices for their three-year-old chil-
dren. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
ment, Washington, DC.
Child Trends. (2001). Facts at a glance. Washington, DC. Retrieved December 8, 2004, from
http://www.childtrends.org
Child Trends. (2003). Facts at a glance. Washington, DC. Retrieved December 8, 2004, from
http://www.childtrends.org
Crockenberg, S. (1986). Are temperamental differences in babies associated with predictable
differences in caregiving. In J. V. Lerner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Temperament and child devel-
opment: New directions for child development (pp. 53–73). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward and punitive control of tod-
dlers by adolescent mothers. Child Development, 58, 964–975.
Crockenberg, S. (1988). Social support and parenting. In H. Fitzgerald, B. Lester, & M.
Yogman (Eds.), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 4, pp. 141–174). New York:
Plenum.
Culp, A., Osofsky, J., & O’Brien, M. (1996). Language patterns of adolescent and older moth-
ers. First Language, 16, 61–75.
Dubow, E., & Luster, T. (1990). Adjustment of children born to teenage mothers: The contri-
bution of risk and protective factors. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 393–404.
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early
childhood development. Child Development, 65, 296–318.
East, P., & Jacobson, L. (2001). The younger siblings of teenage mothers: A follow-up of their
pregnancy risk. Developmental Psychology, 37, 254–264.
Field, T. M. (1980). Interaction of preterm and term infants with their lower and middle-
income teenage and adult mothers. In T. M. Field, S. Goldberg, D. Stern, & E. Sostek
(Eds.), High-risk infants and children: Adult and peer interaction (pp. 113–132). New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
Furstenberg, F., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Morgan, S. (1987). Adolescent mothers in later life. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Geronimus, A., & Korenman, S. (1992). The socioeconomic consequences of teenage child-
bearing reconsidered. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107, 1187–1214.
Geronimus, A., Korenman, S., & Hillemeier, M. (1994). Does young maternal age adversely af-
fect child development? Evidence from cousin comparisons in the United States. Popula-
tion and Developmental Review, 20, 585–609.
Goerge, R., & Lee, B. J. (1997). Abuse and neglect of the child. In R. Maynard (Ed.), Kids hav-
ing kids: Economic costs and social consequences of teen pregnancy (pp. 205–230). Washington,
DC: Urban Institute Press.
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 99
Luster, T., Bates, L., Vandenbelt, M., & Nievar, M. A. (2004). Family advocates’ perspectives on
the early academic success of children born to low-income adolescent mothers. Family Rela-
tions, 53, 68–77.
Luster, T., & Dubow, E. (1990). Predictors of the quality of the home environment that adoles-
cent mothers provide for their school-aged children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19,
475–494.
Luster, T., & McAdoo, H. (1996). Family and child influences on educational attainment: A
secondary analysis of the High/Perry Preschool data. Developmental Psychology, 32, 26–39.
Luster, T., & Okagaki, L. (1993). Parenting: An ecological perspective (1st ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Luster, T., & Rhoades, K. (1989). The relation between childrearing beliefs and the home en-
vironment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Family Relations, 38, 317–322.
Luster, T., & Small, S. A. (1997). Sexual abuse history and number of sex partners among fe-
male adolescents. Family Planning Perspectives, 29(5), 204–211.
Manlove, J., Terry-Humen, E., Papillo, A. R., Franzetta, K., Williams, S., & Ryan, S. (2002). Pre-
venting teenage pregnancy, childbearing, and sexually transmitted diseases: What the research shows.
Washington, DC: Child Trends.
McCullough, M., & Scherman, A. (1998). Family-of-origin interaction and adolescent moth-
ers’ potential for child abuse. Adolescence, 33, 375–384.
McLoyd, V. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psycho-
logical distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development, 61,
311–346.
Miller, B. C. (2002). Family influences on adolescent sexual and contraceptive behavior. Jour-
nal of Sex Research, 39(1), 22–26.
Miller-Johnson, S., Winn, D. M., Coie, J., Maumary-Gremaud, A., Hyman, C., Terry, C., &
Lochman, J. (1999). Motherhood during the teen years: A developmental perspective on
risk factors for childbearing. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 85–100.
Moore, K. A., Miller, B. C., Glie, D., & Morrison, D. R. (1995). Adolescent sex, contraception, and
childbearing: A review of recent research. Washington, DC: Child Trends.
Moore, K., Morrison, D., & Greene, A. (1997). Effects on children born to adolescent mothers.
In R. Maynard (Ed.), Kids having kids: Economic costs and social consequences of teen pregnancy
(pp. 145–180). Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.
Moore, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Adolescent parenthood. In M. Borstein (Ed.), Handbook
of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (pp. 173–214). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Musick, J. S. (1993). Young, poor, and pregnant: The psychology of teenage motherhood. New Haven:
Yale University Press.
Nagin, D., Pogarsky, G., & Farrington, D. (1997). Adolescent mothers and the criminal behav-
ior of their children. Law and Society Review, 31(1), 137–162.
Osofsky, J. D., Hann, D. A., & Peebles, C. (1993). Adolescent parenthood: Risks and opportu-
nities for mothers and infants. In C. H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (pp.
106–119). New York: Guilford Press.
Panzarine, S., Slater, E., & Sharps, P. (1995). Coping, social support, and depressive symptoms
in adolescent mothers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 17, 113–119.
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting breakdown: The making and breaking of intergen-
erational bonds. Aldershot, UK: Avebury.
Rauch-Elnekave, H. (1994). Teenage motherhood: Its relationship to undetected learning
problems. Adolescence, 29, 91–103.
Reis, J. S., & Herz, E. J. (1987). Correlates of adolescent parenting. Adolescence, 22, 599–609.
Rodgers, J. L., & Rowe, D. C. (1993). Social contagion and adolescent sexual behavior: A devel-
opmental EMOSA model. Psychological Review, 100(3), 479–510.
Schellenbach, C. J., Whitman, T. L., & Borkowski, J. G. (1992). Toward an integrative model of
adolescent parenting. Human Development, 35, 81–99.
3. ADOLESCENT MOTHERS 101
Seitz, V., & Apfel, N. (1993). Adolescent mothers and repeated child-bearing: Effects of a
school-based intervention program. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 63, 572–581.
Sommer, K., Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Schellenbach, C., Maxwell, S., & Keogh, D.
(1993). Cognitive readiness and adolescent parenting. Developmental Psychology, 29,
389–398.
Spieker, S. J., Bensley, L., McMahon, R., Fung, H., & Ossiander, E. (1996). Sexual abuse as a
factor in child maltreatment by adolescent mothers of preschool aged children. Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 8, 497–509.
Small, S., & Luster, T. (1994). Adolescent sexual activity: An ecological, risk-factor approach.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 56, 181–192.
Stevens-Simon, C., & McAnarney (1994). Childhood victimization: Relationship to adolescent
pregnancy outcome. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 569–575.
Terry, E., & Manlove, J. (2000). Trends in sexual activity and contraceptive use among teens. Wash-
ington, DC: Child Trends.
Turley, R. N. L. (2003). Are children of young mothers disadvantaged because of their
mother’s age or family background? Child Development, 74, 465–474.
Unger, D., & Wandersman, L. P. (1985). Social support and adolescent mothers: Action re-
search contributions to theory and application. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 29–45.
Vandenbelt, M., Luster, T., & Bates, L. (2001). Caregiving practices of low-income adolescent
mothers and the academic competence of their first-grade children. Parenting Science and
Practice, 1, 185–215.
Voight, J. D., Hans, S. L., & Bernstein, V. J. (1996). Support networks of adolescent mothers:
Effects on parenting experiences and behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 58–73.
Wakschlag, L. S., Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Not just “Ghosts in the
Nursery”: Contemporaneous intergenerational relationships and parenting in young Afri-
can-American families. Child Development, 67, 2131–2147.
Ward, M. J., & Carlson, E. A. (1995). Associations among adult attachment representations,
maternal sensitivity, and infant–mother attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers.
Child Development, 66, 69–79.
Werner, E. (1994). Overcoming the odds. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
15(2), 131–136.
Werner, E. (1990). Protective factors and individual resilience. In S. J. Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff
(Eds.), Handbook of early intervention: Theory, practice and analysis (pp. 97–116). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1989). Vulnerable but invincible. New York: Adams, Bannister & Cox.
Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood.
Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Werner, E., & Smith, R. (2001). Journeys from childhood to midlife. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University
Press.
Whitman, T., Borkowski, J., Keogh, D., & Weed, K. (2001). Interwoven lives: Adolescent mothers
and their children. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Wise, S., & Grossman, F. K. (1980). Adolescent mothers and their infants: Psychological factors
in attachment and interaction. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50, 454–468.
Zoccolillo, M., Meyers, J., & Assiter, S. (1997). Conduct disorder, substance dependence, and
adolescent motherhood. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 67, 152–157.
Zoccolillo, M., & Rogers, K. (1991). Characteristics and outcomes of hospitalized adolescent
girls with conduct disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
30, 973–981.
Zuravin, S. J., & DiBlasio, F. A. (1996). The correlates of child physical abuse and neglect by
adolescent mothers. Journal of Family Violence, 11, 149–166.
This page intentionally left blank
4
Ross D. Parke
Jessica Dennis
Mary L. Flyr
Kristie L. Morris
Melinda S. Leidy
Thomas J. Schofield
University of California, Riverside
Several assumptions guide this chapter. First and most critical is our as-
sumption that the nature of the father–child relationship is best under-
103
104 PARKE ET AL.
There is clear evidence that mothers and fathers contribute through differ-
ent pathways to their children’s development. Perhaps the most well estab-
lished difference between parents of different genders is their relative in-
vestment in play and caregiving. Fathers participate less than mothers in
caregiving but spend a greater percentage of the time available for interac-
tion in play activities than mothers do. In the U.S. Kotelchuck (1976) found
that fathers spent a greater percentage of their time with their infants in
play (37.5%) than mothers did (25.8%), although in absolute terms moth-
ers spent more time than fathers in play with their children. Similar find-
ings have been reported from a longitudinal investigation of parent–infant
interaction in England (Richards, Dunn, & Antonis, 1977). At both 30 and
60 weeks of age, playing with their infants was the most common activity of
4. FATHERS 105
fathers, and over 90% of the fathers played regularly with their infants.
Lamb (1977) observed interactions among mother, father, and infant in
their homes at 7 to 8 months and again at 12 to 13 months. Marked differ-
ences emerged in the reasons that fathers and mothers pick up infants. Fa-
thers were more likely to hold the babies simply to play with them, whereas
mothers were far more likely to hold them for caregiving purposes. It is not
only the quantity of time in play that discriminates between mother and fa-
ther involvement in infancy; the quality of play activity does so as well.
Power and Parke (1982) observed mothers and fathers interacting with
their 8-month-old infants in a laboratory playroom. Fathers played more
bouncing and lifting games, especially with boys, than mothers. In contrast,
mothers played more watching games in which a toy is presented and made
salient by moving or shaking it. Observations of father–and mother–infant
interactions in unstructured home contexts with older infants reveals simi-
lar mother–father differences in play style (Clarke-Stewart, 1980; Lamb,
1977).
Nor are these effects evident only in infancy. MacDonald and Parke
(1984), in an observational study of the play interaction patterns between
mothers, fathers, and 3- and 4-year-olds, found that fathers engaged in
more physical play with their children than mothers, while mothers en-
gaged in more object-mediated play than fathers. However, according to
MacDonald and Parke (1986), the father’s distinctive role as physical play
partner changes with age. Physical play was highest between fathers and 2-
year-olds, and between 2 and 10 years of age there was a decreased likeli-
hood that fathers engaged their children physically.
In spite of the decline in physical play across age, fathers are still more
often physical play partners than mothers. In an Australian study of parents
and their 6- to 7-year-old children (Russell & Russell, 1987), fathers were
more involved in physical/outdoor play interactions and fixing things
around the house and garden than mothers. In contrast, mothers were
more actively involved in caregiving and household tasks and in school
work. Mothers were also involved in more reading, playing with toys, and
helping with arts and crafts.
In all studies reviewed, a reasonably consistent pattern emerges: Fathers
are tactile and physical, and mothers tend to be verbal, didactic, and toy-
mediated in their play. Clearly, infants and young children experience a
qualitatively different stimulatory pattern from their fathers and mothers.
Interaction refers to the father’s direct contact with his child through care-
giving and shared activities. Availability is a related concept concerning the fa-
ther’s potential availability for interaction, by virtue of being present or acces-
sible to the child whether or not direct interaction is occurring. Responsibility
refers to the role the father takes in ascertaining that the child is taken care of
and arranging for resources to be available for the child. (Lamb, Pleck,
Charnov, & Levine, 1987, p. 125)
As several previous authors (e.g., Palkovitz, 2002; Parke, 2000) found, the
focus of research on fathers has been primarily on face-to-face parent–child
interaction. To a large degree this emphasis reflects the common assump-
tion that parental influence takes place directly through face-to-face con-
tact or indirectly through the impact of the interaction on another family
member. Similarly, the availability issue has been addressed, but largely
through the research on father absence which is a consequence of either di-
vorce or unwed parenthood (Mott, 1994; Garfinkel, McLanahan, Meyer, &
Seltzer, 1998). Less is known about the determinants or consequences of
availability of fathers among residential fathers. Only recently have re-
searchers and theorists begun to recognize the managerial function of par-
ents (Lamb et al.’s responsibility notion) and to appreciate the impact of vari-
ations in how this managerial function influences the child’s development
(Parke & O’Neil, 2000). By managerial, we refer to the way in which parents
organize and arrange the child’s home environment and set limits on the
range of the home setting to which the child has access and the opportuni-
ties for social contact with playmates and socializing agents outside the fam-
ily. The managerial role may be just as important as the parent’s role as
stimulator, because the amount of time that children spend interacting
with the inanimate environment far exceeds their social interaction time
(White, Kaban, Shapiro, & Attonucci, 1976).
It is important to distinguish among domains of involvement, since fa-
thers and mothers vary in their distribution of time across different child
and household activities. Several distinctions have been made in the prior
literature including personal care activities, involvement in play, leisure
and affiliative activities with children (Beitel & Parke, 1998; Radin, 1993).
More recently Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, and Hofferth (2001) ex-
panded the domain list to include not just personal care and play but
achievement-related activities (i.e., homework, reading), household activi-
ties (i.e., housework, shopping), social activities (i.e., conversation, social
events) and other activities, and examined how the determinants of in-
volvement in these domains vary across fathers. Finally, recent estimates of
father involvement have usefully distinguished between weekdays and
weekends since both the types of activities and levels of father involvement
vary as a function of the time period being assessed (Yeung et al., 2001).
108 PARKE ET AL.
fathers function as the primary caregivers about 23% of the time. Pleck and
Masciadrelli (2004) noted:
Fathers were the primary care arrangement (23%) as often as child care cen-
ters and preschools combined (24%), as often as family day care homes
(23%), and more often than grandparents (16%). The fact that fathers are
the primary caregivers during mothers’ working hours in more than one out
of five dual-career families with preschool children suggests that a much
higher proportion of fathers have significant childcare responsibility than is
usually thought. Some estimate that fathers’ involvement in all aspects of
child care, not just during their wives’ working hours, is nearly a third of the
total child-care by U.S. dual-career couples in the 1990s. (p. 242)
Although more mothers are entering the work force, current occupational
arrangements still mean that the vast majority of fathers have less opportu-
nity for interaction with their children than mothers.
mately 3 hours a day. By ages 9 to 12, the level of involvement has decreased
to 2 hours, 15 minutes. Activities vary across age as well. Time in personal
care with fathers (either interaction or accessibility) drops from one hour
per day for infants to 30 minutes for 9–12-year-olds. Play/companionship
activities with fathers are more common among infants and toddlers (44
minutes/day) than at later ages (23 minutes for 9–12-year-olds). While in-
door games and toy play as well as outdoor activities and sports decrease,
T.V. and video watching increases across age. Achievement-related activi-
ties which include reading, educational play, and studying increases from 7
to 27 minutes from toddlerhood to pre-adolescence. The pattern is similar
on weekends but the absolute amount of time in which fathers are either in-
volved or accessible nearly doubles. Not unexpected is the rise in house-
hold activities (i.e., shopping) and social activities (i.e., religious services)
on weekends for fathers and their children. As the Yeung et al. study clearly
underscores, both age and type of activity need to be considered in descrip-
tions of father involvement.
fant cries than men with higher baseline testosterone levels. Moreover, fa-
thers with higher baseline prolactin levels are more positive and alert in re-
sponse to infant cries. Since similar links between androgen levels and
responsiveness to cries are evident for non-fathers as well, it suggests that
“human fathers’ androgen levels at baseline are associated with a nurturant
style and are predictive of responsiveness to infant cries” (Corter & Flem-
ing, 2002, p. 167). However, experience does appear to play a role as well.
At 2 days after the birth of a baby, fathers in contrast to non-fathers show
lower levels of testosterone. Moreover, fathers with more experience with
babies have lower testosterone and higher prolactin levels than first time fa-
thers (Corter & Fleming, 2002)—even after controlling for paternal age.
This perspective recognizes the dynamic or transactional nature of the links
between hormones and behavior in which behavior changes can lead to
hormonal shifts and vice versa. In contrast to the myth of the biologically
unfit father, this work suggests that men may be more prepared even bio-
logically for parenting than previously thought. Finally, it is critical to un-
derscore that these hormonal changes are not necessary for the elicitation
of fathering behavior in either animals or humans (Fleming & Li, 2002;
Corter & Fleming, 2002). In humans, for example, studies of father–infant
relationships in the cases of adoption clearly suggest that hormonal shifts
are unnecessary for the development of positive father–infant relationships
(Brodzinsky & Pinderhughes, 2002). Next, we turn to a discussion of the so-
cial determinants of father involvement.
mate that, in turn, may have carryover effects to the enactment of roles in
home settings. The focus is generally on short-term or transitory effects. A
second type of linkage focuses on the types of attitudes and values that
adults acquire in the workplace and on how these variations in job experi-
ence alter their behavior in family contexts. In contrast to the short-term
perspective of the spillover of emotional climate research, this second type
examines more enduring and long-lasting effects of work on family life.
Work in the first tradition has been conducted by Repetti (1994), who
studied the impact of working in a high stress job (air-traffic controller) on
subsequent family interaction patterns. She found that male air traffic con-
trollers were more withdrawn in marital interactions after high-stress shifts
and tended to be behaviorally and emotionally withdrawn during interac-
tions with their children as well. Although high workload is associated with
withdrawal, negative social experiences in the work place have a different
effect. Distressing social experiences at work were associated with higher
expressions of anger and greater use of discipline during interaction with
the child later in the day. Repetti and Wood (1997) found similar effects for
mothers who withdrew from their preschoolers on days when the mothers
experienced greater workloads or interpersonal stress on the job. Repetti
views this as a spillover effect in which there is transfer of negative feeling
across settings. Similarly, Crouter, Bumpus, Maguire, and McHale (1999)
found that parents who reported high work pressure and role overload had
more conflicts with their adolescents.
Research in the second tradition of family–work linkages, namely the ef-
fects of the nature of men’s occupational roles on their fathering behavior,
dates back to the classic work of Kohn (1995). Men who experience a high
degree of occupational autonomy value independence in their children,
recognize children’s intentions when considering discipline, and use rea-
soning and withdrawal of rewards instead of physical punishment. In con-
trast, men who are in highly supervised jobs with little autonomy value con-
formity and obedience, focus on consequences rather than intentions and
use more physical forms of discipline. In short, they repeat their job-based
experiences in their parenting roles. Several researchers extended this
work by focusing on the outcomes of job characteristics for children’s de-
velopment. Cooksey, Menaghan, and Jokielek (1997) found that children
had fewer behavior problems when their mothers’ work involved more au-
tonomy, working with people, and more problem-solving opportunities.
Similarly, others found that fathers with greater job complexity and auton-
omy were less authoritarian (Grimm-Thomas & Perry-Jenkins, 1994), re-
sponded with greater warmth to their children, and provided more verbal
explanations (Greenberger, O’Neil, & Nagel, 1994).
As these studies clearly suggest, fathers’ and mothers’ work experiences
impact their parenting behavior. However, the process probably operates
116 PARKE ET AL.
in both directions in which parents’ home experience affects their job per-
formance as well. In fact, arguments at home with a wife or with a child
were negatively related to father’s work performance (Frone, Yardley, &
Markel, 1997). Perhaps, a positive home-based experience would create a
positive mood and possibly enhance one’s workday. These studies under-
score the importance of moving beyond employment status per se to a de-
tailed exploration of the nature of work in our future investigations of fam-
ily–work linkages.
had taken an active role in rule setting, teaching morals, and disciplining,
and 87% felt that as fathers, they had the unique characteristics that would
best serve the needs of their children.
More recently, Hofferth (2003) has provided an even clearer portrait of
African-American fathers using a nationally representative sample of Afri-
can American, Caucasian, and Hispanic fathers. In comparison to White fa-
thers, African-American fathers spend less time with their children on a
regular basis, although the differences are relatively modest (12.76 hours
per week vs. 15.35 hours per week for African-American and White fathers
respectively). This finding is in part due to family size, family structure and
employment patterns. “Black fathers have larger families, are less likely to
be the biological father and are more likely to be unemployed living with a
female breadwinner, all of which by themselves, would result in less time
spent with children” (Hoffreth, 2003, p. 211). In spite of these differences
in level of involvement, African-American fathers assumed greater responsi-
bility for routine care of their children than white fathers. This level of re-
sponsibility is consistent with the less traditional attitudes of Black fathers
toward maternal roles than White fathers. African-American fathers are less
likely to endorse such views, as “preschool children are likely to suffer if
their mother is employed.” In terms of child-rearing style, Black fathers rate
themselves as lower on warmth and more controlling than white fathers—a
finding consistent with earlier work (McAdoo, 1988; Gadsen, 1999). Finally,
African-American fathers were higher in monitoring their children than
White fathers. As others (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997) have argued
neighborhood characteristics may account for these patterns. African-
American fathers control and monitor their children more because their
neighborhoods are more dangerous. Family size may explain this differ-
ence as well since African-Americans have larger families than Caucasian
families and larger families require more parental control.
Several studies have focused on the determinants of involvement for Af-
rican-American fathers (Hofferth, 2003; Toth & Xu, 1999). As in the case of
White fathers, higher numbers of work hours is linked with less father in-
volvement. When spouses are employed outside the home, Black fathers
spend more time with their children—a pattern found in earlier studies of
White fathers (Coltrane, 1996). Neighborhood characteristics are important
as well. Black fathers who live in predominantly African-American neighbor-
hoods exhibit higher levels of warmth than those who reside in more hetero-
geneous neighborhoods (Hofferth, 2003). In sum, while there are differ-
ences in style and level of involvement, the determinants of variation in
involvement in African-American families and White families are similar.
Although it is important to understand differences between African-
Americans and other cultural groups, it is also necessary to recognize that
there is considerable variability within African-American families as well.
120 PARKE ET AL.
tural values found amongst Latinos. One such value is familism which em-
phasizes family cohesion, cooperation, and closeness (Cauce & Domenech-
Rodriguez, 2002). Other cultural values that are hypothesized to be related
to father involvement include the concept of machismo, which in recent
years has been revised from earlier, largely misguided notions of manly self-
reliance and sexuality to include masculine loyalty and responsibility
(Mirandé, 1997). Empirical research has indeed found that Latino fathers
spend similar lengths of time involved in caring for their children as fathers
of other ethnicities. Specifically, some studies have found that Latino fa-
thers are more likely to be engaged in caretaking than European Ameri-
cans (Bartkowski & Xu, 2000; Toth & Xu, 1999; Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-
Kean, & Hofferth, 2001). Others have found no difference in the amount
of father engagement between fathers of different ethnicities (Hofferth,
2003; Hossain, Field, Malphurs, Valle, & Pickens, 1995; NICHD Early Child
Care Research Network, 2000).
When focusing on different types of engagement activities with their
children, researchers have found both patterns of similarity and differences
between Latino fathers and other ethnicities. Similar to other types of fa-
thers, Latino fathers spend more time in play with their children relative to
mothers (Toth & Xu, 1999; Yeung et al., 2001). Latino fathers have been
found to be more involved in household and personal care activities than
European-American fathers (Yeung et al., 2001). Finally, Latino fathers
show similar levels of affection and warmth to their children as fathers of
other ethnicities (Hofferth, 2003; Toth & Xu, 1999).
Studies of responsibility in Latino fathers have mostly focused on the ex-
tent to which Latino fathers monitor or restrict their children’s behavior.
Toth and Xu (1999) found that Latino fathers reported increased restric-
tion of their children’s behavior over European-American fathers. Hofferth
(2003) found that Latino fathers report less monitoring of their children’s
behavior than European-American fathers. Hofferth did, however, find
that Latino fathers report more responsibility for care of their children
than White fathers in spite of the fact that she found no difference in level
of involvement between Latino and White fathers. Discrepancies between
these two studies in the extent to which Latino fathers monitor their chil-
dren relative to other ethnicities may be due to sample characteristics in
each study; clearly more work is needed on the level of responsibility taken
by fathers in Latino families.
Several studies have examined factors related to increased paternal in-
volvement for Latinos. Among relevant demographic variables, high in-
come, maternal employment, and maternal hours of work have been re-
lated to increased paternal involvement in Latino families (Fagan, 1998).
Similarly, Coltrane, Parke, and Adams (2004) have found that fathers were
more involved with their children in families where mothers earned a
122 PARKE ET AL.
Native American Fathers. In spite of the fact that there are 1.5 million
Native Americans in the United States, the role of Native American fathers
has been largely neglected in research. It is important to note that one can-
not generalize across all Native Americans since there are 280 different
tribal groups and 161 linguistic groups (Staples, 1988). Some Native Ameri-
cans live on reservations, some do not. Thus, the terms Native Americans or
American Indians refer to a variety of cultures, languages, nationalities, and
family systems (Mirandé, 1991), just as in the case of other groups.
While no one group can be considered typical, Mirandé (1991) has used
the Navajo as a way of illustrating some common features of Native Ameri-
can families and how the family structure has been altered by contact with
the dominant, Euro-American society. Traditionally, Navajo women were
equal to, if not greater than, men, in terms of family roles. Women were re-
sponsible for the care and maintenance of children. Many teaching and dis-
ciplinary functions were carried out by the mother’s brothers, not the fa-
ther. As the society came in contact more with Euro-American culture, the
father became increasingly important within the family and the mother lost
much of her influence (Mirandé, 1991).
Native American families rely heavily on the community as a whole.
Children are protected, loved, and nurtured not only by parents, but also by
siblings, aunts, uncles, grandparents, cousins, and other extended family
(Mirandé, 1991). Therefore, fathering in the traditional Native-American
community is best examined in the context of the extended family (Burgess,
1980). The extended family network partakes in child rearing, which pro-
vides a safeguard and protection for children and parents do not have abso-
4. FATHERS 123
lute or even primary authority over children (Mirandé, 1991). Cross (1986)
asserted that community opinion governs parents’ childrearing behavior,
and parents who do not comply are often ridiculed into compliance. In some
Native American tribes, the role of disciplinarian is assigned to a specific
tribal member or sometimes even a mythological figure, thus removing the
burden of physical discipline from parents and placing it on a “wise one.” In
fact, adults seldom, if ever, strike children, nor do they talk loudly or yell at
children, especially when correcting them. Native-American children do not
expect praise for doing what is required of them. Parents occasionally praise
children for doing well, and indicate their approval through a smile, pleasant
tone of voice, or friendly pat (Burgess, 1980). Burgess (1980) claimed that
for children, knowledge of approval and disapproval is undoubtedly a power-
ful means of social control in Native American societies.
In one of the few studies of paternal involvement among Native Ameri-
can fathers, Williams, Radin, and Coggins (1996) examined childrearing
and school performance among the Ojibwa children. Higher amounts of
father involvement were associated with better academic functioning and
better Native American adaptive functioning. Native American functioning
assessed adaptive traits that researchers believed to be important in most
Native American populations. For sons in particular, increased amounts of
father involvement were associated with better academic functioning, lower
levels of internalization, and better Native-American adaptive functioning.
Fathers who expected higher community leadership for their children
spent a higher percentage of their time as primary caregivers. It was also
surprising that children of more nurturant fathers appeared to be less com-
petent in school. However, Williams et al. suggest that this could be a result
of the father responding to a child who is struggling in a primarily Euro-
American classroom with individualistic and competitive atmosphere, which
is very different from the tribal society to which the child is accustomed.
Overall, this study indicated that there is a strong relation between fathers’
greater presence and sons’ better cognitive and social development.
While the role of the father in Native American families is still poorly
understood, it is clear that in traditional Native-American society, ulti-
mate power and authority is vested in neither the father nor the mother,
but rather in the community as a whole (Mirandé, 1991). Fathers have
been found to influence child academic achievement, especially for sons.
However, more research is needed to examine the particular amount of
involvement and responsibility Native-American fathers have in child-
rearing, and, in turn, how this is changing across generations as a conse-
quence of assimilation.
Asian Fathers. Asian fathers have typically been viewed as aloof and
uninvolved with their children. Again, this stereotype has some historical
basis, but the portrait of the modern Asian father is changing due to both
124 PARKE ET AL.
tional adage ‘strict father, kind mother’—wherein fathers exert high de-
grees of authoritarian control and mothers manifest high degrees of
warmth” (p. 73). Consistent with this view, Shek (2000) found that Chinese
adolescents perceived maternal parenting as being more positive than pa-
ternal parenting. Adolescents stated that fathers showed less concern, less
responsiveness, and higher levels of harshness than mothers. Also, father–
adolescent communication was perceived less positively and as occurring
less frequently than mother–adolescent communication. However, Janko-
wiak (1992) reported that fathers spoke to their children, from ages 1 to 6,
in a softer tone than mothers. Fathers were actually very affectionate.
In contrast to the general picture that emerges for East Asian families,
studies of Asian immigrant families in the United States have found few dif-
ferences between mothers and fathers in levels of authoritarian parenting
control and warmth (Chao & Kim, 2000). Again, acculturation clearly plays
a role in helping to understand differences in mother–father childrearing
styles. These patterns present a clear challenge to our stereotype of the ma-
ternal and paternal childrearing styles among Asian-American families. Di-
versity among Asian Americans needs to be recognized in light of the differ-
ences in the recency of entry of some groups (e.g., Hmong, Vietnamese)
relative to other groups (e.g., Chinese, Japanese) into the United States. As
in the case of fathers in other cultural minorities, there is a gap between
cultural stereotypes and reality.
The marital stability of couples who have preschool children is protected. Al-
though new parents may be experiencing increase tension or dissatisfaction
as couples, their joint involvement with managing the baby’s and the family’s
needs may lead them to put off, or possibly work harder on, the problems in
their marriage—at least while the children are young. (p. 110)
In sum, it is clear that there are long-term positive effects of father involve-
ment on marriage, even though there may, in some cases, be a decline in
satisfaction accompanying the transition to fatherhood.
Does fatherhood alter men’s own development? First, men’s self-identity
changes after the onset of parenthood. The Cowans (2000) assessed role
shifts during the transition to fatherhood and found that men who become
fathers decreased the partner/lover aspect of their self and increased the
parent percentage of their self-rating over the 21-month assessment period.
Self-esteem, however, was not affected by the transition to parenthood for
either fathers or mothers in the Cowans’ project. Grossman (1987), who
studied men’s transition to parenthood, found that first time fathers who
were both more affiliative (i.e., importantly connected to others, enjoying
empathetic relationships) and more autonomous (viewing themselves as
separate and distinct from others) had significantly higher life adaptation
scores. Fathers of firstborns who were more affiliative at 1 year also reported
being higher in emotional well-being. These findings suggest that “sepa-
rateness and individuation are not sufficient for men’s well-being; they
need connections as well” (Grossman, 1987, p. 107).
Does fatherhood have a longer-term impact on men’s psychological de-
velopment? Heath and Heath (1991), in a longitudinal study of college
men, found that fatherhood related to men’s ability to understand them-
selves, to understand others sympathetically, and to integrate their own
feelings. Second, just as women have difficulty balancing work and parent-
ing, there is evidence that men experience role strain between family and
occupational demands as well (Parke & Brott, 1999; Levine & Pittinsky,
1998). For example, several studies report that more involved fathers feel
that they have less time for their careers and that their family obligations in-
terfere with their occupational demands (Baruch & Barnett, 1986). Simi-
larly, Greenberger and O’Neil (1990) found that work–family role strain is
lowest when there is low work commitment and high parental involvement.
Although policy debates (e.g., Hochschild, 1989) usually involve role con-
flicts for mothers, there has been increased recognition that in dual-career
families, balancing work and family obligations is clearly an issue that af-
fects parents of both genders (Parke & Brott, 1999).
Does fatherhood affect generativity, a concept derived from Erikson’s
(1975) theoretical writings? Snarey (1993) provided a succinct summary:
The psychosocial task of middle adulthood, Stage 7 (in Erikson’s Stage the-
ory) is the attainment of a favorable balance of generativity over stagnation
and self-absorption . . . Most broadly, Erikson (1975) considers generativity to
mean any caring activity that contributes to the spirit of future generations,
such as the generation of new or more mature persons, products, ideas, or
works of art . . . generativity’s psychosocial challenge to adults is to create,
care for, and promote the development of others from nurturing the growth
128 PARKE ET AL.
Snarey (1993) described three types that apply to fathers, namely (1) bio-
logical generativity (indicated by the birth of a child), (2) parental gen-
erativity (indicated by child rearing activities), and (3) societal generativity
(indicated by caring for other younger adults; serving as a mentor, provid-
ing leadership, and contributing to generational continuity). Is men’s soci-
etal generativity at mid-life related to the level of care and support they pro-
vide their children?
Snarey (1993) found that men who nurtured their children’s social-
emotional development during childhood (0–10 years) and who also con-
tributed to both social-emotional and intellectual-academic development
during the second decade (11–21 years) were at mid-life more likely to be-
come generative in areas outside their family. Again this contribution of fa-
ther participation to societal generativity was evident after controlling for a
variety of background variables. Snarey offered several interpretations of
these findings. First, a disequilibrium explanation suggests that parental
child-rearing responsibility results in demands that are difficult to meet,
and that, in turn, promote “increased complexity in the fathers’ cognitive
emotional and behavioral repertoire. . . . This commitment beyond the
self, in turn, prepares the way for societal generativity which involves a com-
mitment beyond the family” (pp. 117–118). Second, perhaps a nurturing
predisposition may underlie both parenting and societal generativity and
account for the continuity across time. Third, the arrival of children often
leads to increases in men’s participation in neighborhood and community
organizations on behalf of children, which, in turn, may continue into the
mid-life years. In summary, although the processes are not yet well under-
stood, it is clear that involved fathering relates in positive ways to other as-
pects of men’s lives. As Snarey (1993) noted, “men who are parentally gen-
erative during early adulthood usually turn out to be good spouses,
workers, and citizens at mid-life” (p. 119).
Others have found support for these links between childrearing in-
volvement (e.g., taking child on routine errands and jobs; consulting with
teachers, supervising homework) and generativity (McKeering & Paken-
ham, 2000). Moreover, paternal engagement is related to civic engage-
ment in a large representative sample (Eggebeen & Knoester, 2001). At
the same time, other studies report only small or marginal links between
care giving involvement and generativity (Bailey, 1992; Christiansen &
Palkovitz, 1998). Clearly more work is needed before strong conclusions
about the caregiving–generativity link can be firmly established (Pleck &
Masciadrelli, 2004).
4. FATHERS 129
Contact Between Nonresident Fathers and Their Children. Research has fo-
cused on large national samples of fathers and children, such as the Na-
tional Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY), the National Survey of Chil-
dren (NSC), and the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH).
These surveys reveal a high level of disengagement on the part of nonresi-
dent fathers but at the same time sufficient variability to permit an examina-
tion of the issue of the impact of contact on children’s development. A
meta-analysis of 63 studies of the associations between nonresident fathers’
contact and children’s well-being was reported by Amato and Gilbreth
(1999). Although they found weak associations between contact and aca-
demic success and internalizing problems, there was no link between con-
tact and externalizing problems. In general, frequency of contact with non-
resident fathers was not linked to child outcomes. This conclusion is
consistent with other scholarly evaluations of the evidence (Eggebeen,
2002; McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994).
However, quality and not presence/absence alone, is important in as-
sessing the impact of nonresident fathers. In a follow-up study of 18- to 21-
year-old children of African-American adolescent mothers, Furstenberg
and Harris (1993) found little impact of contact alone on young adults’ out-
comes but clear beneficial effects if the quality of the relationship was taken
into account. Those who reported a strong bond or attachment with their
father during adolescence had higher educational attainment, were less
likely to be imprisoned, and were less depressed. These effects were espe-
cially evident in the case of children living with the father and were only
marginally evident for nonresident biological fathers. The data suggest that
both presence and quality matters; but quality is especially important be-
cause fathers’ presence is unrelated to outcomes when quality (degree of
attachment to father) is controlled. The Amato and Gilbreth (1999) meta-
analysis confirms these earlier findings; a measure of the affective relation-
ship between the father and child (feeling close) was positively associated
130 PARKE ET AL.
(p. 1183). First, most studies rely on a single data source which raises the
problem of shared method variance. Second, many researchers do not con-
trol for the quality of the mother–child relationship when examining father
effects. Since the behavior and attitudes of parents are often highly related,
this step is critical. Only 8 of the 72 studies reviewed by Marsiglio et al.
(2000) did, in fact, control for the quality of the mother–child relationship;
however, five of the eight studies continued to show a father effect after tak-
ing into account mother–child effects. For example, Isley, O’Neil, and
Parke (1996) found that fathers’ level of affect and control predicted chil-
dren’s social adaptation with peers both concurrently and one year later af-
ter controlling for maternal effects (see also Mosley & Thompson, 1995;
Hart, Nelson, Robinson, Olsen, & McNeilly-Choque, 1998).
Pleck and Masciadrelli (2004) cited several other recent studies that con-
trolled for maternal involvement and avoided the same reporter problem.
For example, Aldous and Mulligan (2002) using a national data set found
that positive paternal engagement is linked to lower frequency of later be-
havior problems for boys and for difficult-to-rear children. Similarly, in a
large sample of British teenagers, positive paternal engagement predicted
positive school attitudes (Flouri, Buchanan, & Bream, 2002). Pleck and
Masciadrelli (2004) concluded that in over 70% of the studies that were
methodologically sound (controlling for maternal effects and independent
data sources), there were positive correlations between child outcomes and
paternal involvement. Although there is overlap between the effects of
mothers and fathers on their children’s academic, emotional, and social de-
velopment, evidence is emerging that fathers make a unique contribution
to their children’s development (Parke, 2002; Rohner, 1998).
A third caveat concerns problems of inferring direction of causality be-
cause studies are correlational and involve concurrent rather than longitu-
dinal assessments (Marsiglio et al., 2000). However, two strands of evidence
suggest that the direction of effects can plausibly flow from paternal behav-
ior to child outcomes. First, longitudinal studies support the view that fa-
thers influence their children (see Amato & Rivera, 1999; Parke, 1996,
2002; Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004, for reviews). For example, Gottman, Katz,
and Hooven (1997) found that fathers’ acceptance of and assistance with
their children’s emotions (sadness, anger) at 5 years of age were related to
higher levels of social acceptance with peers at age 8. Positive father en-
gagement in tenth grade was related to fewer behavior problems one year
later (Zimmerman, Salem, & Notaro, 2000). Nor are the effects of fathering
on developmental outcomes restricted to childhood. In a follow-up of the
classic childrearing study of Sears, Maccoby, and Levin (1957), Koestner,
Franz, and Weinberger (1990) reassessed a sample of the original children
when they were 31 years old. The most powerful predictor of empathy in
adulthood for both men and women was paternal child-rearing involve-
132 PARKE ET AL.
ment at age 5. This paternal factor was a better predictor than several other
maternal variables. In a further study (Franz, McClelland, & Weinberger,
1991), at age 41, men and women with better social relationships (marriage
quality; extra-familial ties) in mid-life had experienced more paternal
warmth as children. Although these studies support a father effects per-
spective, it is likely that reciprocal relationships will become evident, in
which children and fathers mutually influence each other across the life
course (Parke, 2002).
ated with less aggressive behavior with peers. Later work has isolated other
emotional processes such as emotional regulation (McDowell, Kim, O’Neil,
& Parke, 2002) and knowledge and use of display rules (McDowell & Parke,
2000) that, in turn, are influenced by paternal (as well as maternal) interac-
tion patterns and are predictive of children’s social acceptance.
Although father involvement in infancy and childhood is quantitatively
less than mother involvement, the data suggest that fathers nevertheless do
have an important impact on their offspring’s development.
In light of the evidence it is clear that strategies aimed at increasing father in-
volvement are worthwhile. Since, fathering is multi-determined, suggestions
and policies aimed at facilitating fathers involvement need to be multi-level
in nature as well. Parke and Brott (1999) provide some guidelines for in-
creasing father involvement, by noting various steps that men, women, the
government and the private sector can take to increase father involvement.
Men themselves can increase their involvement in various ways. These in-
clude taking the initiative more rather than waiting to be asked. “If men are
going to be fully involved, they are going to have to share responsibility for
the household and childcare in an active fashion” (Parke & Brott, 1999, p.
193). This means being a partner not merely a helper. Second, men need to
get more practice in parenting skills. Whatever women know about raising
children they learned by doing and men can learn to improve their parent-
ing as well. Programs are available to help fathers learn the basics of child-
care, but men have to be willing to take advantage of these opportunities
(Fagan & Hawkins, 2000). While many women gained these critical child-
care experiences while they were growing up (Parke, 2000), many men have
to catch up when they become fathers. Third, men need to be emotionally
available to their children if they are going to be fully effective parents. As
Gottman (1994) wisely notes: “Men must allow themselves to be aware of
their feelings so they can empathize with their children. Then they must take
whatever steps necessary to make themselves available to their kids. They
must structure their lives so they give more time and attention to their chil-
dren” (p. 183). And nonresidential fathers need to stay involved if they are
single, separated or divorced. To achieve this goal fathers need to know their
legal rights as a noncustodial parent, but also be aware of their responsibili-
ties to continue to play an active role in their children’s lives and to meet
their financial support obligations.
As we noted earlier, fathering is a negotiated set of activities and mothers
play an important part in determining how much fathers are involved. Just
as men need to rethink their family roles as “assistants” to mothers, women
134 PARKE ET AL.
need to change their expectations about their partners as well and commu-
nicate that parenting is a shared enterprise, not just a mother’s responsibil-
ity. “Asking for help makes it seem as if what he’s (fathers) helping with is
really the women’s job and that she should be grateful” (Parke & Brott,
1999, p. 197). Second, women should be gate openers, not gate closers. As
research has shown, when women function as gate closers men’s participa-
tion is less (Allen & Hawkins, 1999; Beitel & Parke, 1998). To increase fa-
ther participation, women need to be facilitors and encourage men to play
a part in family caregiving routines. Third, and closely related to the gate-
keeper issue is the need to adjust standards. Women need to recognize that
men and women may have different standards, if fathers are going to be full
participants. As Parke and Brott (1999) wryly observed, “No child ever suf-
fered long term trauma by having her diaper put on a bit looser than
mother would like” (p. 196). Modifying standards in non-critical domains
may encourage greater father participation.
In view of our ecological systems perspective, it is clear that individuals
and couples cannot be expected to achieve changes in father involvement
without support from societal institutions. Schools can play a role in this
process by providing parenting education for both boys and girls, begin-
ning in elementary school. This is not a new idea; the National PTA recom-
mended that parenthood education be included in the curriculum as early
as 1925. Second, physicians need to treat fathers as parents. Too often hos-
pitals and doctors treat fathers—even expectant fathers—as second class
citizens whose primary usefulness is to pay the bills. Fathers need opportu-
nities to learn about the care and feeding of new babies in the hospital and
to have programs that are sensitive to their needs and roles (Fagan &
Hawkins, 2000). Pediatricians can encourage men to be present at well-
baby checkups and to make evening appointment available so fathers can
be there. The government can play a role too by funding public awareness
campaigns by federal, state, and local governments about the importance
of fathers in the lives of children, and government can overhaul welfare
practices to encourage father involvement by not reducing maternal wel-
fare benefits when fathers are active residential partners. Government poli-
cies, such as the Family and Medical Leave Act which gives fathers the same
right as mothers to take time off after the birth of their children, need to be
upheld and extended. At present, this law does not provide wages for the
parents and by offering a financial safety net, more men (and women)
would take advantage of these opportunities. On the legal front a policy of
encouraging joint custody is an important tool for keeping more fathers in-
volved in their children’s lives and reducing some of the negative effects of
divorce on children. The media can encourage father involvement by por-
traying fathers realistically and not as inept, uninvolved, and unimportant.
Instead television can help shape our visions of men as partners for their
4. FATHERS 135
wives and as involved and equal contributors to the care and up bringing of
their children.
It is imperative that these policies and programs be carefully crafted and
organized to appeal to fathers of different ethnic backgrounds. This kind of
cultural sensitivity is crucial to attracting and retaining fathers in these pro-
grams and for these men to appreciate the relevance of these policies to
their lives.
As Parke and Brott (1999) conclude:
Promoting a cultural change in the ways that society views fathers and the ways
that men view themselves in this role is no easy task. Just as there are many real
barriers that converge and conspire to limit fathers’ involvement, it will take a
coordinated effort by men, women, the media, government and the private sec-
tor to bring about a new and more involved era of fatherhood. It’s no easy task,
but children, women, and men themselves will all benefit if we can increase fa-
ther’s involvement as we enter the twenty-first century. (p. 204)
One goal of this chapter is to extend an invitation to join this effort and to
provide the scholarly foundation that underlies such an undertaking.
CONCLUSIONS
Our review suggests that fathers play important roles in the family and their
involvement has significant consequences for children, mothers, and men
themselves. Father involvement is multiply determined and a variety of in-
fluences, including individual, familial, marital, institutional, and cultural
factors, need to be considered in order to understand variations in level of
involvement. By recognizing fathers’ embeddedness in community and cul-
ture as well as in family contexts, we will more fully understand the father’s
role and be better positioned to design intervention efforts to increase
fathers’ level of involvement in the lives of their children.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Thanks to Faye Harmer for her assistance in the preparation of the manu-
script. Support from NIMH grant RO1MH54154 to Parke, in part, facili-
tated the work described in this chapter.
REFERENCES
Aldous, J., & Mulligan, G. M. (2002). Fathers’ child care and children’s behavior problems.
Journal of Family Issues, 54, 699–707.
136 PARKE ET AL.
Allen, J., & Hawkins, A. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mothers’ beliefs and behaviors that in-
hibit greater father involvement in family work. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61,
199–212.
Amato, P. A., & Gilbreth, J. G. (1999). Nonresident fathers and children’s well-being: Meta
analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 15–73.
Amato, R. R., & Rivera, F. (1999). Paternal involvement and children’s behavior. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 61, 375–384.
Bailey, W. T. (1992). Psychological development in men: Generativity and involvement with
young children. Psychological Reports, 71, 929–930.
Bartkowski, J. P., & Xu, X. (2000). Distant patriarchs or expressive dads?: The discourse and
practice of fathering in conservative Protestant families. Sociological Quarterly, 41, 465–485.
Baruch, G. K., & Barnett, R. C. (1986). Fathers’ participation in family work and children’s sex-
role attitudes. Child Development, 57, 1210–1223.
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and sub-
stance abuse. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11, 56–94.
Beitel, A., & Parke, R. D. (1998). Paternal involvement in infancy: The role of maternal and pa-
ternal attitudes. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 268–288.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55,
83–96.
Belsky, J., Rovine, M., & Fish, M. (1989).The developing family system. In M. Gunnar & E.
Thelen (Eds.), Systems and development: Vol. 22. Minnesota symposia on child psychology (pp.
119–166). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Belsky, J., Ward, H. J., & Levine, M. (1986). Prenatal expectations, postnatal experiences and
the transition to parenthood. In R. Ashmore & D. Brodzinsky (Eds.), Perspectives on the fam-
ily (pp. 111–146). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Belsky, J., Youngblood, L., Rovine, M., & Volling, B. (1991). Patterns of marital change and
parent–child interaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 487–498.
Bianchi, S. M. (2000). Maternal employment and time with children: Dramatic change or sur-
prising continuity? Demography, 37, 401–414.
Bonney, J. F., Kelley, M. L., & Levant, R. F. (1999). A model of fathers’ involvement in child
care in dual-earner families. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 401–415.
Booth, A., & Amato, P. (1994). Parental marital quality, divorce and relations with offspring in
young adulthood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 21–34.
Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.). (1991). Cultural approaches to parenting. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Boyum, L., & Parke, R. D. (1995). Family emotional expressiveness and children’s social com-
petence. Journal of Marriage and Family, 57, 593–608.
Brodzinsky, D. M., & Pinderhughes, E. (2002). Parenting and child development in adoptive
families. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 279–312).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Burgess, B. J. (1980). Parenting in the Native-American community. In M. R. Fantini & R.
Cárdenas (Eds.), Parenting in a multicultural society (pp. 63–73). New York: Longman.
Carson, J., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent–child interactions and
children’s peer competency. Child Development, 67, 2217–2226.
Cauce, A. M., & Domenech-Rodriquez, M. (2002). Latino families: Myths and realities. In J. M.
Contreras, K. A. Kerns, & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Latino children and families in the United
States (pp. 3–25). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Chao, R. K., & Kim, K. (2000). Parenting differences among immigrant Chinese fathers and
mothers in the United States. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 1, 71–91.
Chao, R., & Tseng, V. (2002). Parenting of Asians. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting
(2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 59–94). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
4. FATHERS 137
Christiansen, S. L., & Palkovitz, R. (1998). Exploring Erikson’s psychosocial theory of develop-
ment: Generatively and its relationship to paternal identity, intimacy, and involvement in
childcare. Journal of Men’s Studies, 7, 133–156.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1980). The father’s contribution to children’s cognitive and social devel-
opment in early childhood. In F. Pedersen (Ed.), The father–infant relationship. New York:
Praeger.
Coley, R. L., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (1999). Stability and change in paternal involvement
among urban African-American fathers. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 416–435.
Collins, W. A., & Russell, A. (1991). Mother–child and father–child relationships in middle
childhood and adolescence. Developmental Review, 11, 99–136.
Coltrane, S. (1996). Family man. New York: Oxford.
Coltrane, S., Parke, R. D., & Adams, M. (2004). Complexity of father involvement in low in-
come Mexican-American families. Family Relations, 53, 179–189.
Coltrane, S., & Valdez, E. (1993). Reluctant compliance: Work–family role allocation in dual
earner Chicano families. In J. Hood (Ed.), Men, work, and family (pp. 151–174). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Conger, R., & Elder, G. (1994). Families in troubled times. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Eco-
nomic pressure in African-American families: A replication and extension of the family
stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 179–193.
Cooksey, E. C., Menaghan, E. G., & Jokielek, S. M. (1997). Life course effects of work and fam-
ily circumstances on children. Social Forces, 76, 637–667.
Cooper, J. E., Holman, J., & Braithwaite, V. A. (1983). Self-esteem and family cohesion: The
child’s perspective. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 153–159.
Corter, C., & Fleming, A. S. (2002). Psychobiology of maternal behavior in human beings. In
M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 141–182). Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1992). When parents become partners. New York: Basic Books.
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (2000). When parents become partners. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Cox, M. J., & Paley, B. (1997). Families as systems. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 243–267.
Cross, T. L. (1986). Drawing on cultural tradition in Indian child welfare practice. Social Case-
work, 67, 283–289.
Crouter, A. C. (1994). Processes linking families and work: Implications for behavior and de-
velopment in both settings. In R. D. Parke & S. Kellam (Eds.), Exploring family relationships
with other contexts (pp. 9–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Crouter, A. C., Bumpus, M. F., Maguire, M. C., & McHale, S. M. (1999). Linking parents’ work
pressure and adolescents’ well being: Insights into dynamics in dual-earner families. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 35, 1453–1461.
Cummings, E. M., & O’Reilly, A. W. (1997). Fathers in family context: Effects of marital quality
on child adjustment. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The father’s role in child development (3rd ed.). New
York: Wiley.
Daly, K. (1995). Reshaping fatherhood: Finding the models. In W. Marsiglio (Ed.), Fatherhood:
Contemporary theory, research and social policy (pp. 21–40). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dancy, B., & Handal, P. (1984). Perceived family climate, psychological adjustment, and peer
relationship of Black adolescents: A function of parental marital status or perceived family
conflict. Journal of Community Psychology, 12, 222–229.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. (1997). Externalizing behavior problem and discipline revis-
ited: Nonlinear effect and variation by culture, context and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8,
161–175.
Dickie, J. R., & Matheson, P. (1984, August). Mother–father–infant: Who needs support? Paper pre-
sented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto.
138 PARKE ET AL.
Dienhart, A., & Daly, K. (1997). Men and women co-creating father involvement in a
nongenerative culture. In A. J. Hawkins & D. C. Dollahite (Eds.), Generative fathering (pp.
147–164). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Doherty, W. E. J., Kouneski, E. F., & Erikson, M. (1998). Responsible fathering: An overview
and conceptual framework. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 277–292.
Durrett, M. E., Otaki, M., & Richards, P. (1984). Attachment and the mother’s perception of
support from the father. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 7, 167–176.
Eggebeen, D. J. (2002). Sociological perspectives on fatherhood: What do we know about fa-
thers from social surveys? In C. S. Tamis-LeMonda & N. Cabrera (Eds.), Handbook of father
involvement (pp. 189–210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Eggebeen, D. J., & Knoester, C. (2001). Does fatherhood matter for men? Journal of Marriage
and Family, 63, 381–393.
Erikson, E. (1975). Life history and the historical moment. New York: Norton.
Fagan, J. (1998). Correlates of low-income African American and Puerto Rican fathers’ in-
volvement with their children. Journal of Black Psychology, 24, 351–367.
Fagan, J., & Hawkins, A. J. (Eds.). (2000). Clinical and educational interventions with fathers. New
York: Haworth Press.
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone and prolactin are as-
sociated with emotional responses to infant cues in new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 43,
399–413.
Fleming, A. S., & Li, M. (2002). Psychobiology of maternal behavior and its early determinants
in nonhuman mammals. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp.
61–98). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fleming, A. S., Ruble, D., Krieger, H., & Wong, P. Y. (1997). Hormonal and experimental cor-
relates of maternal responsiveness during pregnancy and the puerperium in human moth-
ers. Hormones and Behavior, 31, 145–158.
Flouri, E., Buchanan, A., & Bream, V. (2002). Adolescents’ perceptions of their fathers’ in-
volvement: Significance to school attitudes. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 575–582.
Franz, C. E., McClelland, D., & Weinberger, J. (1991). Childhood antecedents of conventional
social accomplishment in midlife adults: A 26-year prospective study. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.
Frone, M. R., Yardley, J. K., & Markel, K. S. (1997). Developing and testing an integrative
model of the work–family interface. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 145–167.
Furstenberg, F. F., & Harris, K. M. (1993). When and why fathers matter: Impacts of father in-
volvement on children of adolescent mothers. In R. I. Lerman & T. J. Ooms (Eds.), Young
unwed fathers (pp. 117–138). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Gadsen, V. (1999). Black families in intergenerational and cultural perspective. In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), Parenting and child development in “nontraditional” families (pp. 221–246). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Garfinkel, I., & McLanahan, S. S. (1995). The effects of child support reform on child well be-
ing. In J. Brooks-Gunn & L. Chase-Lansdale (Eds.), Escape from poverty: What a difference for
children? New York: Cambridge University Press.
Garfinkel, L., McLanahan, S., Meyer, D., & Seltzer, J. (1998). Fathers under fire: The revolution of
child support enforcement. New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press.
Gorman-Smith, D., Tolan, P. H., Zelli, A., & Huesmann, R. L. (1996). The relations of family
functioning to violence among inner-city minority youths. Journal of Family Psychology, 10,
115–129.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Greenberger, E., & O’Neil, R. (1990). Parents’ concerns about their child’s development: Im-
plications for fathers’ and mothers’ well-being and attitudes toward work. Journal of Mar-
riage and Family, 52, 621–635.
4. FATHERS 139
Greenberger, E., O’Neil, R., & Nagel, S. K. (1994). Linking workplace and home place: Rela-
tions between the nature of adults’ work and their parenting behaviors. Developmental Psy-
chology, 30, 990–1002.
Grimm-Thomas, K., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (1994). All in a day’s work: Job experiences, self-
esteem and fathering in working-class families. Family Relations, 43, 174–181.
Grossman, F. K. (1987). Separate and together: Men’s autonomy and affiliation in the transi-
tion to parenthood. In P. W. Berman & F. A. Pedersen (Eds.), Men’s transition to parenthood
(pp. 89–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., & McNeilly-Choque, M. K. (1998).
Overt and relational aggression in Russian nursery-school-age children: Parenting style and
marital linkages. Developmental Psychology, 34, 687–697.
Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., McNeilly-Choque, M. K., Porter, C. L.,
& McKee, T. R. (2000). Russian parenting styles and family processes: Linkages with sub-
types of victimization and aggression. In K. A. Kerns, J. M. Contreras, & A. M. Neal-Barnett
(Eds.), Family and peers: Linking two social worlds. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Heath, D. H., & Heath, H. E. (1991). Fulfilling lives; Paths to maturity and success. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Heiss, J. (1996). Effects of African American family structure on school attitudes and perform-
ance. Social Problems, 43, 246–265.
Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Intimate fathers. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Hochschild, A. (1989). The second shift: Working parents and the revolution at home. New York: Vi-
king Press.
Hofferth, S. L. (2003). Race/ethnic difference in father involvement in two-parent families:
Culture, context, or economy? Journal of Family Issues, 24, 185–216.
Hossain, T., Field, I., Malphurs, J. E., Valle, C., & Pickens, J. (1995). Father caregiving in low in-
come African-American and Hispanic American families. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Miami Medical School, Miami, Florida.
Hyde, B. L., & Texidor, M. S. (1988). A description of the fathering experience among Black
fathers. Journal of Black Nurses Association, 2, 67–78.
Ishii-Kuntz, M. (1993). Japanese fathers: Work demands and family roles. In J. C. Hood (Ed.),
Men, work, and family (pp. 45–67). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
Ishii-Kuntz, M. (1995). Paternal involvement and perception toward fathers’ roles: A compari-
son between Japan and the United States. In W. Marsiglio (Ed.), Fatherhood: Contemporary
theory, research, and social policy (pp. 102–118). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
Ishii-Kuntz, M. (2000). Diversity within Asian American families. Handbook of family diversity
(pp. 274–292). New York: Oxford University Press.
Isley, S., O’Neil, R., & Parke, R. D. (1996). The relation of parental effect and control behavior
to children’s classroom acceptance: A concurrent and predictive analysis. Early Education
and Development, 7, 7–23.
Jain, A., Belsky, J., & Crnic, K. (1996). Beyond fathering behavior: Types of dads. Journal of Fam-
ily Psychology, 10, 431–442.
Jankowiak, W. (1992). Father–child relations in urban China. In B. S. Hewlett (Ed.), Fa-
ther–child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 345–363). New York: Aldine De
Gruyter.
Knight, G., Tein, J., Prost, J. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (2002). Measurement equivalence and re-
search on Latino children and families: The importance of culturally informed theory. In
J. M. Contreras, K. K. Kerns, & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Latino children and families in the
United States (pp. 181–202). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Koestner, R., Franz, C., & Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins of empathic concern: A 26-
year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.
Kohn, M. (1995). Social structure and personality through time and space. In P. Moen, G. H.
Elder, & K. Luscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human devel-
opment (pp. 141–168). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
140 PARKE ET AL.
McDermid, S. M., Huston, T., & McHale, S. (1990). Changes in marriage associated with the
transition to parenthood: Individual differences as a function of sex-role attitudes and
changes in the division of household labor. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 475–486.
McDowell, D. J., & Parke, R. D. (2000). Differential knowledge of display rules for positive and
negative emotion: Influence from parents, influences on peers. Social Development, 9,
415–432.
McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., O’Neil, R., & Parke, R. D. (2002). Children’s emotional regulation
and social competence in middle childhood: The role of maternal and paternal interactive
style. Marriage and Family Review, 34, 345–364.
McKeering, H., & Pakenham, K. I. (2000). Gender and generativity issues in parenting: Do fa-
thers benefit more than mothers from involvement in child care activities? Sex Roles, 43,
459–480.
McLanahan, S. S., & Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single parent: What helps, what hurts.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Meany, M. J., Stewart, J., & Beatty, W. W. (1985). Sex differences in social play: The socializa-
tion of sex roles. In J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Bear, C. W. Bushnell, & P. Slater (Eds.), Advances in
the study of behavior (Vol. 15, pp. 1–58). New York: Academic Press.
Mirandé, A. (1991). Ethnicity and fatherhood. In F. W. Bozett & S. M. H. Hanson (Eds.), Fa-
therhood and families in cultural context (pp. 53–82). New York: Springer Publishing Com-
pany.
Mirandé, A. (1997). Hombres et machos: Masculinity and Latino culture. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Mishel, L., Bernstein, J., & Schmitt, J. (1999). The state of working America. Ithaca, NY: Cornell
University Press.
Miwa, S. (1995). Parent–child relationships from the child’s viewpoint. In L. Nagoyaka (Ed.),
Contemporary child rearing and parent–child relationships. Nagoya: Nagayaki.
Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers social coaching, mother–child relationship style and
children’s peer competence: Is the medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312–332.
Montemayer, R. (1982). The relationship between parent–adolescent conflict and the amount
of time adolescents spend alone with parents and peers. Child Development, 68, 312–332.
Montemayer, R., & Brownlee, J. (1987). Fathers, mothers, and adolescents: Gender based dif-
ferences in parental roles during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 281–291.
Mosley, J., & Thompson, E. (1995). Fathering behavior and child outcomes: The role of race
and poverty. In W. Marsigllo (Ed.), Fatherhood: Contemporary theory, research and social policy
(pp. 158–165). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Mott, F. L. (1994). Sons, daughters and fathers’ absence: Differentials in father-leaving proba-
bilities and in home environments. Journal of Family Issues, 5, 97–128.
New, R., & Benigni, L. (1987). Italian fathers and infants: Cultural constraints on paternal be-
havior. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The father role: Cross-cultural perspectives. New York: Wiley.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1997). Child care in the first year of life. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 43, 340–360.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). Factors associated with fathers’
caregiving activities and sensitivities with young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 14,
200–219.
O’Connell, M. (1993). Where’s Papa? Father’s role in child care. Washington, DC: Population Ref-
erence Bureau.
Palkovitz, R. (2002). Involved fathering and men’s adult development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Parcel, J. L., & Menaghan, E. G. (1994). Parents’ jobs and children’s lives. New York: Aldine de
Gruyter.
Parke, R. D. (1981). Fathers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
142 PARKE ET AL.
Robinson, J. (1988). Who’s doing the housework? American Demographics, 10, 24–28.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.
Rohner, R. P. (1998). Father love and child development; History and current evidence. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 157–161.
Roopnarine, J. C., Hooper, F. H., Ahmeduzzaman, M., & Pollack, B. (1993). Gentle play part-
ners: Mother–child, father–child play in New Delhi, India. In K. MacDonald (Ed.), Par-
ent–child play (pp. 287–304). Albany: State University of New York Press.
Rosenblatt, J. (2002). Hormonal basis of parenting in mammals. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Hand-
book of parenting (Vol. 2, pp. 3–25). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Russell, G., & Russell, A. (1987). Mother–child and father–child relationships in middle child-
hood. Child Development, 58, 1573–1585.
Sagi, A. (1982). Antecedents and consequences of various degrees of paternal involvement in
childrearing: The Israeli project. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), Nontraditional families: Parenting and
child development (pp. 205–232). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sagi, A., Lamb, M. E., Shoham, R., Dvir, R., & Lewkowicz, K. S. (1985). Parent–infant interac-
tion in families on Israeli kibbutzim. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8,
273–284.
Sandberg, J. F., & Hofferth, S. L. (2001). Changes in children’s time with parents: United
States, 1981–1997. Demography, 38, 423–436.
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of childrearing. Evanston, IL: Row Peter-
son.
Shek, D. L. T. (2000). Differences between fathers and mothers in the treatment of, and rela-
tionship with, their teenage children: Perceptions of Chinese adolescents. Adolescence, 35,
135–146.
Shwalb, D. W., Kawai, H., Shoji, J., & Tsunetsugu, K. (1997). The middle class Japanese father:
A survey of parents of preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 497–511.
Silverstein, L. B., & Auerbach, C. F. (1999). Deconstructing the essential father. American Psy-
chologist, 54, 397–407.
Smith, P. K., & Daglish, L. (1977). Sex differences in parent and infant behavior in the home.
Child Development, 48, 1250–1254.
Snarey, J. (1993). How fathers care for the next generation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.
Staples, R. (1988). The Black American family. In C. H. Mindel, R. W. Habenstein, & R. W.
Wright, Jr. (Eds.), Ethnic families in America: Patterns and variations (pp. 173–195). New York:
Elsevier.
Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2000). Hormonal corre-
lates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fathers. Evolution and Human Behav-
ior, 21, 79–95.
Sun, L. C., & Roopnarine, J. L. (1996). Mother–infant, father–infant interaction and involve-
ment in child care and household labor among Taiwanese families. Infant Behavior and De-
velopment, 19, 121–129.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Cabrera, N. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of father involvement. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Toth, J. F., & Xu, X. (1999). Ethnic and cultural diversity in fathers’ involvement: A racial/eth-
nic comparison of African American, Hispanic, and White fathers. Youth and Society, 31,
76–99.
U.S. Census Bureau. (1995). Population reports. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2003). Population reports. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.
Wachs, T. (2000). Necessary but not sufficient. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
tion.
White, B. L., Kaban, B., Shapiro, B., & Attonucci, J. (1976). Competence and experience. In
I. C. Uzgiris & F. Weizmann (Eds.), The structuring of experience (pp. 115–152). New York:
Plenum.
144 PARKE ET AL.
White, L., & Keith, B. (1990). The effect of shift work on the quality and stability of marital re-
lations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 453–462.
White, L., & Rogers, S. L. (2000). Economic circumstances and family outcomes: A review of
the 1990’s. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1035–1051.
Williams, E., Radin, N., & Coggins, K. (1996). Paternal involvement in childrearing and the
school performance of Ojibwa children: An exploratory study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42,
578–595.
Yeung, W. J., Sandberg, J. F., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Hofferth, S. L. (2001). Children’s time with
fathers in intact families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, 136–154.
Zimmerman, M. A., Salem, D. A., & Notaro, P. C. (2000). Make room for daddy II: The positive
effects of fathers’ role in adolescent development. In R. D. Taylor & M. C. Wang (Eds.), Re-
silience across contexts: Family, work, culture, and community (pp. 233–258). Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
II
CHARACTERISTICS
OF THE CHILD
This page intentionally left blank
5
Priscilla K. Coleman
Bowling Green University
INTRODUCTION
147
148 KARRAKER AND COLEMAN
child may resent the attention and insist on being left alone. The child’s re-
sponses to the parent’s behaviors then influence the parent’s subsequent
cognitions, emotions, and behaviors, which then affect the child’s re-
sponses, and so on.
Dialectical and contextualistic theoretical developments over the last
several decades have also supported the view of an active child. For exam-
ple, Vygotsky (1978) argued that development proceeds through the proc-
ess of internalization whereby externally experienced events are translated
into personal, mental activity. The view of the child as capable of influenc-
ing his or her own development is further exemplified by the concept of
constructive epigenesis (Bidell & Fischer, 1997), which focuses on the pri-
mary role of self-organizing activity in the development of new abilities.
Bronfenbrenner (1979) also advanced a bioecological model (most re-
cently termed the Process-Person-Context-Time Model; Bronfenbrenner &
Morris, 1998), which offers a theoretical perspective within a contextual
framework for understanding how particular processes involving environ-
mental components interact with characteristics of the individual child to
differentially impact development. In this model, parenting is viewed as a
proximal process in development. The influence of parenting on develop-
ment is conceptualized as one that varies in relation to child characteristics.
Belsky’s (1984) widely cited discussion of the primary influences on the
quality of parenting extends some of Bronfenbrenner’s earlier notions.
Belsky proposed that the main influences on the quality of parenting in-
clude parents’ psychological resources, child characteristics, and socio-
cultural or environmental factors.
Very little scholarly work explored child effects on others until results
from the New York Longitudinal Study (Thomas et al., 1963) effectively
challenged the dominant belief that parents were solely responsible for
their children’s development. Child behavior problems were reported to
emerge occasionally despite the presence of positive parenting practices,
and adaptive child behaviors were sometimes evident when children were
exposed to dysfunctional parenting behavior. Moreover, components of
children’s temperament and particular aspects of their rearing environ-
ments were found to combine to produce positive and negative outcomes
(Chess, Thomas, & Birch, 1965).
Despite the current prevailing consensus that children do have powerful
effects on others, there are surprisingly few studies devoted specifically to
examining the role of such effects in parent–child relations. Researchers,
parents, and society more generally persist in assuming that parent-to-child
effects represent the most salient causal path in the socialization process.
With this assumption rooted heavily in classic theories of development, tra-
dition in the field, intuition, and conventional wisdom (Harris, 1998; Pat-
5. CHILD CHARACTERISTICS AND PARENTING 151
terson & Fisher, 2002), other explanations for correlations between parent
behavior and child outcomes are not as actively researched as might be ex-
pected. Many behavioral geneticists (e.g., Plomin, 1990; Scarr, 1988) and
environmentalists (e.g., Wachs, 1992; Wachs & Kohnstamm, 2001) have at-
tempted to examine the child’s role in models of environmental influence.
Nevertheless, many contemporary examples suggest that researchers are
not taking child effects seriously. For example, in an extensive recent re-
view of literature on parenting related to child and parent psychopathology
by Berg-Nielsen, Vikan, and Dahl (2002), the unidirectional bias is quite
obvious. Although child effects are alluded to at the beginning of the re-
port, they are not mentioned again until the last page under a segment de-
voted to future research.
The tendency to ignore or minimize the effects of the child on parenting
in particular and the childrearing environment more generally cannot be
attributed to declining interest in the study of parenting as the number of
empirically based studies of parenting has increased dramatically in recent
years (Bornstein, 1995, 2002; Patterson & Fisher, 2002). The outpouring of
scholarly work crossing numerous disciplines including psychology, sociol-
ogy, social work, and nursing, among others, is undoubtedly tied to the in-
creased complexity and multiple pressures characterizing the lives of con-
temporary parents (Patterson & Fisher, 2002). Because much parenting
research is motivated by the desire to identify ways that parents can posi-
tively influence their children, the research agenda tends to be biased to-
ward examining parent effects rather than child effects.
As we explore the merits of including child effects in the study of
parenting processes, an examination of the definitions used in the litera-
ture is a logical starting point. Berg-Nielsen and colleagues (2002) offer a
very straightforward definition of parenting when they define it as every-
day parental behavior (including cognitions, emotions, and attributions)
directed toward children in addition to relevant attitudes and values. Most
contemporary researchers define the construct similarly; however, as noted
by O’Connor (2002), different scholars have emphasized distinct compo-
nents (e.g., behavior over socio-cognitive processes) and have adopted
different assessment strategies designed to measure parent and child vari-
ables separately or to examine dyadic behaviors such as synchrony, mutu-
ality, or reciprocity. Efforts have also been made to define competent and
less competent or dysfunctional parenting, with competent parenting typ-
ically referring to internal attributes and behaviors believed to promote
positive physical, emotional, cognitive, and social development in chil-
dren and dysfunctional parenting referring to internal attributes and be-
haviors believed to adversely impact the child’s development (Berg-Nielsen
et al., 2002).
152 KARRAKER AND COLEMAN
the parent) using the same methodology (typically questionnaires). The re-
sulting correlations between the two sets of measures may be inflated as a
result of having the same person provide both measures. Second, correla-
tions observed between parent behavior and child characteristics or behav-
iors may very well be due to third variables in the environment. For exam-
ple, an association observed between inconsistent parenting and aggressive
child behavior may be the result of stress or parental divorce causing both
the inconsistency and the aggression. Third, research suggesting that par-
ent behaviors cause variations in child outcomes often ignores explanations
based on shared genes. For instance, an association between frequent pa-
rental verbal stimulation and children’s academic achievement could very
easily be based on a shared genetic endowment that leads to both verbal
and academic competence.
Many observed correlations between parenting cognitions and behaviors
and child characteristics and outcomes probably do represent the result of
a causal relation. The difficulty lies in determining whether the parenting
variable causes the child variable or the child variable causes the parenting
variable. The remainder of this section describes the possible mechanisms
underlying each of these causal paths, as well as the potential roles of ge-
netic and contextual factors in determining or identifying causation.
The most compelling evidence for child effects on parents comes from ex-
perimental and quasi-experimental studies in which adults’ responses to
children whose target characteristics systematically vary are compared. For
example, research by Barkley and colleagues (cited in Patterson & Fisher,
2002) revealed that when children with ADHD were medicated, and there-
fore demonstrated less hyperactivity than when they were unmedicated,
their mothers’ behavior became less aversive. Similarly, a study by Ander-
son, Lytton, and Romney (1986) showed that women exhibited more nega-
tive behavior when interacting with boys diagnosed with conduct disorder
than when interacting with their own boys who did not have the disorder.
Stern and Hildebrandt (1986) illustrated that women interacted differently
with unfamiliar infants who were labeled as having been born prematurely
and infants labeled as having been born at full term, independent of the in-
fants’ actual birth status.
Experimental manipulation of children’s characteristics or behavior is
often not possible, so again, most of our information about the effects of
children on parents is inferred from correlational data. The mechanisms
underlying the presumed effects of children’s characteristics and behaviors
on parenting behavior include both direct and mediated effects of child
characteristics and behaviors on parenting behavior. The most commonly
identified mediators are parent cognitions, such as beliefs, attitudes, values,
5. CHILD CHARACTERISTICS AND PARENTING 155
and Fulker (1988) found higher correlations for nonadoptive than for
adoptive siblings on measures of parental provision of toys and parental re-
striction–punishment. Similarly, Dunn and Plomin (1986) reported higher
correlations for nonadoptive than for adoptive siblings on a measure of ma-
ternal affection derived from observations of mother–child interaction
from infancy to early childhood. Plomin, Loehlin, and DeFries (1985) also
reported higher correlations in nonadoptive siblings than in adoptive sib-
lings for relations between Home Observation for the Measurement of the
Environment (HOME) scores and 2-year-olds’ Bayley mental scores as well
as scores on a measure of language development. There are also research
findings to suggest that the level of genetic influence on child and adoles-
cent perceptions of parenting differs depending on the aspect of parenting
measured, with support for genetic effects related to measures of warmth
but not control (Braungart, 1994; Elkins, McGue, & Iacono, 1997; Good-
man & Stevenson, 1989). Finally, results from a study of families containing
both biological and adoptive children between the ages of 12 and 18 years
revealed stronger associations between parent reports of family functioning
and adolescent reports of behavioral adjustment for the biological children
(McGue, Sharma, & Benson, 1996).
All of the above examples of evidence for genetic influence illustrate
what is referred to as a passive genotype-environment correlation (Scarr &
McCartney, 1983; Towers, Spotts, & Neiderhiser, 2001) presumably based
on genetic relatedness. Unfortunately the studies conducted to examine
the effects of shared genes have not been able to specify exactly how these
effects operate (Towers et al., 2001), and the ability to describe such mech-
anisms is confounded by the numerous ways that parent–child genetic simi-
larity might be expressed through parenting. There have been recent ef-
forts to identify particular candidate genes among family members using
DNA samples (Plomin & Rutter, 1998; Towers et al., 2001). This methodol-
ogy basically involves associating specific genes with behaviors using statisti-
cal methods such as regression analysis. These studies have also begun to
examine the role of family relationship factors in mediating or moderating
the behavioral expression of candidate genes (Towers et al., 2001).
A child’s genetic endowment may also impact parenting behavior
through nonpassive genotype-environment correlations (Scarr & McCart-
ney, 1983; Towers et al., 2001). These correlations result from the impact of
the child’s genetically determined characteristics on the parent, regardless
of the parent’s genetic makeup. There are two forms of nonpassive geno-
type-environment correlations. First, there are evocative gene-environment
correlations which are defined as reactions of others to a genetically influ-
enced trait of the individual. For example, a mother of a child with a short
attention span is likely to find herself providing many different types of ma-
terials (books, art supplies, games, and so forth) and opportunities to keep
158 KARRAKER AND COLEMAN
her child content and may become very creative in strategies to help the
child remain focused for increasing time intervals. Second, there are active
gene-environment correlations, which involve the individual seeking out a
particular environment or experience as a function of genetically based
traits. For example, a child with high verbal aptitude may seek opportuni-
ties to exercise her talent through social experiences, writing, drama, or a
debate club. Parenting will undoubtedly be influenced as the child pursues
support to engage in such activities.
Genetic processes clearly are an important mechanism underlying some
relations between parenting behavior and child characteristics or behavior.
However, these processes have only been studied in limited contexts and of-
ten are not recognized as a possible influence on parent–child correlations.
stressors including marital conflict and family size increase differential treat-
ment by parents (Crouter, McHale, & Tucker, 1999; Deal, 1996; Jenkins,
Rasbash, & O’Connor, 2003; Volling & Elins, 1998); however, the extent to
which these environmental factors interact with child effects to predict differ-
ential treatment has not been adequately explored.
With widespread acceptance of the transactional model of child develop-
ment, one of the largest challenges facing researchers today will be to find
effective methods to tease apart parent–child and child–parent effects that
are complexly intertwined with each other and with other factors that influ-
ence parenting (Patterson & Fisher, 2002). For example, a correlation be-
tween child rebelliousness and maternal withdrawal behavior may reflect a
causal relation between the two variables flowing from child to mother or
from mother to child, effects of additional child, maternal, genetic, or envi-
ronment variables, or some combination of all of these. More precise speci-
fications of the causal factors involved in the parenting of children should
follow as research moves from simple correlational studies to designs in-
volving experimental factors, complex correlational designs that take ad-
vantage of sophisticated statistical modeling techniques, and adoption or
other designs that allow identification of the role of genetic factors.
Definition of Temperament
later find themselves worn out, frustrated, and unable to maintain the stam-
ina needed to continue to adequately meet their difficult child’s demands.
Empirical data support the notion of declining emotional and verbal re-
sponsiveness, reduced teaching efforts, and increased negativity among
parents of difficult children with increasing child age (Peters-Martin &
Wachs, 1984; Maccoby, Snow, & Jacklin, 1984).
An emerging literature is devoted to describing and understanding how
the effects of temperament on parenting and child outcomes might be
modified by environmental factors. For example, a study by Jenkins et al.
(2003) revealed that associations between difficult temperament and par-
ent negativity were considerably stronger among families with low socioeco-
nomic status (SES) compared to families with high socio-economic status.
As noted by the authors, parents with high SES may be less reactive to their
children’s difficult temperament because they have lower levels of stress
generally or because they have different ways of attributing child misbehav-
ior, with higher SES parents adopting explanations that tend to be constitu-
tionally based. Contrasting results were reported by Prior, Sanson, Carroll,
and Oberklaid (1989), who found nearly twice as many significant correla-
tions between measures of temperament and parenting among high-SES
participants compared to low-SES participants, with the authors suggesting
that high-SES parents may be more aware of their children’s individual
qualities.
acceptable male and female behavior. The available evidence seems to indi-
cate that as a result of this differential parental treatment, inborn gender
differences, and children’s cognitive processing of gender role informa-
tion, boys and girls can be expected to behave differently and to elicit dif-
ferential responses from others. However, in an extensive review of the gen-
der literature, Fagot (1995) concluded that the central question pertaining
to whether parents treat male and female children differently has not been
sufficiently answered. She further notes that differential child outcomes
based on parenting may be tied more to attitudes than behavior and the
study of internal responses of parents to the parenting role has just
emerged in recent years.
Physical attractiveness is another child characteristic that may lead to dif-
ferential parenting behavior. For example, Langlois, Ritter, Casey, and Sawin
(1995) found that mothers of more attractive newborns demonstrated more
warmth and interest in playing with their newborns when compared to moth-
ers of less attractive newborns. Adults also tend to look longer at and pay
more attention to attractive infants and toddlers compared to their less at-
tractive peers (Hildebrandt & Cannan, 1985; Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald,
1978). In view of the high value placed on physical attractiveness in contem-
porary society (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986), the environments of more and
less attractive children may differ in other ways as well.
The studies reviewed previously provide evidence that certain child char-
acteristics may elicit differential responding from parents. Much of the
available evidence for these effects is necessarily correlational rather than
experimental, and thus susceptible to alternate causal and noncausal expla-
nations. In addition, from a transactional perspective, our understanding
of the parenting process would be enhanced by simultaneous consider-
ation of alternate causal pathways, mediators, and moderators. As Putnam
et al. (2002) noted, investigations of complex interactive effects have not al-
ways been theory driven, and more theory-based model development would
enhance our understanding of the parenting process.
REFERENCES
Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., Bell, K. L., & O’Connor, T. G. (1994). Longitudinal assessment of au-
tonomy and relatedness in adolescent–family interactions as predictors of adolescent ego
development and self-esteem. Child Development, 65, 179–194.
Anderson, K. E., Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1986). Mothers’ interactions with normal and
conduct-disordered boys: Who affects whom? Developmental Psychology, 22, 604–609.
Bates, J. E., Bayles, K., Bennett, D. S., Ridge, B., & Brown, M. M. (1991). Origins of
externalizing behavior problems at eight years of age. In D. Pepler and K. Rubin (Eds.), De-
velopment and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 93–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Baumrind, D. (1991). Effective parenting during the early adolescent transition. In P. A. Cow-
an & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Family transitions (pp. 111–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Beal, C. R. (1994). Boys and girls: The development of gender roles. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psy-
chological Review, 75, 81–95.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55,
83–96.
Belsky, J., Rha, J., & Park, S. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent and child effects in the case of
child inhibition in US and Korean samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
24, 338–347.
Berg-Nielsen, T. S., Vikan, A., & Dahl, A. A. (2002). Parenting related to child and parental
psychopathology: A descriptive review of the literature. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychia-
try, 7, 529–552.
Bidell, T. R., & Fischer, K. W. (1997). Between nature and nurture: The role of human agency
in the epigenesis of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & L. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence,
heredity, and environment (pp. 193–242). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (1995). Handbook of parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates.
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Braungart, J. M. (1994). Genetic influences on “environmental” measures. In J. C. DeFries, R.
Plomin, & D. W. Fulker (Eds.), Nature and nurture during middle childhood (pp. 233–248).
Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M. M., & Stifter, C. (1997). Compliance and noncompliance:
The roles of maternal control and child temperament. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
chology, 18, 411–428.
172 KARRAKER AND COLEMAN
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W.
Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical
models of human development (5th ed., pp. 993–1028). New York: Wiley.
Buss, D. M. (1981). Predicting parent–child interaction from children’s activity level. Develop-
mental Psychology, 17, 59–65.
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child rear-
ing practices and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural
study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677–686.
Chess, S., Thomas, A., & Birch, H. G. (1965). Your child is a person. New York: Viking.
Clark, L. A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers’ personality and its interaction with
child temperament as predictors of parenting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 79, 274–285.
Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (1998). Self-efficacy and parenting quality: Findings and fu-
ture applications. Developmental Review, 18, 46–85.
Crockenberg, S. B. (1986). Are temperamental differences in babies associated with predict-
able differences in care giving? In J. V. Lerner and R. M. Lerner (Eds.), New Directions for
Child Development: Vol. 31. Temperament and social interaction during infancy and childhood (pp.
53–73). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Crouter, A. C., McHale, S. M., & Tucker, C. J. (1999). Does stress exacerbate parental differen-
tial treatment of siblings? A pattern analytic approach. Journal of Family Psychology, 13,
286–299.
Cutrona, C., & Troutman, B. (1986). Social support, infant temperament, and parenting self-
efficacy. Child Development, 57, 1507–1518.
Deal, J. (1996). Marital conflict and differential treatment of siblings. Family Process, 35,
333–346.
Deater-Deckard, K., Pike, A., Petrill, S. A., Cutting, A. L., Hughes, C., & O’Connor, T. G.
(2001). Nonshared environmental processes in social-emotional development: An observa-
tional study of identical twin differences in the preschool period. Developmental Science, 4,
F1–F6.
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive processes.
Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25.
Dunn, J. F., & Plomin, R. (1986). Determinants of maternal behavior toward three-year-old sib-
lings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 57 348–356.
Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1990). Separate lives: Why siblings are so different. Basic Books.
Elkins, I. J., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on
parent–son relationships. Evidence for increasing genetic influence during adolescence.
Developmental Psychology, 33, 351–353.
Fagot, B. I. (1995). Parenting boys and girls. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting:
Vol. 1: Children and parenting (pp. 163–183). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Feinberg, M., & Hetherington, E. M. (2001). Differential parenting as a within-family variable.
Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 22–37.
Fowles, D. C., & Kochanska, G. (2000). Temperament as a moderator of pathways to con-
science in children: The contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 37,
788–795.
Galambos, N. L., & Turner, P. K. (1997, April). Shaping parent–adolescent relations: Good-
ness-of-fit in parent–adolescent temperaments. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of
the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.
Gauvain, M., & Fagot, B. (1995). Child temperament as mediator of mother–toddler problem
solving. Social Development, 4, 257–276.
5. CHILD CHARACTERISTICS AND PARENTING 173
Gelfand, D. M., Teti, D. M., & Radin Fox, C. E. (1992). Sources of parenting stress for de-
pressed and non-depressed mothers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 262–272.
Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989). A twin study of hyperactivity-II. The aetiological role of
genes, family relationships and perinatal adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
30, 691–709.
Gross, D., Conrad, B., Fogg, L., & Wothke, W. (1994). A longitudinal model of maternal self-
efficacy, depression, and difficult temperament in toddlerhood. Research in Nursing &
Health, 17, 207–215.
Grusec, J. E., Hastings, P., & Mammone, N. (1994). Parenting cognitions and relationship
schemas. In J. G. Smetana (Ed.), Beliefs about parenting: Origins and developmental implications
(pp. 5–19). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption. New York: Free Press.
Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Mirror, mirror . . . The importance of looks in everyday life. Al-
bany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Hetherington, E. M. (1989). Coping with family transitions: Winners, losers, and survivors.
Child Development, 60, 1–14.
Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1992). Coping with marital transitions. Mono-
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(2–3, Serial No. 227).
Hildebrandt, K. A., & Cannan, T. (1985). The distribution of caregiver attention in a group
program for young children. Child Study Journal, 15, 43–55.
Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1978). Adults’ responses to infants varying in per-
ceived cuteness. Behavioral Processes, 3, 159–172.
Holden, G. W. (1995). Parental attitudes toward childrearing. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Hand-
book of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 359–392). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Jain, A., Belsky, J., & Crnic, K. (1996). Beyond fathering behaviors: Types of dads. Journal of
Family Psychology, 10, 431–442.
Jenkins, J. M, Rasbash, J., & O’Connor, T. G. (2003). The role of the shared family context in
differential parenting. Developmental Psychology, 39, 99–113.
Karraker, K. H., & Coleman, P. (2002). Infants’ characteristics and behaviors help shape their
environments. In H. E. Fitzgerald, K. Karraker, & T. Luster (Eds.), Infant development: Eco-
logical perspectives (pp. 165–191). New York: Garland.
Karraker, K. H., Vogel, D. A., & Lake, M. A. (1995). Parents’ gender-stereotyped perceptions
of newborns: The eye of the beholder revisited. Sex Roles, 33, 687–701.
Kawaguchi, M. C., Welsh, D. P., Powers, S. I., & Rostosky, S. S. (1998). Mothers, fathers, sons,
and daughters: Temperament, gender, and adolescent–parent relationships. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 44, 77–96.
Kingston, L., & Prior, M. (1995). The development of patterns of stable, transient, and school-
age onset aggressive behavior in young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 348–358.
Kowal, A., & Kramer, L. (1997). Children’s understanding of parental differential treatment.
Child Development, 68, 113–126.
Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Casey, R. J., & Sawin, D. B. (1995). Infant attractiveness predicts
maternal behaviors and attitudes. Developmental Psychology, 31, 464–472.
Lerner, J. V. (1993). The influence of child temperamental characteristics on parent behav-
iors. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (1st ed., pp.
101–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Leve, L. D., Scaramella, L. V., & Fagot, B. I. (2001). Infant temperament, pleasure in
parenting, and marital happiness in adoptive families. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22,
545–558.
Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ sex-related differential socialization of boys and
girls: A meta-analysis, Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.
174 KARRAKER AND COLEMAN
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Maccoby, E. E., Snow, M. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1984), Children’s dispositions and mother–child
interaction at 12 and 18 months: A short-term longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,
20, 459–472.
Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Kestenbaum, R., Lang, S., & Andreas, D. (1990). Infant prone-
ness to distress temperament, maternal personality, and mother–infant attachment: Associ-
ations and goodness-of-fit. Child Development, 61, 820–831.
Mash, E. J., & Johnston, C. (1983). Parental perceptions of child behavior problems, parenting
self-esteem, and mothers’ reported stress in younger and older hyperactive and normal
children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 86–99.
Masten, A. S. (1989). Resilience in development: Implications of the study of successful adap-
tation for developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), The emergence of a disci-
pline: Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology (Vol. 1, pp. 261–294). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McBride, B. A., Schoppe, S. J., & Rane, T. R. (2002). Child characteristics, parenting stress,
and parental involvement: Fathers versus mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64,
998–1011.
McGue, M., Sharma, A., & Benson, P. (1996). The effect of common rearing on adolescent ad-
justment: Evidence from a U.S. adoption cohort. Developmental Psychology, 32, 604–613.
McGuire, S., Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1995). Maternal differential treatment of siblings and
children’s behavioral problems: A longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 7,
515–528.
McHale, S. M., & Pawletko, T. M. (1992). Differential treatment in two family contexts. Child
Development, 63, 68–81.
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., & Essex, M. J. (2002). Temper-
amental vulnerability and negative parenting as interacting predictors of child adjustment.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 461–471.
Mulsow, M., Caldera, Y. M., Pursley, M., & Reifman, A. (2002). Multilevel factors influencing
maternal stress during the first three years. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 944–956.
O’Connor, T. G. (2002). Annotation: The ‘effects’ of parenting reconsidered: Findings, chal-
lenges, and applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 555–572.
Okagaki, L., & Divecha, D. J. (1993). Development of parental beliefs. In T. Luster & L.
Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (1st ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Park, S., Belsky, J., Putnam, S., & Crnic, K. (1997). Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year
inhibition: Exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Developmental Psy-
chology, 33, 218–227.
Patterson, G. R., & Fisher, P. A. (2002). Recent developments in our understanding of
parenting: Bidirectional effects, causal models, and the search for parsimony. In M. H.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp.
59–88). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Peters-Martin, P., & Wachs, T. D. (1984). A longitudinal study of temperament and its corre-
lates in the first 12 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 285–298.
Pike, A., McGuire, S., Hetherington, E. M., Reiss, D., & Plomin, R. (1996). Family environment
and adolescent depression and antisocial behavior: A multivariate genetic analysis. Develop-
mental Psychology, 32, 590–603.
Plomin, R. (1990). Nature and nurture: An introduction to human behavioral genetics. Belmont, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Nature and nurture during infancy and early
childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Plomin, R., Loehlin, J. C., & DeFries, J. C. (1985). Genetic and environmental components of
“environmental” influences. Developmental Psychology, 21, 391–402.
5. CHILD CHARACTERISTICS AND PARENTING 175
Plomin, R., & Rutter, M. (1998). Child development and molecular genetics. What do we do
with genes once they are found? Child Development, 69, 1225–1242.
Prior, M., Sanson, A., Carroll, R., & Oberklaid, F. (1989). Social class and differences in tem-
perament ratings of pre-school children. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 35, 239–248.
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child temperament and parenting. In
M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp.
255–277).
Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psy-
chology, 22, 356–365.
Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P., Chen, X., Stewart, S., & McNichol, K. (1998). Interpersonal and ma-
ternal correlates of aggression, conflict, and externalizing problems in toddlers. Child De-
velopment, 69, 1614–1629.
Rubin, K. H., LeMare, L. J., & Lollis, S. (1990). Social withdrawal in childhood: Developmental
pathways to peer rejection. In S. R. Ashey & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood (pp.
217–249). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). The transaction between par-
ents’ perceptions of their children’s shyness and their parenting styles. International Journal
of Behavioral Development, 23, 937–957.
Rubin, J. Z., Provenzano, F. J., & Luria, Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder: Parents’ views on
sex of newborns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44, 47–55.
Rubin, K. H., & Stewart, S. L. (1996). Social withdrawal and inhibition in childhood. In E.
Mash & R. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (pp. 277–307). New York: Guilford Press.
Rubin, K. H., Stewart, S. L., & Chen, X. (1995). Parents of aggressive and withdrawn children.
In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (pp. 255–284).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sameroff, A., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casu-
alty. In F. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4, pp. 303–331). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2002). Temperament and social development. In
P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (pp.
97–115). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Scarr, S. (1988). How genotypes and environments combine: Development and individual dif-
ferences. In N. Bolger, A. Caspi, G. Downey, & M. Moorehouse (Eds.), Persons in context: De-
velopmental processes (pp. 217–244). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of ge-
notype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Evanston, IL: Row, Pe-
terson.
Sheeber, L. B., & Johnson, J. H. (1992). Child temperament, maternal adjustment, and
changes in family lifestyle. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62, 178–185.
Sheeber, L. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1998). Temperament-focused parent training. In J. M.
Briesmeister & C. E. Schaefer (Eds.), Handbook of parent training (pp. 479–507). New York:
Wiley.
Smetana, J. G., & Daddis, C. (2002). Domain-specific antecedents of parental psychological
control and monitoring: The role of parenting beliefs and practices. Child Development, 73,
563–580.
Stern, M., & Hildebrandt, K. A. (1986). Prematurity stereotyping: Effects on mother–infant in-
teraction. Child Development, 57, 308–315.
Stern, M., & Karraker, K. H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: A review of gender labeling
studies. Sex Roles, 20, 501–522.
Stocker, C. M. (1995). Differences in mothers’ and fathers’ relationships with siblings: Links
with children’s behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 499–513.
176 KARRAKER AND COLEMAN
Teti, D. M., & Gelfand, D. M. (1991). Behavioral competence among mothers of infants in the
first year: The mediational role of maternal self-efficacy. Child Development, 62, 918–929.
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H. G., Hertzig, M. E., & Korn, S. (1963). Behavioral individuality in
early childhood. New York: New York University Press.
Towers, H., Spotts, E. L., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences
on parenting and marital relationships: Current findings and future directions. Marriage
and Family Review, 33, 11–29.
van Bakel, H. J. A., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2002). Parenting and development of one-year-
olds: Links with parental, contextual, and child characteristics. Child Development, 73,
256–273.
van den Boom, D. C., & Hoeksma, J. B. (1994). The effect of infant irritability on mother–in-
fant interaction: A growth curve analysis. Developmental Psychology, 31, 581–590.
Vaughn, B. E., Taraldson, B. J., Crichton, L., & Egeland, B. (1981). The assessment of infant
temperament: A critique of the Carey Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Infant Behavior
and Development, 4, 1–17.
Volling, B. L., & Belsky, J. (1991). Multiple determinants of father involvement during infancy
in dual-earner and single-earner families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 461–474.
Volling, B. L., & Elins, J. L. (1998). Family relationships and children’s emotional adjustment
as correlates of maternal and paternal differential treatment: A replication with toddler
and preschool siblings. Child Development, 69, 1640–1656.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). The mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wachs, T. D. (1992). The nature of nurture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Wachs, T. D., & Grandour, M. J. (1983). Temperament, environment, and six-month cogni-
tive-intellectual development: A test of the organismic specificity hypotheses. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 135–152.
Wachs, T. D., & Kohnstamm, G. A. (Eds.). (2001). The bidirectional nature of temperament-
context links. In T. D. Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context (pp.
201–222). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Woodworth, S., Belsky, J., & Crnic, K. (1996). The determinants of fathering during the child’s
second and third years of life: A developmental analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
58, 679–692.
6
Robert M. Hodapp
Vanderbilt University
Tran M. Ly
University of California–Los Angeles
INTRODUCTION
177
178 HODAPP AND LY
Down syndrome 95% involve trisomy 21 1–1.5/1,000 Moderate MR: slowing rate of development as child gets older; social
strengths; weaknesses in grammar & speech
Fragile X syndrome Fragile site on X-chromosome .73–.92/1,000 Moderate MR; more males than females; strength in simultaneous
processing & weakness in sequential processing; slowed develop-
ment from puberty; hyperactivity and autistic-like behaviors
Prader-Willi 2/3 involve deletions on chromo- 1/15,000 Mild MR; failure to thrive in infancy followed by hyperphagia; prone-
syndrome some 15; 1/3 involve both ness to obesity, food foraging, and compulsive behaviors; strength
chromosome 15s from mother in jigsaw puzzles; stubbornness; skin picking
Smith-Magenis Deletion on chromosome 17 1/25,000–50,000 Mild to moderate MR; minor physical and facial anomalies; sleep dis-
syndrome turbance; self-injurious behaviors and high levels of many
maladaptive behaviors
Williams syndrome Deletion on chromosome 7 1/7,500 Mild to moderate MR; strengths in language and facial recognition;
difficulties in visual-spatial construction tasks; interest in (and some-
times fairly talented in) music; sociable but difficulty in making and
keeping friends; very high rates of anxiety, fears, and phobias
5p- syndrome Deletion on short arm of chro- 1/50,000 Severe MR; high-pitched, infantile, cat-like cry; better developed re-
(Cri du Chat) mosome 5 ceptive than expressive language; self-stimulatory and repetitive be-
haviors; hyperactivity and inattention
179
180 HODAPP AND LY
begin with a brief history, emphasizing the ways that family researchers
have changed in how they view parents of children with disabilities. We
then describe some parenting studies of children with different genetic
mental retardation syndromes, before discussing such remaining issues as:
who affects whom in parent–child interactions; what parents know and how
parental knowledge relates to parental behaviors; and the ways in which var-
ious levels of the environment might be linked when discussing the
parenting of children with disabilities.
In the field of disabilities, family studies are not new. The sociologist Ber-
nard Farber’s (1959; 1960) early work dates to the late 1950s and early
1960s, and families have been an area of research within the disabilities
field for over 100 years (Blacher & Baker, 2002). But earlier studies—those
up to the early 1980s—differ from more modern studies in several ways.
Five changes illustrate the movement from old to new studies of families of
children with disabilities.
sor on the family, but one that could be handled differently by different
parents and families. Parenting the child with disabilities thus became like
handling any stressor—for example, like coping with the illness of a parent
or child, or of one or both parents losing their jobs, or of the family moving.
Like reactions to all stressors, parents and families can react either posi-
tively or negatively. As we discuss below, the search for risk and protective
factors—within children, parents, or families—has increasingly character-
ized research on families of children with disabilities.
From More General to More Specific Disability Groups. Older family stud-
ies also lacked attention to the child’s type of disability. To some extent, this
inattention to child problems also stems from Solnit and Stark (1961). If
parents are reacting to the loss of the idealized infant, then any violation of
expectations, involving any type of child problem, might bring about ma-
ternal mourning. Solnit and Stark (1961) even speculated that mothers of
twins might suffer from maternal-mourning reactions.
A good example of this approach can be seen in a study by Drotar,
Baskiewicz, Irvin, Kennell, and Klaus (1975). In that study, the authors in-
terviewed 20 mothers and 5 fathers of children with disabilities to examine
a stage theory of the mourning reaction. But within their group with dis-
abilities were children with cleft lip and palate, spina bifida, Down syn-
drome, and multiple congenital malformations. In line with the thinking of
the time, mothers were reacting to parenting a child with disabilities; the
exact nature of the disability seemed unimportant.
As recent findings demonstrate, however, children with different disabil-
ities may differentially affect their parents. In mental retardation, for exam-
ple, we now know that children with different genetic mental retardation
disorders show different, etiology related maladaptive behaviors, profiles of
cognitive-linguistic strengths and weaknesses, and periods of faster or
slower development (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001). Such child behaviors, in
turn, may elicit different reactions and behaviors from parents. In contrast
to the perspectives used in early family work, then, later family researchers
concluded that specific characteristics of the child matter for how parents
and families cope with parenting the child with mental retardation. As we
mention later, children with Down syndrome may elicit better coping from
their parents, siblings, and families; children with syndromes that predis-
pose children to perform higher or lower on specific intellectual tasks may
also elicit different reactions from their parents.
From Mothers Alone to Mothers, Fathers, and Others. In most early studies,
mothers were the sole parent considered. This emphasis on mothers was
due to a variety of factors. First, mothers were (and remain) the primary
caregivers for most children. In addition, particularly until approximately
1980, lower percentages of mothers worked outside the home, and these
percentages were likely even lower for mothers of children with disabilities.
Finally, given a predominant Freudian perspective, there may have been an
implicit emphasis on mothers as the child’s main parent. For example, even
those who disagreed with Solnit and Stark’s (1961) model—feeling that
maternal reactions were a series of ups and downs, depressive and non-
depressive reactions (Olshansky, 1962)—were still implicitly using Freud as
their touchstone and mothers as their focus.
Although fathers remain underexamined in modern studies, most re-
searchers nevertheless acknowledge the important role that fathers play in
families of children with disabilities. Moreover, various researchers are dis-
covering that the needs of fathers may differ from those of mothers. More
than fathers, mothers express needs for more social and familial support,
information to explain the child’s disability to others, and help with child
care (Bailey, Blasco, & Simeonsson, 1992). In contrast, fathers are particu-
larly concerned about the costs of caring for a child with disabilities and
what the child will mean to the family as a whole (Price-Bonham & Addison,
1978). Comparing factors affecting mothers versus fathers of young chil-
dren with mental retardation, Krauss (1993) noted, “mothers reported
more difficulty than did fathers in adjusting to the personal aspects of
6. CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 183
From Older to More Modern Cultural Views of Disability. A final issue dis-
tinguishing older from more modern studies involves the culture itself.
American culture has changed dramatically in its reactions to children with
disabilities. As a result, over the past several decades the lives of these chil-
dren and of their parents have changed dramatically.
The first area of change involves the timing of services to children and
parents. In addition to school services from age 3 through 21 years, states
now provide early intervention services during the 0- to 3-year period. And
later, after the school-age years, services help young adults with disabilities
to work and live as independently as possible during adulthood (Hallahan
& Kauffman, 2002). Services for individuals and their families are thus life-
long, and one must consider the interplay between children–parents–fami-
lies and the service–delivery system from a life-span perspective. Changing
services constitute one important level of the overall ecology of that per-
son’s life.
A second change relates to how services are conceptualized. Partly due
to the change from pathological to stress-and-coping models, service deliv-
ery is now considered in terms of supporting families. No longer are par-
ents, siblings, and families conceptualized as patients who need to be
cured, but instead as persons—or consumers—who require long- or short-
term support to enable themselves to cope more effectively. One family
may require more information about a range of state-supported services,
another respite care (i.e., short term out-of-family care) in order that the
family can get a break from the full-time care of the offspring with disabili-
ties. Still other parents may need to be put in touch with parents of children
with similar problems, or parents who have dealt with the same school dis-
trict, or who can otherwise help in their particular situation. This support
revolution has changed the nature of services and how such services are un-
derstood by families and professionals (Hallahan & Kauffman, 2002).
Third, knowledge about many disorders has changed dramatically in re-
cent years, and many family-based services transmit knowledge to parents
and families. Considering genetic mental retardation disorders, parent-
professional groups exist for most every disorder (see Table 6.2 for a list of
some helpful Web sites). Organizations such as NORD—the National Orga-
nization of Rare Disorders—run informational Web sites that provide up-
to-date information about the disorder and about the relevant parent-
professional groups and their annual or biennial meetings. The National
184 HODAPP AND LY
TABLE 6.2
Web Sites of Parent-Professional Groups
Syndrome-Specific Organizations
Down syndrome
National Down syndrome Society http://www.ndss.org
National Down syndrome Congress http://www.ndsccenter.org
National Association for Down syndrome http://www.nads.org
National Fragile X Foundation http://www.nfxf.org
Prader-Willi syndrome Association (USA) http://www.pwsausa.org
PRISMS: Parents and Researchers Interested http://www.smithmagenis.org
in Smith-Magenis syndrome
Williams syndrome http://www.williams-syndrome.org
http://www.wsf.org
http://www.liliclairefoundation.org
5p- syndrome Society (Cri du Chat) http://www.fivepminus.org
In discussing the many ways in which the ecology of parenting has changed
over the past few decades, one important advance concerns knowledge.
Simply put, we now know much more about the behaviors of children with
many different etiologies or causes of mental retardation (Dykens &
Hodapp, 2001). Indeed, a new area of behavioral phenotypes has arisen
(Dykens, 1995), a field that examines the ways in which particular genetic
disorders affect behaviors. Although not every affected person necessarily
shows that disorder’s characteristic behaviors, genetic syndromes do ap-
pear to influence the behaviors of most children in many areas. To give a
few examples, compared to other children with mental retardation:
with Down syndrome to parents of children with autism or with conduct dis-
order. Likewise, Hodapp et al. (2003) found that, relative to parents of chil-
dren with heterogeneous causes of mental retardation, mothers of children
with Down syndrome considered their child more acceptable and more re-
warding.
It may even be the case that parents of children with Down syndrome ex-
perience equal levels of rewardingness as parents of typically developing
children. Interestingly, these low levels of PSI Reinforcement stress oc-
curred even when parents reported higher levels of stress overall. Such was
the case in the study by Roach et al. (1999). Mothers and fathers reported
more stress on most of the PSI subscales compared to their counterparts of
same-aged typically developing children. On the PSI Reinforcement sub-
scale, however, no group differences emerged (and the means of parents of
typical children and of children with Down syndrome were almost identi-
cal). Similarly in Noh et al. (1989), although parents of children with Down
syndrome rated their children as less attractive, socially appropriate, and in-
telligent, they regarded “their children as happier and as a greater source
of positive reinforcement than the parents of normal children” (p. 460).
were chosen because, as a group, children with each of these two disorders
are either exceptionally strong (Prader-Willi syndrome) or exceptionally
weak (Williams syndrome) in puzzles and other visuospatial tasks. Thus,
Dykens (2002) has recently found that, even compared to typical children
of similar chronological ages, children with Prader-Willi syndrome perform
exceptionally well in putting together jigsaw puzzles. In contrast, children
with Williams syndrome perform particularly poorly (i.e., below mental
age-levels) on a wide variety of visuospatial tasks (Bellugi, Mills, Jernigan,
Hickok, & Galaburda, 1999; Mervis, Morris, Bertrand, & Robinson, 1999).
In one study, Ly and Hodapp (2003) examined child effects on parents’
behaviors in parent–child dyads of 20 children with Prader-Willi syndrome
and 21 children with Williams syndrome. We measured parents’ amount of
helping and reinforcement behaviors in response to interacting with their
child to complete a jigsaw puzzle task. The jigsaw puzzle task was used to
capitalize on the strength of Prader-Willi syndrome and the weakness of
Williams syndrome. From attribution theory (Graham, 1991), parents of
children with Williams syndrome (vs. with Prader-Willi syndrome) were hy-
pothesized to provide more help and reinforcement during the jigsaw puz-
zle interaction task.
Prior to the interaction task, children completed a jigsaw puzzle inde-
pendently to obtain an objective measure of the child’s puzzle abilities. Al-
though children with Prader-Willi syndrome, on average, completed more
jigsaw puzzle pieces than children with Williams syndrome, each group
showed a fair amount of within-syndrome variability. Two groups were
therefore created—low and high puzzle ability.
Findings showed that, compared to parents of children with Prader-Willi
syndrome, parents of children with Williams syndrome provided both more
helping and more reinforcement behaviors. Within the 5-minute interac-
tion session, parents of children with Williams syndrome helped their child
49 times, compared to slightly less than 24 times for the Prader-Willi par-
ents. Similarly, parents of children with Williams syndrome reinforced their
children over twice as often (Ms = 14.14 and 5.75, SDs = 8.46 and 5.41 for
Williams and Prader-Willi syndromes, respectively). Although parents did
not differ in the amount of reinforcement behaviors in children with
higher versus lower puzzle abilities, parents provided more instances of
help when their children had lower (M = 45.96, SD = 23.43) versus higher
(M = 24.89, SD = 20.10) puzzle abilities.
These findings show that different genetic mental retardation syn-
dromes may have indirect effects on parents, and possibly on others in the
child’s environment. If, indeed, parents of children with Down syndrome
are reacting to the child’s more pleasant personalities (and general lack of
maladaptive behaviors), and those of children with Prader-Willi and Wil-
liams syndromes are reacting to high versus low puzzle abilities (respec-
6. CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 191
(2) Can One Use Genetic Disorders as Behavioral Proxies? In many studies
examining the direct effects of genetic disorders on children themselves,
researchers emphasize how this or that genetic syndrome can serve as a
model for a particular gene–brain–behavior pathway. In research on indi-
192 HODAPP AND LY
more etiology related behaviors, which in turn lead parents to certain, pre-
dictable responses. The parent’s response was to the child’s behavior, not
to the child’s etiology per se. Considering cases in which the behavior of an
individual child ran counter to the general etiology related profile, then,
parents should essentially react to the behavior, not to the etiology. Thus, if
a specific child with Down syndrome is not particularly pleasant or has
many behavior problems, parents should feel more stressed and less re-
warded. If a specific child with Prader-Willi syndrome is poor at puzzles,
parents should react with more help. Conversely, if a certain child with Wil-
liams syndrome is good at puzzles, parents should offer less help. In effect,
then, an etiology general approach assumes that parents react to the child’s
behavior, and that any child showing that behavior should elicit similar pa-
rental reactions and behaviors, regardless of the etiology of mental retarda-
tion.
In several studies, however, parents react to the child’s behavior, but
mainly in the context of what parents know or perceive of their child’s syn-
drome. In one study, Ly and Hodapp (2002) asked parents of children with
Down syndrome versus with other forms of mental retardation to make
causal attributions in response to hypothetical vignettes in which their child
performed each of two common, noncompliant behaviors. Parents of chil-
dren considered more sociable and outgoing attributed their child’s
noncompliant behaviors as due to normative concerns (“my child is acting
like other children his age”). But such connections between child personal-
ity and normalizing behaviors were found only within the Down syndrome
group. Thus, among parents of children with Down syndrome, those who
saw their child as more sociable, also rated more highly normalizing as the
reason for their child’s noncompliance, r = .43, p < .01. But among parents
of children with mixed forms of mental retardation, no such connections
existed between child personality and parental ratings of normalizing attri-
butions, r = .05, ns.
A similar, maybe even more extreme, finding occurred in the jigsaw puz-
zle study (Ly & Hodapp, in press). In addition to comparing parental help
and reward behaviors to children with Williams syndrome versus with
Prader-Willi syndromes, we also divided children into high and low puz-
zlers. Given the usual amount of within-syndrome variance, dividing chil-
dren at the overall median score for the two groups combined produced
some children in each etiological group who were against the etiology. Some
children with Prader-Willi syndrome were therefore poor puzzlers, and
some with William syndrome were good puzzlers. To separate the contribu-
tions of the child’s level of puzzle ability and parents’ knowledge of the
child’s etiological diagnosis on parental behaviors, we then performed step-
wise multiple regressions, using both etiology group and puzzle ability as in-
dependent predictors. When looking at parents’ helping behaviors, the
194 HODAPP AND LY
child’s etiology accounted for 28% of the variance, with the child’s puzzle
ability accounting for an additional 12%. For reinforcement behaviors,
only the child’s etiology emerged as a significant predictor, accounting for
27% of the variance.
Such findings suggest a complex interplay between the child’s behavior,
on one hand, and parental knowledge or perceptions of etiology related
behavior on the other. In the case of Down syndrome, increasing amounts
of etiology related personality elicited greater degrees of parental normaliz-
ing attributions. More etiology related behavior was associated with a partic-
ular parental reaction, but only within the Down syndrome group. In the
puzzle study, the important issue seemed to be parents’ perceptions of what
that etiology means, less so the child’s behavior per se. There seems, then,
to be a complex interaction between the behaviors themselves and the
child’s type of mental retardation. Sometimes both are important, some-
times etiology trumps behavior, and sometimes behavior matters, but only
within the context of what parents know or think that they know about eti-
ology related behaviors of their child’s etiology. We again return to Bell,
and his idea that reactions and behaviors must be considered within the
context of a “thinking parent” (Bell, 1979).
(4) What Do Parents Know and How Do They Know It? If parental percep-
tions and knowledge of their child’s etiology are so important, what do par-
ents know? This involves the issue of parental beliefs and their origins
(Okagaki & Divecha, 1993; Okagaki & Bingham, chap. 1, this volume), a
significant area within parenting studies of typically developing children,
which is relatively unexplored among parents of children with disabilities.
In a few syndromes, however, we do have some sense of what parents know
about their child’s etiology. In one study, Fidler, Hodapp, and Dykens (2002)
asked parents of children with Prader-Willi syndrome, Down syndrome, and
Williams syndrome about several education-related aspects of their child’s
behavioral characteristics and school accommodations. Whereas parents of
children with Down syndrome were aware of their child’s cognitive-linguistic
strengths (e.g., visual short-term memory) and weaknesses (e.g., expressive
language), parents in the two other groups knew much less. Among parents
of children with Prader-Willi syndrome, parents knew of their child’s ex-
treme overeating and tantrums, but were less aware of their child’s strong
visuospatial abilities. In Williams syndrome, parents were aware of the many
fears and anxieties shown by these children (Dykens, 2003), but less aware of
these children’s general propensity to have relatively strong linguistic skills
and relatively weak visuospatial skills.
Another, somewhat unexpected finding concerned delivery of etiology
related information into the classroom setting. Thus, one of our questions
asked parents about who among ten different people (the parent and nine
6. CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 195
(5) How Do We Link Various Levels of the Environment? As the many unex-
plored questions imply, the field of parenting studies in mental retarda-
tion—and even in disabilities in general—has not yet joined the various
levels of the child’s environment (e.g., parents and families, schools, neigh-
borhoods). Although an ecological approach to parenting is a booming en-
terprise for parents of typically developing children, our knowledge of
Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) many different levels, or of how such levels inter-
relate is almost non-existent for children with mental retardation.
And yet, as shown above, if we are to understand parenting children with
different genetic disorders, we need to know how children with these disor-
ders live within their families, with friends, and in their schools, neighbor-
hoods, and other environments. In addition to knowing about each level
separately, we will also need to know how the various levels of ecology go to-
gether. To follow our example above, we will need to know when and how
parents access information, how such information might be gathered or
used differently by parents who are of different ages, ethnicities, SES levels,
or who live in different geographic locales or types of locale (urban, subur-
ban, rural).
196 HODAPP AND LY
CONCLUSIONS
abilities. So, too, have advances occurred in how these families are under-
stood by researchers and practitioners, as represented by the movement
from more pathological and less differentiated views to more stress-and-
coping perspectives. Scientific advances have also helped us know much
more about the biomedical and behavioral characteristics of children with
a wide variety of genetic syndromes. Although we have far to go, we remain
hopeful that we will continue to learn more about parenting children with
disabilities, thereby benefiting the families themselves as well as the larger
parenting field in general.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank Tom Luster, Lynn Okagaki, Marc Bornstein, and
Elisabeth Dykens for their comments on earlier drafts of this chapter. The
support of NSF post-doctoral fellowship #0310013 to the second author is
also gratefully acknowledged.
REFERENCES
Abidin, R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index: Professional manual (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychologi-
cal Assessment Resources.
Anderson, L. L., Lakin, K. C., Mangan, T. W., & Prouty, R. W. (1998). State institutions: Thirty
years of depopulation and closure. Mental Retardation, 36, 431–443.
Bailey, D., Blasco, P., & Simeonsson, R. (1992). Needs expressed by mothers and fathers of
young children with disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 1–10.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emory, G. (1971). Cognitive theory of depression. New York:
Guilford Press.
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psycho-
logical Review, 75, 81–95.
Bell, R. Q. (1979). Parent, child, and reciprocal influences. American Psychologist, 34, 821–826.
Bellugi, U., Mills, D., Jernigan, T., Hickok, G., & Galaburda, A. (1999). Linking cognition,
brain structure, and brain function in Williams syndrome. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.),
Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 111–136). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Blacher, J. (1984). Sequential stages of parental adjustment to the birth of the child with hand-
icaps: Fact or artifact? Mental Retardation, 22, 55–68.
Blacher, J., & Baker, B. (Eds.). (2002). Best of AAMR: Families and mental retardation. A collection
of notable AAMR journal articles across the 20th century. Washington, DC: American Associa-
tion on Mental Retardation.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
Cahill, B. M., & Glidden, L. M. (1996). Influence of child diagnosis on family and parent func-
tioning: Down syndrome versus other disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation,
101, 149–160.
Carr, J. (1995). Down’s syndrome: Children growing up. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
198 HODAPP AND LY
Chapman, R. S., & Hesketh, L. J. (2000). Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syn-
drome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 84–95.
Crnic, K., Friedrich, W., & Greenberg, M. (1983). Adaptation of families with mentally handi-
capped children: A model of stress, coping, and family ecology. American Journal of Mental
Deficiency, 88, 125–138.
Cummings, S. (1976). The impact of the child’s deficiency on the father: A study of fathers of
mentally retarded and chronically ill children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 46,
246–255.
Cummings, S., Bayley, H., & Rie, H. (1966). Effects of the child’s deficiency on the mother: A
study of mentally retarded, chronically ill, and neurotic children. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 36, 595–608.
Down, H. L. (1866). Observations of an ethnic classification of idiots. London Hospital Clinical
Lecture and Report, 3, 259–262.
Drotar, D., Baskiewicz, A., Irvin, N., Kennell, J., & Klaus, M. (1975). The adaptation of parents
to the birth of an infant with congenital malformation: A hypothetical model. Pediatrics, 56,
710–717.
Dunn, P. M. (1991). Dr. Langdon Down (1828–1896) and “mongolism.” Archives of Disease in
Childhood, 66, 827–828.
Dykens, E. M. (1995). Measuring behavioral phenotypes: Provocations from the “New Genet-
ics.” American Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 522–532.
Dykens, E. M. (2002). Are jigsaw puzzle skills spared in persons with Prader-Willi syndrome?
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 343–352.
Dykens, E. M. (2003). Anxiety, fears, and phobias in persons with Williams syndrome. Develop-
mental Neuropsychology, 23, 291–316.
Dykens, E. M., & Hodapp, R. M. (2001). Research in mental retardation: Toward an etiologic
approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 49–71.
Dykens, E. M., Hodapp, R. M., & Finucane, B. (2000). Genetics and mental retardation syndromes:
A new look at behavior and treatments. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
Dykens, E. M., & Kasari, C. (1997). Maladaptive behavior in children with Prader-Willi syn-
drome, Down syndrome, and non-specific mental retardation. American Journal on Mental
Retardation, 102, 228–237.
Dykens, E. M., Leckman, J. F., & Cassidy, S. B. (1996). Obsessions and compulsions in Prader-
Willi syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 995–1002.
Essex, E. L., Seltzer, M. M., & Krauss, M. W. (1999). Differences in coping effectiveness and
well-being among aging mothers and fathers of adults with mental retardation. American
Journal on Mental Retardation, 104, 454–563.
Farber, B. (1959). The effects of the severely retarded child on the family system. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 24, no. 2.
Farber, B. (1960). Family organization and crisis: Maintenance of integration in families with a
severely mentally retarded child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
25, no. 1.
Fidler, D. J., Hodapp, R. M., & Dykens, E. M. (2000). Stress in families of young children with
Down syndrome, Williams syndrome, and Smith-Magenis syndrome. Early Education and De-
velopment, 11, 395–406.
Fidler, D. J., Hodapp, R. M., & Dykens, E. M. (2002). Behavioral phenotypes and special educa-
tion: Parent report of educational issues for children with Down syndrome, Prader-Willi
syndrome, and Williams syndrome. Journal of Special Education, 36, 80–88.
Freud, S. (1917). Mourning and melancholia. (Reprinted from A general selection from the works
of Sigmund Freud, pp. 124–140, by J. Rickman, Ed., 1957, Garden City, NY: Doubleday)
Friedrich, W. L., & Freidrich, W. N. (1981). Psychosocial assets of parents of handicapped and
nonhandicapped children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 85, 551–553.
6. CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 199
Friedrich, W. N., Greenberg, M. T., & Crnic, K. (1983). A short-form of the Questionnaire on
Resources and Stress. American Journal on Mental Deficiency, 88, 41–48.
Gibson, D. (1978). Down syndrome: The psychology of mongolism. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Glidden, L. M. (2002). Parenting children with developmental disabilities: A ladder of influ-
ence. In J. L. Borkowski, S. L. Ramey, & M. Bristol-Powers (Eds.), Parenting and the child’s
world: Influences on academic, intellectual, and socio-emotional development (pp. 329–344). Mono-
graphs in Parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Graham, S. (1991). A review of attribution theory in achievement contexts. Educational Psychol-
ogy Review, 3, 5–39.
Hallahan, D. P., & Kauffman, J. M. (2002). Exceptional children: Introduction to Special Education
(9th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Hanson, M., & Hanline, M. F. (1990). Parenting a child with a disability: A longitudinal study
of parental stress and adaptation. Journal of Early Intervention, 14, 234–248.
Hodapp, R. M. (1996). Down syndrome: Developmental, psychiatric, and management issues.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5, 881–894.
Hodapp, R. M. (1997). Direct and indirect behavioral effects of different genetic mental retar-
dation disorders. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, 67–79.
Hodapp, R. M. (1999). Indirect effects of genetic mental retardation disorders: Theoretical
and methodological issues. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 22, 27–50.
Hodapp, R. M. (2002). Parenting children with mental retardation. In M. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of parenting, 2nd ed.: Vol. 1. How children influence parents (pp. 355–381). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hodapp, R. M. (2004). Studying interactions, reactions, and perceptions: Can genetic disor-
ders serve as behavioral proxies? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 29–34.
Hodapp, R. M., & Dykens, E. M. (2004). Studying behavioral phenotypes: Issues, benefits, chal-
lenges. In E. Emerson, C. Hatton, T. Parmenter, & T. Thompson (Eds.), International hand-
book of applied research in intellectual disabilities (pp. 203–220). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Hodapp, R. M., Evans, D. W., & Gray, F. L. (1999). Intellectual development in children with
Down syndrome. In J. Rondal, J. Perera, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Down syndrome: A review of current
knowledge (pp. 124–132). London: Whurr.
Hodapp, R. M., & Fidler, D. J. (1999). Special education and genetics: Connections for the
21st century. Journal of Special Education, 33, 130–137.
Hodapp, R. M., Ly, T. M., Fidler, D. J., & Ricci, L. A. (2001). Less stress, more rewarding: Par-
enting children with Down syndrome. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1, 317–337.
Hodapp, R. M., Ricci, L. A., Ly, T. M., & Fidler, D. J. (2003). The effects of the child with Down
syndrome on maternal stress. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 137–151.
Holroyd, J., & MacArthur, D. (1976). Mental retardation and stress on parents: A contrast be-
tween Down’s syndrome and childhood autism. American Journal on Mental Deficiency, 80,
431–436.
Hoppes, K., & Harris, S. (1990). Perceptions of child attachment and maternal gratification in
mothers of children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Child Clinical Psychology, 19,
365–370.
Hornby, G. (1995). Fathers’ views of the effects on their families of children with Down syn-
drome. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 4, 103–117.
Kasari, C., & Freeman, S. F. N. (2001). Task related social behavior in children with Down syn-
drome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106, 253–264.
Kasari, C., Mundy, P., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1990). Affect and attention in children with
Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 55–67.
Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1997). Linking parental perceptions to interactions in young chil-
dren with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 39–57.
200 HODAPP AND LY
Keogh, B. K., Garnier, H. E., Bernheimer, L. P., & Gallimore, R. (2000). Models of child-family
interactions for children with developmental delays: Child-driven or transactional? Ameri-
can Journal on Mental Retardation, 105, 32–46.
Krauss, M. (1993). Child-related and parenting stress: Similarities and differences between
mothers and fathers of children with disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97,
393–404.
Lakin, C., Prouty, B., Braddock, D., & Anderson, L. (1997). State institution populations:
Smaller, older, more impaired. Mental Retardation, 35, 231–232.
Lejeune, J., Gautier, M., & Turpin, R. (1959). Etudes des chromosomes somatique de neuf en-
fants mongoliens. Comptes Rendus de l’Academie des Sciences, 48, 1721.
Lobato, D. (1983). Siblings of handicapped children: A review. Journal of Autism and Develop-
mental Disorders, 13, 347–364.
Ly, T. M., & Hodapp, R. M. (2002). Maternal attribution of child noncompliance in children
with mental retardation: Down syndrome versus other etiologies. Journal of Developmental
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23, 322–329.
Ly, T. M., & Hodapp, R. M. (in press). Children with Prader-Willi syndrome vs. Williams syn-
drome: Indirect effects on parents during a jigsaw puzzle task. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research.
Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Bertrand, J. M., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Williams syndrome:
Findings from an integrated program of research. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.),
Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 65–110). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Meyers, B. A., & Pueschel, S. M. (1991). Psychiatric disorders in persons with Down syndrome.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 609–613.
Miller, J. (1999). Profiles of language development in children with Down syndrome. In J. F.
Miller, M. Leddy, & L. A. Leavitt (Eds.), Improving the communication of people with Down syn-
drome (pp. 11–39). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
Noh, S., Dumas, J. E., Wolf, L. C., & Fisman, S. N. (1989). Delineating sources of stress in par-
ents of exceptional children. Family Relations, 38, 456–461.
Okagaki, L., & Divecha, D. J. (1993). Development of parental beliefs. In T. Luster & L.
Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (pp. 35–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Olsen, C. L., Cross, P. K., Gensburg, L. J., & Hughes, J. P. (1996). The effects of prenatal diag-
nosis, population aging, and changing fertility rates on the live birth prevalence of Down
syndrome in New York State, 1983–1992. Prenatal Diagnosis, 16, 991–1002.
Olshansky, S. (1962). Chronic sorrow: A response to having a mentally defective child. Social
Casework, 43, 190–193.
Pitcairn, T. K., & Wishart, J. G. (1994). Reactions of young children with Down’s syndrome to
an impossible task. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 485–489.
Price-Bonham, S., & Addison, S. (1978). Families and mentally retarded children: Emphasis
on the father. The Family Coordinator, 27, 221–230.
Pueschel, S. R., Gallagher, P. L., Zartler, A. S., & Pezzullo, J. C. (1986). Cognitive and learning
profiles in children with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 8, 21–37.
Roach, M. A., Orsmond, G. I., & Barratt, M. S. (1999). Mothers and fathers of children with
Down syndrome: Parental stress and involvement in childcare. American Journal on Mental
Retardation, 104, 422–436.
Ruskin, E. M., Kasari, C., Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1994). Attention to people and toys during
social and object mastery in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Re-
tardation, 99, 103–111.
Rutter, M., Pickles, A., Murray, R., & Eaves, L. (2001). Testing hypotheses on specific environ-
mental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 291–324.
Sanders, J. L., & & Morgan, S. B. (1997). Family stress and adjustment as perceived by parents
of children with autism or Down syndrome: Implications for intervention. Child and Family
Behavior Therapy, 19, 15–32.
6. CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 201
CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES
ON PARENTING
This page intentionally left blank
7
Frank D. Fincham
Florida State University
Julie H. Hall
University at Buffalo, The State University of New York
INTRODUCTION
205
206 FINCHAM AND HALL
second-order effects, points out how the marital relationship can affect in-
teractions between parent and child (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). The eco-
logical model also emphasizes the importance of the marital relationship
as a support system for parenting; Bronfenbrenner (1986) reviewed evi-
dence that mothers who felt supported by their husbands tended to have
higher marital satisfaction and more positive attitudes toward parenting.
This research helped lay the foundation for much current work on mari-
tal and parenting processes, and illustrated the importance of the envi-
ronment in such processes.
In keeping with Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model, this chapter ex-
plores the direct effects of the marital relationship on child development,
as well as the indirect effects that occur through parenting. We review
briefly direct effects, before turning to the primary focus of the chapter,
how marital and parenting processes interact to influence child develop-
ment. Following consideration of the major research in this area, the next
section identifies promising avenues for future research. The chapter con-
cludes with a summary of the main arguments.
Parenting has consistently been shown to play a mediating role in the rela-
tion between marital quality and child functioning. High marital quality is
associated with sensitive parenting and optimal toddler functioning (e.g.,
Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984), whereas marital discord undermines and
disrupts effective parenting practices and is associated with poor child ad-
justment (Belsky, 1984; Fauber & Long, 1991; Reid & Crisafulli, 1990).
However, before one can fully explore such associations, it is critical to ex-
amine central constructs. For example, Erel and Burman (1995) were not
able to test adequately whether relevant variables moderated the associa-
tion between the marital and parent–child relationships owing to the heter-
ogeneity of effect sizes within categories of the moderator variables that
they examined (e.g., different operationalizations of marital quality).
Erel and Burman (1995) found a positive association between marital qual-
ity and parenting (the composite mean weighted effect size was .46). Thus,
on the whole, harmonious marriages tend to be associated with effective
parenting, whereas troubled marriages are linked to maladaptive parent-
ing. This finding supports a spillover model whereby the affective tone of
the marriage spills over into the parent–child relationship; it stands in
sharp contrast to the alternative compensatory model in which marital
quality and parenting quality are posited to be negatively related (Erel &
Burman, 1995; Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984).
However, there is some empirical support for the compensatory model
in which parents are hypothesized to compensate for marital frustration
and dissatisfaction by channeling these negative emotions into positive
parenting behaviors (Amato, 1986; Belsky, Youngblade, Rovine, & Volling,
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 211
1991). Parents may attempt to fill the void left by their unhappy marriage
by establishing fulfilling relationships with their children. Nevertheless,
Cox, Paley, and Harter (2001) point out that it is difficult to differentiate
between genuinely positive parenting and parent–child relationships that
seem positive but actually serve to meet the needs of the parent. As a whole,
support for the compensatory model is limited and difficult to interpret.
Krishnakumar and Buehler’s (2000) meta-analysis of interparental con-
flict and parenting behaviors suggests a third model, compartmentaliza-
tion, in which parents are able to maintain the boundaries between their
spousal and parenting roles. Such a model requires partners to contain
their feelings about the marriage and to not let them contaminate the par-
ent–child relationship. This model implies that there is no relationship be-
tween marital quality and parenting. However, neither Erel and Burman
(1995) nor Krisnakumar and Buehler (2000) found support for this model
in their meta-analyses. In fact the latter authors found an average weighted
effect size of −.62 between interparental conflict and positive parenting be-
haviors which lends further support to the spillover model.
What Processes Account for the Spillover Effect? Grych (2002) reviewed sev-
eral conceptual models that might explain the association between marital
and parenting domains. Family systems theory accounts for this association
through the idea of circular causality in which relationships within the family
are reciprocally related. From a stress and coping perspective, the marital re-
lationship is conceptualized as a source of stress or support for parenting
processes. Third, the affective spillover model posits that emotional experi-
ences in the marriage carry over to affective expression in parent–child rela-
tionships. A fourth conceptual model proposes that spouses in distressed
marriages withdraw from their children, leading to problems in parenting.
However, it is also possible that a third variable influences behavior in both
marital and parenting domains. Although each of these models is theoreti-
cally sound, it is difficult to distinguish among them empirically because the
processes described in each lead to the same outcomes.
A general theoretical framework would facilitate research on the interre-
lationship of marriage and parenting, but it is equally important to explore
empirically specific associations to identify which elements of marital func-
tioning are linked to parenting. As noted, one important association sup-
ported by empirical research is the link between marital satisfaction and
quality of parenting.
2000). Marital conflict is also related to child rejection, low parental in-
volvement, and low emotional responsivity (Buehler & Gerard, 2002; Fau-
ber, Forehand, Thomas, & Wierson, 1990; Webster-Stratton & Hammond,
1999). Parents often become so consumed by marital conflicts that their
parenting behavior grows less effective and more inconsistent (Fauber &
Long, 1991). Indeed, marital conflict may drain parent resources to the
point that it reduces parents’ ability to recognize and respond to the child’s
emotional needs (Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984). Children may perceive
parental inattention as rejection, leading to emotional and behavioral
problems (Fincham et al., 1994). In addition to diminishing the quantity of
parent–child interactions, marital conflict may also influence the quality of
these exchanges. Negative affect from the marital context may spillover
into parent–child relationships (Kerig, Cowan, & Cowan, 1993), leading to
disciplinary techniques that are harsh, critical, and rely on guilt and anxiety
induction (Buehler & Gerard, 2002; Fauber et al., 1990; Fincham et al.,
1994; Gable, Belsky, & Crnic, 1992; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1999).
When compared to nondistressed families, tension in distressed families
endures for a longer period of time, and marital conflict increases the prob-
ability of parent–child conflict (Christensen & Margolin, 1988; Margolin,
Christensen, & John, 1996).
ceive themselves as lacking power are most likely to use coercive control tac-
tics with their children.
Brody, Arias, and Fincham (1996) further explored how marital attribu-
tions are related to parenting, and also examined the link with children’s
attributions for negative parental behavior. Consistent with prior research,
they found that husbands’ and wives’ conflict promoting attributions were
related to negativity in the marital context, which was associated with harsh,
punitive parenting, less involved communicative parenting, and ineffective
parent–child communication. Harsh, punitive parenting tends to be associ-
ated with conflicted and distant parent–child relationships, whereas in-
volved communicative parenting is associated with harmonious and cohe-
sive parent–child relationships (Collins, 1990). The direct effects of
attributions on parent–child relationships were also stronger for fathers
than for mothers. Brody et al. (1996) also found that parenting behavior
was related to children’s attributions for negative parental behavior. In-
volved communicative parenting was negatively related to children’s con-
flict promoting attributions, and harsh, punitive parenting was positively re-
lated (Brody et al., 1996).
Implicit in our discussion thus far is the assumption that the processes
discussed are gender neutral. But the same behavior performed by a father
versus a mother may be experienced quite differently. Not only might re-
sponses differ because boys and girls develop different relationships with fa-
thers and mothers, but factors such as size and strength differences can
make a slap delivered with the same force more or less threatening depend-
ing on whether the mother slaps the father or vice versa. We therefore turn
to consider the role of gender.
Gender Matters
There has been a great deal of controversy as to whether there are system-
atic differences in the association between marital quality and fathering ver-
sus mothering. Several studies have suggested that fathering is more af-
fected by marital quality than is mothering (Belsky et al., 1991; Coiro &
Emery, 1998; Easterbrooks & Emde, 1988; Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984;
Kitzmann, 2000). Some researchers have posited that this is because male
familial roles are less defined than those of females, and/or that mothers
are better able to maintain boundaries between familial roles (Belsky et al.,
1991).
Marital satisfaction and conflict also have been linked to gender differ-
ences in parenting behaviors and attitudes. Marital satisfaction is positively
related to fathers’ childrearing attitudes but is unrelated to the attitudes of
mothers (Easterbrooks & Emde, 1988). Fathering is also more likely to be
affected by marital conflict than mothering (Coiro & Emery, 1998), with fa-
216 FINCHAM AND HALL
thers being more likely to withdraw from their wives and children following
marital conflict or when in distressed marriages (Christensen & Heavey,
1990; Howes & Markman, 1989). There is an especially strong link between
destructive forms of marital conflict and negative fathering behaviors (Lin-
dahl & Malik, 1999). Fathers tend to be less supportive and engaged when
interacting with their sons after a conflictual marital discussion whereas
mothers do not show such an effect (Kitzmann, 2000). There is also evi-
dence of gender specific longitudinal associations between marital quality
and parenting, as deterioration in marital quality over time is associated
with more negative and intrusive fathering behavior but facilitative mother-
ing behavior (Belsky et al., 1991).
While these findings may suggest that there is a null or compensatory re-
lation between marital quality and maternal parenting behavior, there is
evidence that marital distress is linked to lower maternal involvement and
increased negativity (Erel & Burman, 1995; Grych & Fincham, 1990). Fur-
thermore, Osborne and Fincham (1996) found that boys’ perceptions of
interparental conflict were more strongly associated with mother–child re-
lationships than father–child relationships, and that there was no differ-
ence in the associations for girls. As these findings show, it is insufficient
merely to introduce gender at the parental level. Only by considering both
parent and child gender is it possible to show that interparental conflict is
especially deleterious for cross-gender parent child relationships (e.g., Ke-
rig, Cowan, & Cowan, 1993) which, unlike same-sex relationships, predict
child outcome even though boys and girls report similar levels of exposure
to interparental conflict (Osborne & Fincham, 1996). Despite the consider-
able attention given to child gender, our understanding of the impact of
the interparental relationship on boys versus girls remains limited (see
Davies & Lindsay 2001).
Kerig, Cowan, and Cowan (1993) found that, overall, both fathers and
mothers tend to respond negatively towards daughters more often than
sons. Also, less maritally adjusted fathers behave most negatively towards
daughters, whereas mothers in less satisfied marriages are the least accept-
ing of daughter’s assertiveness and are more likely to reciprocate their sons’
negative affect. However, Reid and Crisafulli (1990) found that there is a
stronger association between marital conflict and externalizing child be-
havior for sons than for daughters. Similarly, Gordis, Margolin, and John
(2001) showed that parent-to-child hostility increases the effects of marital
hostility on child adjustment among boys but not among girls. This could
be due to the fact that boys are less shielded from family conflict than girls
(Grych & Fincham, 1990), or that girls are more likely to react with inter-
nalizing behaviors than externalizing behaviors. Thus, evidence of gender
effects in the association between marriage and parenting is somewhat
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 217
mixed. It is important that future research examine not only the effects of
parent and child gender, but also their interaction (Grych, 2002; Snyder,
1998).
In sum, consideration of parent and child gender is likely to yield a more
textured picture of the associations among the marital relationship, parent-
ing and child development. Moving beyond consideration of family dyads is
similarly likely to yield a more sophisticated understanding. We therefore
turn to briefly consider triadic family interactions.
In recent years, it has become increasingly apparent that parents treat their
children differently when their spouse is present than they do when alone
with their children. However, triadic interactions are extremely complex to
study, as they require a focus on three different dyadic relationships, as well
as an examination of how the third person influences each of these dyads.
Vuchinich, Emery, and Cassidy (1988) found that in situations with dyadic
conflict, a third family member intervened more than one-third of time,
and often in a way that formed an alliance with one of the parties. Children
tended to use distraction tactics to intervene, and girls were more likely
than boys to intervene in all family disputes except marital conflicts. Fathers
tended to use authority strategies to intervene, whereas mothers used
mediational tactics, but mothers and fathers were unlikely to side against
each other when intervening in dyadic conflicts.
Early studies showed that parents tend to have less physical contact and
talk less to their infants when in triadic situations than they do in dyadic
parent–child interactions (Belsky, 1979). However, when looking at adoles-
cents, Gjerde (1986) found that both the quantity and the quality of
parenting behaviors changed across contexts, as well as by gender. The
quality of mother–son interactions improved when the father was present,
with mothers becoming more engaged, secure, consistent, and affective
than they were when they were alone with their sons. However, father–son
interactions deteriorated in quality when in the presence of the mother,
with fathers decreasing in involvement, engagement, and egalitarianism,
and increasing in criticism and antagonism. In contrast, Johnson (2001)
found that both parents displayed less negative affect and were less en-
gaged with their children in triadic interactions than in dyadic parent–
child interactions, but that parental warmth and responsiveness were stable
across contexts. There also tends to be greater role differentiation between
mothering and fathering in triadic interactions than in dyadic parent–child
interactions. Mothers are more at ease and traditional in their mothering
218 FINCHAM AND HALL
role when in the presence of the father. In contrast, fathers are more likely
to withdraw from a primary parenting role when in the presence of their
wives (Gjerde, 1986; Johnson, 2001).
Marital Power and Triadic Interaction. Lindahl and Malik (1999) exam-
ined how the balance of power within marriage relates to parenting in
triadic situations, and classified marriages as balanced, male dominant, or
power struggle. The results varied according to gender, level of marital dis-
tress, and ethnicity. Mothers from male-dominant marriages were more
emotionally supportive in triadic interactions than mothers from power
struggle marriages, but only when they were European American and were
not maritally distressed. However, maritally distressed fathers from male-
dominant marriages were more coercive with sons than fathers from bal-
anced or power struggle marriages. Hispanic American fathers from
balanced marriages were more emotionally supportive of sons in triadic in-
teractions than fathers from male-dominant or power struggle marriages.
Meanwhile, European American fathers from power struggle marriages
were more withdrawn than fathers from the other power groups. These re-
sults illustrate the complexity of triadic interactions, and the variety of ways
in which marital processes can interact with parenting behavior.
In sum, it is clear that the marital relationship–parenting association
cannot be adequately understood without consideration of the broader
context of triadic family processes. At the same time, it is apparent that
work on triadic family processes is in its infancy. In the next section of the
chapter we turn explicitly to identifying directions for future research that
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 219
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
lems then peers (McHale & Rasmussen, 1998). Thus, both overt and covert
coparenting processes may influence child adjustment.
esses are actually risk indicators (Rutter, 1994). Floyd, Gilliom, and Costi-
gan (1998) also found that coparenting (parenting alliance) mediated the
effects of marital quality on parenting and did not find evidence of a recip-
rocal relation (i.e., parenting → marital quality).
Parenting behavior may have a particularly strong influence on child be-
havior when coparenting is unsupportive; parental rejection plays an espe-
cially important role in predicting attention, passivity/dependence, and
grades when supportive coparenting is low (Stright & Neitzel, 2003). Simi-
larly, unsupportive coparenting influences externalizing behavior when
family structure is less adaptive (Schoppe et al., 2001). This interaction also
extends to positive affect in the family—when positive affect is low, suppor-
tive coparenting protects against the development of externalizing prob-
lems, but when positive affect is high, supportive coparenting is not associ-
ated with such problems (Schoppe et al., 2001).
As research on coparenting as a mechanism continues, it will be neces-
sary to explore what aspects of marriage are related to coparenting, and
which aspects of coparenting are related to parenting. Nevertheless, it is im-
portant to recognize that coparenting is only one of several likely mecha-
nisms operating in the relation between marriage and parenting. Other
possibilities include parent’s own adjustment (Cowan & Cowan, 2002;
Seifer & Dickstein, 2000), individual personality differences (Belsky et al.,
1995), and/or disrupted sibling relationships (Deal, Hagan, Bass, Hether-
ington, & Clingempeel, 1999). The field will benefit from further explora-
tion of possible mechanisms in this relationship.
such longitudinal studies are still correlational, they do permit one to make
slightly stronger causal inferences. Second, intervention studies allow experi-
mental manipulation and thus stronger causal inference. We therefore high-
light the role of intervention research in the next section.
Intervention Research
than just child adaptation. The bottom line is that the field of intervention
research on parenting and coparenting needs to move beyond asking if an
intervention works, to ask whom it works for and under what conditions,
and to focus on specific intervention targets (Cowan & Cowan, 2002).
In addition to an increased focus on intervention our understanding of
the relation among marital processes, parenting, and child development
will benefit from attention to recent advances in marital research, a topic
that we now briefly address.
Recent calls have been made to reconsider the role of conflict in marriage
(e.g., Bradbury, Rogge & Lawrence, 2001; Fincham, 2003). These calls re-
flect, in part, recognition of the fact that couple conflict varies according to
contextual factors. For example, conflictual marital interactions occur
more frequently in couples’ homes on days of high general life stress (e.g.,
Repetti, 1989), and at times and places associated with multiple competing
demands (e.g., Halford, Gravestock, Lowe, & Scheldt, 1992). Similarly the
interior context of the marriage is important for understanding marital
conflict. For example, in the context of poor support communication, con-
flict produces the greatest risk of marital deterioration (Pasch & Bradbury,
1998; see also Carels & Baucom, 1999; Saitzyk, Floyd, & Kroll, 1997),
whereas in the context of high levels of affectional expression between
spouses, the inverse correlation between negative spouse behavior and mar-
ital satisfaction decreases significantly (Huston & Chorost, 1994).
As a result, greater attention is being paid to positive aspects of marriage,
a development further facilitated by the emergence of positive psychology
(Sheldon & King, 2001), with its emphasis on shifting attention from re-
pairing distressed marriages to building up happy, satisfying marriages
(Seligman, 2002). This shift is paralleled in the more general family litera-
ture. As noted by McHale, Kuersten, and Lauretti (1996), much of the cur-
rent research on family processes focuses on conflict, and we must look be-
yond that in order for the field to progress.
The implications are relatively straightforward. First, the ecological niche
of the couple—their life events, family constellation, socioeconomic stand-
ing, and stressful circumstances—can no longer be ignored. For example,
Conger, Rueter, and Elder (1999) found that economic pressure in a sample
of predominantly rural families at Time 1 predicted individual distress and
observed marital conflict at Time 2, which in turn predicted marital distress
at Time 3. Such findings have lead to the argument that it may be “at least as
important to examine the struggle that exists between the couple . . . and the
environment they inhabit as it is to examine the interpersonal struggles that
are the focus of our work” (Bradbury et al., 2001, p.76). To the extent that
226 FINCHAM AND HALL
work on marriage and parenting takes into account the broader ecological
niche of the couple it is likely to be more informative.
Second, it is important to recognize the processes that promote and main-
tain good marriages, and examine how they relate to parenting. Focusing on
positive marital processes and strengths will allow researchers to broaden
their conceptual frameworks and look at a range of emotions, not solely neg-
ative ones. It has become increasingly apparent that marital satisfaction and
marital conflict are not mutually exclusive, and that positive, satisfying mari-
tal processes reflect much more than the absence of negative processes
(Fincham, 1998; Reis & Gable, 2003). However, at the same time, we must
not conclude that marital conflict is wholly negative; there are no doubt con-
structive elements that might even be beneficial (e.g., in building resilience
in the couple, providing children with a model of conflict resolution). The
field will benefit from incorporating into research on parenting and mar-
riage these and other constructive variables such as positive affect (Dix,
1991), communication (McHale et al., 1996), marital intimacy (O’Brien &
Peyton, 2002), and spousal supportive behavior (Pasch & Bradbury, 1998).
CONCLUSIONS
In this chapter we map the terrain that stands at the interface of marital and
parenting research. Our initial brief review of the direct effects of marital
processes on child development serves as a springboard for an extended
discussion of how the marital relationship may be linked to child develop-
ment via parenting behavior. We consider several models of the association
between marital processes and parenting before examining specific ways in
which marital conflict might influence parent behavior. This, in turn, high-
lights the need to consider marital attributions, child gender and triadic in-
teractions to gain a more complete understanding of the marital relation-
ship–parenting association. In the penultimate section of the chapter, we
offer some signposts that might guide future exploration, focusing in par-
ticular on coparenting as a process that links marital processes to mother-
ing and fathering and, to a lesser extent, the implications of recent develop-
ments in marital research. Although our map is far from complete, we hope
it will provide a heuristic stepping stone for cartographers as they develop a
more detailed map of the ecology of parenting and its implications for un-
derstanding child development.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This chapter was written while the first author was supported by a
grant from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
(R21AA013690).
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 227
REFERENCES
Abidin, R., & Brunner, J. (1995). Development of a parenting alliance inventory. Journal of
Clinical Child Psychology, 24, 31–40.
Amato, P. (1986). Marital conflict, the parent–child relationship, and child self-esteem. Family
Relations, 35, 403–410.
Appel, A. E., & Holden, G. W. (1998). The co-occurrence of spouse and physical child abuse: A
review and appraisal. Journal of Family Psychology, 12(4), 578–599.
Bearss, K., & Eyberg, S. (1998). A test of the parenting alliance theory. Early Education and De-
velopment, 9, 179–185.
Belsky, J. (1979). Mother–father–infant interaction: A naturalistic observational study. Develop-
mental Psychology, 15(6), 601–607.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55,
83–96.
Belsky, J., Crnic, K., & Gable, S. (1995). The determinants of coparenting in families with tod-
dler boys: Spousal differences and daily hassles. Child Development, 66(3), 629–642.
Belsky, J., & Hsieh, K. (1998). Patterns of marital change during the early childhood years: Par-
ent personality, coparenting, and division-of-labor correlates. Journal of Family Psychology,
12, 511–528.
Belsky, J., Lang, M. E., & Rovine, M. (1985). Stability and change in marriage across the transi-
tion to parenthood: A second study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47, 855–865.
Belsky, J., Putnam, S., & Crnic, K. (1996). Coparenting, parenting and early emotional devel-
opment. In J. P. McHale & P. A. Cowan (Eds.), Understanding how family-level dynamics affect
children’s development: Studies of two-parent families (pp. 45–56). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Belsky, J., Youngblade, L., Rovine, M., & Volling, B. (1991). Patterns of marital change and
parent–child interaction. Journal of Marriage and Family, 53, 487–498.
Block, J., Block, J., & Morrison, A. (1981). Parental agreement–disagreement on child-rearing
orientations and gender-related personality correlates in children. Child Development, 52,
965–974.
Bradbury, T. N., Beach, S. R., Fincham, F. D., & Nelson, G. M. (1996). Attributions and behav-
ior in functional and dysfunctional marriages. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 64,
569–576.
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Attributions and behavior in marital interaction.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 63, 613–628.
Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. (2000). Research on the nature and determi-
nants of marital satisfaction: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62(4),
964–980.
Bradbury, T. N., Rogge, R., & Lawrence, E. (2001). Reconsidering the role of conflict in mar-
riage. In A. Booth, A. C. Crouter, & M. L. Clements (Eds.), Couples in conflict (pp. 59–81).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brody, G. H., Arias, I., & Fincham, F. D. (1996). Linking marital and child attributions to fam-
ily processes and parent–child relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 10(4), 408–421.
Brody, G. H., & Forehand, R. (1985). The efficacy of parent training with maritally distressed
and nondistressed mothers: A multimethod assessment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23,
291–296.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge: Harvard University
Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Re-
search perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723–742.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child de-
velopment: Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187–249). London: Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
228 FINCHAM AND HALL
Buehler, C., Anthony, C., Krishnakumar, A., & Stone, G. (1997). Interparental conflict and
youth problem behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 6, 223–247.
Buehler, C., & Gerard, J. M. (2002). Marital conflict, ineffective parenting, and children’s and
adolescents’ maladjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 78–92.
Bugental, D. B., Blue, J., & Cruzcosa, M. (1989). Perceived control over caregiving outcomes:
Implications for child abuse. Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 532–539.
Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child cognitions in the context of the
family. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 315–344.
Burman, B., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1993). America’s angriest home videos: Behavioral
contingencies observed in home reenactments of marital conflict. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 61(1), 28–39.
Carels, R. A., & Baucom, D. H. (1999). Support in marriage: Factors associated with on-line
perceptions of support helpfulness. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 131–144.
Christensen, A., & Heavey, C. L. (1990). Gender and social structure in the demand/withdraw
pattern of marital conflict. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 59(1), 73–81.
Christensen, A., & Margolin, G. (1988). Conflict and alliance in distressed and non-distressed
families. In R. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families (pp.
263–282). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Christensen, A., & Walczynski, P. T. (1997). Conflict and satisfaction in couples. In R. J. Stern-
berg & M. Hojjat (Eds.), Satisfaction in close relationships (pp. 249–274). New York: The
Guilford Press.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.
Coiro, M. J., & Emery, R. E. (1998). Do marriage problems affect fathering more than mother-
ing? A quantitative and qualitative review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1(1),
23–40.
Collins, W. A. (1990). Parent–child relationships in the transition to adolescence: Continuity
and change in interaction, affect, and cognition. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, & T.
Gullotta (Eds.), Advances in adolescent development: Vol. 2. From childhood to adolescence: A tran-
sitional period? (pp. 85–106). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Conger, R. D., Rueter, M. A., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1999). Couple resilience to economic pres-
sure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 54–71.
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1992). When partners become parents: The big life change for couples.
New York: Basic Books.
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (2000). When partners become parents: The big life change for couples.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). Interventions as tests of family systems theories: Marital
and family relationships in children’s development and psychopathology. Development and
Psychopathology, 14, 731–759.
Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., Ablow, J., Johnson, V. K., & Measelle, J. (Eds.). (in press). The family
context of parenting in children’s adaptation to elementary school. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., Heming, G., Garrett, E., Coysh, W. S., Curtis-Boles, H., & Boles,
A. J. (1985). Transition to parenthood: His, hers, and theirs. Journal of Family Issues, 6,
451–481.
Cox, M. J., Paley, B., & Harter, K. (2001). Interparental conflict and parent–child relation-
ships. In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conflict and child development: The-
ory, research, and applications (pp. 249–272). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and marital conflict: The impact of family dispute
and resolution. New York: The Guilford Press.
Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Campbell, S. B. (2000). Developmental psychopathology and fam-
ily process. New York: Guilford Press.
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 229
Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Simpson, K. S. (1994). Marital conflict, gender, and chil-
dren’s appraisals and coping efficacy as mediators of child adjustment. Journal of Family Psy-
chology, 8(2), 141–149.
Cummings, E. M., Iannotti, R. J., & Zahn-Walker, C. (1985). The influence of conflict between
adults on the emotions and aggression of young children. Developmental Psychology, 21,
495–507.
Cummings, E. M., Zahn-Walker, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1981). Young children’s responses to
expressions of anger and affection by others in the family. Child Development, 52,
1274–1282.
Dadds, M. R., Schwartz, S., & Sanders, M. R. (1987). Marital discord and treatment outcome in
behavioral treatment of child conduct disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 55, 396–403.
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional
security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 387–411.
Davies, P. T., & Lindsay, L. L. (2001). Does gender moderate the effects of marital conflict on
children? In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conflict and child development:
Theory, research, and applications (pp. 64–97). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Deal, J. E., Hagan, M. S., Bass, B., Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, G. (1999). Marital in-
teraction in dyadic and triadic contexts: Continuities and discontinuities. Family Process,
38(1), 105–115.
DeVito, C., & Hopkins, J. (2001). Attachment, parenting, and marital dissatisfaction as predic-
tors of disruptive behavior in preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 215–231.
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive processes.
Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25.
Easterbrooks, M. A., & Emde, R. N. (1988). Marital and parent–child relationships: The role of
affect in the family system. In R. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within
families (pp. 83–103). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Emery, R. E., Fincham, F. D., & Cummings, E. M. (1992). Parenting in context: Systemic think-
ing about parental conflict and its influence on children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 60, 909–912.
Emery, R. E., & Tuer, M. (1993). Parenting and the marital relationship. In T. Luster & L.
Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (pp. 121–148). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent–child rela-
tions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 108–132.
Fauber, R., Forehand, R., Thomas, A. M., & Wierson, M. (1990). A mediational model of the
impact of marital conflict on adolescent adjustment in intact and divorced families: The
role of disrupted parenting. Child Development, 61, 1112–1123.
Fauber, R. L., & Long, N. (1991). Children in context: The role of the family in child psycho-
therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 813–820.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Ward, C. (1997). Marital satisfaction and spousal interaction. In R. J.
Sternberg & M. Hojjat (Eds.), Satisfaction in close relationships (pp. 160–189). New York: The
Guilford Press.
Feinberg, M. E. (2002). Coparenting and the transition to parenthood: A framework for pre-
vention. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5(3), 173–195.
Fincham, F. D. (1998). Child development and marital relations. Child Development, 69,
543–574.
Fincham, F. D. (2001). Attributions and close relationships: From balkanization to integra-
tion. In G. J. Fletcher & M. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology (pp. 3–31).
Oxford: Blackwells.
Fincham, F. D. (2003). Marital conflict: Correlates, structure, and context. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 12, 23–27.
230 FINCHAM AND HALL
Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. (1999). Conflict in marriage: Implications for working with cou-
ples. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 47–77.
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R., & Kemp-Fincham, S. I. (1997). Marital quality: A new theoretical
perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & M. Hojjat (Eds.), Satisfaction in close relationships (pp.
275–304). New York: The Guilford Press.
Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1987). The assessment of marital quality: A reevaluation.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 797–809.
Fincham, F. D., & Grych, J. H. (1991). Explanations for family events in distressed and
nondistressed couples: Is one type of explanation used consistently? Journal of Family Psy-
chology, 4, 341–353.
Fincham, F. D., Grych, J. H., & Osborne, L. N. (1994). Does marital conflict cause child malad-
justment? Directions and challenges for longitudinal research. Journal of Family Psychology,
8, 128–140.
Fincham, F. D., & Linfield, K. J. (1997). A new look at marital quality: Can spouses feel positive
and negative about their marriage? Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 489–502.
Floyd, F. J., Gilliom, L. A., & Costigan, C. L. (1998). Marriage and the parenting alliance: Lon-
gitudinal prediction of change in parenting perceptions and behaviors. Child Development,
69, 1461–1479.
Floyd, F. J., & Zmich, D. E. (1991). Marriage and the parenting partnership: Perceptions and
interactions of parents with mentally retarded and typically developing children. Child De-
velopment, 62, 1434–1448.
Frosch, C., Mangelsdorf, S., & McHale, J. (2000). Marital behavior and the security of pre-
schooler-parent attachment relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 144–161.
Gable, S., Belsky, J., & Crnic, K. (1992). Marriage, parenting, and child development: Progress
and prospects. Journal of Family Psychology, 5(3/4), 276–294.
Gjerde, P. F. (1986). The interpersonal structure of family interaction settings: Parent–adoles-
cent relations in dyads and triads. Developmental Psychology, 22(3), 297–304.
Goldberg, W. A., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1984). Role of marital quality in toddler develop-
ment. Developmental Psychology, 20, 504–514.
Gordis, E. B., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (2001). Parents’ hostility in dyadic marital and triadic
family settings and children’s behavior problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 69, 727–734.
Grych, J. H. (2002). Marital relationships and parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and applied parenting (pp. 203–225). Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: A cognitive-
contextual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 267–290.
Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Interventions for children of divorce: Toward greater in-
tegration of research and action. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 434–454.
Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (2001). Interparental conflict and child development: Theory, research,
and applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Halford, W. K., Gravestock, F. M., Lowe, R., & Scheldt, S. (1992). Toward a behavioral ecology
of stressful marital interactions. Behavioral Assessment, 14, 199–217.
Harold, G. T., Fincham, F. D., Osborne, L. N., & Conger, R. D. (1997). Mom and dad are at it
again: Adolescent perceptions of marital conflict and adolescent psychological distress. De-
velopmental Psychology, 33, 333–350.
Heinicke, C. M. (1995). Determinants of the transition to parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 277–303). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Howes, P., & Markman, H. J. (1989). Marital quality and child functioning: A longitudinal in-
vestigation. Child Development, 60, 1044–1051.
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 231
Huston, T. L., & Chorost, A. F. (1994). Behavioral buffers on the effect of negativity on marital
satisfaction: A longitudinal study. Personal Relationships, 1, 223–239.
Johnson, V. K. (2001). Marital interaction, family organization, and differences in parenting
behavior: Explaining variations across family interaction contexts. Family Process, 40,
333–342.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stabil-
ity: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3–34.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the trajectory of
marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1075–1092.
Karney, B. R., Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., & Sullivan, K. T. (1994). The role of negative
affectivity in the association between attributions and marital satisfaction. Journal of Person-
ality & Social Psychology, 66(2), 413–424.
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1996). Spillover effects of marital conflict: In search of parenting
and coparenting mechanisms. In J. P. McHale & P. A. Cowan (Eds.), Understanding how fam-
ily-level dynamics affect children’s development: Studies of two-parent families (pp. 57–76). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Kerig, P. K. (1995). Triangles in the family circle: Effects of family structure on marriage,
parenting, and child adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 28–43.
Kerig, P. K., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (1993). Marital quality and gender differences in
parent–child interaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 931–939.
Kitzmann, K. M. (2000). Effects of marital conflict on subsequent triadic family interactions
and parenting. Developmental Psychology, 36, 3–13.
Krishnakumar, A., & Buehler, C. (2000). Interparental conflict and parenting behaviors: A
meta-analytic review. Family Relations, 49, 25–44.
Kurdek, L. A. (1998). Prospective predictors of parenting satisfaction for fathers and mothers
with young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 56–65.
Larrance, D. T., & Twentyman, C. T. (1983). Maternal attributions and child abuse. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 92(4), 449–457.
Lewis, C. E., Siegal, J. M., & Lewis, M. A. (1984). Feeling bad: Exploring sources of distress
among pre-adolescent children. American Journal of Public Health, 74, 117–122.
Lindahl, K. M., Clements, M., & Markman, H. (1997). Predicting marital and parent function-
ing in dyads and triads: A longitudinal investigation of marital processes. Journal of Family
Psychology, 11, 139–151.
Lindahl, K. M., & Malik, N. M. (1999). Observations of marital conflict and power: Relations
with parenting in the triad. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 320–330.
Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital adjustment prediction tests: Their reliabil-
ity and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251–255.
Margolin, G., & Christensen, A. (1981). The treatment of families with marital and child problems.
Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy,
Toronto.
Margolin, G., Christensen, A., & John, R. S. (1996). The continuance and spillover of everyday
tensions in distressed and nondistressed families. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 304–321.
Margolin, G., Gordis, E. B., & John, R. S. (2001). Coparenting: A link between marital conflict
and parenting in two-parent families. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 3–21.
McHale, J. P. (1995). Coparenting and triadic interactions during infancy: The roles of marital
distress and child gender. Developmental Psychology, 31, 985–996.
McHale, J. P. (1997). Overt and covert parenting processes in the family. Family Process, 36,
183–201.
McHale, J. P., Johnson, D., & Sinclair, R. (1999). Family dynamics, preschoolers’ family repre-
sentations, and preschool peer relationships. Early Education & Development, 10, 373–401.
McHale, J. P., Kuersten, R., & Lauretti, A. (1996). New directions in the study of family-level dy-
namics during infancy and early childhood. In J. P. McHale & P. A. Cowan (Eds.), Under-
232 FINCHAM AND HALL
standing how family-level dynamics affect children’s development: Studies of two-parent families (pp.
5–26). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
McHale, J. P., Kuersten-Hogan, R., Lauretti, A., & Rasmussen, J. L. (2000). Parental reports of
coparenting and observed coparenting behavior during the toddler period. Journal of Fam-
ily Psychology, 14, 220–236.
McHale, J. P., Lauretti, A., Talbot, J., & Pouquette, C. (2002). Retrospect and prospect in the
psychological study of coparenting and family group process. In J. P. McHale & W. S.
Grolnick (Eds.), Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of families (pp. 127–165).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McHale, J. P., & Rasmussen, J. L. (1998). Coparental and family group-level dynamics during
infancy: Early family precursors of child and family functioning during preschool. Develop-
ment & Psychopathology, 10, 39–59.
Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the dependent variable. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141–151.
O’Brien, M., & Peyton, V. (2002). Parenting attitudes and marital intimacy: A longitudinal
analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 118–127.
O’Leary, K. D., Slep, A., & O’Leary, S. (2000). Co-occurrence of partner and parent aggres-
sion: Research and treatment implications. Behavior Therapy, 31, 631–648.
Oltmanns, T. F., Broderick, J. E., & O’Leary, K. D. (1977). Marital adjustment and the efficacy
of behavior therapy with children. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 45(5),
724–729.
Osborne, L. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1996). Marital conflict, parent–child relationships, and
child adjustment: Does gender matter? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42(1), 48–75.
Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (1997). Marital conflict and the development of infant–parent at-
tachment relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 11(2), 152–164.
Pasch, L. A., & Bradbury, T. N. (1998). Social support, conflict, and the development of mari-
tal dysfunction. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 66(2), 219–230.
Reid, W. J., & Crisafulli, A. (1990). Marital discord and child behavior problems: A meta-
analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18(1), 105–117.
Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2003). Toward a positive psychology of relationships. In C. L. Keyes
& J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 129–159). Washing-
ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Repetti, R. (1989). Effects of daily workload on subsequent behavior during marital interac-
tion: The roles of social withdrawal and spouse support. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 57, 651–659.
Rutter, M. (1994). Family discord and conduct disorder: Cause, consequence, or correlate?
Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 170–186.
Saitzyk, A. R., Floyd, F. J., & Kroll, A. B. (1997). Sequential analysis of autonomy-
interdependence and affiliation-disaffiliation in couples’ social support interactions. Per-
sonal Relationships, 4, 341–360.
Schoppe, S. J., Mangelsdorf, S. C., & Frosch, C. A. (2001). Coparenting, family process, and
family structure: Implications for preschoolers’ externalizing behavior problems. Journal of
Family Psychology, 15(3), 526–545.
Schumm, W. R., Paff-Bergen, L. A., Hatch, R. C., Obiorah, F. C., Copeland, J. M., Meens, L. D.,
& Bugaighis, M. A. (1986). Concurrent and discriminant validity of the Kansas Marital Sat-
isfaction Scale. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 381–387.
Seifer, R., & Dickstein, S. (2000). Parental mental illness and infant development. In C. H.
Zeanah, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd ed., pp. 145–160). New York:
Guilford Press.
Seligman, M. E. (2002). Positive psychology, positive prevention, and positive therapy. In C. R.
Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 3–9). London: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
7. PARENTING AND THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIP 233
Shek, D. T. (1998). Linkage between marital quality and parent–child relationship: A longitu-
dinal study in the Chinese culture. Journal of Family Issues, 19(6), 687–703.
Sheldon, K. M., & King, L. (2001). Why positive psychology is necessary. American Psychologist,
56(3), 216–217.
Slep, A. S., & O’Leary, S. (1998). The effects of maternal attributions on parenting: An experi-
mental analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 12(2), 234–243.
Snyder, J. R. (1998). Marital conflict and child adjustment: What about gender? Developmental
Review, 18, 390–420.
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of
marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28.
Stocker, C. M., & Youngblade, L. (1999). Marital conflict and parental hostility: Links with
children’s sibling and peer relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 13(4), 598–609.
Stright, A. D., & Neitzel, C. (2003). Beyond parenting: Coparenting and children’s classroom
adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(1), 31–39.
Vandewater, E. A., & Lansford, J. E. (1998). Influences of family structure and parental con-
flict on children’s well-being. Family Relations, 47(4), 323–330.
Vuchinich, S., Emery, R. E., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Family members and third parties in dyadic
family conflict: Strategies, alliances, and outcomes. Child Development, 59(5), 1293–1302.
Webster-Stratton, C., & Hammond, M. (1999). Marital conflict management skills, parenting
style, and early-onset conduct problems: Processes and Pathways. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 917–927.
Wilson, B. J., & Gottman, J. M. (1995). Marital interaction and parenting. In M. Bornstein
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Applied and practical parenting (pp. 33–55). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Zimet, D. M., & Jacob, T. (2001). Influences of marital conflict on child adjustment: Review of
theory and research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 4, 319–335.
This page intentionally left blank
8
Moncrieff Cochran
Cornell University
Susan K. Walker
University of Maryland, College Park
INTRODUCTION
235
236 COCHRAN AND WALKER
Social networks are specific sets of linkages among defined sets of people
(Mitchell, 1969). The type of social network of particular interest to the
readers of this chapter is personal, that is, anchored to a specific person or
family. In this case our focus is on the personal networks of parents or chil-
dren, or the whole family. These networks consist of those relatives, neigh-
bors, co-workers, and other friends who are directly linked to a family mem-
ber, and who may be linked to one another as well.
Some years ago, in an article linking child development with personal so-
cial networks, Cochran and Brassard (1979) defined the network of interest
as consisting of “those people outside the household who engage in activi-
ties and exchanges of an affective and/or material nature with the mem-
bers of the immediate family” (p. 601). In that original formulation the
spouse and children were specifically excluded from the parent’s personal
network, and siblings from the child’s, so long as they lived together with
the parent or child anchoring the network. Bryant (1985), examining
sources of support in middle childhood, defined the network as including
the family members in the child’s household, explicitly rejecting this earlier
definition.
From a conceptual standpoint the important distinction here is between
the nuclear family and the personal network. Bott (1957), in her networks
study Family and Social Network, emphasized the distinction to show that the
definition of roles in a marital relationship is a function in part of the struc-
ture of the personal networks that each person brings to the new family. In
so doing she distinguished membership in the nuclear family from mem-
bership in the networks of husband and wife. Study of nuclear families has a
long tradition in sociology and anthropology, and the subdiscipline of fam-
ily sociology has become well established during the past half century. Fam-
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 237
ily historians and others conceive of the family as an emotional entity rest-
ing on sentimental ties between husband and wife and parents and
children, and as a social unit with economic significance (Haraven, 1984).
Thus, the nuclear family is a concept that has meaning in the real world
and significance for the development of the individual, separate from the
impacts of other kin, associates and friends.1
There is empirical evidence that spousal and parent–child relations are
qualitatively different from those relationships maintained by parents or
children with people living outside the household. Brassard (1982) applied
the in versus outside the family distinction in the design of her own study of
mother–child interaction and personal social networks, by comparing
stress and support in one- and two-parent families and measuring the con-
tributions of the father separately from those of other kin and nonkin. She
found that the effects of a supportive father on mother–child relations were
quite different from the effects of a supportive network. Her research un-
derscores the value of making a distinction between members of the nu-
clear family and the rest of the personal network. Others have also identi-
fied the independent effect of a supportive partner on the parenting
behavior of the mother (Quinton & Rutter, 1985; Crockenberg, 1987). For
instance, Simons, Lorenz, Wu, and Conger (1993) used a sample of 451
two-parent families to examine a number of possible determinants of
parenting behavior, including economic pressure, parental depression
(mother and father), spouse support, and social network support. The
LISREL statistical model-building technique was used to examine how the
significant relations between economic pressure and supportive parenting
might be mediated by spouse support, network support, and depression.
Best fit models showed no significant association between spouse support
and network support and the two variables demonstrated differential rela-
tionships to parenting. Spouse support had a positive, direct influence on
parenting, and an indirect influence through depression. Network support
was only indirectly influential on parenting through mothers’ emotional
well-being. There was some evidence that spousal support was especially sa-
lient when social support was low, again suggesting that the two kinds of
support should be considered independent of one another.
1
1We recognize that the distinction between nuclear and extended families may not be valid
leads an individual to believe that he or she is cared for and loved, valued,
and a member of a network of mutual obligation. Crockenberg (1988) stated
that social support refers to the emotional, instrumental, or informational
help that other people provide to an individual. She went on to say that:
The personal social network has been defined earlier as a specific set of
linkages among a defined set of persons. The content of those linkages
ranges from information of various kinds (where to find work, how to rear
your child, which day care arrangement to choose) to emotional and mate-
rial assistance and access to role models (Cochran & Brassard, 1979; Mitch-
ell, 1969). Thus the social support concept focuses primarily on the types of
support provided (emotional, instrumental, informational) and the psy-
chological state of the receiving individual (cared for and loved, valued),
whereas with the personal network the emphasis is both on the characteris-
tics of the set of linkages (structure) and on a broader range of types of ex-
changes between the anchoring individual and members of the network
(content).
Researchers interested only in support have tended to map the networks
of their respondents with the use of probes that are oriented explicitly to
support, like “Please give me the names of all the people who provide you
with emotional support.” These particular defining characteristics lead to
identification of a partial network, excluding all of those people in a per-
son’s life who are not thought of primarily in terms of support. Such other
people are more likely to be included in response to an orienting question
like “Please give me the names of all the people who make a difference to
you in one way or another.” This more inclusive approach was the one
adopted in the Cornell studies described later in this chapter.
The emphasis by social support theorists and researchers on function
(what is provided) much more than on role (the socially proscribed parame-
ters of the relationship) helps to explain why they include partners as mem-
bers of the social support network along with friends and relatives living out-
side the household. Those interested in the broader social network concept
are as concerned with the limits imposed by society on personal relationships
as we are on the content of those relationships. For this reason we assign
spouses and partners living with the mother to the immediate family, and re-
serve network membership for those living outside the household.
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 239
Both the social network and the social support concepts are valuable.
The distinction can be maintained, in part, by acknowledging that network
relations are stressful as well as supportive, and that network members can
influence parenting in ways that extend well beyond those included in the
support concept.2 In her study of low-income mothers with young children
Belle (1982) was interested in the costs as well as the benefits of social ties,
and concluded that “one cannot receive support without also risking the
costs of rejection, betrayal, burdensome dependence, and vicarious pain”
(p. 143). Research has documented the negative consequences for develop-
ment in adolescence and early adulthood of network membership that is a
source of conflict as well as support (see for example, Crockenberg, 1987).
Based on their study of the support networks of 25 African-American ado-
lescent first-time mothers, Voight, Hans, and Bernstein (1996) concluded
that “when support is provided by someone who is also a source of stress,
the combination of support and conflict may be related to poor maternal
adjustment” (p. 70). Wellman (1981) wrote an entire chapter on the appli-
cation of network analysis to the study of support, in which he articulated
the various ways that the concept of social support can oversimplify the na-
ture of social networks:
What forces and factors influence how personal networks develop? What
determines their size and shape, and how they change over time? What is
the role of the individual parent in the network-building process? How do
the resulting networks affect parenting attitudes and behaviors? A frame-
work for addressing these questions is provided in Fig. 8.1, as a summariz-
ing model. A detailed presentation of the model and empirical evidence on
which the framework is based are presented elsewhere (Cochran, Larner,
Riley, Gunnarsson, & Henderson, 1990). The model incorporates the
forces constraining or shaping network development, the factors stimulat-
2
2Perhaps
the most powerful articulation of network influences that are distinct from social
support can be found in Granovetter’s 1973 article “The strengths of weak ties.”
240 COCHRAN AND WALKER
ways in which parents may be restrained by societal forces beyond their con-
trol from establishing social relations that are crucial to their development
as parents. At the same time, we also recognize that many of the constraints
by societies placed on individual behavior are constructive. Limits placed
on violence against children, for example, and on the right of parents to re-
linquish responsibility for financial support of their children are positive
constraints.
It is also important to note the use of the word initiatives, rather than
Fischer’s (1982) choices, to characterize the behavioral contribution of the
individual parent to the construction of his or her network. Initiatives
rather than choices is preferred because the word conveys action without
necessarily assuming that alternatives exist. That is, a parent can initiate the
act of including or excluding a person available in the “pool of eligibles”
without necessarily choosing among alternatives available in the pool.
Evidence presented later in this chapter indicates that a substantial num-
ber of parents experience severe constraints on free choice because of the
structural forces arraying on the left hand side of the figure. Fischer (1982)
wrote early in To Dwell Among Friends that, “In general, we each construct our
own networks” (p. 4). The evidence presented in this chapter, taken as a
whole, indicates: (a) that it is inappropriate to generalize across ecological
niches, and (b) that the networks of poor and undereducated parents—who
make up 20% to 25% of all parents in the United States—are largely con-
structed for them by their life circumstances. The model in Fig. 8.1 conveys
the power struggle between cultural and social structural forces on the one
hand and the individual on the other for control over the content of the per-
sonal social network. For any given adult the tilt in the power balance is de-
termined by that person’s location in the social structure of the society to
which he or she belongs. Recent studies indicate, for instance, that in the
United States an unemployed, poor, African-American, single mother has far
less control over who is included in her personal network than does a Euro-
pean-American, married, middle-income mother working outside the home
(Cochran et al., 1990). Wilson and Tolson (1990), in a review of the struc-
tural, functional, and interactional aspects of African-American familial net-
works, attribute the depletion of network resources ( jobs, father presence,
stable family membership) to social and economic opportunities. They cited
generational poverty, ongoing racial discrimination, and differences in the
economic status of African-American and European-American families as
some primary contributors to network vulnerability.
During the past decade there has been an expansion of knowledge related
to stability and change in the social networks of parents over time. The in-
augural longitudinal study of parents’ networks, reported by Larner
242 COCHRAN AND WALKER
(1990), showed that the networks of parents with young children show sta-
bility in structural characteristics such as overall size and kin/nonkin distri-
bution, but that considerable change can occur at the level of the individ-
ual network member, as one friend is replaced by another, for instance.
Manetti and Schneider (1996) have since replicated those findings with an
Italian sample of parents with 4- and 5-year-olds, examining social networks
with children’s school transition over two years. Bost, Cox, Burchinal, and
Payne (2002) focused on parents’ networks during the transition to parent-
hood, gathering networks and support data during the prenatal period and
over three time points until the firstborn’s second birthday. They also
found considerable stability in network size and amount of support during
that transition, yet noted important dynamic changes across networks dur-
ing this period.
Toward the end of his chapter Wellman (1981) reminded the reader that
personal networks operate within and must be influenced by the attributes
of larger social systems. He referred in that regard to the social and spatial
division of labor, the ways that bureaucracies are organized, and the social
classes to which respondents belong. These more macro-level constraints
were included on the left side of the model shown in Fig. 8.1. They repre-
sent aspects of the social ecology surrounding families and children that
shape their social and economic opportunities. In Fig. 8.2 they are shown in
greater detail. We discuss only certain of these constraint factors, given the
limited space available in a single chapter and our desire to illustrate other
aspects of the overall model.
Social Class
Family income, the educational level of parents, and the status and com-
plexity of the occupations parents engage in are thought of by sociologists
as contributing to determination of the social class in which a family is lo-
cated. The first sociologist to provide empirical evidence for the relations
between dimensions of social class and network ties was Fischer (1982), in
his book To Dwell Among Friends: Personal Networks in Town and City. Fischer’s
team interviewed slightly more than 1,000 people in a 20 county area
around San Francisco about their personal social networks. These people
were all English-speaking, at least 18 years old, and permanent residents.
Some were parents, others were not. They reflected rather well the diversity
in educational, occupational, economic, gender and life-cycle characteris-
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 243
tics of that part of California. It is these variations that are of particular im-
portance to us, because Fischer was able to consider the extent to which var-
ious aspects of the personal networks described by the people in his study
were associated with such social structural factors. Of these factors, Fischer
found that educational level had the most consistent effect upon the per-
sonal networks. He stated:
Other things being equal, the more educational credentials respondents had,
the more socially active they were, the larger their networks, the more com-
panionship they reported, the more intimate their relations, and the wider
the geographic range of their ties. In general, education by itself meant
broader, deeper, and richer networks. (p. 252)
Fischer also found that household income made a sizeable difference in the
networks reported, even with education held constant. People with more
income included more nonkin in their networks, and were more likely to
report adequate amounts of companionship and practical support than
were the poor. In considering the meaning of these relations between so-
cioeconomic factors and network ties Fischer offered three, not necessarily
contradictory, possibilities. Perhaps, he suggested, certain kinds of person-
alities result in both higher socioeconomic status and greater sociability.
Another explanation, and the one he seemed to prefer, emphasized the so-
244 COCHRAN AND WALKER
cial skills and concrete resources that come with more education and in-
come, and the ways that these skills and resources can be used to build and
maintain network ties. The third explanation, which he thought of as oper-
ating “in a straightforward structural manner” (p. 252), is one in which
schooling is viewed as providing the direct opportunity to meet and make
friends with people of like mind.
Fischer’s work also provided insight into the impacts of life cycle stage
and gender on personal networks. Married people named more relatives
and neighbors than did those who were unmarried, whereas single people
were more involved with nonkin. Children restricted the social involvement
of their parents, and especially of their mothers. “Women with children at
home had fewer friends and associates, engaged in fewer social activities,
had less reliable social support, and had more localized networks than did
otherwise similar women without children” (p. 253). From the gender per-
spective, women tended to be more involved with kinfolk and to report
more intimate ties than did men.
Some years ago Gunnarsson and the first author participated with Welsh and
West German colleagues in a coordinated set of studies of mothers’ social
networks, which extended Fischer’s general approach to a comparison of
networks across cultures (Sweden, the United States, Wales, West Germany),
social class (blue collar vs. white collar), and family structure (one vs. two par-
ent families; Cochran & Gunnarsson, 1990). The mothers in all four coun-
tries were Caucasian, and each had a 3-year-old child when first interviewed.
Concentrating first on mothers in two-parent families, the researchers found
in all four countries that mothers in white-collar families reported larger net-
works than did women in blue-collar households. Interestingly, the magni-
tude of the difference proved to be about the same in every country except
the United States, where the differences by occupational status were larger.
Network size however, differed by culture. Networks of mothers in the
United States and in Sweden were larger than those of mothers in West Ger-
many and in Wales. Looking beyond simply network size for the functions
performed by network members, again differences by class were readily ap-
parent. Mothers in the white-collar families reported involvement with a
higher number of network members in every category of social support. This
difference was most visible for social and recreational activities. The data sug-
gested, as a hypothesis, that mothers in white-collar families had more leisure
time at their disposal (especially in Sweden and the United States), and spent
some of that extra time in social and recreational activities with network
members. Across cultures, network support differed as well. Mothers in the
United States and in Sweden identified greatly higher percentages of net-
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 245
work members whom they could call on for all types of social support, com-
pared with the mothers in Germany and Wales.
Within the United States, MacPhee and colleagues (MacPhee, Fritz, &
Miller-Heyl, 1996) compared the social networks of Latin-American, Na-
tive-American and European-American parents. Their sample included 500
low-income parents of 2 to 5 year old children (74% mothers) living in ur-
ban and rural areas in a western state. The three ethnic groups were equally
likely to identify a domestic partner, children and parents in their social
networks. Though overall network size was similar across ethnic groups
(each mentioning approximately 15 persons), Native Americans had signif-
icantly more extended kin and siblings in their networks than did Euro-
pean Americans. More friends were found in the European-American re-
ports of network membership. Even with socioeconomic status held
constant, ethnic differences appeared in network density and contact.
The authors characterized Native-American caregivers as having a close
knit social system, one with which they felt satisfaction with support and fre-
quency of contact. The authors suggested that closeness of the Native-
American network may be a cultural phenomenon, but may also be due to
reservation dynamics and a response to local prejudice. Latin-American
parents also had large, close-knit networks yet relied on smaller numbers of
immediate and extended kin for emotional support. Offers of babysitting as
practical assistance were more prevalent within the larger network. A site
difference was noted within this ethnic group. Urban Latin Americans had
smaller, more cohesive networks with more expression of emotional sup-
port. MacPhee and his colleagues observed that urban areas afford more
opportunities for interaction and the formation of subcultures. The Latin
American families in this particular geographic area also showed relatively
little migration, suggesting residential stability as a potential factor in cohe-
sive networking. European-American parents, on the other hand, had more
diffuse networks in terms of contact and connectedness, yet reported more
offers of emotional support within the network. The authors suggested that
this may be due to geographic mobility or a preference to turn to a wider
range of different individuals for support and assistance.
Roschelle (1997), using data from the National Survey of Families and
Households (NSFH) looked at the structure and function of social support
across culture, gender, and class. In particular, Roschelle was interested in
determining if culture and/or economic need dominate in determining
participation in support networks and whether such differences are the re-
sult of cultural or gendered attitudes, economic resources, or a combina-
tion of these structural factors. Limitations of the data set precluded an
analysis of network size. Rather, Roschelle was able to examine the proxim-
ity of network members (specifically at least one parent, siblings, and adult
children) by distance from the respondent, the presence of extended fam-
246 COCHRAN AND WALKER
ily living in the home, and the exchange of support functions (child care,
monetary resources, and household assistance received from and given to
network members). The sample included 13,017 individuals 19 and older,
living in a household, and was divided by race/ethnicity to include Euro-
pean Americans, African Americans, Chicanos, and Puerto Ricans.
The examination of household help revealed that European Americans
were significantly more likely to be involved with their social networks in of-
fers of help than were the other three race/ethnic subgroups. Yet, examina-
tion of the receipt of assistance did not find this group’s participation with
their networks greater than the others. In fact, no predictable pattern
emerged across the race/ethnic groups on the type of support or source
within the network with whom support was exchanged. However, within
gender lines, offers of child care (for women) and household assistance
(for men) cut across all race/ethnic categories.
Looking at offers of child care to women by their families and extended
networks and by women to their networks, Roschelle found unexpected
race or ethnic differences across the four groups. In fact, she found that Eu-
ropean-American women, especially as compared with African American
mothers, were more likely than women in other groups to offer and receive
child care help. Economic circumstances, the proximity of extended family
members, and migration status played a part in the dynamics. As women’s
economic situations improved, so did their offers of child care to and from
family members. It was also determined that the closer a respondent lived
to family members, the greater the amount of child-care exchange. That
non U.S.-born Puerto Rican mothers exchanged less child care with family
members was believed to reflect the back and forth migration patterns of
immigrant parents. Roschelle observed that the lack of participation in so-
cial network exchanges of child care may not indicate that cultural norms
toward familism are irrelevant, rather that limited resources may prevent
poor women from these exchanges.
When she shifted her attention to the household help men contribute to
other family members, as well as the help they receive from family and ex-
tended family, Roschelle again found an impact based on economic condi-
tions. African American, Chicano, and Puerto Rican men were not more
likely than non-Latin American men to offer or receive household assistance.
Puerto Rican men’s assistance was similarly affected by their migration status.
The relation between exchanges of household assistance and Puerto Rican
ethnicity disappeared when migration status was controlled for. Roschelle
considered her results a replication of those of the first author and his col-
leagues (p. 177) and suggested that more impoverished families do not have
the time and resources necessary to become involved in offering or receiving
support from others in their family and extended family networks. She
suggested that the finding of limited availability of kin networks with declin-
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 247
ing economic conditions dispels a common belief that in tough times, and in
the face of fewer public services, families can turn to each other.
Limited resources also affect mothers’ satisfaction with support. If, as
Roschelle asserted, network participation is based on exchange reciprocity,
it would make sense that satisfaction with support from the network would
be tied to socioeconomic status and the ability to return assistance and sup-
port. Miller-Loncar and colleagues (Miller-Loncar, Erwin, Landry, Smith, &
Swank, 1998) examined the social network differences among low-income
African-American, Latin-American and European-American mothers and
determined that across all ethnic groups, mothers higher in socioeconomic
status were more likely to be satisfied with help from family and friends
than were those of lower SES.
Immigration status among non-U.S. parents also can affect the structure
and function of their network relations. In Roschelle’s analyses she found a
clear relation between migration status and participation of social networks
among Latin American mothers. Chicano and Puerto Rican respondents
born in the United States were far more active participants with their net-
works than were those not born in the United States. This was due to proxim-
ity; second and third generation mothers had more access to family mem-
bers. First generation mothers had to rely on visits to or from family members
who were in their native countries, and thus offered less support overall and
less offers of instrumental support specifically. Yet even this phenomenon
may be tied to economic status and motivation. Some racial–ethnic groups
may choose to live near family as a way to maintain cultural and familial ties.
Others may choose to move away from kin and nonkin, including migration
to another country as a means to seek greater access to economic resources.
Roschelle also noted that for Puerto Ricans circular migration patterns may
lead to the perception among network members of their unavailability to
participate in network exchanges and fulfill obligations. Therefore, migra-
tion among Puerto Ricans in the United States may discourage connections
to kin and nonkin in both countries.
Societies, and classes within societies, differ in terms of the roles women
are permitted or encouraged to adopt (mother vs. worker, for instance),
and the extent to which they can develop identities beyond those roles.
From a developmental perspective, one can distinguish between develop-
ment as a parent (parent role) and development as a person (personal
identity). Network members can be thought of as contributing more or less
to one or another of these developmental trajectories. It is reasonable to
suggest, for instance, that kinfolk contribute heavily to definition and rein-
forcement of the parental role, whereas other friends are more likely to
contribute to “development of self as person” or personal identity.
Our general understanding of class differences indicates that blue-collar
expectations are somewhat more conservative than those of white-collar
248 COCHRAN AND WALKER
workers regarding the roles of men and women. This pattern was reflected
in the network data presented by Cochran and colleagues, and Roschelle.
Cutting across cultures was the picture of lower-SES networks as somewhat
smaller, more kin-dominated, less geographically dispersed and more
child-related and practical in content than their higher income, more
highly educated counterparts. The overall impression from the Cornell net-
works study is that cultural expectations regarding the woman’s place over-
ride those of class; that is, the network differences were greater between
pairs of countries than among occupational levels within country. Adding
to this, Roschelle suggested that stereotypical behaviors about gender roles
extend beyond culture, race and socioeconomic status. In her analysis of
the NSFH data, she determined that men and women, across race and eth-
nic groups, taught their like-gendered offspring skills that maintain tradi-
tional male (occupational) and female (nurturing) skills.
Cross (1990) examined the personal networks of African-American sin-
gle and married mothers, and compared the size and functioning of those
networks with the social ties reported by White ethnic and nonethnic moth-
ers from similar socioeconomic circumstances living in the same city. His
study included a dimension of cross race membership in networks of the
mothers, testing the likelihood that at least one opposite race contact
would appear at the functional level of the network. The results of this anal-
ysis showed that 21% of the African American mothers and 16% of the
White parents in one-parent families had at least one opposite race friend.
This modest disparity by race increased as family structure changed and so-
cioeconomic level became higher; within the two-parent sample 41% of the
African American women but only 11% of the European Americans re-
ported friends of the opposite race.
Cross postulated a relation between the relative lack of African-Ameri-
can people in the networks of European-American mothers and the smaller
number of other friends reported by the African-American women. On the
one hand, he suggested that the exclusion of African Americans as poten-
tial friends would not have much of an effect on the overall size of Euro-
pean-American networks (reduce the pool of eligibles very much), since
the large numbers of European Americans in all sectors of everyday life pro-
vide numerous opportunities to meet and incorporate new European-
American nonkin contacts. On the other hand, he pointed out that this is
not the case for African-American people living in the same community. In
the northeastern city where this study was conducted only about 12% of the
population was African American. So the pool of potential same-race net-
work members was much smaller for African American than for European-
American mothers, meaning that any cross-race avoidance that might have
occurred would have placed the African American women at a relative dis-
advantage.
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 249
The evidence that Cross assembled strongly suggests that in the United
States race continues to be a social divide. He questions what the conse-
quences of such segregation might be for the development of children in
these families, and points for evidence to studies of group identity showing
that African-American children develop a much more bi-racial orientation
than European-American children, and so are better equipped to function
in a truly multi-racial society.
In this section of the chapter we present evidence that structural factors
operating at the levels of culture, class, race, ethnicity, and family structure
constrain the network-building opportunities of some parents more than of
others. That is, the pool of network eligibles available to some parents is
smaller than that available to others. African-American parents, parents with
relatively little education, first generation immigrants, and parents living in
cultures shaped by beliefs that lead to narrow definitions of the woman’s role
all have a smaller pool of potential network membership available to them
than do their more socioeconomically and socially advantaged counterparts.
Constraints accumulate for single parents, who often have less access to rela-
tives, to further education, to jobs paying a decent salary, and to housing in
neighborhoods that are supportive of neighboring activities.
One-Parent Families
cessity. Some single mothers in the U.S. sample were actually living with
their parents, because they could not afford to live elsewhere. Beyond hous-
ing there are other ways of relieving stress that require rather close proxim-
ity: child care, housework, transportation, and babysitting. Knowing of the
greater need by single parents for these services, it was not surprising to
find in the U.S. data that they were living closer to their network contacts
than were mothers living with their spouses. Therefore, it was puzzling to
find the opposite pattern in the Swedish data. One hypothesis is that the
sharp difference in proximity patterns is due to the much larger set of for-
mal supports available to such mothers in Sweden—parental leave, housing
allowance, child allowance, subsidized child care arrangements—supports
that may well reduce the need to live together with or right next to relatives
or friends.
Roschelle (1997) made an interesting observation about the exchanges
of social support involving different types of single mothers in the NSFH
data set. With single, never-married mothers she found fewer exchanges of
child care than in two-parent families. Single mothers were far less likely to
offer child care services to family members, suggesting that the presence of
a spouse enabled married women to offer care for others. This may also
have been due to co-residence of single mothers with other single mothers,
keeping the exchange of child care help within the household. Yet, when
the one parent family was headed by a woman who was divorced, separated
or widowed, exchanges of child care between the parent and her non-
family social network were more frequent than in two-parent families. The
social network membership of these women featured more relationships
with friends and neighbors. Roschelle hypothesized that divorced, sepa-
rated or widowed women with young children may bond in a friendship
network, or an intimate community of shared experiences. This may not be
as common or available to single, never-married mothers.
The underlying thesis of the previous section was that cultures are structured
in ways that may limit the range of social relations available to some, and per-
haps many, of their members. Equally important to acknowledge is the role
played by the individual in challenging those limits. One key feature of the
ecological orientation is its recognition that influence is bi-directional. Not
only are individuals and families shaped by the larger forces within which
they are embedded, but they also play active, initiating roles in altering those
larger systems to fit their needs. In his preface to Network Analysis: Studies in
Human Interaction (Boissevain & Mitchell, 1973), Boissevain refers to “the
concept of man as an interacting social being capable of manipulating others
as well as being manipulated by them” (p. viii). This concept of the human
252 COCHRAN AND WALKER
Personal Identity
McLanahan and her colleagues were the first researchers to link personal
identity with personal social networks (McLanahan, Wedemeyer, & Adel-
berg, 1981). Working with data gathered through in-depth interviews with 45
divorced mothers, they distinguished women who wished to maintain their
existing identity after the divorce from those trying to create a different and
less traditional identity for themselves. These researchers suggested that a
close-knit network may be especially supportive to those interested in retain-
ing their pre-divorce identity, and a loose-knit network can be helpful to
those seeking change. In practice, involvement in a close-knit network typi-
cally meant that the mother had frequent contact with her own mother and
other female relatives, who usually lived nearby. Women wishing to transition
from one role orientation to another—for instance, from homemaker to
working outside the home—seemed to be better served by more extended
networks, which included significant numbers of nonkin, female friends.
The study by McLanahan and colleagues alerted us to the fact that differ-
ent kinds of network resources are useful to women with differing defini-
tions of their own roles, and different future orientations. The differing
identities of the women studied translated into greater or lesser expecta-
tions for change. Where role transition was sought, some kinds of social re-
sources were more helpful than others. Implied by the findings, but not
studied directly, is the notion that when the needed resources are not pres-
ent in the network, women desiring change will seek them elsewhere, and
attempt to recruit them into their networks.
This reference to networks fitting with psychosocial needs serves as a re-
minder that the relations between personal identity and the network are dy-
namic. Alcalay (1983) summarized the differing functions of high and low
density networks under different conditions in this way:
Social networks can be both a source of direct help and a source of linkage to
other resources. In crises a small, dense network with strong ties may be most
valuable. At times of psychosocial transition, a low density network with weak
ties . . . may be most useful. Under these circumstances social support net-
works provide a much needed sense of identity, a feeling of belonging, of be-
ing wanted and worthwhile. (p. 73)
Stage of Development
create social presses for the individual that are different from the demands
stemming from developmental change. The causes of these events may be
independent of the individual. However, they are not necessarily direct ex-
tensions of the environmental constraints discussed earlier (see Fig. 8.2).
The interest here is in how such occurrences might stimulate networking
initiatives by the individual.
The transition to parenthood is a major life event for adults, with high
potential for change in both network composition and function. Bost et
al. (2002) examined patterns of change in couples’ social networks in the
months during their first child’s birth through 24 months postpartum.
One hundred and thirty-seven primarily European-American, working-
class couples from rural prenatal clinics in North Carolina participated in
the study. Network dimensions of size, composition and frequency of con-
tact, satisfaction with support, depression and adjustment scores for hus-
bands and wives were examined separately prenatally and three times
postpartum (3 months, 12 months and 24 months). All social network and
support dimensions remained relatively stable over the two years for both
parents, with network size the most consistent variable. At various points
across the years, some flux in network dimensions was observed. For ex-
ample, although family network size decreased after the birth of the chil-
dren, the frequency of contact with family members increased. Yet, as the
authors observed, amidst great change, and over time, social network
characteristics can remain stable.
Depression and adjustment scores were also found to be stable across
time for husbands and wives. Variability in these indicators of well-being re-
lated to mothers’ and fathers’ interactions with and the size of their social
networks. Increased network size was related to parental adjustment, yet
more frequent contact was associated with decreased adjustment. The au-
thors suggested that less adjusted parents may feel the need for more con-
tact with family members with the onset of parenting. Depression was nega-
tively linked to perceived reciprocity and satisfaction with network support,
particularly from friends, and the importance of this relationship increased
over time. Bost and her colleagues concluded that social relationships and
the support they offer remain critical aids to adjustment and well-being
during the transition to parenthood. They recommended however, that the
dynamic nature of social structures and support should be examined more
closely to determine their roles in familial adaptation over time.
The research by McLanahan and her associates and by Roschelle dis-
cussed earlier underscore the importance of the divorcing woman’s per-
sonal identity in determining the utility of existing network ties. Thus it is
plausible to suggest, pending better evidence, that separation or divorce
acts as a stimulus to network reorganization primarily in combination with
an interest in establishing a new identity and use of resources.
256 COCHRAN AND WALKER
Personality Characteristics
In the first section of this chapter we focus on the structural constraints de-
fining the pool of network potential available to parents in differing ecolog-
ical niches. In the second section we identify some of the factors that might
determine the propensity of parents to actively seek out relationships with
people in the pool of potential membership. We shift now to the question
of how networks with differing characteristics, once they are in place and
functioning, affect parenting attitudes and behavior. (See social processes, in
Fig. 8.1.)
Network Size
size and offers of support were associated with perception of the parenting
experience and observations of parenting behavior. Having more friends in
her social network was positively related to the quality of the adolescent’s
interactions with her one-year-old child, as was the number of types of sup-
ports that her mother offered (though the latter was negatively related to
the parenting experience). The relationship effect was conditional on the
source of the network support (more siblings and more sibling support re-
lated to poorer parenting behavior, support from male partners was not sig-
nificant), and on support from the network being positive. The authors de-
termined that larger network membership was related to the quality of the
parenting experience if the quality of the relationship was marred by con-
flict. More sources of conflicted support were negatively associated with
parenting behavior.
The study by MacPhee and associates (1996) referred to previously also
examined influences from social network structure and support on parent-
ing practices, and was based on the premise that ethnic differences in the
formation of and processes within personal social networks may help to ex-
plain subcultural variations in socialization practices of parents. As they
noted in their review of cultural literature related to parenting, network ex-
changes convey childrearing norms through approval or criticism. More
frequent exchanges with smaller numbers of individuals increase the op-
portunities for social sanctions, modeling, and reinforcement of behavior.
Values of familism and interdependence frequently seen in collectivistic
cultures, such as those associated with the Latin American and Native
American groups represented in their study, can reinforce the reliance
upon family members as the primary support network. As a result, parents’
exposure to information outside the kin network may be potentially lim-
ited. Social support from the cultural network that increases personal well-
being, and buffers parenting stress, may be most influential on parenting
when psychosocial risk is greatest.
These authors were cautious about teasing out ethnic network influ-
ences from those by geographic area, population density and social stratifi-
cation. Economic well-being is a critical dimension of ethnic differences in
both social network composition and function and parenting practices. Of-
ten when economic variables such as income, education, and occupation
are controlled, ethnic differences in social networks disappear. In fact, in
this study, though some variation was noted surrounding parents’ value for
autonomy, controlling for socioeconomic variables lessened the impact of
ethnicity on parenting.
Despite differences observed in structural aspects of the social networks
in the three ethnic groups, MacPhee and colleagues found that the quality
of emotional support proved to be a more important influence on
parenting. Controlling for psychosocial risk in within-ethnicity regression
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 261
mother. Analyses were carried out separately for families with and without
an employed mother.
Riley found that in the two-earner families both nonkin allies and local
female kin affected the child care participation of the father. Nonkin allies
are those highly elective and supportive network members who provided
three or more of the following six kinds of support to the father: practical
borrowing, financial assistance, work-related support, a person to talk with
about marital issues, emotional support, and social activities. Local female
kin consisted of the number of adult female relatives (including in-laws) in
the father’s network who lived in the same section of town. Of these two
sources of network support, the nonkin allies variable was a much stronger
predictor than was the number of local female kin. Riley noted that the two
were related; fathers with fewer local kinfolk had more nonkin allies. He
suggested the intriguing possibility that today some men are substituting
multiply-supportive nonkin bonds for the traditional extended family
bonds sustained by other men.
In the case of the one-earner families the pattern was different: There
was a powerful effect for what Riley called the male network variable, showing
that as the percentage of men in the network increased, the father’s share
of parent–child play went down. The existence of local female relatives also
decreased the father’s play involvement. The local female kin seemed to re-
duce the demand for the father’s assistance in childrearing, and the male
peer group to maintain activities and attitudes in competition with the
father’s home role. Riley concluded by emphasizing that in two-parent
households maternal employment status appeared to represent a crucial
ecological divide. His findings indicated that fathers are often pushed into
childrearing involvement, or away from it, by situational demands. The ex-
istence of a local female kin network appeared to relieve the pressure on
the father to participate in childrearing (or it may have competed with him
if he wanted more responsibility for his children). At the same time, there
was evidence that fathers may to some extent select or construct the social
environment that thereafter influences them. The male peer group and
nonkin allies are highly amenable to active construction by the individual.
This is especially true for the nonkin allies, since they represent social
bonds encompassing diverse kinds of support content, often with an indi-
vidual known through no regular role-context.
Cotterell (1986), working in rural towns of inland Australia, was inter-
ested in the influences on childrearing of the father’s workplace, the
mother’s social network, and the community itself. With a total of 96 mar-
ried mothers he compared the personal networks of and the childrearing
milieus provided by those with husbands regularly present with those whose
partners’ jobs routinely required periods of absence from home. Character-
istics of the childrearing environment were measured with the Caldwell
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 263
HOME inventory, and the quality of maternal expectations and beliefs was
assessed with the Parent as a Teacher (PAAT) inventory. Analysis of the net-
work data indicated that mothers with absentee husbands relied more
heavily on their neighbors than those with husbands regularly at home.
When the quality of childrearing variables were analyzed by father presence/
absence and mother’s amount of informational support from the network,
the characteristics of the father’s work exerted independent influence on
only two of the variables, while the effects of support were statistically signif-
icant for six of the seven childrearing measures. In order to assess the sepa-
rate and joint contributions of the three environmental dimensions, Cot-
terell entered them all with the childrearing variables in a regression
equation. In his own words:
Between 40% and 60% of the variance explained in the full model could be
attributed to the factors of father absence, community characteristics, and
mother’s informational support. Of the three factors, the support factor had
the greatest prominence in terms of its power to add significantly to the vari-
ance in the measures of childrearing quality. (p. 369)
Cotterell was careful to point out the danger of assuming that these three
environmental forces—general character of the community, father’s work
situation and network support—operate independently and at the same
level of influence. He suggested that “the chain of influence of father’s
work is connected to maternal behavior via the patterns of social relation-
ships established by the mother” (p. 371). His evidence indicated that the
wives of absentee husbands had smaller networks, and that these women
had a more limited range of settings available for contacting network mem-
bers.
It is important to keep in mind the effects of context on the value of sup-
port as an influence on parenting. Ceballo and McLloyd (2002) examined
whether living under stressful neighborhood conditions influences social
support’s effects on parenting behavior. Their sample was 262 low income,
African-American single mothers of middle school age children who re-
sided in lower and working-class urban neighborhoods. As neighborhood
quality decreased, based on mothers’ perceptions, violent crime rate statis-
tics, and levels of poverty by families in the area, emotional support from
friends and relatives had less influence on mothers’ nurturant parenting.
Similarly, the negative relation between instrumental support and maternal
punishment was also weakened as neighborhood quality diminished. The
findings suggest that in certain conditions, social support may have limited
value as a cushion to mothers’ stress. The authors cautioned that a fuller
understanding of parenting behavior should include the consideration of
context as potential moderator of psychological relations.
264 COCHRAN AND WALKER
The adequacy of the mother’s social support is clearly and consistently associ-
ated with the security of the infant–mother attachment: low social support
was associated with high resistance, high avoidance, and with anxious attach-
ment. Moreover, that support had its strongest effects on the irritable babies
and their mothers suggests that the availability of social support is particularly
critical when the family is under particular stress. (p. 862)
The fact that the positive relation between social support for the mother
and the attachment behaviors of the child was obtained primarily in the
case of irritable babies is important to note. An environmental demand, the
irritable baby, appears to have created the conditions calling for mobiliza-
tion of existing support.
Melson, Ladd, and Hsu (1993) tested a model of maternal network char-
acteristics as direct influences on children’s social and cognitive develop-
ment and as indirect influences through mother’s attributions about
parenting. The sample was comprised of 69 primarily European-American,
moderately high SES mothers and their preschool age children from a uni-
versity preschool setting. Network size and quality (including components
of contact frequency, duration, and perceived helpfulness, conflict, satisfac-
tion and a willingness to seek help), directly predicted child cognitive per-
formance. Children’s exposure to stimulation and network influences on
maternal interactions were cited as probable processes at play. Indirectly,
network size positively influenced cognitive performance and peer accep-
tance through maternal attributions about helping the child. Network size
was also related to differing patterns of attributions depending on the ease
of the helping situation. Mothers who had larger social networks were more
likely to attribute an easy situation of cognitive or social assistance to the
child, and they were less likely to make a child attribution when the situa-
tion of assistance was difficult. The authors suggested that more support
from a larger social network may expose mothers to more information
about caring for their children, generate a greater sense of well-being in
mothers, and help them feel more positive toward the children.
Manetti and Schneider (1996) examined the direct connection between
the structure and satisfaction with parents’ social networks and children’s ad-
justment to the transition to elementary school. They studied an Italian sam-
ple of mothers, their spouses, and their children over a three-year period, be-
ginning when the children were 4- and 5-year-olds. The size of the mothers’
networks at year one predicted the adjustment of the children to school two
years later, as measured by teacher ratings. Network support to mothers who
experienced stressful life events showed a stronger impact. For these moth-
266 COCHRAN AND WALKER
ers, both network size and frequency of contact were predictive of children’s
school adjustment, at one point in time (year three) and over time (year one
data predicting year three adjustment). The findings were related only to the
mother’s social network. The authors cautioned that the lack of findings for
the fathers may be related to small numbers in the sample over time.
With an ethnically and economically diverse sample, Marshall and col-
leagues (Marshall, Noonan, McCartney, Marx, & Keefe, 2001) looked at the
social networks of African-American, European-American and Latin-Ameri-
can parents with an elementary school age child (grades 1 to 4) to deter-
mine the relation, if any, between parents’ social networks and several
aspects of children’s well-being. This study differed from others in that non-
standard measures of size and structure were used to measure the social
network. Network homogeneity was determined by whether or not kin were
within walking distance, and network size was determined by the number of
neighbors the parent knew well enough to stop and talk with outside the
home. Also, the children were involved by answering questions about the
availability of adults to talk about their feelings.
There was no difference in emotional support provided by the social net-
works to parents of different races or ethnic backgrounds. European-Amer-
ican parents had less extended family nearby and were more involved with
their neighbors. African-American parents were more likely to rely on rela-
tives than on friends in aspects of caring for their children and, like Latin-
American families, had more extended family nearby. Although neighbor-
hood ties were positively associated with children’s social competence and
negatively associated with children’s depression, when parent gender, ma-
ternal education and ethnicity were controlled, these direct relations were
no longer detected. However, indirect effects of social networks on child
outcomes were found through the influence of the network on parental be-
havior and efficacy. Parents with more heterogeneous networks demon-
strated more responsive, warm parenting practices, and their children had
fewer behavior problems. This outcome was also observed in the children
of those parents who reported feeling more effective in their parenting
role, which was influenced by the degree of emotional support from their
networks. And, parents with heterogeneous networks also provided a more
stimulating home environment for their children, who showed more posi-
tive social skills.
ter and discussing and sharing it with others in the social network were sig-
nificantly linked, but not so strongly that network involvement mediated
the effect of participation; they were largely separate processes. Walker and
Riley concluded from the findings that social network processes and par-
enting education intervention may exist as parallel learning environments
for new parents and function perhaps, as separate channels of information
about childrearing. But the analyses also revealed that they operate con-
jointly. The authors observed that the processes were correlated and that
the measure of network involvement referred only to interactions around
program (newsletter) content. This research encourages the question
whether other results reported by parenting programs may, in part, be the
result of stimulated interaction with the parents’ personal support network.
CONCLUSIONS
ary education would appear to deserve highest priority, because those par-
ents who had acquired such schooling were much richer in network re-
sources than those who had not. College scholarships, student loans, and
programs providing incentives to adolescents and young adults for continu-
ation of schooling through high school and beyond would seem to be an es-
pecially good investment to society from this standpoint.
Policies that stimulate job creation and continuation would also deserve
high priority from a networks in context perspective. The workplace has been
seen in our studies to be an important context for network building, and
the economic returns associated with working provide the resources
needed to sustain network relations. Just as importantly, the absence of job
security for much of today’s semi- or unskilled and non-unionized work
force reduces the capacity of the workplace to support the development of
social relationships among co-workers. Economic instability also contrib-
utes to residential mobility as families leave the state to seek better fortune
in another part of the country. The protection of job security and assur-
ances of fair treatment for employees during hard times would support the
social as well as the economic well-being of working families.
We would be remiss, however, if in closing we left the impression that en-
hancement of network development can come only through removal of so-
cietal constraints. Several of the factors contributing to parental initiative in
developing their network relations are accessible to public policies and pro-
grams. Educational opportunity has already been mentioned. Home visit-
ing and parent support groups, if carried out in an empowering way,
enhance parental self-esteem, and thereby increase network-building po-
tential. Any family support program that provides parents with respite, and
more time to devote to family life, increases the capacity of parents to en-
gage in the social activities that develop and maintain network ties. And ini-
tial research about the intersection of parenting program influences and
parents’ social networks suggests great potential for strengthening network
ties and relations through parent education and support programs.
At the same time, the most important policy lesson to be learned from
the various studies of networks and parenting is that it is not nearly enough
simply to provide opportunities for social interaction, and the message that
social ties are important, if parents are to be supported on behalf of their
own development and that of their children. Public policies must give first
priority to freeing parents, and those who will become parents, from the
constraints of inequality and oppression, by insuring the provision of ade-
quate and sufficient education, employment, and housing conditions. The
evidence suggests that freedom to grow and develop, through schooling,
work, and leisure activities, will lead in turn to the social network connec-
tions that form the basis for healthy, productive communities.
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 271
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Permission to use the material in Figs. 8.1–8.3 has been provided by Cam-
bridge University Press.
REFERENCES
Alcalay, R. (1983). Health and social support networks: A case for improving interpersonal
communication. Social Networks, 4, 71–88.
Belle, D. (1982). Social ties and social support. In D. Belle (Ed.), Lives in stress: Women and de-
pression (pp. 223–241). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Boissevain, J., & Mitchell, J. C. (1973). Network analysis: Studies in human interaction. The Hague:
Mounton.
Bost, K. K., Cox, M. J., Burchinal, M. R., & Payne, C. (2002). Structural and supportive changes
in couples’ family and friendship networks across the transition to parenthood. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 64, 517–531.
Bott, E. (1957). Family and social networks. London: Havistock.
Brassard, J. (1982). Beyond family structure: Mother–child interaction and personal social networks.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York.
Bryant, B. (1985). The neighborhood walk: Sources of support in middle childhood. Mono-
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(3, Serial No. 210).
Ceballo, R., & McLloyd, V. (2002). Social support and parenting in poor, dangerous neighbor-
hoods. Child Development, 73, 1310–1321.
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1984). Origins and evolution of behavior disorders. New York: Bruner/
Mazel.
Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 39,
300–314.
Cochran, M. (1990). The network as an environment for human development. In M. Cochran,
M. Larner, D. Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr. (Eds.), Extending families: The social
networks of parents and their children (pp. 265–276). London/New York: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Cochran, M., & Brassard, J. (1979). Child development and personal social networks. Child De-
velopment, 50, 609–615.
Cochran, M., & Bø, I. (1989). The social networks, family involvement and pro- and anti-social
behavior of adolescent males in Norway. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18(4), 377–398.
Cochran, M., & Gunnarsson, L. (1990). The social networks of married mothers in four cul-
tures. In M. Cochran, M. Larner, D. Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr. (Eds.), Ex-
tending families: The social networks of parents and their children (pp. 86–104). London/New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Cochran, M., & Riley, D. (1990). The social networks of six-year-olds: Context, content, and
consequence. In M. Cochran, M. Larner, D. Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr.
(Eds.), Extending families: The social networks of parents and their children (pp. 154–178). Lon-
don/New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cochran, M., Larner, M., Riley, D., Gunnarsson, L., & Henderson, C., Jr. (1990). Extending fam-
ilies: The social networks of parents and their children. London/New York: Cambridge University
Press.
272 COCHRAN AND WALKER
Colletta, N. (1981). Social support and the risk of maternal rejection by adolescent mothers.
Journal of Psychology, 109, 191–197.
Cotterell, J. (1986). Work and community influences on the quality of childrearing. Child De-
velopment, 57, 362–374.
Crittenden, P. (1981). Abusing, neglecting, problematic, and adequate dyads: Differentiating
by patterns of interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 27, 1–18.
Crittenden, P. (1985a). Maltreated infants: Vulnerability and resistance. Journal of Child Psychol-
ogy and Psychiatry, 26, 85–96.
Crittenden, P. (1985b). Social networks, quality of child rearing, and child development. Child
Development, 56, 1299–1313.
Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social support influ-
ences on the security of infant–mother attachment. Child Development, 52, 857–865.
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Support for adolescent mothers during the postnatal period: Theory
and research. In C. F. Z. Boukydis (Ed.), Research on support for parents and infants in the
postnatal period. New York: Ablex.
Crockenberg, S. (1988). Social support and parenting. In H. Fitzgerald, B. Lester, & M.
Yogman (Eds.), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 4, pp. 67–92). New York/
London: Plenum.
Cross, W. (1990). Race and ethnicity: Effects on social networks. In M. Cochran, M. Larner, D.
Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr. (Eds.), Extending families: The social networks of
parents and their children (pp. 67–85). London/New York: Cambridge University Press.
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1993). Parenting in two generations of Mexican American families. Inter-
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 16(3), 409–427.
Fischer, C. (1982). To dwell among friends: Personal networks in town and city. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Granovetter, M. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology, 78, 1360–1380.
Gunnarsson, L., & Cochran, M. (1990). The social networks of single parents: Sweden and the
United States. In M. Cochran, M. Larner, D. Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr.
(Eds.), Extending families: The social networks of parents and their children (pp. 105–116). Lon-
don/New York: Cambridge University Press.
Haraven, T. (1984). Themes in the historical development of the family. In R. D. Parke (Ed.),
Review of child development research (Vol. 7, pp. 86–101). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Hinde, R., Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Tamplin, A. (1985). Characteristics of 3- to 4-year-olds as-
sessed at home and their interactions in pre-school. Developmental Psychology, 21, 130–140.
Jennings, K. D., Stagg, V., & Connors, R. (1991). Social networks and mothers’ interactions
with their preschool children. Child Development, 62, 966–978.
Larner, M. (1990). Changes in network resources and relationships over time. In M. Cochran,
M. Larner, D. Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr. (Eds.), Extending families: The social
networks of parents and their children (pp. 181–204). London/New York: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
MacPhee, D., Fritz, J., & Miller-Heyl, J. (1996). Ethnic variations in personal social networks
and parenting. Child Development, 67, 3278–3295.
Manetti, M., & Schneider, B. H. (1996). Stability and change in patterns of parental social sup-
port and their relation to children’s school adjustment. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
chology, 17, 101–115.
Marshall, N., Noonan, A., McCartney, K., Marx, F., & Keefe, N. (2001). It takes an urban vil-
lage. Parenting networks of urban families. Journal of Family Issues, 22, 163–182.
McLanahan, S., Wedemeyer, N., & Adelberg, T. (1981). Network structure, social support and
psychological well-being in the single-parent family. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43,
601–612.
8. PERSONAL SOCIAL NETWORKS 273
Melson, G. F., Ladd, G. W., & Hsu, H. (1993). Maternal support networks, maternal
cognitions, and young children’s social and cognitive development. Child Development, 64,
1401–1417.
Miller-Loncar, C. L., Erwin, L. J., Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (1998). Character-
istics of social support networks of low socioeconomic status African American, Anglo
American, and Mexican American mothers of full-term and preterm infants. Journal of Com-
munity Psychology, 26, 131–143.
Mitchell, J. C. (1969). Social networks in urban situations. Manchester, England: Manchester Uni-
versity Press.
Polansky, N., Gaudin, J., Ammons, P., & Davis, K. (1985). The psychological ecology of the ne-
glectful mother. Child Abuse & Neglect, 9, 265–275.
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1985). Parenting behavior of mothers raised “in care.” In R. Nicol
(Ed.), Longitudinal studies in child psychology and psychiatry. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Riley, D. (1990). Network influences on father involvement in childrearing. In M. Cochran, M.
Larner, D. Riley, L. Gunnarsson, & C. Henderson, Jr. (Eds.), Extending families: The social
networks of parents and their children. London/New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rogosch, F. A., Mowbray, C. T., & Bogat, G. A. (1992). Determinants of parenting attitudes in
mothers with severe psychopathology. Development and Psychology, 4, 469–487.
Roschelle, A. (1997). No more kin: Exploring race, class and gender in family networks. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sheldon, S. B. (2002). Parents’ social networks and beliefs as predictors of parent involvement.
The Elementary School Journal, 102(4), 301–316.
Simons, R. L., Lorenz, F. O., Wu, C., & Conger, R. D. (1993). Social network and marital sup-
port as mediators and moderators of the impact of stress and depression on parental be-
havior. Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 368–381.
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Bruner/Mazel.
Tracy, E. M., Whittaker, A. P., Kapp, S., & Overstreet, E. J. (1994). Support networks for pri-
mary caregivers receiving family preservation services: An exploratory study. Families in Soci-
ety: The Journal of Contemporary Human Services, 75(8), 481–489.
Voight, J. D., Hans, S. L., & Bernstein, V. J. (1996). Support networks of adolescent mothers:
Effects on parenting experience and behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17(1), 58–73.
Walker, S. K., & Riley, D. A. (2001). Involvement of the personal social network as a factor in
parent education effectiveness. Family Relations, 50, 186–193.
Wellman, B. (1979). The community question: The intimate networks of East Yorkers. Ameri-
can Journal of Sociology, 84, 129–131.
Wellman, B. (1981). Applying network analysis to the study of support. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.),
Social networks and social support (pp. 166–183). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Wilson, M. N., & Tolson, T. F. J. (1990). Familial support in the Black community. Journal of
Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 347–355.
This page intentionally left blank
9
Ann C. Crouter
Susan M. McHale
The Pennsylvania State University
INTRODUCTION
In Middletown, the classic monograph about U.S. culture, Lynd and Lynd
(1929) coined the phrase “the long arm of the job” to describe the consid-
erable influence that fathers’ employment situations had on their families.
As the Lynds explained, the temporal rhythms of work life and the specter
of unemployment helped to shape many aspects of family life in Middle-
town, including parents’ childrearing activities. In the years since that pub-
lication, the phrase “the long arm of the job” has been borrowed by other
scholars too. Waller (1938) used it as a chapter title in his book, The family:
A dynamic interpretation, as did Komarovsky (1962) in Blue Collar Marriage.
Menaghan (1991) used the term “the long reach of the job” in her chapter
on work and family for the Annual Review of Sociology. We were pleased to
join this tradition in our chapter (Crouter & McHale, 1993a) in the first
edition of Parenting: An Ecological Perspective and to continue it in our cur-
rent reassessment of the influences of parental work on childrearing.
In our 1993 chapter on the long arm of the job, we acknowledged that
the influence of work on family life was no less important then than it was in
the Lynds’ day, but we noted that the relationship between these two social
institutions had become more complex with the entrance of large numbers
of mothers into the paid labor force. Indeed, the movement of women, es-
pecially mothers of young children, into the labor force is one of the most
dramatic demographic trends of the twentieth century (Hernandez, 1997).
275
276 CROUTER AND MCHALE
When the impact of parental work on parenting is viewed through the lens
of the workplace, there are three levels of analysis to consider. At the broad-
est level, work is adults’ means of subsistence and, as such, is a central deter-
minant of their world views (Kanter, 1977). Work informs parents’ concep-
tualizations of how their environments operate and the qualities required
to be successful in these environments (Kohn, 1977; Ogbu, 1981). These
ideas in turn shape parents’ values about the characteristics that are impor-
tant to inculcate in their young.
At an intermediate level, the characteristics of parents’ jobs and the orga-
nizations in which they are employed pose opportunities and constraints for
parenting. For example, some jobs encourage the acquisition of skills that
are applicable not only on the job but at home. Other work settings provide
informal support to working parents via helpful co-workers and supervisors
or access to formal workplace benefits and programs that enable parents to
carry out their parenting roles more effectively. Jobs also come with charac-
teristic stressors, such as low control, fast pace, or frequent deadlines, stress-
ors that have implications over time for the quality of parenting.
Finally, the work–parenting system can be examined at a very proximal,
immediate level, a view of work–family dynamics that focuses on daily work
experiences. Although most jobs have fairly stable characteristics, such as a
schedule, an assortment of supportive or unsupportive co-workers, and cer-
tain activities that make up the nature of the work itself, there are also daily
fluctuations in work demands and interpersonal interactions that shape the
daily emotional state of the employee. “How was your day?” is a familiar
question in the post-reunion script of family members. The answer to that
question usually is based on the unusual occurrences of that particular day
and often has to do with how those events made the person feel. Here, work
is seen as an influence on parents’ emotional states, moods that in turn may
be brought home to influence the tenor and content of parent–child and
mother–father interactions.
278 CROUTER AND MCHALE
Anthropologist Ogbu (1981) argued that adults acquire ideas about what it
takes to be successful in their culture by observing prevailing subsistence
patterns, that is, by observing work roles in the culture that are associated
with success. In a hunting and gathering society, for example, adults iden-
tify the characteristics of successful hunters (e.g., individualism, bravery,
risk-taking, and independence) as those traits that they value and wish to in-
culcate in their children (Barry, Child, & Bacon, 1959). Ogbu (1981) ex-
plained that adults form theories of childrearing that correspond to those
values and, in turn, they develop childrearing techniques designed to foster
these desirable qualities in children. Miller and Swanson (1958) provided
early evidence for this idea in their comparison of parental values of moth-
ers whose husbands were employed in entrepreneurial versus bureaucratic
work settings. The former group emphasized achievement and striving in
their children, whereas their bureaucratic counterparts emphasized inter-
personal skills and getting along well with others in their reports of
childrearing values.
Note that the early work in this area simplified the picture considerably.
The causal arrow was seen to run in one direction: from work (notably fa-
thers’ work) to parenting values. There was little recognition that powerful
sorting mechanisms function to distribute people into jobs on a non-ran-
dom basis, as is the case in western, industrialized societies. People select
(and are selected for) jobs based on a complex set of variables including ap-
titudes, educational background, specific job related training, interests,
skills, perceptions of opportunity, attraction to the lifestyle represented by
the job, and so forth. Thus, before they even take their first jobs, future en-
trepreneurs and bureaucrats undoubtedly differ on many characteristics
that may in turn have implications for parenting. The reciprocal linkages
between work and the individual are rarely acknowledged in work and fam-
ily research but are critically important. Jobs shape individual functioning,
but individuals also actively select in and out of careers, occupations, and
specific jobs.
One of the leading researchers in the area of work and parental values
who has recognized the complex, reciprocal linkages between work and in-
dividual characteristics is sociologist Kohn. In Class and Conformity, Kohn
(1977) argued that men who differ in their occupational position in the
stratification system come to see the world differently and that the charac-
teristics associated with success in men’s occupational niches influence the
qualities they value in their children and, consequently, the characteristics
they will support or discourage in the context of daily childrearing activi-
ties. In a survey of more than 3,000 employed men, Kohn (1977) found that
men in middle-class occupations tended to value independence and initia-
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 279
larly pronounced for unmarried women who took on jobs low in complex-
ity and low in wages. The quality of home environments in turn was related
to children’s receptive vocabulary at ages 3 to 6 and reading and math skills
at ages 5 to 8 (Parcel & Menaghan, 1994). Interestingly, Parcel and Men-
aghan (1994) did not find statistical evidence that the quality of the home
environment mediated the effects of occupational self-direction on child
outcomes. Thus, the processes through which occupational complexity af-
fects children’s development remain unclear.
In a small scale study of 65 mother–infant dyads, Luster, Rhoades, and
Haas (1989) focused more precisely on what the mechanisms might be that
may link social class and parenting behavior during the first year of life.
These researchers replicated the finding that social class is related to par-
ents’ values of conformity and self-direction in their children. They also
identified a set of specific parental beliefs that mediated the relation be-
tween parental values and parenting behavior. Global parenting values
were related to specific attitudes about spoiling the baby, giving babies free-
dom to explore the home environment, discipline, and the importance of
verbal stimulation. These specific beliefs, in turn, were related to mothers’
supportive and constraining behaviors toward their babies as reported by
the mothers themselves and as rated by interviewers.
The dual-earner arrangement, so prevalent today, raises the issue that
mothers and fathers, by virtue of their occupational positions, may identify
different qualities as important and come to favor different childrearing
practices. A mismatch in childrearing values and strategies may be unusual,
however, given patterns of assortative mating in this culture (i.e., men and
women who marry tend to have similar levels of education and thus would
probably gravitate to jobs at similar levels in the occupational hierarchy)
and given the fact that husbands and wives socialize one another through-
out the life course (Gruber-Baldini, Schaie, & Willis, 1995). On the other
hand, due to the prevalence of occupational segregation along gender
lines, even within occupations, men and women tend to hold quite differ-
ent jobs (Baron & Bielby, 1985). A hypothesis for future research is that in-
congruence in occupational position (and hence, world view) for husbands
and wives is linked to differences in childrearing values and philosophy.
By virtue of the job that a parent holds and the nature of the specific work-
place in which that job is carried out, mothers and fathers are exposed to
certain opportunities and constraints with regard to their parenting role. In
his review of the “determinants of parenting,” Belsky (1984) conceptual-
ized the influence of work on parenting in terms of stress and support. Al-
though stress and support are important, a full consideration of the place
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 281
. . . for 5 years these women lived much of their lives together in that room.
Their menial jobs did not deaden their vibrant personalities, and in their con-
versations with one another one can see a clear reflection of the totality of
their lives. When I visit with these women through their conversations, I get to
participate in their constant dramas, their continual flow of humor, their sto-
ries, their worries, their frivolity, their omnipresent planning for the next
grand event, and above all, their intimacy . . . they talk in detail about their
family members, especially if one of them is in any kind of difficulty. Here
they reveal themselves not only as kin keepers but also as their coworkers’ kin
keepers. (pp. 160–161)
worker support was a significant correlate of role strain for married fathers
and single mothers, although not for married mothers: the greater the per-
ceived co-worker support, the less role strain married fathers and single
mothers reported. Supervisor support was not associated with parents’ re-
ports of role strain in the Greenberger et al. study, although Repetti (1987)
found that supervisor support mitigated depression in female bank tellers
who experienced stressful work conditions. Future research is needed to
examine whether co-worker support and supervisor support are linked to
mothers’ and fathers’ actual parenting behavior.
Such research will have to grapple with the ubiquitous selection effects
discussed earlier. Socially competent people may develop and engage in
constructive relationships at work, and they may also develop and engage in
constructive parent–child relationships. Finding a correlation between the
two does not imply that workplace relationships cause effective parenting.
This is another situation in which a workplace intervention, such as training
supervisors to be supportive of workers’ family roles, might be evaluated
not only in terms of whether or not it reduces absenteeism and turnover or
increased job satisfaction and productivity—traditional “outcomes” of in-
terest to the workplace—but also in terms of whether it enhances employ-
ees’ parenting.
cies, assistance with child care) more than fathers did and that, for single
mothers, use of formal benefits was related to reduced role strain, even con-
trolling for other sources of support at work.
Hyde and colleagues (Hyde, 1995; Hyde, Essex, Clark, Klein, & Byrd,
1996) have conducted research on maternity leave and its implications for
mothers themselves as well as for the quality of their interactions with their
children. Clark, Hyde, Essex, and Klein (1997), for example, compared the
quality of mother–infant interaction when babies were about 4 months of
age for mothers who had taken a maternity leave of about 6 weeks (short) vs.
a leave of about 12 weeks (long). In general, mothers who experienced a
short leave evidenced more negative affect and behavior than other moth-
ers. Clark et al. (1997), however, also reported interaction effects. Mothers
who experienced a short leave tended to interact with their offspring with
less positive affect, responsiveness, and sensitivity if they also reported
higher levels of depressive symptoms or perceived their baby as having a
more difficult temperament.
Studying the effects of formal workplace policies should be relatively
straightforward, but it is not. In some organizations, there is a discrepancy
between the policies that are “on the books” and the behaviors that are in-
formally encouraged or discouraged in the organization’s culture: during
lunch breaks, around the water cooler, and in hallway interactions. Paren-
tal leave may be technically available to employees, for example, but there
may be strong informal discouragement of people actually taking that
leave, discouragement that is hard to measure in surveys but may be detect-
able in qualitative research (see, for example, Fried, 1998). Likewise, long,
inflexible work hours may not be a formal requirement of some organiza-
tions, but there may be subtle messages about the importance of “face
time,” messages that convey the difficulties of getting ahead in the organiza-
tion if the employee does not put in long hours that are visible to co-
workers and supervisors or go the extra mile to work on weekends and eve-
nings.
Further complicating matters, the organization or larger cultural con-
text may send different messages to men and women about taking advan-
tage of workplace policies and benefits. Haas (1992) documents, for exam-
ple, that, although both men and women are permitted by Swedish social
policy to take a generous leave at the birth of a child, men are often infor-
mally discouraged from taking the time to which they are entitled. Thus, a
careful examination of whether family friendly benefits and practices do in
fact support parents in their childrearing roles would have to carefully as-
sess not only the availability of the specific policies and practices but also
the extent to which male and female workers are informally encouraged or
discouraged from actually using them.
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 285
ever, their moods during these tasks were more positive than was the case for
their wives when they performed the same activities. Larson and Richards
(1994) proposed that housework and child care elicit a more positive reac-
tion from husbands than wives because it is seen as voluntary work by hus-
bands. Men get involved when they are in the mood, and their efforts are
noted and appreciated by wives, but wives are expected to handle these tasks
whether or not they are in the mood to do them.
Much research on emotional transmission across the work–family inter-
face has focused on stress. In a pioneering study, for example, Bolger et al.
(1989) found an increased probability of arguments with spouse (but not
children) after days in which the husband had had arguments at work. For
wives, in contrast, there was no association between arguments at work and
arguments with husbands (or children). Repetti (1989) examined the con-
nections between work-induced stress and parents’ behavior at home using
a somewhat different strategy. She zeroed in on air traffic controllers, an oc-
cupation that is notoriously stressful, to take advantage of daily FAA records
of work conditions and demands. Twenty seven air traffic controllers, all
parents, completed daily reports on work stress and parent–child interac-
tions on three consecutive days. Repetti found that parents tended to be
more socially withdrawn and less emotionally expressive on days when work
had been stressful.
To follow up Repetti’s findings about parental withdrawal following
stressful work days, Repetti and Wood (1997) collected mood data at the
end of mothers’ work shifts and self-report and observational parenting
data during subsequent mother–child reunions at work-site child care cen-
ters. Consistent with their expectations, they found that mothers of pre-
schoolers were more likely to withdraw from their children, both emotion-
ally and behaviorally, on days during which they had experienced either
overloads or negative interpersonal interactions at work. Interestingly,
stress on the job generally was not followed by aversive mother–child inter-
action. Indeed, Repetti and Wood found some evidence in the observa-
tional component of their study that mothers were somewhat more patient
with their children after high stress work days, a pattern they interpreted as
part of the package of emotional and behavioral withdrawal. The extent to
which mothers were able to refrain from engaging in negative interaction
depended, however, on the mother’s own general level of psychosocial
functioning. Mothers characterized as high on Type A behavior were less
able to refrain from interacting negatively with their children than were
mothers with low scores on that measure. This finding is a good reminder
that the effects of work conditions on parents are not uniform but undoubt-
edly depend on the individual strengths and vulnerabilities that parents
bring to their work and family situations (Perry-Jenkins, Repetti, & Crouter,
2000).
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 287
The second way researchers have examined the opportunities and con-
straints that working outside the home presents to parents pays little atten-
tion to the nature of the work that parents perform or to the setting in
which that work is done but focuses instead on variations in family lifestyles
as a function of parents’ work-related choices. The first generation of stud-
ies in this area focused on the implications for two-parent families of having
one or two parents working outside the home by comparing family proc-
esses or child outcomes in single-earner versus dual-earner families (see re-
view by Bronfenbrenner & Crouter, 1982). Early studies in this tradition
were often explicitly or implicitly designed to illuminate the potentially
negative effects of maternal employment for parenting and, in turn, for
children. We will briefly touch on this literature.
In recent years, however, the focus of this genre has shifted rather dra-
matically. As more and more mothers have moved into the paid labor force,
with the concomitant trend that dual-earner families have become the most
common form of two-parent family (Hernandez, 1997), and as the dual-
288 CROUTER AND MCHALE
earner lifestyle has become more widely accepted, researchers have paid
less attention to maternal employment status and more attention to how
much time mothers and fathers are spending at work. Like the old litera-
ture on employment status, the concern underlying some of the research
on mothers’ and fathers’ work time is that long hours represent a risk factor
for children because they reduce parents’ temporal and (some worry) emo-
tional availability to their children. As will be seen, however, the empirical
evidence provides mixed support for these ideas.
If we look simply at how much time parents spend with their children, the
general picture is that, although there are minimal differences in involve-
ment for mothers as a function of their employment status, maternal em-
ployment has the effect of pulling fathers into a more active parenting role.
Demographer Bianchi (2000) examined children’s time with their par-
ents from 1981 to 1997 and concluded that there has been surprisingly little
change in the amount of time that mothers spend with their children across
that period. In addition, she summarized evidence from several national
studies showing that, after controlling for many of the variables that reflect
parents’ tendencies to select (or be selected) into one lifestyle or another,
employed mothers today spend only slightly less time with their offspring,
overall, than homemaker mothers do. The differences are modest. Bianchi
suggests that rather than taking time away from parenting, employed moth-
ers cut back “in other areas, reallocating priorities to protect time with
children” (Bianchi, 2000, p. 406). Specifically, she notes that, during the
historical period she studied, employed mothers reduced time spent in
housework, volunteer work, personal care, leisure time, and even sleep!
Given that there are only small differences in maternal involvement with
children as a function of earner status, it is perhaps not surprising that
there is little consistent evidence that maternal employment has positive or
negative implications for children’s development. In an analysis of data
from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, Parcel and Menaghan
(1994) reported that, holding a variety of possible confounding variables
constant, mothers’ work hours have few consistent, direct effects on young
children’s cognitive and social outcomes, a conclusion underscored by
Harvey’s (1999) analyses of the same data set. In her review of the litera-
ture, Bianchi (2000) noted bits of evidence here and there that maternal
employment may have some negative effects, for example in the first year of
life or for children in certain family situations, but she underscored the
paucity of significant outcomes for children:
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 289
. . . given the effort that has been devoted to searching for negative effects of
maternal employment on children’s academic achievement and emotional
adjustment, coupled with the scarcity of findings (either positive or negative),
it would appear that the dramatic movement into the labor force by women of
childbearing age in the United States has been accomplished with relatively
little consequence for children. (p. 401)
whereabouts, and companions. Crouter et al. (1999) reported that, not sur-
prisingly, mothers of school-aged children are more knowledgeable about
their children’s daily lives than fathers are. They also found that, although
mothers did not differ in knowledge as a function of their level of involve-
ment in paid work, fathers married to women who worked longer hours
were more knowledgeable than their counterparts whose wives worked few
hours.
Fathers’ tendencies to respond to their wives’ longer work hours with
greater parental involvement and parental knowledge is suggested in a
short-term longitudinal study conducted by Crouter and McHale (1993b)
that capitalized on seasonal variability in mothers’ work involvement. Using
data on time use and daily parental knowledge collected during a series of
evening telephone interviews with parents and children, they compared
mothers and fathers at three time points: the school year, the following
summer when children were not in school, and the following school year.
They divided families into three groups on the basis of stability or change in
mothers’ work hours (all fathers were employed full-time). The three
groups were (a) consistently dual-earner (both parents employed at all
three time points); (b) consistently single-earner (fathers were consistently
employed outside the home and their wives were employed very little or not
at all); and (c) a group in which parents both worked during the school
year but mothers cut their work hours way back or ceased working at all
during the summer months. In comparison to the first two groups which
were marked by stability in parents’ work time the third group of families
was characterized by substantial change from the winter to the summer in
parenting processes. The division of parenting between mothers and fa-
thers became significantly more traditional in the summer and then re-
turned to a more egalitarian arrangement during the subsequent school
year. In addition, although the fathers in the consistently dual-earner
group maintained a relatively high level of knowledge about their chil-
dren’s daily activities throughout all three occasions of measurement, fa-
thers in the group in which mothers cut way back in work involvement in
the summer dropped substantially in their levels of knowledge in the sum-
mer months and recovered again in the subsequent school year when
mothers had returned to their prior levels of involvement in paid work.
1998, 2001), so presumably there are more examples at the high end of fa-
thers putting in extraordinarily long work weeks than of mothers doing so,
but, with a few exceptions, paternal long hours have generally not been
conceptualized as a social issue worthy of concern.
Parcel and Menaghan (1994) reported that, controlling for a variety of
background and individual characteristics and for baseline levels of the
quality of the home environment, the quality of children’s home environ-
ments deteriorated over a two-year period when fathers worked part-time.
This finding may reflect part-time employed men’s tenuous position in the
labor force. Parcel and Menaghan (1994) also underscored the importance
of looking at the combination of fathers’ and mothers’ work hours, noting
that when both parents held part-time work schedules they provided their
children with lower quality home environments than was the case when
only one parent worked part-time. Likewise, they found that the combina-
tion of two parents working overtime was associated with higher levels of
problem behavior in children and lower levels of verbal competence (i.e.,
vocabulary) than was the case when only one parent worked overtime. In
households in which both parents are putting in long hours, it may be
harder to maintain consistent limit-setting, interact positively and contin-
gently with children, and find time for conversations, story telling, songs,
and other forms of verbal stimulation.
Crouter et al. (1999) found no connection between fathers’ work hours
and either fathers’ or mothers’ knowledge of their children’s daily experi-
ences. This may be due to the fact that fathers have a variety of different
sources of information about their children’s daily lives. Indeed, Bumpus
and Crouter (2003) developed a typology of fathers based on their sources
of knowledge about their children’s daily experiences. One group of fa-
thers was characterized by high levels of reliance on wives and other chil-
dren in the family. Interestingly, these fathers tended to work long hours
and to have close marital relationships. Mothers and fathers in two-parent
families may catch their partners up on child-oriented events, another way
in which children are buffered from the effects of long work hours. Under
some conditions, however, long paternal work hours may erode the quality
of father–child relationships. Crouter, Bumpus, Head, and McHale (2001)
examined the connections among fathers’ overwork (defined as paid em-
ployment of more than 60 hours a week), overload (e.g., fathers’ subjective
perceptions of being overwhelmed by having too much to do), and fathers’
and adolescents’ reports of the quality of their relationship. They found no
associations between overwork or overload and the sheer amount of time
fathers spent with their adolescent offspring in daily activities, data gath-
ered through a series of seven evening telephone interviews about time use.
In contrast, the combination of long paternal work hours and high paternal
overload was consistently related to both fathers’ and adolescents’ subjec-
292 CROUTER AND MCHALE
My one concern is that I have given the impression that women have found it
quite easy to balance increased labor force participation with child rearing, to
reduce hours of employment so as to juggle childcare, and to get their hus-
bands more involved in child rearing; and that fathers have found it easy to
add more hours with children to those they already commit to supporting
children financially. I do not think these changes have been easy for Ameri-
can families, particularly for American women. (p. 412)
A century may seem a long time, but it is a short time to alter the basic struc-
ture of an institution. The new egalitarian system of sex roles still lacks norma-
tive guidelines. Each couple has to work out its own arrangement which
means in practice a great deal of experimentation and failure. (p. 413)
CONCLUSIONS
For dual-earner families, mother’s job and father’s job constitute an impor-
tant exosystem for the developing child. Scholars interested in the interre-
lationship between work and parenting have taken quite different ap-
proaches depending upon whether their focus is that of work, and the
roles, activities, and relationships associated with employment, or family
and the parenting dynamics associated with that context. Work influences
childrearing via its effects on parents’ views of the world, the opportunities
and constraints jobs pose for parents who need to balance multiple roles,
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 293
and the daily stresses (and exhilarations) of the work day that shape par-
ents’ emotional states as they leave their workplaces to resume their parent-
ing roles. All three levels are important; the next challenge is to design re-
search that bridges two or more. A second general approach researchers
have taken to examining work and family life has been to focus primarily on
whether and how family processes, such as parent–child involvement, pa-
rental knowledge, and parent–child closeness, differ in families that vary in
terms of parents’ connections to the paid labor force. Both perspectives are
needed, alone and in tandem, to inform the next generation of research in
this area.
A challenge for the future is to design studies that look in detail at the
implications for parenting and parent–child relationships of the combina-
tion of mothers’ and fathers’ work situations. This means taking a dyadic
approach to the study of work and parenting in two-parent families (see
Crouter & Helms-Erikson, 1997). This can be done in at least two ways. A
variable-oriented approach is to create interaction terms that combine vari-
ables reflecting “his and her” occupational conditions (e.g., Parcel & Men-
aghan, 1994). A pattern-analytic approach is to create typologies based on
the patterning of mothers’ and fathers’ work characteristics (e.g., Bumpus,
Crouter, & McHale, 1999; Crouter & Manke, 1997), using a statistical pro-
cedure like cluster analysis. Once interesting family groups have been iden-
tified and described, they can be examined in relation to parenting and
parent–child relationships. Dyadic approaches will get us closer to a more
holistic understanding of the diverse ecologies that are included under the
rubric of dual-earner families—and hence to a more detailed, nuanced un-
derstanding of the work and family contexts of child development.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful for the many insights provided by our current and former
graduate students, Tom Luster, Lynn Okagaki, and Marc Bornstein, as well
as for sustained funding for our work from the National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development.
REFERENCES
Baron, J. N., & Bielby, W. T. (1985). Organizational barriers to gender equality: Sex segrega-
tion of jobs and occupations. In A. S. Rossi (Ed.), Gender and the life course (pp. 233–251).
New York: Aldine.
Barry, H., Child, I. L., & Bacon, M. K. (1959). Relation of child training to subsistence econ-
omy. American Anthropologist, 61, 51–63.
294 CROUTER AND MCHALE
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting. A process model. Child Development, 55(1),
83–96.
Bianchi, S. M. (2000). Maternal employment and time with children: Dramatic change or sur-
prising continuity? Demography, 37, 401–414.
Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C., & Wethington, E. (1989). The contagion of stress
across multiple roles. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 175–183.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1982). Work and family through time and space. In S.
Kamerman & C. Hayes (Eds.), Families that work: Children in a changing world (pp. 39–83).
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Bumpus, M. F., & Crouter, A. C. (2003, April). Fathers’ strategies for acquiring knowledge
about their adolescents’ daily lives. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society
for Research in Child Development, Tampa Florida.
Bumpus, M. F., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (1999). Work demands of dual-earner couples:
Implications for parents’ knowledge about children’s daily lives in middle childhood. Jour-
nal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 465–475.
Clark, R., Hyde, J., Essex, M. J., & Klein, M. H. (1997). Length of maternity leave and quality of
mother–infant interactions. Child Development, 68, 364–383.
Coltrane, S. (1996). Family man. New York: Oxford University Press.
Crouter, A. C. (1984). Participative work as an influence on human development. Journal of Ap-
plied Developmental Psychology, 5, 71–90.
Crouter A. C., & Bumpus, M. F. (2001). Linking parents’ work stress to children’s and adoles-
cents’ psychological adjustment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 156–159.
Crouter, A. C., Bumpus, M. F., Head, M. R., & McHale, S. M. (2001). Implications of overwork
and overload for the quality of men’s family relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63,
404–416.
Crouter, A. C., Bumpus, M. F., Maguire, M. C., & McHale, S. M. (1999). Linking parents’ work
pressure to adolescents’ well-being: Insights into dynamics in dual-earner families. Develop-
mental Psychology, 35, 1453–1461.
Crouter, A. C., & Head, M. R. (2002). Parental monitoring and knowledge of children. In
M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed.,
pp. 461–483). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Crouter, A. C., & Helms-Erikson, H. (1997). Work and family from a dyadic perspective: Varia-
tions in inequality. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships. New York: John
Wiley.
Crouter, A. C., Helms-Erikson, H., Updegraff, K., & McHale, S. M. (1999). Conditions underly-
ing parents’ knowledge about children’s daily lives in middle childhood: Between- and
within-family comparisons. Child Development, 70, 246–259.
Crouter, A. C., & Manke, B. (1997). Development of a typology of dual-earner families: A win-
dow into differences between and within families. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 62–75.
Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (1993a). The long arm of the job: Influences of parental work
on childrearing. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (1993b). Temporal rhythms in family life: Seasonal variation
in the relation between parental work and family process. Developmental Psychology, 29,
198–205.
Davis, K. (1984). Wives and work: The sex role revolution and its consequences. Population and
Development Review, 10, 397–417.
Fried, M. (1998). Taking time: Parental leave policy and corporate culture. Pennsylvania: Temple
University Press.
Friedman, D. E. (1990). Corporate responses to family needs. In D. G. Unger & M. B. Sussman
(Eds.), Families in community settings: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 77–98). New York:
Haworth.
9. LONG ARM OF THE JOB 295
Gottfried, A. E., Gottfried, A. W., & Bathurst, K. (2002). Maternal and dual-earner employ-
ment status and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology
and ecology of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 207–229). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenberger, E., Goldberg, W. A., Hamill, S., O’Neil, R., & Payne, C. K. (1989). Contributions
of a supportive work environment to parents’ well-being and orientation to work. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 17, 755–783.
Gruber-Baldini, A. L., Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (1995). Similarity in married couples: A lon-
gitudinal study of mental abilities and rigidity flexibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 69, 191–203.
Haas, L. (1992). Equal parenthood and social policy. New York: State University of New York Press.
Harvey, E. (1999). Short-term and long-term effects of early parental employment on children
of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Developmental Psychology, 35, 445–459.
Hernandez, D. J. (1997). Child development and the social demography of childhood. Child
Development, 68, 149–169.
Hyde, J. S. (1995). Women and maternity leave: Empirical data and public policy. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 19, 299–313.
Hyde, J. S., Essex, M. J., Clark, R., Klein, M. H., & Byrd, J. E. (1996). Parental leave: Policy and
research. Journal of Social Issues, 52, 91–110.
Jacobs, J. A., & Gerson, K. (1998). Who are the overworked Americans? Review of Social Econ-
omy, 56, 442–459.
Jacobs, J. A., & Gerson, K. (2001). Overworked individuals or overworked families? Explaining
trends in work, leisure, and family time. Work and Occupations, 28, 40–63.
Kanter, R. M. (1977). Work and family in the United States: A critical review and agenda for research
and policy. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of ado-
lescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental
Psychology, 36, 366–380.
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2003). Parenting of adolescents: Action or reaction? In A. C. Crouter &
A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships
(pp. 121–151). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kline, M., & Cowan, P. A. (1989). Rethinking the connections among “work” and “family” and
“well-being.” Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 3, 61–90.
Kohn, M. L. (1977). Class and conformity: A study in values (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chi-
cago Press.
Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality: An inquiry into the impact of social strati-
fication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Komarovsky, M. (1962). Blue collar marriage. New York: Random House.
Larson, R. W., & Almeida, D. M. (1999). Emotional transmission in the daily lives of families: A
new paradigm for studying family process. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 5–20.
Larson, R., & Richards, M. (1994). Divergent realities: The emotional lives of mothers, fathers, and ad-
olescents. New York: Basic Books.
Luster, T., & Okagaki, L. (Eds.). (1993). Parenting: An ecological perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Luster, T., Rhoades, K., & Haas, B. (1989). Relations between parenting values and parenting
behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 139–147.
Lynd, R. S., & Lynd, H. M. (1929). Middletown: A study in modern American culture. New York:
Harcourt, Brace & World.
Marks, S. R. (1994). Studying workplace intimacy: Havens at work. In D. L. Sollie & L. A. Leslie
(Eds.), Gender, families, and close relationships: Feminist research journeys (pp. 145–168). Cali-
fornia: Sage.
Menaghan, E. G. (1991). Work experiences and family interaction processes: The long reach
of the job? Annual Review of Sociology, 17, 419–444.
296 CROUTER AND MCHALE
Menaghan, E. G., & Parcel, T. L. (1995). Social sources of change in children’s home environ-
ments: The effects of parental occupational experiences and family conditions. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 57, 69–84.
Miller, D. D., & Swanson, G. E. (1958). The changing American parent: A study in the Detroit area.
New York: Wiley.
Ogbu, J. U. (1981). Origins of human competence: A cultural-ecological perspective. Child De-
velopment, 52, 413–429.
Parcel, T. L., & Menaghan, E. G. (1994). Parents’ jobs and children’s lives. New York: Aldine De
Gruyter.
Perry-Jenkins, M., Repetti, R. L., & Crouter, A. C. (2000). Work and family in the 1990s. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 62, 981–998.
Repetti, R. L. (1987). Individual and common components of the social environment at work
and psychology well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 710–720.
Repetti, R. L. (1989). Effects of daily workload on subsequent behavior during marital interac-
tion: The roles of social withdrawal and spouse support. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 57, 651–659.
Repetti, R. L., & Wood, J. (1997). Effects of daily stress at work on mothers’ interactions with
preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 90–108.
Roethlisberger, F., & Dickson, W. J. (1939). Management and the worker. Cambridge, MA: Har-
vard University Press.
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71,
1072–1085.
Steinberg, L., Elmen, J. D., & Mounts, N. S. (1989). Authoritative parenting, psychosocial ma-
turity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development, 60, 1425.
Waller, W. W. (1938). The family: A dynamic interpretation. New York: Cordon.
10
James Garbarino
Cornell University
Catherine P. Bradshaw
Johns Hopkins University
Kathleen Kostelny
Erikson Institute for Advanced Study in Child Development
INTRODUCTION
Families are, to a certain degree, “open systems,” and are thus influenced
by the larger social context (Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Furstenberg, Cook,
Eccles, Elder, & Sameroff, 1999; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Minu-
chin, 1974; Stinnett, Chesser, & DeFrain, 1979). Families and environ-
ments continually negotiate and re-negotiate their relationships, each in-
fluencing, changing, and depending on the other. Although the effects
are usually neither simple nor direct, there is a constant shifting and
evolving interplay among the child’s biology, the environment, and the
parents’ behavior. Consequently, the success of parents to a great extent
depends on the difficulties posed by the children and the degree to which
the social environment is toxic or nurturing to both children and their
parents (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000;
Garbarino, 1995). This interaction is particularly important to consider
when examining how neighborhoods and communities influence parent-
ing behaviors in modern societies.
In this chapter, we examine several aspects of neighborhoods and com-
munities that have the potential to influence parents and the children they
rear. We review some of the consequences of living in high-risk environ-
ments for both children and parents. A crucial indicator of successful
parenting and family functioning is the absence of child maltreatment;
297
298 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
On the individual level, we know that one of the most important functions
of parents is to provide stability and security. Parents are or should be a se-
cure emotional base from which their children and adolescents explore the
environment (Allen & Land, 1999). Similarly, parents serve as emotional
and social buffers by creating boundaries for their children and deciding
how permeable the boundaries will be to the flow of information, energy,
and people in and out of the family system (Garbarino, 1995). Although
parents play a significant role in promoting healthy development, socially
toxic forces present in many American communities are often working
against the goals of parents and impeding positive youth development.
40% or more) of parents falling below the poverty level (Wilson, 1996).
Studies indicate that child abuse and infant mortality rates are significantly
higher in poverty stricken neighborhoods than in unafflicted communities.
In Chicago, for example, the rates of child abuse in the poorest neighbor-
hoods were four times higher than in the more affluent areas, and for in-
fant mortality, the rate was higher by a factor of five (Garbarino, Dubrow,
Kostelny, & Pardo, 1992). In the following section, we discuss research on
child victimization and the numerous challenges posed to parents who live
in high-risk communities as they struggle to keep their children physically
safe while promoting positive youth development.
that are higher or lower than would be expected in that community (given
its socioeconomic and demographic composition). For example, consider
two communities which have the same actual child maltreatment rates.
One community might be labeled high-risk because its rate exceeds what it
should be given its socioeconomic and demographic characteristics. In con-
trast, the other would be labeled low-risk because the rate is lower than ex-
pected, given its socioeconomic and demographic profile.
Building on this model, Garbarino and Kostelny (1992) examined com-
munity level factors as possible explanations for different patterns of infant
mortality in Chicago’s 77 communities during the mid-1980s. They pre-
dicted infant mortality rates for each community by computing a multiple
regression analysis with nine socioeconomic and demographic variables,
and then compared the predicted rates with the actual rates. Not surpris-
ingly, there was a strong correlation between economically impoverished
communities and high rates of infant mortality. However, closer examina-
tion of the data indicated more complex associations between other com-
munity level variables and infant mortality. Rates of infant mortality in 4 of
the 77 communities were not adequately explained by either socioeco-
nomic and demographic variables or the additional factors of low birth
weight and births to teens. In these neighborhoods, idiosyncratic commu-
nity factors, such as participation in the community’s prenatal class, parent-
ing education and support programs were associated with lower rates of in-
fant mortality, whereas barriers to medical care, such as a hospital closure,
were associated with higher rates.
This approach of comparing actual risks with expected risks illustrates
how institutional policies and the concentration of poverty can have a nega-
tive influence on parents and childbearing. Conditions within poor com-
munities (e.g., hospital closings) can exacerbate the problems faced by par-
ents and increase the risk for children. In contrast, programs (e.g.,
community prenatal classes) for parents living in some impoverished com-
munities have the potential to strengthen and support families, thereby re-
ducing the actual risk of infant mortality. Comparing communities with
similar predicted rates of risk factors, but different actual rates can provide
useful insights for the development of intervention strategies in high-risk
neighborhoods (Garbarino & Kostelny, 1992).
tims. Approximately one quarter of all juvenile murders in the United States
occur in only five out of the nearly 3,000 U.S. counties—these five counties
include the cities of Los Angeles, Chicago, New York, Philadelphia, and De-
troit (Sickmund et al., 1997). Poor, inner-city, minority neighborhoods are
the sites for most of these murders and serious assaults. For example, the ho-
micide rate in Chicago hovers around 22 per 100,000 and 17 for Los Angeles,
when the national rate is only about 5.5 (Federal Bureau of Investigation,
n.d.). Consequently, some children and their parents face environmental vi-
olence on a weekly, if not daily, basis. One study of children living in Chicago
in the inner-city communities indicated that by age 17, 30% of the children
had witnessed a homicide (Bell & Jenkins, 1991).
It is clear that children are sensitive and responsive to stressors in their
environments; however, what parents do and say to their children can me-
diate these influences (Garbarino et al., 1992; Furstenberg et al., 1999). To
a large degree, the effect of stressful neighborhoods and communities on
children is influenced by the impact of that stress on the parents, most no-
tably on mothers. Children in the care of their parents or familiar parent
substitutes can cope with stressful life circumstances if parents are able to
maintain their day-to-day routines, project high morale, and continue to be
responsive to their children’s basic physical and emotional needs (Richters
& Martinez, 1993).
has shown that overly restrictive and controlling parenting practices are as-
sociated with increased behavior problems and greater affiliation with devi-
ant peers among adolescents and pre-adolescents (Brody et al., 2001). This
finding is particularly important, since several studies have shown that asso-
ciation with deviant peers is major risk factor for aggressive and delinquent
behavior during adolescence (for a review see Thornberry & Krohn, 1997).
It also illustrates the transactional nature of parenting, whereby ineffective
parenting behavior leaves an open door to the influences of deviant peers
(Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989).
An overly restrictive parenting style also likely heightens aggression on
the child’s part and models aggressive responses to threat. More restrictive
parenting practices do appear to be somewhat effective in terms of reduc-
ing delinquency for younger African American children, but only when
coupled with high levels of maternal warmth (Dunifon & Kowalski-Jones,
2002; Mason, Cauce, Gonzales, Hiraga, & Grove, 1994). Although these ef-
fects do not appear to hold for European American children, they remind
us that the emotional context of any parenting behavior is extremely im-
portant (Steinberg, 2001).
In these examples, the parental adaptation was likely well-intentioned
and may have appeared to be practically sensible, but its side effects may be
detrimental in the long run. Attempts to shield the child from negative
forces by punitive restrictiveness is generally much less successful as a strat-
egy than expressing confidence in the child’s capacities and promoting
positive alternatives to the negative subculture feared by the parent
(Scheinfeld, 1983). In the following section, we consider some of the other
more successful strategies that parents use to mediate the negative influ-
ences of high-risk communities.
The $70 million dollar MTO program is modeled after the smaller
Gautreaux Program, in which African American families residing in public
housing in inner-city Chicago were provided with a voucher to move to sub-
sidized suburban or urban housing (Orr et al., 2003; Shroder, 2001). The
families who moved to the suburbs demonstrated numerous gains in em-
ployment and education. However, the program was not truly experimental
(with random assignment to conditions), and thus it cannot be determined
whether the changes resulted from the relocation or other factors (e.g.,
self-selection bias). To examine more specifically the influence of residen-
tial mobility on parents and children, HUD developed the MTO program
and utilized a controlled experimental design (Shroder, 2001).
Beginning in 1994, the MTO program identified 4,608 eligible families
living in neighborhood with poverty rates of 40% or higher across five large
cities: Baltimore, Boston, Chicago, Los Angeles, and New York (Orr et al.,
2003). Approximately half of the sample consisted of African American
families and another 39% were Hispanic. An average of 2.5 children under
age 18 resided in each household, and the mean family income was just un-
der $9,400; 75% of families received assistance through Aid to Families with
Dependent Children (AFDC) or Temporary Assistances for Needy Families
(TANF; Shroder, 2001).
The families were randomly assigned into one of three groups. Partici-
pants in the experimental group were given a “Section 8” rental voucher to
move to private-market rental housing and were provided counseling assis-
tance in finding a new residence and working with prospective landlords.
Their relocations were restricted, such that families were required to move
into a community where no more than 10% of the population was below
the poverty line. The families in the Section 8 only group also received a
rental voucher for private-market housing, but there were no relocation re-
strictions, nor were residential counseling services provided. Families in the
comparison group continued residing in project-based housing (Orr et al.,
2003). Approximately 48% of the experimental group (i.e., Section 8
voucher plus residential counseling) and 60% of the Section 8 only group
“leased-up” through the use of the voucher system (Shroder, 2001). It is not
clear why some families did not lease-up; some may have had a difficult time
finding housing in low-poverty neighborhoods, whereas others may have
simply chosen not to move.
A longitudinal study of the MTO program is currently underway to ex-
amine the several aspects of mobility including, mental and physical health,
educational achievement, delinquency and crime, employment, and earn-
ings. The families in the experimental and Section 8 only groups who
leased-up, moved to significantly better neighborhoods, as indicated by
higher proportions of employed adults, high school graduates, and home-
owners. The movers in the two voucher groups also reported greater satis-
308 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
faction with the new neighborhood, greater perceptions of safety, less expo-
sure to neighborhood disorder (e.g., litter, graffiti, public drinking), and
reduced likelihood of witnessing or being victimized by violence in the
neighborhood (Orr et al., 2003).
With regard to health outcomes, there was a reduced incidence of obe-
sity among the parents in the experimental and Section 8 only groups, as
well as reductions in psychological distress and depression for the parents
in the experimental group. For the children in the two voucher groups, in
contrast to the comparison group, there were no significant differences in
the educational performance measures. The girls in the two voucher
groups reported reductions in mental health problems (e.g., psychological
distress, depression, anxiety), whereas the findings regarding the children’s
behavior problems (e.g., delinquency, risky behavior) were mixed. There is
some indication that the effects of the move through the MTO voucher
program yielded positive effects for girls, but some negative effects for boys
(Orr et al., 2003).
Overall, the short-term findings from the first four years of the MTO
demonstration are promising, despite the fact that only about half of the
families in the experimental and Section 8 only groups actually moved
(Shroder, 2001). Even with this limitation, the effects observed for families
in the two voucher groups are positive, with those in the experimental
group demonstrating the greatest improvements. And it is likely that the
impacts will become stronger with time. These and other studies on neigh-
borhood effects illustrate the role context can play in promoting healthy
behaviors among parents and positive development among youth. In the
following section, we highlight some of the other ways in which communi-
ties can support parents and contribute to the well-being of children.
TABLE 10.1
Percent of Youth Demonstrating High-Risk
Behavior Grouped by Number of Assets
Number of Assets
Alcohol Abuse 53 30 11 3
Tobacco Use 45 21 6 1
Illicit Drug Use 42 19 6 1
Sexual Intercourse 33 21 10 3
Depression/Suicide 40 25 13 4
Antisocial Behavior 52 23 7 1
Violence 61 35 16 6
School Problems 43 19 7 2
Drinking and Driving 42 24 10 4
Source: Scales, P., & Leffert, N. (1999). Developmental assets: A synthesis of the scientific research
on adolescent development. Minneapolis, MN: Search Institute, p. 8. (N = 99,462 U.S. students.)
310 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
of the 40 assets are under the direct control of parents (e.g., “parent(s) are
actively involved in helping young person succeed in school”; Scales &
Leffert, 1999, p. 2). This means that the other 30 assets are primarily char-
acteristics of the school or community (e.g., “school provides a caring, en-
couraging environment” and “young person experiences caring neigh-
bors”; Scales & Leffert, 1999, p. 2). Assets found in contexts outside the
family may complement those provided by the parents, and for some chil-
dren, may help compensate for what parents cannot provide.
Quite a bit can be done to support children outside the actions of par-
ents. Parents and the parent-oriented assets can benefit from social support
provided by the larger community. Convergent findings from several stud-
ies of life course responses to stressful early experiences suggest a series of
ameliorating factors that lead to prosocial and healthy adaptation (Losel &
Bliesener, 1990):
school readiness scores between children living in high and low poverty
communities (Pfannenstiel, Seitz, & Zigler, 2002).
For parenting to be successful in high-risk environments, parents must
overcome powerful negative forces that include the depressive effects of liv-
ing in chronic stress. Chicago’s Center for Successful Child Development
(“The Beethoven Project”) is an effort which prevents developmental de-
lays among an entire birth cohort in a public housing project (i.e., all the
children born in one year who live in the same kindergarten catchment
area; Barclay-McLaughlin, 1987; Center for Successful Child Development,
1998). The program provides several supportive services, such as home
health visits, early developmental screening, prenatal health care, parent
education, job training for parents, infant day care, child abuse prevention
programming, and Head Start programming (Barclay-McLaughlin, 1987).
When such efforts are conducted in the context of thoughtful evaluation
research, they can serve as the kind of transforming experiments that ad-
vance an ecologically valid science of parenting (Bronfenbrenner, 1979).
Community influences also are important in providing the open, sup-
portive educational climate, that is a source of resilience for children. Com-
munities can help set the tone and the context of individual parenting deci-
sions. For example, the growing campaign to end corporal punishment
(i.e., physical assault as discipline) in U.S. schools is driven by the belief that
this will help shape the context for disciplinary practices in the home. The
informal nature of the community and neighborhood are an important
source of resilience for children and, in a sense, are defined by the social
support from individuals outside the family.
Informal social support, either through emotional support (e.g., some-
one to confide in) or instrumental support (e.g., someone to depend on),
can be particularly important for parents, especially single parents. A study
of 262 low-SES, African American single mothers indicated that emotional
support was associated with greater warmth and more nurturing parenting
behaviors (Ceballo & McLoyd, 2002). But these effects varied by context,
such that within disordered neighborhoods, the association was weaker. Re-
ceipt of instrumental support by the mothers was associated with reductions
in the single mothers’ use of punitive parenting behaviors (e.g., yelling, hit-
ting, threatening), but this association also weakened as the neighborhood
conditions worsened (Ceballo & McLoyd, 2002). These findings suggest
that parents may benefit less from social and instrumental support in disor-
dered contexts. Furthermore, in these contexts, the costs of initiating or
maintaining social networks may outweigh the benefits. Some parents may
consciously withdraw—physically, emotionally, and socially—from the dis-
order present in their neighborhood as a way of protecting their children
(Ceballo & McLoyd, 2002).
312 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
and single-parenthood more and more families have only one potential
wage earner. This was not the case at the outset of the economic depression
of the 1930s, when most families with children contained two adults, and
wives represented a largely untapped resource that had the potential to be,
and in fact was, mobilized to generate cash income in response to the un-
employment and income loss experienced by male workers (Elder, 1974).
Now, this resource has already been tapped to meet basic family expenses,
and therefore does not represent a reserve in the same sense that it did in
the 1930s. Children are increasingly an economic burden, directly because
of what it costs to rear them and indirectly because of what they “cost” in
lost parental income (i.e., time away from the job that over a childhood
comes to tens of thousands if not hundreds of thousands of dollars; Gar-
barino & Bedard, 2001).
Economic issues play quite a large role in the dynamics of early risk, for
family structure and activities interact with the parents’ participation in the
workforce. To the degree to which the community’s day-to-day life is mone-
tarized, families will be drawn or driven into the cash economy. The great-
est risks come when families lack the financial resources to purchase sup-
port services in the market place and are cut off from the informal helping
relationships. This is seen most clearly in the urban underclass that has be-
come the focal point for emergency intervention. Marginal or submarginal
economic resources interact with diminished psychosocial resources born
of violence, academic failure, exploitation, despair, fear, and deteriorated
community infrastructure. In such environments most females experience
their first pregnancy while still an unmarried teenager, living with little
prospect of economic self-sufficiency or two-parent family status. Many of
these pregnancies result from sexual exploitations by much older men
(Barclay-McLaughlin, 1987). These are the environments in which prenatal
care is inadequate, intervals between births are often too brief, beliefs
about child care too often dysfunctional, access to and use of healthcare for
infants is inadequate, and early intervention for child disabilities insuffi-
cient. It is perhaps not surprising that it is also in these environments that
large numbers of child mortality and morbidity are rampant.
CONCLUSIONS
In the Middle Ages, only half of all children lived past age five. Now, child
death is relatively rare and the nature of parenting has changed. As stan-
dards and expectations for the care and life prospects of children have im-
proved in the last century, developmental risk has become a focal point for
research and policy. Thus, parental focus has shifted from sheer quantita-
tive concern with child survival to a qualitative concern with development;
314 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This chapter was supported in part by the Family Life Development Center
at Cornell University, the National Institute of Justice, and the National
Consortium on Violence Research. We thank Katrina Davy for helpful com-
ments on an earlier version of this chapter.
10. NEIGHBORHOOD AND COMMUNITY INFLUENCES ON PARENTING 315
REFERENCES
Allen, J. P., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 319–335). New York:
Guilford Press.
Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 62, 1269–1287.
Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment: In-
dividual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Jour-
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 16(5), 427–454.
Barclay-McLaughlin, G. (1987). The Center for Successful Child Development, The Ounce of
Prevention Fund, Chicago.
Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative control on child behavior. Child Development,
37(4), 887–907.
Bell, C. C., & Jenkins, E. J. (1991). Traumatic stress and children. Journal of Health Care for the
Poor and Underserved, 2(1), 175–185.
Bell, C. C., & Jenkins, E. J. (1993). Community violence and children on Chicago’s Southside.
Psychiatry, 56, 46–54.
Beyers, J. M., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2003). Neighborhood structure,
parenting processes, and the development of youths’ externalizing behaviors: A multilevel
analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31(1–2), 35–53.
Brody, G. H., Conger, R., Gibbons, F. X., Ge, X., McBride Murry, V., Gerrard, M., & Simons,
R. L. (2001). The influence of neighborhood disadvantage, collective socialization, and
parenting on African American children’s affiliation with deviant peers. Child Development,
72(4), 1231–1246.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development re-
search perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 6, 723–742.
Ceballo, R., & McLoyd, V. C. (2002). Social support and parenting in poor, dangerous neigh-
borhoods. Child Development, 73(4), 1310–1321.
Center for Successful Child Development (1998, March). Promising practices. Retrieved Janu-
ary 10, 2004, from http://www.financeprojectinfo.org/WIN/promising/
centerforsuccessfulchilddevelopment.htm
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000).
Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psycholo-
gist, 55(2), 218–232.
Collins, A., & Pancoast, D. (1976). Natural helping networks. Washington, DC: National Associa-
tion of Social Workers.
Danieli, Y. (1985). The treatment and prevention of long-term effects and intergenerational
transmission of victimization: A lesson from holocaust survivors and their children. In C. R.
Figley (Ed.), Trauma and its wake (pp. 295–313). New York: Bruner/Mazel.
Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1996). Physical discipline among
African American and European American mothers: Links to children’s externalizing be-
haviors. Developmental Psychology, 32(6), 1065–1072.
Dishion, T. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1998). Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and
adolescent problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and
Family Psychology Review, 1(1), 61–75.
Dubrow, N. F., & Garbarino, J. (1989). Living in the war zone: Mothers and young children in
public housing development. Journal of Child Welfare, 68, 3–20.
316 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
Dunifon, R., & Kowaleski-Jones, L. (2002). Who’s in the house? Race differences in cohabita-
tion, single parenthood, and child development. Child Development, 73(4), 1249–1264.
DuRant, R. H., Cadenhead, C., Pendergrast, R. A., Slavens, G., & Linder, C. W. (1994). Factors
associated with the use of violence among urban Black adolescents. American Journal of Pub-
lic Health, 84(4), 612–617.
Egeland, B., & Farber, E. (1984). Infant–mother attachment: Factors related to its develop-
ment and changes over time. Child Development, 55, 753–771.
Elder, G. H. (1974). Children of the great depression. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Erikson, K. (1976). Everything in its path: Destruction of community in the Buffalo Creekflood. New
York: Simon & Schuster.
Federal Bureau of Investigation. (n.d.). Offenses known to law enforcement by city 10,000 and over
in population, 2002. Retrieved February 24, 2003, from http://www.fbi.gov/ucr/02cius.htm
Fletcher, A. C., Darling, N. E., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. (1995). The company they keep:
Relation of adolescents’ adjustment and behavior to their friends’ perceptions of authori-
tative parenting in the social network. Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 300–310.
Freud, A., & Burlingham, D. (1943). War and children. New York: International Universities
Press.
Furstenberg, F. F., Cook, T., Eccles, J., Elder, G. H., & Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to make it:
Urban families and adolescent success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Garbarino, J. (1995). Raising children in a socially toxic environment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Garbarino, J., & Bedard, C. (2001). Parents under siege. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Garbarino, J., & Crouter, A. (1978a). Defining the community context of parent–child rela-
tions. Child Development, 49, 604–616.
Garbarino, J., & Crouter, A. (1978b). A note on assessing the construct validity of child mal-
treatment report data. American Journal of Public Health, 68, 598–599.
Garbarino, J., Dubrow, N., Kostelny, K., & Pardo, C. (1992). Children in danger: Coping with the
consequences of community violence. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Garbarino, J., & Kostelny, K. (1992). Child maltreatment as a community problem. Child Abuse
and Neglect, 16(4), 455–464.
Garbarino, J., & Sherman, D. (1980). High risk neighborhoods and high risk families: The hu-
man ecology of child maltreatment. Child Development, 51, 188–198.
Gelles, R., & Lancaster, J. (1987). Child abuse and neglect: Biosocial dimension. New York: Aldine
De Gruyter.
Groves, B. M. (2002). Children who see too much: Lessons from the Child Witness to Violence Project.
Boston: Beacon.
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Si-
mon & Schuster.
Ianni, F. A. (1989). The search for structure and the caring community, The search for structure:
A report on American youth today (pp. 260–283). New York: Free Press.
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of ado-
lescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental
Psychology, 36(3), 366–380.
Klebanov, P. K., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1994). Does neighborhood and family pov-
erty affect mothers’ parenting, mental health, and social support? Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 56(2), 441–455.
Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (2003). Change in parents’ monitoring
knowledge: Links with parenting, relationship quality, adolescent beliefs, and antisocial be-
havior. Social Development, 12(3), 401–419.
Lehmann, N. (1986). The origins of the underclass. Atlantic, 257, 31–61.
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neigh-
borhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2),
309–337.
10. NEIGHBORHOOD AND COMMUNITY INFLUENCES ON PARENTING 317
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2003). Moving to Opportunity: What about the kids? In J.
Goering & J. Feins (Eds.), Choosing a better life: Evaluating the moving to opportunity social exper-
iment. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute Press.
Losel, F., & Bliesener, T. (1990). Resilience in adolescence: A study on the generalizability of
protective factors. In K. Hurrelmann & F. Losel (Eds.), Health hazards in adolescence (pp.
299–320). New York: Walter de Gruyter.
Mason, C. A., Cauce, A. M., Gonzales, N., Hiraga, Y., & Grove, K. (1994). An ecological model
of externalizing behaviors in African-American adolescents: No family is an island. Journal
of Research on Adolescence, 4(4), 639–655.
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
National Clearinghouse on Child Abuse and Neglect Information. (2003). Child abuse and ne-
glect fatalities: Statistics and interventions. Retrieved February 24, 2004, from http://
nccanch.acf.hhs.gov/pubs/factsheets/fatality.cfm
Orr, L., Feins, J. D., Jacob, R., Beecroft, E., Sanbonmatsu, L., Katz, L. F., Liebman, J. B., &
Kling, J. R. (2003). Moving to Opportunity for fair housing demonstration program: Interim impacts
evaluation (Executive Summary). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Ur-
ban Development. Retrieved January 10, 2004, from http://www.huduser.org/publica-
tions/fairhsg/mtofinal.html
Osofsky, J. (1995). The effect of exposure to violence on young children. American Psychologist,
50(9), 782–788.
Osofsky, J. D. (Ed.). (1997). Children in a violent society. New York: Guilford Press.
Parents as Teachers National Center. (2003, December 12). Retrieved December 15, 2003,
from http://www.patnc.org
Paternoster, R. (1988). Examining three-wave deterrence models: A question of temporal or-
der and specification. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 79, 135–179.
Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective on anti-
social behavior. American Psychologist, 44(2), 329–335.
Patterson, G., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1984). The correlation of family management prac-
tices and delinquency. Child Development, 55, 1299–1307.
Pfannenstiel, J. C., Seitz, V., & Zigler, E. (2002). Promoting school readiness: The role of the
Parents as Teachers Program. NHSA Dialog: A Research-to-Practice Journal for the Early Interven-
tion Field, 6, 71–86.
Price, R. H., Cioci, M., Penner, W., & Trautlein, B. (1993). Webs of influence: School and com-
munity programs that enhance adolescent health and education. Teachers College Record, 94,
487–521.
Richters, J. E., & Martinez, P. E. (1993). Violent communities, family choices, and children’s
chances: An algorithm for improving the odds. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4),
609–627.
Rymond-Richmond, W. (2003, November). Perceptions of safety and danger in a public housing de-
velopment. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Society of Criminology,
Denver, CO.
Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Earls, F. (1999). Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of
collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review, 64(5), 633–660.
Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A
multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277(5328), 918–924.
Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Leffert, N., & Blyth, D. A. (2000). Contribution of developmental
assets to the prediction of thriving among adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 4(1),
27–46.
Scales, P., & Leffert, N. (1999). Developmental assets: A synthesis of the scientific research on adolescent
development. Minneapolis, MN: Search Institute.
Scheinfeld, D. (1983). Family relationships and school achievement among boys of lower-
income urban Black families. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 53(1), 127–143.
318 GARBARINO, BRADSHAW, KOSTELNY
Sedlak, A. J., & Broadhurst, D. D. (1996). Third national incidence study (NIS-3) of child abuse and
neglect. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Sickmund, M., Snyder, H. N., & Poe-Yamagata, E. (1997). Juvenile offenders and victims: 1997 Up-
date on violence. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
Shroder, M. (2001). Moving to opportunity: An experiment in social and geographic mobility.
In S. M. Wachter & R. L. Penne (Eds.), Housing policy in the new millennium: Proceedings (pp.
334–348). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development. Re-
trieved January 10, 2004, from http://www.huduser.org/intercept.asp?loc=/Publications/
pdf/brd/17Shroder.pdf
Snyder, H. N., & Sickmund, M. (1995). Juvenile offender and victims: A national report. Washing-
ton, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
Snyder, H. N., & Sickmund, M. (1999). Juvenile offenders and victims: 1999 national report. Wash-
ington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Relationships, self, and individual adaptation. In A. J. Sameroff & R. N.
Emde (Eds.), Relationship disturbances in early childhood: A developmental approach (pp.
70–96). New York: Basic Books.
Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things: Parent–adolescent relationships in retrospect and
prospect. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11(1), 1–19.
Steinberg, L., Catalano, R., & Dooley, D. (1981). Economic antecedents of child abuse and ne-
glect. Child Development, 52, 975–985.
Stinnett, N., Chesser, B., & DeFrain, J. (1979). Building family strengths: Blueprints for action. Lin-
coln: University of Nebraska Press.
Thornberry, T. P., & Krohn, M. D. (1997). Peers, drug use, and delinquency. In D. M. Stoff, J.
Breiling, & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 218–233). New York: John
Wiley and Sons.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2003). Maltreatment 2001 (Washington, DC:
U.S. Government Printing Office, 2003). Retrieved February 24, 2004, from www.acf.
hhs.gov/programs/cb/publications/cm01/outcover.htm
U.S. Department of Justice (2002). Homicide trends in the U.S.: Age trends. Retrieved February 24,
2004, from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/homicide/teens.htm
Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged: The inner city, the underclass, and public policy. Chi-
cago: The University of Chicago Press.
Wilson, W. J. (1991). Studying inner-city social dislocations: The challenge of public agenda
research. American Sociological Review, 56(1), 1–14.
Wilson, W. J. (1995). Jobless ghettos and the social outcome of youngsters. In P. Moen, G. H.
Elder, & K. Luscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human devel-
opment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wilson, W. J. (1996). When work disappears: The world of the new urban poor. New York: Random
House.
11
Socioeconomic Status,
Ethnicity, and Parenting
Birgit Leyendecker
Ruhr-University Bochum, Germany
Robin L. Harwood
Texas Tech University
Lisa Comparini
Clark University
Alev Yalç2nkaya
University of Connecticut
319
320 LEYENDECKER ET AL.
Measuring SES
empirical research the measures are often collapsed to create groups be-
yond or above a cut-off point and thus reduced to discrete categories.
These discrete categories group people who receive points based on their
occupation and education within a certain range into discrete social classes.
The classes derived from these or other measures are disadvantageous inas-
much as they obscure individual sources of SES effects, such as mother’s or
father’s education, income, and family structure. In addition, they assume
meaningful differences between people who might be only a point apart.
Although the grouping into distinct social classes may be problematic,
there is little doubt that SES—just like ethnicity—is a social construct highly
relevant to the study of differences in individual’s developmental trajecto-
ries. Many external factors such as family structure and neighborhood, as
well as internal factors such as parenting behavior, parent–child interac-
tion, and school achievement vary with socioeconomic status (Duncan &
Magnuson, 2003; Entwisle & Astone, 1994; Hoff, Laursen, & Tardif, 2002;
McLoyd, 1998). According to Entwisle and Astone’s (1994) measurement
of socioeconomic status, we can differentiate between three kinds of capital
that parents provide for their children. First, there is the financial capital
that allows parents to provide their children with the basic necessities such
as food and clothes. Second, there is the human capital which represents
the nonmaterial resources. Here, parents’ education is of special impor-
tance as this is an indicator of the extent to which parents can support chil-
dren with homework and other school demands, and are likely to encour-
age higher education. In addition, a higher level of parental education is
likely to facilitate communication with teachers. Third, parents have an im-
pact on the social capital which provides a connection between children
and the larger world outside their household. Here, the people living in a
youngster’s household, including the social networks of parents and other
household members, are of special importance as they are likely to share
these links to the larger community with these youngsters.
Unfortunately, there is no single best stand-in for the effects of SES on
parenting; moreover, parenting demands are likely to vary with children’s
age. Instead, for any consideration of the possible impact of SES on parent-
ing and child development, we need to bear in mind two questions: First,
what are the components of SES that are likely to influence parenting at
each stage of child development? Second, are these components of SES a
stable characteristic of the family or are they likely to fluctuate greatly?
There is evidence that specific aspects of financial, human, and social
capital have differing influences on parenting and child development and
that the magnitude changes with children’s ages. For example, research has
documented that for parents of young children, human capital, particularly
a mother’s participation in formal schooling, is likely to be the single best
measurement of SES. In an observational study on parenting behavior of
322 LEYENDECKER ET AL.
mothers with young infants, Bornstein, Hahn, Suwalsky, and Haynes (2003)
examined the effects of SES (Hollingshead four-factor index as well as the
Socioeconomic Index of Occupations) on 324 Euro-American mother–in-
fants dyads. Two of their findings are particularly noteworthy in the context
of this paper: First, they found mothers’ education to be the most robust
and thus the single most important individual sociodemographic predictor
of their parenting behavior, and second the results of their study indicate
that, because maternal education was a unique and consistent predictor of
maternal and infant behaviors, various components of the SES indexes are
not interchangeable but provide different information on the parenting of
young children. The relative importance of mothers’ education on their in-
teractions with young children is also supported by an earlier study by
Richman, Miller, and LeVine (1992). Although mothers are likely to be the
ones who spend the most time with their infants and thus may exert the
greatest single influence on an infant’s development, the impact of the ed-
ucational status of the father and of other caregivers is likely to increase
with children’s age. As children grow older, other parameters such as the
social and financial capital are also likely to become more important in the
assessment of socioeconomic influences on parenting. Thus, SES compos-
ites are not more than the sum of its parts but may be best analyzed sepa-
rately rather than combined into a single scale (Bornstein et al., 2003). This
approach is also strongly advocated by Entwisle and Astone (1994). Accord-
ing to Entwisle and Astone (1994), human capital, specifically parents’ edu-
cation, is the single best measurement of SES. However, this indicator
should not be used by itself but only in combination with income and meas-
ures of household structure to assess potential social capital: number of
birth parents, and whether there is a stepparent or a grandparent in the
home. They strongly advise that any analysis of SES should include meas-
ures of the financial, human, and social capital separately rather than rely-
ing on a composite measure. For example, if we are interested in studying
parents’ preferences for differing childcare arrangements, the social capi-
tal as well as the financial capital are both likely to impact parents’ decisions
separately and in concert. At a later point in time, parents’ education as
well as the current and the anticipated financial capital of a family may in-
fluence parents’ plans and hopes for their children’s economic future.
There appears to be, however, a non-linear relationship between paren-
tal education and income on parenting and children’s socioemotional and
cognitive development. For example, a 100% increase in parents’ income is
more likely to affect children’s development in a family living around the
poverty line than in an already affluent family. The same is true for educa-
tion—the difference between dropping out of high school after 10th grade
or completing high school is likely to have greater effects on parenting and
child development than between having parents with an M.A. versus a
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 323
Ph.D. degree. The smaller the numbers of the yearly income or of the years
spent in formal education, the greater the likelihood that any variation and
change is likely to affect children, inasmuch as development is particularly
likely to be impaired by lack of parental resources (Duncan & Magnuson,
2003; McLoyd, 1998). In addition, there is evidence that social capital is
particularly critical for families with low financial and human capital, inas-
much as social networks and professional support can serve to buffer the ef-
fects of poverty on parenting and child development (Field, Widmayer,
Adler, & De Cubas, 1990; Hurrelmann, Klocke, Melzer, & Ravens-Sieberer,
2003).
Although education is likely to remain stable over an adult’s life, income
and social support may fluctuate greatly and may not be a permanent char-
acteristic of a family (Hauser, 1994). For children living in low SES families,
the income of their parents is likely to influence later school attainment
(Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Smith, 1998). Although parents’ education tends
to remain stable (or may even increase), financial and social capital are
both very volatile. However, there is a general tendency for a family’s in-
come to increase as parents grow older, acquire experience, and obtain
promotions. Aside from unemployment, decreases and increases in a house-
hold’s disposable income are not unusual and may fluctuate considerably
across a child’s childhood. Assessing the monthly or yearly income of a fam-
ily is a sensitive issue, making it difficult in some cases to obtain accurate
data. In addition, these numbers might not represent the actual disposable
income because high mortgages, loan payments, property taxes, and medi-
cal care may reduce available income considerably. Duncan and Magnuson
(2003) suggest that to avoid a somewhat erroneous measure of a house-
hold’s income, a combination of income measured over several years
combined with measurement of wealth and occupation provide a better
estimate of the influence of financial capital on parenting and child devel-
opment. Similar to income, social capital is also subject to change due to
fluctuations in the social network due to divorce or relocation of family
members or friends.
The fluctuation of the financial and social capital is particularly impor-
tant when studying parents below or around the threshold of poverty. In
2003, the official U.S. Department of Health and Human Services’ defini-
tion of poverty was based on a combination of a household’s income and
the size of a household, e.g., the threshold for two-, three-, and four-person
families living in poverty were $12,120, $15,260, and $18,400, respectively,
in the 48 contiguous states and Washington, D.C. Duncan and Magnuson
(Duncan & Magnuson, 2003; Magnuson & Duncan, 2002) allude to the
need to differentiate between persistent poverty and shorter spells of pov-
erty. Persistent poverty has stronger negative association with parenting
and child development than shorter spells of poverty. They summarize lon-
324 LEYENDECKER ET AL.
One of the problems for research in this domain has been how to define
ethnicity. Although often used as a synonym for minority status, technically
the term means people, from the Greek ethnos, and thus everyone can be con-
sidered to have an ethnic background. From this perspective, it becomes an
aspect of culture that corresponds to an individual’s national heritage.
However, the tendency to equate culture with national heritage runs the
risk of perpetuating group stereotypes by treating large, diverse groups of
people as all of the same kind. The challenge is to find a way of conceptual-
izing culture that recognizes group differences without reifying them. For
our purposes in this chapter, we elaborate briefly on a perspective articu-
lated by Harwood, Handwerker, Schoelmerich and Leyendecker (2001)
which locates culture not in the group but in the contextualized individual;
culture is viewed not as an entity equivalent to group membership labels
but as a shifting continuum of shared commonality among individuals. In
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 325
Along these lines, Lee (2002) supports and specifies this view by pointing
out that it is important to study the variation as well as the commonalities
within an ethnic community. With regard to parenting, we can expect to
find variation in an ethnic community inasmuch as families differ accord-
ing to their socioeconomic status as well as to the degree that they partici-
pate in various other cultural communities.
In research on parenting practices, however, separate sources of influ-
ence have been frequently confounded. For example, much of what we
know about parenting in ethnic minority groups is derived from studies on
low SES families and is thus not representative of the entire community
(Garcia Coll & Pachter, 2002). By contrast, most of our knowledge of what
constitutes optimal parenting is derived from studies on middle-class Euro-
pean American families. Since these particular parenting practices and the
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 327
beliefs and values associated with them are reflected in mainstream institu-
tions, children from ethnic minority families, in particular those from low
SES families, are likely to experience a discontinuity between the home
culture and societal culture and may be faced with conflicting messages
(Greenfield & Suzuki, 1998; Garcia Coll & Pachter, 2002).
In sum, the two errors that are most likely to occur when we look at
parenting within an ethnic community is either to treat families of very di-
verse backgrounds as all of the same kind, or to associate a particular ethnic
community with a particular SES-based parenting style. If we want to under-
stand how parenting beliefs and practices are shaped, we need to pay closer
attention to the separate effects of parents’ ethnic background, their socio-
economic status, and to the matrices of cultural communities in which they
participate.
dulgence, particularly in the first years of life, has also been associated with
ethnic background.
Authoritarian (also labeled authoritarian-autocratic) parents place
power-assertive strict limits on the allowable expression of their children’s
needs and wants. They are parent-centered, high on demandingness and
control, and value the maintenance of their authority. They are also low on
parental responsiveness, and so do not discuss and do not attempt to reach
mutual agreements. Maccoby and Martin (1983) summarize the following
characteristics of this parenting pattern: “1. Attempting to shape, control,
and evaluate the behavior and attitudes of their children in accordance
with an absolute set of standards. 2. Valuing obedience, respect for author-
ity, work, tradition, and preservation of order. 3. Discouraging verbal give-
and-take between parent and child” (p. 40).
In contrast, parents who are authoritative or authoritative–reciprocal ex-
hibit a pattern in which they are responsive to their children’s needs yet ex-
pect their children to be responsive to their demands as well. Children are
allowed to express their wishes and desires, to discuss these with their par-
ents, and to reach a mutual agreement. Parents firmly implement rules but
are willing to discuss and to amend these rules if necessary. Authoritative
parenting is child-centered but in a reciprocal fashion which combines
high bidirectional communication with parental demandingness and con-
trol. Parents encourage their children’s independence and individuality
and emphasize the rights of both parents and children. Fairly high levels of
mutual involvement, responsiveness, and compliance between parents and
children are associated with children’s optimal functioning and high self-
esteem. However, as Maccoby and Martin (1983) point out, in “making
such generalizations we are, of course, glossing over the enormous varia-
tions in family styles and in the individual personalities that emerge from
the family system” (p. 83).
In comparison to the authoritarian and authoritative patterns, the parent-
ing style labeled indulgent-permissive refers to those who are tolerant and ac-
cepting toward children’s impulses, moods, and demands, use little punish-
ment, and reduce parental authority to a minimum. These parents are less
likely to impose and to enforce strict rules and to make maturity demands,
but more likely to allow their children to make their own decisions, e.g. re-
garding schedules for sleeping or for the amount of TV watching. In contrast
to neglectful parents, these parents are described as relatively warm.
habit” (p. 213). Kusserow (1999) took a closer look at the relationship be-
tween preferences for independence/individualism and for interdepend-
ence/sociocentrism in three groups of 4-year old European American
children and their parents in Manhattan and Queens. These families repre-
sented lower working-class families living in a dangerous environment, as
well as upper working-class and upper middle-class families. Kusserow de-
scribes three different styles of individualism that exist alongside socio-
centric socialization practices. The low SES parents who live in a dangerous
environment prefer a style of individualism that combines independence
and self-defense in the form of “not relying on anyone else,” “not trusting
anyone but yourself.” This attempt to toughen up children from early on by
teasing them and by teaching them to speak up and fight back is also de-
scribed by Miller (1996) in her study of working-class families in South Bal-
timore. Preparing children for a rough world outside by fostering their in-
dependence, self-defense skills, and self-reliance can be combined with the
encouragement of strong moral principles (Kusserow, 1999), as well as with
parents’ and teachers’ expectations of respect and adherence to strict rules
(Corsaro, 1996). In addition, the controlling behavior of low-SES parents
living in a high-risk inner-city community has been related to positive child
outcomes (Baldwin, Baldwin, & Cole, 1990; Deater-Deckard & Dodge,
1997). Thus, depending on the environment that children are raised in,
parents might teach their children to be emotionally independent and self-
reliant but at the same time they might expect them to follow strict rules.
The upper working-class parents in Kusserow’s study were more likely to
emphasize the self-assertive aspects of individualism with the goal of up-
ward mobility. Success was often linked to hard work, self-confidence, te-
nacity, good grades, and sports (Kusserow, 1999). In contrast to the two
groups of working-class parents, the socialization practices of the upper
middle-class, well educated Manhattan parents were in most respects identi-
cal with the ones generally associated both with individualism and with an
authoritative parenting style. These parents, as well as their preschool
teachers, practiced a “soft psychologized individualism, in which self-con-
fidence and assertion of the child’s unique feelings were paramount” (Kus-
serow, 1999, p. 222). Parents pursued the goal of raising children by gently
assisting them to discover their own unique qualities, thoughts and feelings
(Kusserow, 1999). Parenting practices were geared toward the reduction of
the power differential between child and parents by instilling a feeling of
empowerment and control in the child while at the same time maintaining
discipline. Thus, direct commands were avoided. As has been described by
other researchers as well (e.g., Nucci, 1994; Nucci, Killen, & Smetana,
1996), the children were either offered choices or commands which were
phrased as suggestions, even though the children knew how to interpret
them correctly. At the same time, parents and teachers balanced the inde-
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 331
trol; and (b) the expectations and extent of familial duties and obligations.
In the independent framework, instrumental autonomy is practiced from
early on, with children sleeping by themselves in their own rooms (Leyen-
decker, Lamb, Schoelmerich, & Fracasso, 1995; Morelli, Rogoff, Oppen-
heim, & Goldsmith, 1992) and feeding themselves from an early age (Har-
wood, Schoelmerich, Ventura-Cook, Schulze, & Wilson, 1996; Harwood,
Miller, Carlson, & Leyendecker, 2002; Schulze, Harwood, & Schoelmerich,
2001). However, the children are closely supervised, e.g., by parents, baby-
sitters, or a baby monitor. By encouraging early self-feeding, middle-class
Euro-American parents foster the child’s sense of control over his or her
own body, as well as autonomy and choice in decisions about how much to
eat and at what pace (Schulze, Harwood, Schoelmerich, & Leyendecker,
2002). However, parents may also tend to carefully structure their chil-
dren’s day, and to provide and follow a schedule for feeding, sleeping, and
napping (Leyendecker et al., 1995). In contrast, early autonomy in feeding,
combined with close supervision and the establishment of a schedule, may
be of much less importance among parents who emphasize a more interde-
pendent orientation. For example, Central American immigrant parents in
the U.S. were found to place little emphasis on schedules for their children
(Leyendecker et al., 1995).
With regard to early autonomy, Lebanese-born mothers now living in
Australia thought that their 5 to 6 year old children were still babies and
should not be given jobs aside from asking them to help mind or amuse
their younger siblings (Goodnow, Cashmore, Cotton & Knight, 1984). Sim-
ilarly, Mexican-born mothers in California did not expect their young chil-
dren to help with household chores, nor did they coax their children to de-
velop self-care skills or to perform household tasks. “Interdependence in
these families means expecting and accepting help when you are very
young, then learning to give help as you get older” (Delgado-Gaitan, 1994,
p. 69).
This principle has also been described for parenting in Muslim families.
According to Khounani (2000), early childhood in Muslim families is char-
acterized by great indulgence towards the child (see Kagitcibasi, 1996, for a
description of parent–child relationships in Turkish families). This serves
the goals of establishing strong emotional ties from children to their par-
ents and their families. Young children are not expected to follow rules and
even their misbehavior is met with great tolerance (Pfluger-Schindlbeck,
1989). This permissive–indulgent parenting style is limited to the first 5 to 7
years, and is likely to be gradually replaced by a parenting style that is much
more demanding and authoritarian, and in which children are expected to
help their younger siblings or their elders. However, although instrumental
independence is taught and valued within an interdependent framework,
emotional interdependence remains strong and is less likely to change dur-
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 333
but has to be viewed over the course of a lifetime and within the entire family
network. For example, the individual who is pampered as a young child is
later expected to pamper younger siblings and family members and to take
care of aging parents. At the same time, however, while growing older, one
learns how to respect authority and also how to grow into the role of some-
one who is treated with respect due to being an older sibling or the oldest
male or female in the family. With a reduction in family size and increased fi-
nancial capital, many of these aspects are likely to change, yet research, for
example, on American-born adolescents of Asian or Latin-American descent
shows that emotional solidarity and obligation remain especially high. Ac-
cording to Fuligni, Tseng, and Lam (1999) parenting practices in Asian and
Latin American families resulted in greater expectations of adolescents re-
garding their duty to assist, respect, and support their families when com-
pared to their peers with European backgrounds. These feelings of closeness
towards their families were consistent across youths’ generation, family com-
position, and socioeconomic backgrounds. Similarly, adolescents from immi-
grant families with an interdependent cultural orientation, such as Asian and
Latin American families, were found to be more willing to accept rather than
to fight parental control and to sustain positive family relations in part by de-
laying their pursuit of autonomy (overviews in Greenfield et al., 2003;
Kagitcibasi, 2003; Kwak, 2003).
In sum, parenting styles and practices vary greatly within as well as across
ethnic groups. In particular, the types of relationships as well as the adher-
ence to hierarchy varies according to the relative importance of an inde-
pendent or an interdependent cultural framework. Both frameworks coexist
in all ethnic communities and overlap greatly but differ in their emphasis.
These parallel effects of SES within families from differing ethnic back-
grounds, however, do not capture the entire picture. First, despite the same
tendencies, overall differences between ethnic groups should not be ig-
nored. Second, particular attitudes toward personal autonomy and family
obligation appear to remain salient in families from differing ethnic
groups, regardless of SES or degree of acculturation. For example, Chao
(1994) suggests that it is somewhat ethnocentric to regard the authoritative
parenting style as the most optimal and the authoritarian as less optimal:
“For Asians, parental obedience and some aspects of strictness may be
equated with parental control, caring, or involvement. Just as important,
for Asians parental control may not always involve ‘domination’ of children
per se, but rather a more organizational type of control for the purpose or
goal of keeping the family running smoothly and fostering family harmony”
(Chao, 1994, p. 1112). She further points out that, despite some parallel
features, the concept of training (guan) should not simply be equated with
the concept of authoritarian parenting, inasmuch as the concept of train-
ing includes high involvement on the part of the parents, physical closeness
as well as parental warmth. Numerous studies indirectly support this view in
reporting that emotional closeness with the family, the acceptance of de-
creasing autonomy with age, and the importance attached to family obliga-
tions, all remain high in children and adolescents with roots in ethnic com-
munities who endorse an interdependent framework, regardless of their
SES (e.g., Phinney, Ong, & Madden, 2000).
CONCLUSIONS
tive development in all its diverse complexity. Unfortunately, this task is nei-
ther simple nor straightforward. First, SES is a multifaceted construct con-
sisting of many components that influence parenting both directly (e.g.,
the association between a mother’s educational level and her vocalizations
toward the infant) and indirectly (e.g., the quality of neighborhood or the
availability of a variety of toys and books in the home). Thus, depending on
our focal group and possible sources of variation, it is necessary to identify
those components which are likely to be a relevant indicators of socioeco-
nomic status, and which are likely to influence parenting practices and chil-
dren’s development. When studying lower SES groups, particularly those
living at or below the poverty level, small differences in available human, so-
cial, or financial capital might have relatively large explanatory power.
Second, the key values of middle-class Euro-American parents have been
studied extensively, and central aspects of parenting beliefs and practices
prevalent among this group have been associated with socioeconomic stat-
us. In particular, many key features of the authoritative parenting style,
widely considered to be most optimal for child development, appear linked
to both a higher socioeconomic status and an independent framework. For
example, more educated European American parents are more likely to
provide explanations rather than relying on simple strict orders. However,
some socioeconomic effects might be parallel in families from diverse eth-
nic backgrounds, but others are likely to be dwarfed by the possibly more
powerful key cultural values. A recognition of the role of cultural values is
necessary to avoid stereotyping normative parenting in minority families.
Third, neither ethnicity nor socioeconomic status can be treated as static
index variables. This is particularly true both for first and second genera-
tion immigrants who might find it necessary to accept jobs well below their
educational attainment and skills due to limited English skills. In addition,
although limited education and/or language skills increase the risk that
families will live below the poverty line, it is important to recognize that this
situation may only be temporary. Similarly, ethnicity is not a clearly defined
marker variable but may also be subject to change. Changes may be in-
duced, for example, by shifts within one’s own or in other ethnic communi-
ties, changes in the composition of the ethnic background of household
members, or (in the case of immigrant families) by changes in accultura-
tion. As a result, the increasing diversity (both ethnically and socioecono-
mically) of the United States as well as of other industrialized countries,
forces us to move beyond the safe realm of a developmental psychology
based primarily on our knowledge of Euro-American middle-class families.
The relations among parenting practices, parenting styles, and children’s
development are much less clearcut once ethnic, socioeconomic, and
acculturative influences are all taken into account. As Miller (1996) points
out, going beyond the vantage point of the privileged sector of our society
338 LEYENDECKER ET AL.
REFERENCES
Baldwin, A. L., Baldwin, C., & Cole, R. E. (1990). Stress-resistant families and stress-resistant
children. In J. Rolf, A. S. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. H. Nuechterlein, & A. S. Weintraub
(Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 257–280). Cam-
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Baltes, M. M., & Silverberg, S. (1994). The dynamics between dependency and autonomy: Il-
lustrations across the life-span. In P. B. Baltes (Ed.), Life-span development and behavior (pp.
41–90). New York: Wiley.
Baumrind, D. (1967). Childcare practices anteceding 3 patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic
Psychology Monographs, 75, 43–88.
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., Suwalsky, J. T. D., & Haynes, O. M. (2003). Socioeconomic sta-
tus, parenting, and child development: The Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of social sta-
tus and the Socioeconomic Index of Occupations. In M. H. Bornstein, R. H. Bradley, & A.
von Eye (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 29–82). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bradley, R. H., Whiteside, L., Mundform, D. J., Casey, P. H., Kelleher, K. J., & Pope, S. K.
(1994). Early indications of resilience and their relation to experiences in the home envi-
ronments of low birthweight, premature children living in poverty. Child Development, 65,
346–360.
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understand-
ing Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65,
1111–1119.
Chao, R. K. (2000). What is the place of culture in describing ethnic diversity in the U.S.? Cross-
Cultural Psychology Bulletin, 34, 7–13.
Corsaro, W. (1996). Transitions in early childhood. In R. Jessor, A. Colby, & R. Shweder
(Eds.), Ethnography and Human Development. Context and meaning in social inquiry (pp.
419–457). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline re-
visited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological In-
quiry, 8, 161–175.
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1994). Socializing young children. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking
(Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates.
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (Eds.). (1997). Consequences of growing up poor. New York: Rus-
sell Sage.
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Smith, J. (1998). How much does childhood poverty affect
the life chances of children? American Sociological Review, 63, 406–423.
Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. A. (2003). Off with Hollingshead: Socioeconomic resources,
parenting, and child development. In M. H. Bornstein, R. H. Bradley, & A. von Eye (Eds.),
Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 83–106). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 339
Entwisle, D. R., & Astone, N. M. (1994). Some practical guidelines for measuring youth’s race/
ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 65, 1521–1540.
Field, T. M., Widmayer, S., Adler, S., & De Cubas, M. (1990). Teenage parenting in different
cultures, family constellations, and caregiving environments: Effects on infant develop-
ment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 158–174.
Fuligni, A. J., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudes toward family obligations among Ameri-
can adolescents with Asian, Latin American, and European family backgrounds. Child De-
velopment, 70, 1030–1044.
Garcia Coll, C., & Pachter, L. M. (2002). Ethnic and minority parenting. In M. H. Bornstein
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and applied parenting (pp. 1–20).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Goodnow, J. J., Cashmore, J., Cotton, S., & Knight, R. (1984). Mother’s developmental time ta-
bles in two cultural groups. International Journal of Psychology, 19, 193–205.
Greenfield, P., Keller, H., Fuligni, A. J., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through uni-
versal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461–490.
Greenfield, P., & Suzuki, L. K. (1998). Culture and human development: Implications for
parenting, education, pediatrics, and mental health. In I. E. Sigel & K. A. Renninger
(Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice (5th ed., pp. 1059–1109.
New York: Wiley.
Harwood, R. L., Handwerker, W. P., Schoelmerich, A., & Leyendecker, B. (2001). Ethnic cate-
gory labels, parental beliefs, and the contextualized individual: An exploration of the indi-
vidualism-sociocentrism debate. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1, 217–236.
Harwood, R. L., Leyendecker, B., Carlson, V. J., Asencio, M., & Miller, A. M. (2002). Parenting
among Latino families in the U.S. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4.
Social conditions and applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 21–46). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Harwood, R. L., Miller, A. M., Carlson, V. J., & Leyendecker, B. (2002). Parenting beliefs and
practices among middle-class Puerto Rican mother–infant pairs. In J. M. Contreras, K. A.
Kerns, & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Latino children and families in the United States (pp.
133–154). Westport, CT: Praeger Publisher.
Harwood, R. L., Schoelmerich, A., Ventura-Cook, E., Schulze, P. A., & Wilson, S. P. (1996).
Culture and class influences on Anglo and Puerto Rican mothers’ beliefs regarding long-
term socialization goals and child behavior. Child Development, 67, 2446–2461.
Hauser, R. M. (1994). Measuring socioeconomic status in studies of child development. Child
Development, 65, 1541–1545.
Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and family socialization strategies
and children’s mental health: Low-income Mexican-American and Euro-American moth-
ers and children. Child Development, 74(1), 189–204.
Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic Status and Parenting. In M. H.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 231–252).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hoffman, L. W. (1988). Cross-cultural differences in childrearing goals. New Directions for Child
Development, 40, 99–122.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). The four-factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale
University, New Haven, CT.
Hurrelmann, K., Klocke, A., Melzer, W., & Ravens-Sieberer, U. (Eds.). (2003). Jugend-
gesundheitssurvey. Internationale Vergleichsstudie im Auftrag der Weltgesundheitsorganisation
WHO. Weinheim: Juventa.
Imamoglu, E. O. (1988). An interdependence model of human development. In C.
Kagitcibasi (Ed.), Growth and progress in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 138–146). Berwyn: Swets
North America.
340 LEYENDECKER ET AL.
Kagitcibasi, C. (1996). Family and human development across cultures. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Kagitcibasi, C. (2003). Autonomy, embeddedness and adaptability in immigration contexts.
Human Development, 46, 145–150.
Kagitcibasi, C. (2003). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: Implications for self and
human development. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Khounani, P. M. (2000). Binationale Familien in Deutschland und die Erziehung der Kinder: eine
Vergleichsuntersuchung zur familären Erziehungsituation in mono- und bikulturellen Familien im
Hinblick auf multikulturelle Handlungsfähigkeit. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
Kohn, M. L. (1969). Class and conformity: A study in values. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.
Kohn, M. L. (1979). The effects of social class on parental values and practices. In D. Reiss &
H. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The American family: Dying or developing (pp. 45–68). New York: Ple-
num.
Kusserow, A. S. (1999). De-homogenizing American individualism: Socializing hard and soft
individualism in Manhattan and Queens. Ethos, 27(2), 210–234.
Kwak, K. (2003). Adolescents and their parents: A review of intergenerational family relations
for immigrant and non-immigrant families. Human Development, 46, 114–136.
Lee, C. D. (2002). Interrogating race and ethnicity as constructs in the examination of cultural
processes in developmental research. Human Development, 45, 282–290.
Leyendecker, B., Lamb, M. E., Schoelmerich, A., & Fracasso, M. P. (1995). The social world of
8- and 12-month-old infants: Early experiences in two subcultural contexts. Social Develop-
ment, 4(2), 194–208.
Leyendecker, B., Harwood, R. L., Lamb, M. E., & Schoelmerich, A. (2002). External factors
versus internal factors: Parental evaluations of desirable and undesirable everyday situa-
tions in two diverse cultural groups. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26,
248–258.
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child
interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization,
personality, and social development (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley.
Magnuson, K. A., & Duncan, G. J. (2002). Parents in poverty. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Hand-
book of parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and applied parenting (pp. 95–122). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion,
and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
McLoyd, V. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist,
53(2), 185–204.
Miller, P. J. (1996). Instantiating culture through discourse practices. In R. Jessor, A. Colby, &
R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Ethnography and human development. Context and meaning in social in-
quiry (pp. 181–204). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Morelli, G., Rogoff, B., Oppenheim, D., & Goldsmith, D. (1992). Cultural variation in infants’
sleeping arrangements. Developmental Psychology, 28, 604–613.
Nucci, L. (1994). Mothers’ beliefs regarding the personal domain of children. In J. G.
Smetana (Ed.), Beliefs about parenting: Origins and developmental implications (Vol. 66, New Di-
rections for Child Development, pp. 81–97). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Nucci, L., Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (1996). Autonomy and the personal: Negotiations and
social reciprocity in adult–child social exchange. In M. Killen (Ed.), Children’s autonomy, so-
cial competence, and interactions with adults and other children: Exploring connections and conse-
quences: Vol. 73. New directions for child development (pp. 7–24). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
11. SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS, ETHNICITY, AND PARENTING 341
Pfluger-Schindlbeck, I. (1989). “Achte die Älteren, liebe die Jüngeren”. Sozialisation türkischer Kinder.
Frankfurt: Athenäum.
Phinney, J. S. (1996). When we talk about American ethnic groups, what do we mean? Ameri-
can Psychologist, 51, 918–927.
Phinney, J. S., Ong, A., & Madden, T. (2000). Cultural values and intergenerational value dis-
crepancies in immigrant and non-immigrant families. Child Development, 71, 528–539.
Richman, A., Miller, P. M., & LeVine, R. (1992). Cultural and educational variations in mater-
nal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 28, 81–96.
Rogoff, B., & Angelillo, C. (2002). Investigating the coordinated functioning of multifaceted
cultural practices in human development. Human Development, 45, 211–225.
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). Autonomy, relatedness, and the self: Their
relation to development and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Devel-
opmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. 618–655). New York: Wiley & Sons.
Schulze, P. A., Harwood, R. L., & Schoelmerich, A. (2001). Feeding practices and expectations
among middle-class Anglo and Puerto Rican mothers of 12-month old infants. Journal of
Cross Cultural Psychology, 32, 397–406.
Schulze, P. A., Harwood, R. L., Schoelmerich, A., & Leyendecker, B. (2002). The cultural
structuring of parenting and universal developmental tasks. Parenting: Science and Practice.
Shweder, R. A. (1996). True ethnography: The lore, the law and the lure. In R. Jessor, A.
Colby, & R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Ethnography and human development (pp. 15–52). Chicago:
The University of Chicago Press.
Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory of cultural meaning. New York: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
This page intentionally left blank
12
Douglas R. Powell
Purdue University
INTRODUCTION
343
344 POWELL
mous number or determine the power of the contexts within which human
beings find themselves” (Berliner, 2002, p. 19).
Rigorous investigation of questions about what works puts a spotlight on
program processes and population characteristics long viewed as static enti-
ties in intervention studies (Powell & Sigel, 1991). For certain, “black box
experiments are out” (Cook & Payne, 2002, p. 160). Less certain is how to
appropriately conceptualize, measure, and analyze program process and
population variables, especially in the context of program features and
community systems in which families and programs function. The treat-
ment variable is actually an intervention package comprised of many vari-
ables (e.g., curriculum, intensity, staffing), usually organized in a complex
manner (Campbell, 1986).
First and foremost, investigators of questions about what works need to
identify variables and their potential linkages in a coherent model of how a
program is expected to make a difference. The model needs to specify how
anticipated actions in a parenting program are expected to change parent
knowledge or behavior and how, in turn, those changes might lead to spe-
cific early, intermediate, and long-term outcomes. This framework can be
used as a road map for examining the extent to which a program’s theory
actually holds (Weiss, 1995). Typically a program’s theory of change, also
known as a logic model, is implicit. Designers of interventions generally do
not articulate in precise terms the assumptions of program strategies (St.Pi-
erre & Layzer, 1998). Moreover, researchers cannot expect program staff to
readily provide a short list of variables or a hypothesized model of causal
linkages. Although service providers are a valuable and overlooked source
of insight into intervention processes (e.g., Kitzman, Cole, Yoos, & Olds,
1997), it appears that program staff often have difficulty articulating the
theory or theories upon which their program is based. Researchers who
have collaborated with program staff to develop a testable model or theory
of change for program evaluation purposes report that early discussions
with staff may yield vague terms and ideas, multiple and sometimes compet-
ing theories of change for the same program; these “Grand Canyon mod-
els” are characterized by a wide gap between the intervention design and
desired results (Philliber, 1998, p. 92), and a reluctance to make program
assumptions explicit due to political risks of alienating or offending one or
more constituencies in a community (Weiss, 1995). Accordingly, the devel-
opment of a model with program staff may be best approached as an itera-
tive process that moves toward successive refinement of a set of program ac-
tions that are both doable and plausibly linked to program goals and
anticipated outcomes (Connell & Kubisch, 1998).
As indicated earlier, this chapter examines three domains of variables in
searches for what works in parenting interventions: program features; pro-
12. PARENTING INTERVENTIONS 347
pected by chance (i.e., mothers in the intervention centers had higher de-
pressive symptom scores and a higher likelihood of having a substance
abuse history; Baydar, Reid, & Webster-Stratton, 2003). Nonetheless, the
best (albeit not always politically or logistically feasible) way to eliminate se-
lection bias is to randomly assign participants to either intervention or con-
trol conditions.
Several across-program analyses of outcomes of family-oriented interven-
tions that included parenting components have been conducted recently
with an eye toward identifying critical components of program effective-
ness. One is a study of the Early Head Start Program in 17 different sites
(Love et al., 2002), all of which included a parenting component. The oth-
ers are meta-analyses, one of 665 studies of 260 family support programs,
98% of which offered parenting education (Layzer, Goodson, Bernstein, &
Price, 2001), and a second of 88 preventive interventions aimed at enhanc-
ing parental sensitivity and infant attachment (Bakermans-Kranenburg,
van IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003). Random assignment of families to pro-
gram or control conditions was used in each of the Early Head Start sites,
and randomization was employed in studies of 109 of the interventions in-
cluded in the family support program analyses. Cross-program compari-
sons and meta-analyses are inherently limited in the contextual informa-
tion that can be provided in relation to results. Nonetheless, meta-analyses
are particularly useful in helping researchers use all existing data on a topic
of programmatic and policy importance (McCartney & Rosenthal, 2000).
PROGRAM FEATURES
Illustrative Studies
Links to Outcomes (path a). Discussion groups and home visits are time-
honored methods for delivering parenting education and support. Each
has rich practice and theoretical traditions that embrace different assump-
tions about how best to facilitate change in parents, namely the peer learn-
ing and support presumed to exist in discussion groups and the individual-
ization to particular circumstances afforded in home visits.
Research comparing the relative effectiveness of these two delivery meth-
ods spans several decades. Peer discussion groups produced stronger ef-
fects than home visits in an early quasi-experimental study (Slaughter,
1983), and the Layzer et al. (2001) meta-analysis of family support pro-
grams found that parent groups produced stronger effects than home visit-
ing. Specifically, the average effect sizes for parent groups as the primary
method of delivering parent education were .54 when targeted to children
at biological risk and .27 when used with nontargeted populations, com-
pared to average effect sizes for home visiting of .36 when targeted to chil-
dren at biological risk and .09 when used with nontargeted populations.
Further, a cross-program study of Early Head Start found that a combina-
tion of center-based and home-based strategies yielded a stronger pattern
of impacts than center-based or home-based alone (Love et al., 2002). Per-
12. PARENTING INTERVENTIONS 355
PROGRAM PROCESSES
Illustrative Studies
POPULATION CHARACTERISTICS
Illustrative Studies
children who were heavier. The researchers speculated that there may be a
ceiling effect on the amount of improvement that can be expected in low
birth weight children who were lighter and reside in relatively enriched en-
vironments (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1997). This is one of the few intervention
studies to consider a child characteristic as a moderator of program effects.
The finding that the Infant Health and Development Program was more
effective with higher-risk families is consistent with results of other interven-
tion researches. In studies of the Nurse Home Visiting Program noted
above, treatment differences for child abuse and neglect and emergency
room visits were more significant among women who had a lower sense of
control over their lives in the Elmira, New York trial (Olds, Henderson,
Chamberlin, & Tatelbaum, 1986), and program effects on children’s health
care encounters for injuries and ingestions were focused on children born
to women with few psychological resources in the Memphis, Tennessee trial
(Olds, Henderson, Kitzman, Eckenrode, Cole, & Tatelbaum, 1998). Also,
the experimental study by Booth et al. (1989) of the Mental Health and In-
formation/Resource intervention models, described previously, found the
intervention was more beneficial for mothers who, at the point of program
entry, had low social skills. Further, a quasi-experimental study (no ran-
domization) of a home visiting program for mothers of at-risk infants found
no main effect of the intervention on infant mental development scores,
but a significant interaction effect between intervention and maternal de-
pression. Infants of depressed mothers who received the home visiting in-
tervention achieved significantly higher mental development scores and
were twice as likely to be classified as securely attached than infants of de-
pressed mothers who did not receive the home visiting intervention (Lyons-
Ruth, Connell, Grunebaum, & Botein, 1990).
(i.e., infant death rate, low birth weight rate) or when isolated from imme-
diate family and friendship networks (McGuigan, Katzev, & Pratt, 2003).
In a multi-site study of Hawaii’s Healthy Start Program, child and parent
risk factors were predictive of prospective participants’ willingness to enroll
in the paraprofessional home visiting program aimed at improving family
functioning, promoting child health and development, and preventing
child abuse and neglect. Infant biologic risk greatly increased an at-risk
family’s willingness to participate in the program. Adolescent mothers who
had not completed high school also were more likely to initially agree to
participate in the program. Families where the father had multiple risks
were more likely to receive more visits (≥12 visits) than mothers who were
unilaterally violent toward their partner (Duggan et al., 2000).
Research Design
Because the ultimate goal of research on what works is to identify critical el-
ements of effective interventions, future studies should focus on variables
that hold promise of occupying a salient role in the design and implemen-
tation of parenting programs. Promising variables and methodological
approaches are noted later in each of the three domains of variables consid-
ered previously in this chapter. An emerging domain of potential signifi-
cance also is briefly described.
Program Features. The use of professionals as the primary staff for work-
ing with parents is a promising candidate for designation as a critical element
of effective interventions. As reviewed in this chapter, results of a randomized
trial offer compelling evidence about the merits of using professionals (Olds
et al., 2002), and a growing literature, mostly descriptive in nature, points to
challenges encountered by paraprofessionals in working with low-income
and high-risk populations. We cannot however generalize to all parenting in-
terventions from one study demonstrating the benefits of nurses as staff in
prenatal and infancy home visiting programs with low-income, first-time
mothers. There may be different outcomes if paraprofessionals were trained
and consistently supported in a program model uniquely suited to their
backgrounds and abilities (Korfmacher, 2001). Hence, experimental studies
on the staffing status question are needed with other types of professionals
and with group-based interventions targeted at different types of populations
and parenting issues. Further research also is needed on specific practices
(e.g., flexible adaptation of program resources) and conditions (e.g., case-
loads, staff turnover rates, supervision) that enable staff to function effec-
tively with parents. This information is essential to developing research-based
guidance on the supports needed for large-scale adoption of a parenting in-
tervention.
The other program features considered in this chapter—delivery meth-
od, curriculum, and pedagogical or clinical approach—are conceptually in-
teresting and programmatically important but there is insufficient research
information to determine what works. Data are needed to address existing
inferences and assumptions about each of these variables. For example,
there are suggestions that a clear and consistent content focus on child de-
velopment and parenting may yield positive intervention effects on parent-
ing and child outcomes, but currently this idea emanates chiefly from the
inferences of intervention results indicating that the absence of a con-
tent focus on child development and parenting is associated with no mean-
ingful effects on parent and child outcomes (e.g., Hebbeler & Gerlach-
Downie, 2002).
12. PARENTING INTERVENTIONS 367
with families (Duggan et al., 2000). Perhaps this line of investigation will de-
fine the third generation of research on parenting interventions.
REFERENCES
Anderson, J., Fagan, W. T., & Cronin, M. (1998). Insights in implementing family literacy pro-
grams. Literacy and community: Twentieth yearbook of the College Reading Association (pp.
269–281). Carrollton, GA: College Reading Association.
Arnold, D. H., Lonigan, C. J., Whitehurst, G. J., & Epstein, J. N. (1994). Accelerating language
development through picture book reading: Replication and extension to a videotape
training format. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 235–243.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-
analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulle-
tin, 129, 195–215.
Barnard, K. E., Magyary, D., Sumner, G., Booth, C. L., Mitchell, S. K., & Spieker, S. (1988). Pre-
vention of parenting alterations for women of low social support. Psychiatry, 51, 248–253.
Baydar, N., Reid, M. J., & Webster-Stratton, C. (2003). The role of mental health factors and
program engagement in the effectiveness of a preventive parenting program for Head
Start mothers. Child Development, 74, 1433–1453.
Belsky, J. (1985). Experimenting with the family in the newborn period. Child Development, 56,
407–414.
Belsky, J. (1986). A tale of two variances: Between and within. Child Development, 57, 1301–1305.
Berlin, L. J., O’Neal, C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1998). What makes early intervention programs
work: The program, its participant, and their interaction. Zero to Three, 19, 4–15.
Berliner, D. C. (2002). Educational research: The hardest science of all. Educational Researcher,
31, 18–20.
Blackwell, B. (1998). Compliance. In G. A. Fava & H. Freyberger (Eds.), Handbook of psychoso-
matic medicine (pp. 625–638). Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Booth, C. L., Mitchell, S. K., Barnard, K. E., & Spieker, S. J. (1989). Development of maternal
social skills in multiproblem families: Effects on the mother–child relationship. Developmen-
tal Psychology, 25, 403–412.
Bradley, R. H., Burchinal, M. R., & Casey, P. H. (2001). Early intervention: The moderating
role of the home environment. Applied Developmental Science, 5, 2–8.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Gross, R. T., Kraemer, H. C., Spiker, D., & Shapiro, S. (1997). Enhancing the
cognitive outcomes of LBW, premature infants: For whom is the intervention most effec-
tive? In R. T. Gross, D. Spiker, & C. W. Haynes (Eds.), Helping low birth weight, premature ba-
bies: The Infant Health and Development Program (pp. 181–189). Stanford, CA: Stanford Uni-
versity Press.
Campbell, D. T. (1986). Relabeling internal and external validity for applied social scientists.
In W. M. K. Trochim (Ed.), Advances in quasi-experimental design and analysis (pp. 67–77).
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Connell, J. P., & Kubisch, A. C. (1998). Applying a theory of change approach to the evalua-
tion of comprehensive community initiatives: Progress, prospects, and problems. In K.
Fulbright-Anderson, A. C. Kubisch, & J. P. Connell (Eds.), New approaches to evaluating com-
munity initiatives: Vol. 2. Theory, measurement, and analysis (pp. 15–44). Washington, DC: The
Aspen Institute.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field
settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
370 POWELL
Cook, T. D., & Payne, M. R. (2002). Objecting to the objections to using random assignment in
educational research. In F. Mosteller & R. Boruch (Eds.), Evidence matters: Randomized trials
in education research (pp. 150–178). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.
Cowan, P. A. (1988). Developmental psychopathology: A nine-cell map of the territory. In E.
Nannis & P. Cowan (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology and its treatment. New directions for
child development (No. 39, pp. 5–30). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cowan, P. A., Powell, D. R., & Cowan, C. P. (1998). Parenting interventions: A family systems
perspective. In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:
Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice (5th ed., pp. 3–72). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Duggan, A., Windham, A., McFarlane, E., Fuddy, L., Rohde, C., Buchbinder, S., & Sia, C.
(2000). Hawaii’s Healthy Start Program of home visiting for at-risk families: Evaluation of
family identification, family engagement, and service delivery. Pediatrics, 105, 250–259.
Dunst, C. J., & Trivette, C. M. (1997). Early intervention with young at-risk children and their
families. In R. Ammerman & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of prevention and treatment with
children and adolescents: Intervention in the real world (pp. 157–180). New York: John Wiley &
Sons.
Eisenstadt, J. W., & Powell, D. R. (1987). Processes of participation in a mother–infant pro-
gram as modified by stress and impulse control. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8,
17–37.
Emde, R. M., & Robinson, J. (2000). Guiding principles for a theory of early intervention: A
developmental-psychoanalytic perspective. In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Hand-
book of early childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. 160–178). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Erickson, F., & Gutierrez, K. (2002). Culture, rigor, and science in educational research. Edu-
cational Researcher, 31, 21–24.
Gomby, D. S., Culross, P. L., & Behrman, R. E. (1999). Home visiting: Recent program evalua-
tions—analysis and recommendations. The Future of Children, 9, 4–26.
Goodson, B. D., & Hess, R. D. (1975). Parents as teachers of young children: An evaluative review of
contemporary concepts and programs. Stanford, CA: School of Education, Stanford University.
Greenstein, B. (1998). Engagement is everything. Zero to Three, 18, 16.
Gross, D., Spiker, D., & Haynes, C. W. (Eds.). (1997). Helping low birth weight, premature babies:
The Infant Health and Development Program. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Guralnick, M. J. (1997). Second-generation research in the field of early intervention. In M. J.
Guralnick (Ed.), The effectiveness of early intervention (pp. 3–20). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes
Publishing.
Halpern, R. (1992). Issues of program design and implementation. In M. Larner, R. Halpern,
& O. Harkavy (Eds.), Fair start for children: Lessons learned from seven demonstration projects (pp.
179–197). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hebbeler, K. M., & Gerlach-Downie, S. G. (2002). Inside the black box of home visiting: A
qualitative analysis of why intended outcomes were not achieved. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 17, 28–51.
Heinicke, C. M., Fineman, N. R., Ponce, V. A., & Guthrie, D. (2001). Relation-based interven-
tion with at-risk mothers: Outcome in the second year of life. Infant Mental Health Journal,
22, 431–462.
Heinicke, C. M., & Ponce, V. A. (1999). Relation-based early family intervention. In D. Cichetti
& S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on psychopathology: Vol. 9. Developmental approaches to
prevention and intervention (pp. 153–193). New York: University of Rochester Press.
Kisker, E. E., Paulsell, D., Love, J. M., & Raikes, H. (2002). Pathways to quality and full implemen-
tation in Early Head Start Programs. Princeton, NJ: Mathematica Policy Research.
Kitzman, H. J., Cole, R., Yoos, H. L., & Olds, D. (1997). Challenges experienced by home visi-
tors: A qualitative study of program implementation. Journal of Community Psychology, 25,
95–109.
12. PARENTING INTERVENTIONS 371
Kitzman, H., Yoos, H. L., Cole, R., Korfmacher, J., & Hanks, C. (1997). Prenatal and early
childhood home-visitation program processes: A case illustration. Journal of Community Psy-
chology, 25, 27–45.
Korfmacher, J. (2001). Early childhood interventions: Now what? In H. E. Fitzgerald, K. H.
Karraker, & T. Luster (Eds.), Infant development: Ecological perspectives (pp. 275–294). New
York: Routledge Falmer.
Korfmacher, J., Kitzman, H., & Olds, D. (1998). Intervention processes as predictors of out-
comes in a preventive home-visitation program. Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 49–64.
Korfmacher, J., O’Brien, R., Hiatt, S., & Olds, D. (1999). Differences in program implementa-
tion between nurses and paraprofessionals providing home visits during pregnancy and in-
fancy: A randomized trial. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1847–1851.
Lamb, M. E. (1998). Nonparental child care: Context, quality, correlates, and consequences.
In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child
psychology in practice (5th ed., pp. 73–133). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Layzer, J. I., Goodson, B. D., Bernstein, L., & Price, C. (2001). National evaluation of family sup-
port programs. Final report volume A: The meta-analysis. Cambridge, MA: Abt Associates.
Lieberman, A. F., Weston, D., & Pawl, J. H. (1991). Preventive intervention and outcome with
anxiously attached dyads. Child Development, 62, 199–209.
Liaw, F., Meisels, S. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1995). The effects of experience of early interven-
tion on low birth weight, premature children: The Infant Health and Development Pro-
gram. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 405–431.
Love, J. M., Kisker, E. E., Ross, C. M., Schochet, P. Z., Brooks-Gunn, J., Paulsell, D., et al.
(2002). Making a difference in the lives of infants and toddlers and their families: The impacts of
Early Head Start: Vol. 1. Final technical report. Princeton, NJ: Mathematica Policy Research.
Luster, T., Perlstadt, H., McKinney, M., Sims, K., & Juang, L. (1996). The effects of a family
support program and other factors on the home environments provided by adolescent
mothers. Family Relations, 45, 255–264.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D. B., Grunebaum, H. U., & Botein, S. (1990). Infants at social risk:
Maternal depression and family support services as mediators of infant development and
security of attachment. Child Development, 61, 85–98.
McCartney, K., & Rosenthal, R. (2000). Effect size, practical importance, and social policy for
children. Child Development, 71, 173–180.
McGuigan, W. M., Katzev, A. R., & Pratt, C. C. (2003). Multi-level determinants of mothers’ en-
gagement in home visitation services. Family Relations, 52, 271–278.
Mosteller, F., & Boruch, R. (Eds.). (2002). Evidence matters: Randomized trials in education re-
search. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.
Musick, J. S., & Barbera-Stein, L. (1988). The role of research in an innovative preventive ini-
tiative. In D. R. Powell (Ed.), Parent education as early childhood intervention: Emerging direc-
tions in theory, research, and practice (pp. 209–227). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
National Research Council. (2002). Scientific research in education. Committee on Scientific
Principles for Education Research. Shavelson, R. J. & Towne, L. (Eds.). Center for Educa-
tion, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The
science of early childhood development. Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Child-
hood Development. J. P. Shonkoff & D. A. Phillips (Eds.). Board on Children, Youth, and
Families, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.
Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Chamberlin, R., & Tatelbaum, R. (1986). Preventing child
abuse and neglect: A randomized trial of nurse home visitation. Pediatrics, 78, 65–78.
Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Kitzman, H. J., Eckenrode, J. J., Cole, R. E., & Tatelbaum, R. C.
(1999). Prenatal and infancy home visitation by nurses: Recent findings. The Future of
Children, 9, 44–65.
372 POWELL
Olds, D. L., Robinson, J., O’Brien, R., Luckey, D. W., Pettitt, L. M., Henderson, C. R., et al.
(2002). Home visiting by paraprofessionals and by nurses: A randomized, controlled trial.
Pediatrics, 110, 486–496.
Olson, D. R. (2004). A triumph of hope over experience in the search for “what works”: A re-
sponse to Slavin. Educational Researcher, 33, 24–26.
Pfannenstiel, J. C., & Seltzer, D. A. (1989). New parents as teachers: Evaluation of an early par-
ent education program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 4, 1–18.
Philliber, S. (1998). The virtue of specificity in theory of change evaluation. In K. Fulbright-
Anderson, A. C. Kubisch, & J. P. Connell (Eds.), New approaches to evaluating community ini-
tiatives: Vol. 2. Theory, measurement, and analysis (pp. 87–99). Washington, DC: The Aspen
Institute.
Powell, C., & Grantham-McGregor, S. (1989). Home visiting of varying frequency and child
development. Pediatrics, 84, 157–164.
Powell, D. R. (1984). Social network and demographic predictors of length of participation in
a parent education program. Journal of Community Psychology, 12, 13–20.
Powell, D. R. (1988). Emerging directions in parent–child early intervention. In D. R. Powell
(Ed.), Parent education as early childhood intervention: Emerging directions in theory, research and
practice (pp. 1–22). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Powell, D. R., & Eisenstadt, J. W. (1988). Informal and formal conversations in parent educa-
tion groups: An observational study. Family Relations, 37, 166–170.
Powell, D., Okagaki, L., & Bojczyk, K. (2004). Evaluating parent participation and outcomes in
family literacy programs: Cultural diversity considerations. In B. H. Wasik (Ed.), Handbook
of family literacy (pp. 551–566). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Powell, D. R., & Sigel, I. E. (1991). Searches for validity in evaluations of young children and
early childhood programs. In B. Spodek & O. Saracho (Eds.), Yearbook of early childhood edu-
cation: Vol. 2. Issues in early childhood curriculum (pp. 190–212). New York: Teachers College
Press.
Ramey, C. T., Bryant, D. M., Wasik, B. H., Sparling, J. J., Fendt, K. H., & LaVange, L. M. (1992).
Infant Health and Development Program for low birth weight, premature infants: Program
elements, family participation, and child intelligence. Pediatrics, 3, 454–465.
Reid, M. J., Webster-Stratton, C., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2001). Parent training in Head Start: A
comparison of program response among African American, Asian American, Caucasian,
and Hispanic mothers. Prevention Science, 2, 209–227.
Roggman, L. A., Boyce, L. K., Cook, G. A., & Jump. V. K. (2001). Inside home visits: A collabo-
rative look at process and quality. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 16, 53–71.
Roth, A., & Fonagy, P. (Eds.). (1996). What works for whom: A critical review of psychotherapy re-
search. New York: Guilford Press.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1988). Far from home: An experimental evaluation of the
Mother–Child Home Program in Bermuda. Child Development, 59, 531–543.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental de-
signs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Slaughter, D. T. (1983). Early intervention and its effects on maternal and child development.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48(4, Serial No. 202).
Slavin, R. E. (2002). Evidence-based education policies: Transforming educational practice
and research. Educational Researcher, 31, 15–21.
Slavin, R. E. (2004). Education research can and must address “what works” questions. Educa-
tional Researcher, 33, 27–28.
Sparling, J., Lewis, I., Ramey, C. T., Wasik, B. H., Bryant, D. M., & LaVange, L. M. (1991). Part-
ners: A curriculum to help premature, low birthweight infants get off to a good start. Topics
in Early Childhood Special Education, 11, 36–55.
St.Pierre, R. G., & Layzer, J. I. (1998). Improving the life chances of children in poverty: As-
sumptions and what we have learned. Social Policy Report, Society for Research in Child Develop-
ment, 12, 1–25.
12. PARENTING INTERVENTIONS 373
Thompson, B., Diamond, K., McWilliam, R., Snyder, P., & Snyder, S. (2005). Evaluating the
quality of evidence from correlational research for evidence-based practice. Exceptional
Children, 71, 181–194.
Traub, J. (2002, November 10). Does it work? The New York Times Education Life, pp.
4A24–4A27, 4A30–4A31.
Travers, J., Irwin, N., & Nauta, M. (1981). The culture of a social program: An ethnographic study of
the Child and Family Resource Program. Report prepared for the Administration for Children,
Youth and Families. Cambridge, MA: Abt Associates.
Travers, J., Nauta, M., & Irwin, N. (1982). The effects of a social program: Final report of the Child and
Family Resource Program’s infant–toddler component. Cambridge, MA: Abt Associates.
Valentine, J. C., & Cooper, H. (2003). What works clearinghouse study design and implementation de-
vice. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Wagner, M. M., & Clayton, S. L. (1999). The Parents as Teachers Program: Results from two
demonstrations. The Future of Children, 9, 91–115.
Wagner, M., Spiker, D., Linn, M. I., Gerlach-Downie, S., & Hernandez, F. (2003). Dimensions
of parental engagement in home visiting programs: Exploratory study. Topics in Early Child-
hood Special Education, 23, 171–187.
Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct problems in Head Start children:
Strengthening parent competencies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66,
715–730.
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Hammond, M. (2001). Preventing conduct problems, and
promoting social competence: A parent and teacher training partnership in Head Start.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 283–302.
Weiss, C. H. (1995). Nothing as practical as a good theory: Exploring theory-based evaluation
for comprehensive community initiatives for children and families. In J. P. Connell, A. C.
Kubisch, L. B. Schorr, & C. H. Weiss (Eds.), New approaches to evaluating community initia-
tives: Concepts, methods, and contexts (pp. 65–92). Washington, DC: The Aspen Institute.
Worobey, J., & Brazelton, T. (1986). Experimenting with the family in the newborn period: A
commentary. Child Development, 57, 1298–1300.
This page intentionally left blank
IV
PARENTAL BEHAVIOR
AND CHILDREN’S
DEVELOPMENT
This page intentionally left blank
13
Research on Parental
Socialization of Child Outcomes:
Current Controversies
and Future Directions
Lynn Okagaki
Purdue University
Tom Luster
Michigan State University
377
378 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
In the 1980s and early 1990s, findings from behavioral genetics research
were cited to raise questions about the extent to which parenting practices
contribute to individual differences in personality and cognitive abilities. For
example, researchers using studies of twins and adopted siblings concluded
that shared environment (experiences that are shared by people who live to-
gether) accounts for little if any (0–10%) of the variance in personality out-
comes (Dunn & Plomin, 1990; Plomin & Daniels, 1987). In addition, studies
382 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
of adult identical twins who were reared together or separately indicated that
the correlations for personality tended to be around .50 for both types of
twins (Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal, & Tellegen, 1990). In other words,
identical twins who were reared in the same household tended to be no more
alike in terms of the personality characteristics that were measured than
identical twins who were reared by different parents.
If identical twins who share 100% of their genes and are reared in the
same families are different in terms of personality, behavioral geneticists ar-
gued that personality differences among these twins must be attributed to
nonshared environmental influences (experiences that are unique to indi-
viduals) (Plomin & Daniels, 1987). Unique experiences in a variety of set-
tings could contribute to these differences in personality; differential treat-
ment by parents, siblings, peers, teachers, and other extra-familial adults
could contribute to differences in personality, as could chance events such
as accidents, illnesses, or fortuitous opportunities (being in the right place
at the right time). Some behavioral geneticists concluded that parenting
may contribute to individual differences in personality among children but
however parenting affects children in the same family, it seems to make
them different rather than alike (Dunn & Plomin, 1991; Plomin & Daniels,
1987). Critics of parenting research argued that if parents have different
styles of parenting (e.g., authoritative, authoritarian, or permissive) that
they use with their children and some parenting styles produce more desir-
able outcomes in children than others, why are children who are reared by
the same parents not more alike in the area of personality (Harris, 1995,
1998)?
Whereas researchers were reporting that monozygotic and dizygotic twin
studies indicated that genetic differences among the twins in the samples
typically accounted for less than half of the variance in personality out-
comes (McCartney, Harris, & Bernieri, 1990), studies of cognitive out-
comes generally obtained stronger estimates of heritability effects, and esti-
mates of heritability increased as individuals get older (McCartney, Harris,
& Bernieri, 1990; McGue, Bouchard, Iacono, & Lykken, 1993). For exam-
ple, estimates of heritability in children were around .45 (Neisser et al.,
1996). In contrast, McGue and his colleagues (1993) estimated heritability
of IQ to be approximately .75 among adults (although Neisser and col-
leagues, 1996, observed that adults from the lowest socioeconomic groups
were under-represented in these samples; hence variation due to contex-
tual factors might be less than what would be found in the entire popula-
tion). With respect to cognitive abilities, most scientists agreed that genetics
plays a significant role in accounting for individual differences in cognitive
abilities (Neisser et al., 1996). Indeed, some suggested that further research
to determine the heritability of cognitive traits would make little contribu-
tion to science (McCartney et al., 1990).
13. PARENTAL SOCIALIZATION 383
included a large proportion of families that were below the poverty level
at the time of the study, allowed the researchers to examine the interac-
tion of SES with genotype, shared environment, and non-shared environ-
ment. At age 7, children’s IQ scores (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children) were obtained. Comparing the extent to which the traditional
main effects model (i.e., only genotype, shared environment, and non-
shared environment) explained variation in IQ scores with an interaction
model (i.e., including the main effects and the interactions of each com-
ponent with SES), the researchers found that the interaction model better
accounted for the variation in child outcomes. Specifically, at higher SES
levels, shared and non-shared environments account for very little of the
variation in IQ scores; however, when SES is low, the opposite is true. To
compare their study to typical analyses, the researchers split the sample
above and below the median SES. For the higher SES group, 72% of the
variation in IQ scores was explained by genotype; shared environment ex-
plained only 15%. In contrast, for the low SES group, genotype accounted
for only 10% of the variation in IQ scores; whereas, shared environment
explained 58%.
Turkheimer and his colleagues (2003) were careful to observe that varia-
tion in SES does not simply reflect an environmental difference, but also
captures genetic differences between parents. Because the twins in this
study lived in the same household, the analyses used in this study cannot
tease apart the environmental and genetic aspects of SES. Nonetheless, the
study supports the contention that we cannot naively assume that herita-
bility is constant across environmental contexts. Why might environmental
factors be more important and genetic factors less important in some con-
texts than in others? Bronfenbrenner posited that in more advantaged en-
vironments heritability effects would be stronger because individuals would
be closer to fully actualizing their potential (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994;
Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998).
Furthermore, as many have observed, even when heritability is high,
there is reason to believe that environment makes a difference (e.g., Bron-
fenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; Ceci, Rosenblum, de Bruyn, & Lee, 1993). In
adoption studies, for example, finding a correlation between biological
mother and adoptive child does not mean that the environment did not in-
fluence the child. In the French adoption study reported by Schiff and his
colleagues (Schiff et al., 1978), children from working class mothers were
adopted early into upper-middle-class families and were later compared to
their non-adopted half-siblings (i.e., both sets of children had the same
biological mothers) and to selected samples of children from the general
population. The average IQ score of the adopted children was 110.6, as
compared to 94.5, the average IQ score of their non-adopted siblings. Com-
parisons of school failure rates indicated that the adopted children did
13. PARENTAL SOCIALIZATION 385
much better than their non-adopted siblings and were much like other chil-
dren from upper-middle-class homes.
For purposes of providing evidence that parenting makes a difference,
the French adoption study has an obvious flaw. Being raised in upper-
middle-class families meant that the adopted children were exposed to en-
vironmental influences other than just the parents that may have contrib-
uted to the differences in IQ scores. But consider the classic longitudinal
adoption study of Skodak and Skeels (1949). They showed that the correla-
tions between adopted children and their biological mothers, who did not
live together for more than a few months, were significant from early child-
hood through adolescence (rs = .28 to .44); these correlations provide evi-
dence of genetic influence on intellectual development. IQ tests were not
given to the adoptive mothers but their level of education was used as an in-
dicator of their intellectual ability. Correlations between the education
level of the adoptive mothers and the IQ scores of the children hovered
near zero, suggesting little influence of shared environment. Yet from age 2
through age 13 (the last time the children were tested) their IQ scores
tended to be about 20 points higher than the IQ scores of their biological
mothers, and their IQ scores tended to be about a standard deviation above
the mean for the population. Although the higher IQ scores of the children
may not be due solely to the parenting they received from their adoptive
parents, the evidence suggests that their adoptive parents helped put them
on a positive developmental trajectory in early childhood and the children
continued to do well after they entered the world of schools and peer
groups in middle childhood and adolescence. The lack of relation between
the adoptive mothers’ education and children’s IQ scores may be due to a
restricted range on the maternal education variable; on average the adop-
tive mothers had high levels of education relative to other people their age
who lived in the same area. Thus, in this study, the correlational evidence
suggests little or no effect of shared environment on intellectual develop-
ment, but the higher mean scores of the children suggests a substantial en-
vironmental effect; the fact that the higher mean scores were evident when
the children were very young and spent most of their time at home is consis-
tent with the view that parenting played a role in these outcomes.
Where does all of this research leave us in terms of understanding the
role of parental socialization on child outcomes? First, although genotype
has in many cases a substantial influence on development, this does not
mean that environmental factors in general and parental socialization in
particular do not play an important and significant role in development.
Even in studies showing a strong heritability effect, there can be an impor-
tant environmental effect (Schiff et al., 1978; Skodak & Skeels, 1949). Sec-
ond, quantitative genetic data on the degree to which siblings are alike does
not definitively demonstrate the relative effects of shared and nonshared
386 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
Many researchers have argued that peers play an important role in develop-
ment (e.g., Berndt, 1999, 2002; Ladd & Burgess, 2001). Building in part on
this research, Judith Rich Harris (1995) proposed the Group Socialization
Theory in which she argued that the variance in child outcomes that is not
explained by genetic differences among individuals can largely be ex-
plained by experiences in peer groups during childhood and adolescence.
According to Harris (1995), “children would develop into the same sort of
adults if we left them in their homes, their schools, their neighborhoods,
and their cultural or subcultural groups, but switched all the parents
around” (p. 461).
Harris (1995, 1998) argued that behavior is very context specific. Chil-
dren learn how to behave inside the home and outside the home, and the
expectations regarding appropriate behavior may be quite different in vari-
ous contexts. Children’s behavior inside the home is influenced consider-
ably by their parents, but children learn how to behave outside the home
from their peer groups. Outside the home, children associate with a peer
group comprised of individuals who tend to be similar to the child on such
characteristics as age, gender, ethnicity, attitudes and interests, and they
tend to adopt the attitudes and behavior of their peer group.
Children want to fit in with and be accepted by their peers. Harris (1995,
1998, 2000, 2002) asserted that if what children learn at home is not ac-
cepted by the peer group, children are likely to adopt the attitudes and be-
havior of the peer group outside the home. For example, if an immigrant
child speaks his parents’ language at home and English in the peer group,
eventually English will become the preferred language of the child. Like-
wise, if parents encourage androgynous behavior on the part of their chil-
dren, and a same-sex peer group has strong rules about behaving in sex-
typed ways, children should display sex-typed behavior outside the home.
Thus, children are socialized in terms of how to behave outside their home
by the peer group they identify with, and what they learn in the peer group
continues to influence their behavior into adulthood. Socialization outside
the home becomes a part of their adult personality (Harris, 1995, p. 467).
With respect to intellectual development, she theorized that children’s atti-
tudes toward school and intellectual achievement are shaped by their
peers. If a child is a member of a group of children who like school and do
13. PARENTAL SOCIALIZATION 387
well in school, he or she will also like school and do well in school. Children
conform to the norms of their peer group.
If the influence of children’s peers is as strong as Harris (1995) pur-
ported, is there any role for parents to play in Group Socialization Theory?
Even though Harris asserted that parenting practices have no influence on
individual differences in adulthood, she acknowledged that parents have
some influence on their children. For example, children need parents or
parental surrogates to meet their most basic survival needs by providing
food, clothing, shelter, protection and other necessities. Parents set and en-
force the rules for how children behave inside the home influencing what
children do when they are at home. If a parent teaches a child skills and at-
titudes at home that are valued by the peer group, these skills and attitudes
can be helpful to the child outside the home. Parents can also influence
outcomes that the peer group does not care about one way or another (e.g.,
political party preference, adult careers, religious beliefs). According to
Harris, the relationship between parent and child can influence how happy
the child is or how miserable the child is especially in the home context, but
these dyadic relationships typically do not have long-term effects on person-
ality or behavior outside the home. Finally, Harris acknowledged that par-
ents can influence the child’s peer group composition by determining
where the family lives and what schools the children attend and can influ-
ence how peers treat their children by the clothes they buy (cool or not
cool), the names they give their children (common or unusual names), and
things they do to make their children relatively attractive (e.g., braces, acne
treatments, hair styling). Nonetheless, the primary social influence on chil-
dren’s development is their peers.
In response to Harris’ proposal, several researchers have written cri-
tiques of Group Socialization Theory and taken the position that Harris’
contention that parents have relatively little influence on their children’s
long-term development is too extreme (Collins et al., 2000; Vandell, 2000).
Those who have been critical of Harris acknowledge that peers exert con-
siderable influence on the behavior of children and adolescents, but they
believe that parental practices contribute to individual differences in chil-
dren’s outcomes as well. In the following section, we draw upon these
critiques of Group Socialization Theory and other published studies to
summarize some key studies that provide evidence that parents play an im-
portant role in the development of their children in various domains.
behaviors than their peers and many more problems than children who
had been adopted into supportive homes. The study by Hodges and Tizard
is interesting because the children being compared had different experi-
ences early in life (institutional rearing vs. rearing in a family) and later in
life (adopted vs. restored). The evidence from both parts of the study sug-
gests that the best outcomes for children are found when they are cared for
by supportive parents who really want them. It is also important to point out
that because of differences in family income, the peer groups of the
adopted and restored children may have also been quite different. Adop-
tive children may have fewer problems than restored children both because
of their adoptive parents and the peers they encounter in more affluent
neighborhoods. Both parents and peer groups, in combination with ge-
netic predispositions, may play significant roles in whether or not children
develop behavior problems.
Neglected infants are another group of children who do not get enough
of what parents are supposed to provide—affection, touch, attention, sen-
sory stimulation, a safe environment, and other basic necessities. Neglected
children show a range of problem outcomes in the cognitive, language, so-
cial, and emotional domains (Perry, 2002). Evidence reviewed by Perry
(2002) shows that severe, global neglect in infancy has a negative effect on
brain development. When neglected children were placed in more sup-
portive foster homes, they showed some degree of recovery, but the amount
of recovery depended upon the length of time that the children had lived
in deprived circumstances. Children who were rescued from their neglect-
ful homes at an earlier age showed greater recovery than those who were
older when the neglectful circumstances were identified. Neuroimaging
techniques suggest that the experiences in Romanian orphanages also af-
fected the brain development of these children (Chugani et al., 2001).
Thus, neglect during infancy seems to fundamentally affect brain develop-
ment; the prospects for recovery appear to be limited for children who ex-
perienced severe neglect for the first two years of their lives.
Animal studies provide experimental evidence of parental influence on
their offspring (O’Connor, 2002); unlike humans, rhesus monkeys can be
randomly assigned to their parents. In 1987, Suomi published an initial re-
port of a rhesus monkey cross-fostering project in which infant rhesus mon-
keys were placed within 96 hours of birth either with foster mothers who
had displayed low levels of punitive behaviors and high levels of nurturing
behaviors with their own offspring or with foster mothers who had dis-
played moderate levels of punitive behaviors with their own offspring. Half
of the infant monkeys were genetically predisposed to be highly reactive;
half were genetically predisposed to be relatively calm. In addition, half of
the foster mothers within each group were highly reactive monkeys; half
were not. During their first 6 months, the infant monkeys were raised by
390 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
their foster mothers in their home cages and were only separated from the
mother once each week for a brief 20-minute testing period. Although the
initial report of this research was based on a sample of 12 dyads,1 analyses of
the observations of the dyads during the first 6 months indicated that while
the infant monkeys were in their cage with their foster mother, the best pre-
dictor of their behavior was whether the foster mother was a punitive or a
nurturing mother. At 6 months, the infant monkeys were separated from
their foster mothers for brief periods of times. During separations, the best
predictor of their behavior was whether or not the infant monkey was in the
highly reactive group. At 9 months, the infant monkeys were permanently
separated from their foster mother. Preliminary observations suggested
that the highly reactive infant monkeys who were placed with nurturing
mothers were able to become dominant members of their peer groups, but
that highly reactive monkeys reared by highly reactive or punitive mothers
may not do as well. These observations are consistent with research on hu-
man children that found that children with certain temperamental quali-
ties respond best to certain parenting styles (Kochanska, 1995, 1997).
Suomi (2002) also studied the consequences of growing up without a
mother and other adults on rhesus monkeys who were reared with their
peers. Peer reared monkeys showed less exploratory behavior than mon-
keys raised by their mothers; monkeys reared by their mothers use the
mother as a secure base for exploration much as securely attached human
infants use their mothers. Peer reared monkeys also showed less developed
play behavior and tended to be less competent play partners than monkeys
reared by their mothers. These less competent peer reared monkeys tended
to end up in the bottom of the dominance hierarchies of their social
groups. Peer reared male monkeys were found to be more impulsive and
aggressive than males reared by their mothers. Peer-reared females were
more likely than those reared by their mothers to exhibit neglectful or abu-
sive behavior with their first-born offspring. Females reared in social isola-
tion were even less competent in the parenting role than peer reared fe-
males.
Turning to research on human parenting, we consider a particular hy-
pothesis offered by Harris (1995) that dyadic relationships in general have
little or no long-term influence on behavior outside of the contexts where
the relationships occur. That is, that dyadic relationships, such as parent–
child, teacher–pupil, or best friend–best friend, do not have a lasting influ-
ence on personality and rarely affect behavior in other contexts unless the
peer group also endorses these behaviors; over the long term, behavior and
personality are affected by peer groups (three or more people). Vandell
(2000) addressed this issue proposing that the multiple dyadic relation-
1
1Number of dyads based on personal communication with S. Suomi, November 30, 2004.
13. PARENTAL SOCIALIZATION 391
ships that children have are important because they meet different develop-
mental needs. Vandell (2000) wrote:
reading with their young child (Whitehurst et al., 1988). In this study,
mothers were randomly assigned to intervention or control conditions.
Those receiving the one-month home-based intervention were taught to use
specific types of questions, to elaborate on their child’s responses, and other
strategies to actively engage their child in the book reading activity and to in-
crease the child’s language development. Not only were the researchers able
to confirm that their intervention changed mothers’ reading behaviors but
were able to detect an effect of the intervention on standardized measures of
children’s language development. That is, when children were tested by a
member of the research team on several standardized measures of language
development, children of mothers in the intervention group behaved differ-
ently from children of mothers in the control group.
The final example of the effects of a parent intervention program on the
behavior of the child comes from David Olds’ work on a home visitation
parent intervention program (Olds et al., 1998). In the late 1970s, young
women who were less than 25 weeks pregnant with no previous live birth,
unmarried or from low income homes were randomly assigned to interven-
tion or comparison conditions. The mothers in the comparison conditions
received regular prenatal and well child care. Mothers in one intervention
group received home visits from nurses during their pregnancy (on aver-
age, nine visits); mothers in the second intervention group received home
visits from nurses during their pregnancy and through the child’s second
birthday (on average, 23 visits). During the home visits, nurses provided
guidance on health-related behaviors, parenting, and maternal personal
development (e.g., family planning, education, jobs). When the offspring
were 15, the investigators conducted a follow-up study collecting data on
the adolescents. Significant differences were obtained between the adoles-
cents whose mothers were in the intervention groups and adolescents
whose mothers were in the comparison condition on a number of variables
representing negative and antisocial behavior. For instance, adolescents
whose mothers received home visits reported fewer arrests and convictions
and fewer violations of probation. These data were corroborated by court
records for those adolescents who had not moved away from the county in
which the study took place; adolescents whose mothers received home visits
through the child’s infancy were less likely to have been adjudicated as a
person in need of supervision. Differences between treatment and compar-
ison groups were more consistent among adolescents whose mothers were
most at risk (both low-SES and unmarried) at the time of intervention.
Does parenting affect children’s behavior outside the parent-child rela-
tionship? The data from these four intervention studies indicates that it
does. Given the importance of this question, however, the data supporting
this conclusion is weaker than it ought to be. Fortunately, we do know how
to strengthen research designs so that we can increase knowledge of par-
394 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
Future Directions
enced severe deprivation early in their lives but were later adopted into
families in Great Britain and in Canada and have been compared with chil-
dren who were adopted in early infancy.
In addition, there is a need for more research using experimental and
quasi-experimental designs to test specific and competing hypotheses. Ulti-
mately to determine if specific parenting practices are causally related to
specific child outcomes, we will need more experimental studies in which
parents are randomly assigned to conditions in which they are taught differ-
ent parenting skills or information about parenting and both parent behav-
ior and child behavior are observed before and after the intervention. (see
Powell, chap. 12, this volume). Researchers need to determine whether an
intervention changes specific parenting practices and in turn, whether
there is an effect of the intervention on child outcomes that is mediated
through the parenting practices. In particular, we need studies that assess
the impact of specific parenting practices on measures of child outcomes
assessed outside the home.
Furthermore, to facilitate studies of parent interventions, better meas-
ures of parenting practices are needed. Global measures of parenting, such
as measures of parenting styles, are not likely to be sensitive enough to cap-
ture the effects of specific parenting practices on child outcomes. Re-
searchers need to consider measures of parental behavior that are concep-
tually related to specific child outcomes. For example, in the previously
described experimental evaluation of the Dialogic Reading program
(Whitehurst et al., 1988), the parenting measures were specific measures of
maternal behavior while the mother and child engaged in a book reading
activity and included the frequency with which mothers labeled pictures,
asked specific types of questions, and elaborated on what the child said.
These behaviors, which mothers in the intervention group were taught and
subsequently exhibited, are theoretically related to children’s language de-
velopment. To assess the effects of parenting on child outcomes, we need to
focus on specific parenting behaviors that are conceptually related to the
targeted child outcomes.
Finally, our view of the challenges to parental socialization research from
behavioral geneticists and peer socialization theorists is that such chal-
lenges have made an important contribution to the literature by highlight-
ing some of the limitations of the parental socialization research. Socializa-
tion researchers (including the authors of this chapter) have tended to
interpret the results of their study as parental influence on children with-
out ruling out alternative explanations for the findings. Challenges to the
rigor and interpretation of our work should push those of us doing re-
search on parenting to adopt and develop more rigorous research designs,
including intervention designs, studies of parenting in families in which the
398 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
children are adopted (i.e., biologically unrelated to the parents), and stud-
ies of parenting of more than one child in a family. It is through such effort
that the field will move forward.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Baumrind, D. (1993). The average expectable environment is not good enough: A response to
Scarr. Child Development, 64, 1299–1317.
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psy-
chological Review, 75, 81–95.
Berndt, T. J. (1999). Friends’ influence on students’ adjustment to school. Educational Psycholo-
gist, 34, 15–28.
Berndt, T. J. (2002). Friendship quality and social development. Current Directions in Psychologi-
cal Sciences, 11, 7–10.
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting: 2nd ed., Vol. 4. Social conditions and applied
parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bouchard, T., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of hu-
man psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science, 250,
223–228.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1972). Is 80% of intelligence genetically determined? In U. Bronfen-
brenner (Ed.), Influences on human development (pp. 118–127). Hinsdale, IL: Dryden Press
Inc.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). Developmental research, public policy, and the ecology of child-
hood. Child Development, 45, 1–5.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Recent advances in research on the ecology of human develop-
ment. In R. K. Silbereisen, K. Eyforth, G. Rudinger (Eds.), Development as action in context:
Problem behavior and normal youth development (pp. 286–309). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (2000). Ecological systems theory. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 129–133). New York: Oxford University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized: A bioecological
model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W.
Damon (Editor-in-chief) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. I. Theo-
retical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 993–1028). New York: John Wiley & Sons,
Inc.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A., & Poulton, R.
(2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297,
851–854.
13. PARENTAL SOCIALIZATION 399
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Morgan, J., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., Arseneault, L., Tully, L., et al.
(2004). Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behavior problems:
Using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral devel-
opment. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 149–161.
Ceci, S. J., Rosenblum, T., de Bruyn, E., & Lee, D. Y. (1993). A bio-ecological model of intellec-
tual development: Moving beyond h2. In R. J. Sternberg & E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelli-
gence, heredity, and environment (pp. 303–322). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Chisholm, K. (1998). A three-year follow-up of attachment and indiscriminate friendliness in
children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Child Development, 69, 1092–1106.
Chisholm, K., Carter, M., Ames, E. W., & Morrison, S. J. (1995). Attachment security and indis-
criminately friendly behavior in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development
and Psychopathology, 7, 283–294.
Chugani, H. T., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Juhasz, C., Nagy, F., & Chugani, D. C. (2001). Local
brain functional activity following early deprivation: A study of post-institutionalized Roma-
nian orphans. Neuroimage, 14, 1290–1301.
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., Bornstein, M. H. (2000).
Contemporary research on parenting: The case of nature and nurture. American Psycholo-
gist, 55, 218–232.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Charles Schribner’s Sons.
Condry, J., & Condry, S. (1976). Sex differences: A study of the eye of the beholder. Child Devel-
opment, 47(3), 812–819.
Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). What an intervention design reveals about how parents
affect their children’s academic achievement and behavior problems. In J. G. Borkowski,
S. L. Ramey, & M. Bristol-Power (Eds.), Parenting and the child’s world: Influences on academic,
intellectual, and social-emotional development (pp. 75–98). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates.
DeBaryshe, D. B., Patterson, G. R., & Capaldi, D. M. (1993). A performance model for aca-
demic achievement in early adolescent boys. Developmental Psychology, 29, 795–804.
Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1990). Separate lives: Why siblings are so different. New York: Basic Books.
Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1991). Why are siblings so different? The significance of differences in
sibling experiences within the family. Family Process, 30, 271–283.
English, D. (1998). The extent and consequences of child maltreatment. The Future of Children,
8(1), 39–51.
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment: A group socialization theory of develop-
ment. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption. New York: The Free Press.
Harris, J. R. (2000). Socialization, personality development, and the child’s environments:
Comment on Vandell (2000). Developmental Psychology, 36, 711–723.
Harris, J. R. (2002). Beyond the nurture assumption: Testing hypotheses about the child’s en-
vironment. In J. G. Borkowski, S. L. Ramey, & M. Bristol-Power (Eds.), Parenting and the
Child’s World: Influences on academic, intellectual, and social-emotional development (pp. 3–20).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Harter, S. (1988). Developmental processes in the construction of self. In T. D. Yawkey & J. E.
Johnson (Eds.), Integrative processes and socialization: Early to middle childhood (pp. 45–78).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Harter, S. (1990). Causes, correlates and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span
perspective. In R. Sternberg & J. Kolligian (Eds.), Competence considered (pp. 67–97). New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) &
Nancy Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and person-
ality development (5th ed., pp. 553–618). New York: Wiley.
400 OKAGAKI AND LUSTER
Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989). IQ and behavioral adjustment of ex-institutional adolescents.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 53–75.
Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mother’s discipline, and the security of at-
tachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597–615.
Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different tempera-
ments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.
Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (2001). Do relational risks and protective factors moderate the
linkages between childhood aggression and early psychological and school adjustment?
Child Development, 72, 1579–1601.
LeVine, R. A. (1988). Human parental care: Universal goals, cultural strategies, and individual
behavior. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. A. Levine, P. M. Miller, & M. M. West (Vol. Eds.),
New directions for child development: Parental behavior in diverse societies (Vol. 40, pp. 3–11). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Levy, F., & Hay, D. (2001). Attention, genes, and ADHD. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates.
Loehlin, J. C., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Reiss, D. (2003). The behavior genetics of personality and
the NEAD study. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 373–387.
McAdoo, H. P. (Ed.). (1993). Family ethnicity: Strength in diversity. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Pub-
lications, Inc.
McCartney, K., Harris, M. J., & Bernieri, F. (1990). Growing up and growing apart: A develop-
mental meta-analysis of twin studies. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 226–237.
McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Behavioral genetics of
cognitive ability: A life-span perspective. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nur-
ture, and psychology (pp. 59–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern,
D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns
and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51(2), 77–101.
O’Connor, T. G. (2002). Annotation: The ‘effects’ of parenting reconsidered: Findings, chal-
lenges, and applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 555–572.
O’Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Keaveney, L., Kreppner, J. M., and the English and
Romanian Adoptees Study Team (2000a). The effects of severe privation on cognitive com-
petence: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Child Development, 71, 376–390.
O’Connor, C., Rutter, M., English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team (2000b). Attachment
disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: Extension and longitudinal follow-
up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 703–712.
Ogbu, J. U. (1981). Origins of human competence: A cultural-ecological perspective. Child De-
velopment, 52, 413–429.
Olds, D., Henderson, C. R., Jr., Cole, R., Eckenrode, J., Kitzman, H., Luckey, D., Pettitt, L.,
Sidora, K., Morris, P., & Powers, J. (1998). Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on
children’s criminal and antisocial behavior: 15-year follow-up of a randomized controlled
trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280(14), 1238–1244.
Perry, B. D. (2002). Childhood experience and the expression of genetic potential: What
childhood neglect tells us about nature and nurture. Brain and Mind, 3, 79–100.
Plassman, B. L., & Breitner, J. C. S. (1996). Recent advances in the genetics of Alzheimer’s dis-
ease and vascular dementia with an emphasis on gene-environment interactions. Journal of
the American Geriatric Society, 44, 1242–1250.
Plomin, R. (1997). Identifying genes for cognitive abilities and disabilities. In R. J. Sternberg &
E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 89–104). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from each
other? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 1–16.
13. PARENTAL SOCIALIZATION 401
Rowe, D. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experiences, and behavior. New York: Guilford
Press.
Rowe, D. (2002). What twin and adoption studies reveal about parenting. In J. G. Borkowski, S.
L. Ramey, & M. Bristol-Power (Eds.), Parenting and the child’s world: Influences on academic, in-
tellectual, and social-emotional development (pp. 21–34). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates.
Rubin, J. Z., Provenzano, F. J., & Luria, Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder: Parents’ views on
sex of newborns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44, 47–55.
Rutter, M. (2002). Nature, nurture, and development: From evangelism through science to-
ward policy and practice. Child Development, 73, 1–21.
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990’s: Development and individual differ-
ences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.
Scarr, S. (1996). How people make their own environments: Implications for parents and pol-
icy makers. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2(2), 204–228.
Scarr, S. (1997). Behavior-genetic and socialization theories of intelligence: Truce and recon-
ciliation. In R. J. Sternberg & E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment
(pp. 3–41). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schiff, M., Duyme, M., Dumaret, A., Stewart, J., Tomkiewicz, S., & Feingold, J. (1978). Intellec-
tual status of working-class children adopted early into upper-middle-class families. Science,
200(4349), 1503–1504.
Skodak, M., & Skeels, H. (1949). A final follow-up of one hundred adopted children. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 75, 85–125.
Spitz, R. A. (1945). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early
childhood. In A. Freud et al. (Eds.), The psychoanalytic study of the child (Vol. 1, pp. 53–74).
New York: International Universities Press.
Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family environments for esti-
mates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption studies. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 125, 392–409.
Suomi, S. J. (1987). Genetic and maternal contributions to individual differences in rhesus
monkey biobehavioral development. In N. A. Krasnegor, E. M. Blass, M. A. Hofer, & W. P.
Smotherman (Eds.), Perinatal development: A psychological perspective (pp. 397–419). New
York: Academic Press.
Suomi, S. J. (2002). Parents, peers, and the process of socialization. In J. G. Borkowski, S. L.
Ramey, & M. Bristol-Power (Eds.), Parenting and the child’s world: Influences on academic, intel-
lectual, and social-emotional development (pp. 265–279). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates.
Tizard, B., & Hodges, J. (1978). The effect of early institutional rearing on the development of
eight-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 99–118.
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 9(5), 160–164.
Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeco-
nomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6),
623–628.
van den Boom, D. C. (1995). Do first-year intervention effects endure? Follow-up during
toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants. Child Development, 66, 1798–1816.
Vandell, D. L. (2000). Parents, peer groups, and other socializing influences. Developmental Psy-
chology, 36, 699–710.
Whitehurst, G. J., Falco, F. L., Lonigan, C. J., Fischel, J. E., DeBaryshe, B. D., Valdez-Menchaca,
M. C., et al. (1988). Accelerating language development through picture book reading. De-
velopmental Psychology, 24(4), 552–559.
This page intentionally left blank
About the Authors
Jay Belsky, Ph.D., is Founding Director of the Institute for the Study of
Children, Families and Social Issues and Professor of Psychology at Birk-
beck College, University of London. He earned his doctorate in Human
Development and Family Studies at Cornell University, served on the fac-
ulty of Penn State University for 21 years, and was awarded the title of Dis-
tinguished Professor. He currently serves as Research Director on the Na-
tional Evaluation of Sure Start in England. He is an internationally
recognized expert in the field of child development and family studies and
is the author of more than 200 scientific articles and chapters and the au-
thor of several books.
403
404 ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Robin L. Harwood got her Ph.D. in 1991 from Yale University. She is cur-
rently a Professor in the Department of Human Development and Family
Studies at Texas Tech University. She has received several major grants
from NIH to study cultural differences in childrearing beliefs and practices,
and is the author of numerous journal articles, as well as a book, Culture and
Attachment: Perceptions of the Child in Context.
Kathleen Kostelny is Research Associate with the Eikson Institute for Ad-
vanced Study in Child Development in Chicago. She serves as an interna-
tional consultant on issue of children and youth involved in violence and
trauma in war zones around the world. She is co-author of Children in Dan-
ger (1991) and No Place to Be a Child (1992).
Birgit Leyendecker got her doctoral degree in 1991 from the University of
Osnabrueck. She is currently a Research Fellow at the University in
Bochum and she is on the faculty of the University of Dortmund. She is au-
thor of numerous journal articles. Her research interests include cultural
perspectives on child development and parenting; cultural and psychologi-
cal adaptation of immigrant children and their families, and linking quali-
tative and quantitative research.
lescent mothers and their children; risk and resilience; and influences on
parental behavior. His latest research includes: a 9-year longitudinal study
of adolescent mothers and their children in Flint, Michigan, and a study of
the Sudanese refugees known as the “Lost Boys” who lived for most of their
lives in refugee camps in Ethiopia and Kenya without contact with their par-
ents.
Helen Bittmann Sysko, Ph.D. received her Masters of Science in Child De-
velopment and doctorate in Counseling Psychology from the University of
Pittsburgh. As a clinician, she has worked with children and adults in outpa-
tient community mental health programs in child care centers and urban
drug and alcohol treatment facilities. Her research interests include home-
less mothers and family reunification intervention.
411
412 AUTHOR INDEX
Black, J. E., 25, 30 Brassard, J., 236, 237, 238, 252, 259, 271
Black, M. M., 92, 99 Braungart, J. M., 157, 171
Blackwell, B., 356, 369 Braungart-Rieker, J., 162, 171
Blasco, P., 182, 197 Braver, S., 117, 142
Blevins-Knabe, B., 15, 32 Brazelton, T., 360, 373
Bliesener, T., 310, 317 Bream, V., 131, 138
Block, A., 221, 227 Breitner, J. C. S., 395, 400
Block, J., 221, 227 Brewster, K., 76, 98
Blue, J., 214, 228 Briggs-Gowan, M., 37, 62
Blum, J. S., 47, 62 Broadhurst, D. D., 300, 318
Blyth, D. A., 308, 317 Broderick, J. E., 224, 232
Bø, I., 254, 271 Brody, G., 40, 43, 62, 67
Bogat, G. A., 258, 272 Brody, G. H., 22, 28, 114, 137, 215, 224,
Bohman, M., 50, 63 227, 304, 306, 315
Boissevain, J., 251, 271 Brody, N., 382, 400
Bojczyk, K., 357, 372 Brodzinsky, D. M., 112, 136
Boles, A. J., 205, 228 Bronfenbrenner, U., xi, xiv, xv, 3, 28, 150,
Bolger, N., 285, 286, 294 153, 159, 172, 195, 196, 197, 205,
Bolton, F., 84, 97 206, 224, 227, 276, 287, 294, 297,
Bonney, J. F., 113, 114, 136 308, 311, 312, 315, 377, 378, 383,
Bontempo, R., 9, 33 384, 394, 398
Boodoo, G., 382, 400 Brooks-Gunn, J., 17, 28, 46, 47, 62, 68, 78,
Boomsma, I., 51, 52, 70 80, 81, 82, 85, 91, 93, 98, 100, 101,
Booth, A., 113, 136 297, 299, 303, 306, 316, 317, 320,
Booth, C. L., 348, 356, 363, 369 323, 324, 338, 344, 348, 350, 351,
Borkowski, J., 79, 80, 81, 82, 84, 86, 89, 91, 354, 359, 361, 363, 365, 369, 371
97, 101 Brott, A., 103, 113, 127, 133, 134, 135,
Borkowski, J. G., 88, 89, 90, 100, 101 142
Bornstead-Bruns, M., 5, 30 Brown, A., 40, 62
Bornstein, M. H., 5, 8, 9, 28, 29, 116, 136, Brown, B. B., 6, 33
147, 151, 171, 297, 315, 322, 338, Brown, G. W., 49, 66
379, 387, 388, 398, 399 Brown, J., 26, 28
Borthwick-Duffy, S., 117, 142 Brown, M., 18, 28
Boruch, R., 349, 371 Brown, M. M., 162, 171
Bosch, J. D., 52, 68 Brownlee, J., 109, 141
Bost, K. K., 47, 62, 242, 255, 271 Brunner, J., 221, 227
Botein, S., 363, 371 Bryant, B., 236, 254, 271
Bott, E., 236, 271 Bryant, D., 48, 62
Bouchard, T., 382, 398 Bryant, D. M., 358, 359, 361, 372
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 382, 400 Buchanan, A., 131, 138
Bowlby, J., 44, 55, 58, 60, 62 Buchbinder, S., 364, 369, 370
Bowman, T., 187, 201 Buck, M. J., 3, 4, 25, 31
Boyce, L. K., 360, 372 Buehler, C., 206, 207, 211, 212, 213, 228, 231
Boyer, D., 75, 95, 98 Bugaighis, M. A., 209, 232
Boykin, A. W., 382, 400 Bugental, D., 43, 62
Boyum, L., 132, 136 Bugental, D. B., 5, 20, 29, 214, 228
Bradbury, T., 43, 62 Bumpus, M. F., 115, 137, 287, 291, 293,
Bradbury, T. N., 209, 210, 214, 225, 226, 294
227, 230, 231, 232 Burchinal, M., 48, 62
Braddock, D., 177, 200 Burchinal, M. R., 47, 62, 242, 255, 271,
Bradley, R. H., 324, 338, 362, 369 362, 369
Braithwaite, V. A., 117, 137 Burgess, B. J., 122, 123, 136
414 AUTHOR INDEX
Burgess, K. B., 386, 400 Ceci, S. J., 377, 382, 384, 398, 399, 400
Buriel, R., 9, 30, 116, 142 Cen, G., 5, 7, 29, 159, 172
Burks, V. S., 52, 64 Chamberlin, R., 363, 371
Burlingham, D., 302, 316 Champion, L. A., 46, 63
Burman, B., 208, 210, 211, 216, 228, 229 Chan, S. Q., 9, 30
Burns, K., 37, 62 Chandler, M., 149, 175
Burns, W. J., 37, 62 Chao, R., 124, 136
Burton, L., 75, 98 Chao, R. K., 5, 6, 29, 125, 136, 320, 326,
Burts, D. C., 4, 31 336, 338
Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., 17, 33 Chao, W., 40, 69
Bush, K. R., 335, 339 Chapman, R. S., 185, 198
Buss, D. M., 163, 172 Charnov, E. L., 107, 140
Butler, J., 75, 98 Chase-Lansdale, P. L., 91, 101, 113, 137
Byrd, J. E., 284, 295 Chavez, F., 37, 65
Chazan-Cohen, R., 37, 62
Chen, C., 12, 33
C Chen, F., 90, 98
Chen, H., 5, 7, 29, 159, 172
Cabrera, N., 103, 143 Chen, X., 5, 7, 29, 159, 162, 163, 172, 175
Cadenhead, C., 303, 316 Chen, Z. Y., 12, 29
Cadoret, R. J., 50, 62 Chess, S., 149, 150, 158, 160, 172, 176, 256,
Cahill, B. M., 187, 197 271, 272
Cain, R., 39, 71 Chesser, B., 297, 318
Caldera, Y. M., 161, 174 Chethik, L., 37, 62
Callor, S., 41, 42, 43, 67 Chiang, T., 9, 29
Campbell, D. T., 346, 349, 369, 372 Chih-Mei, C., 5, 9, 31
Campbell, S. B., 51, 58, 61, 62, 207, 228 Chilamkurti, C., 53, 67
Cannan, T., 168, 173 Child, I. L., 278, 293
Capaldi, D. M., 381, 399 Child Trends, 73, 74, 78, 98
Cardemil, E. V., 50, 62 Chisholm, K., 388, 399
Carels, R. A., 225, 228 Chorost, A. F., 225, 231
Carlson, E. A., 95, 101 Christal, R. C., 38, 70
Carlson, V. J., 325, 326, 332, 333, 339 Christensen, A., 210, 213, 216, 220, 224,
Carr, J., 189, 197 228, 231
Carroll, R., 166, 175 Christian, J. L., 45, 68
Carson, J., 132, 136 Christiansen, S. L., 128, 137
Carter, A., 37, 62 Chugani, D. C., 389, 399
Carter, M., 388, 399 Chugani, H. T., 389, 399
Casey, P. H., 324, 338, 362, 369 Cicchetti, D., 53, 59, 63, 70
Casey, R. J., 168, 173 Cicognani, E., 18, 29
Cashmore, J., 332, 339 Cioci, M., 309, 317
Caspi, A., 38, 53, 54, 62, 63, 65, 66, 75, 80, 81, Clark, L., 39, 42, 66, 69, 70
82, 83, 84, 85, 99, 394, 395, 398, 399 Clark, L. A., 42, 43, 63, 162, 165, 172
Caspi, D., 46, 52, 61 Clark, R., 37, 62, 284, 294, 295
Cassidy, B., 89, 98 Clarke, G. N., 50, 63
Cassidy, J., 45, 56, 63, 217, 233 Clarke-Stewart, K. A., 105, 137
Cassidy, S. B., 185, 198 Clayton, S. L., 352, 358, 363, 373
Castellino, D., 43, 67 Clements, M., 212, 218, 231
Catalano, R., 298, 318 Clingempeel, G., 222, 229
Cattell, R. B., 38, 63 Clingempeel, W. G., 166, 173
Cauce, A. M., 121, 136, 304, 317 Cloninger, C. R., 50, 63
Ceballo, R., 263, 271, 306, 311, 315 Cobb, S., 237, 259, 271
AUTHOR INDEX 415
Cochran, M., 236, 238, 239, 240, 241, 243, Costigan, C. L., 222, 230
244, 249, 252, 254, 267, 271, 272 Cote, L. R., 5, 29
Coggins, K., 123, 144 Cotterell, J., 262, 272
Cohen, J., 206, 228 Cotton, S., 332, 339
Cohen, M., 56, 70 Cowan, C. P., 125, 126, 127, 137, 205, 212,
Cohen, M. M., 56, 63 213, 216, 222, 223, 224, 225, 228,
Cohn, J. F., 58, 61 231, 345, 353, 370, 391, 399
Coie, J., 75, 100 Cowan, P., 125, 126, 127, 137
Coie, J. D., 52, 63 Cowan, P. A., 126, 137, 205, 212, 213, 216,
Coiro, M. J., 215, 228 222, 223, 224, 225, 228, 231, 276,
Colbert, K. K., 94, 97 295, 345, 353, 370, 391, 399
Cole, R., 346, 355, 357, 370, 371, 393, 400 Cox, M. J., 47, 58, 62, 64, 104, 137, 206,
Cole, R. E., 330, 338, 362, 363, 371 211, 228, 232, 242, 255, 271
Coleman, P., 159, 173 Coysh, W. S., 205, 228
Coleman, P. K., 15, 19, 29, 164, 172 Craig, I. W., 394, 398
Coley, R. L., 113, 137 Crichton, L., 161, 176
Colletta, N., 259, 272 Crick, N. C., 52, 64
Collins, A., 298, 315 Crisafulli, A., 208, 216, 232
Collins, N. L., 47, 63 Crittenden, P., 257, 264, 272
Collins, W. A., 3, 4, 25, 30, 109, 137, 215, Crittenden, P. M., 51, 64
228, 297, 315, 379, 387, 388, 399 Crnic, K., 41, 42, 44, 56, 61, 120, 139, 162,
Coltrane, S., 103, 112, 117, 119, 121, 122, 163, 173, 174, 176, 180, 186, 198,
137, 142, 289, 294 199, 213, 220, 222, 227, 230
Comfort, M., 81, 82, 85, 99 Crockenberg, S., 39, 64, 86, 89, 92, 94, 95,
Compas, B. E., 46, 68 98, 237, 238, 239, 256, 264, 272
Condry, J., 380, 399 Crockenberg, S. B., 162, 165, 172
Condry, S., 380, 399 Cronin, M., 357, 369
Conger, K., 40, 54, 63 Cross, C. E., 54, 65
Conger, R., 40, 54, 63, 64, 69, 114, 137, Cross, P. K., 188, 200
304, 306, 315 Cross, T. L., 123, 137
Conger, R. D., 38, 54, 65, 66, 69, 114, 137, Cross, W., 248, 249, 272
207, 225, 228, 230, 237, 272 Crouter, A., 298, 300, 316
Connell, D. B., 363, 371 Crouter, A. C., 10, 29, 114, 115, 137, 142,
Connell, J. P., 346, 369 160, 172, 275, 276, 281, 282, 286,
Connors, R., 258, 272 287, 289, 290, 291, 293, 294, 296
Conrad, B., 164, 173 Crowell, J., 58, 70
Contrerae, J., 39, 64 Cruzcosa, M., 214, 228
Contreras, J. M., 47, 64 Culp, A., 80, 86, 98, 99
Cook, G. A., 360, 372 Culross, P. L., 47, 59, 65, 345, 357, 370
Cook, T., 297, 302, 303, 304, 316 Cummings, E. M., 51, 64, 104, 113, 137,
Cook, T. D., 346, 349, 365, 369, 370, 372 206, 207, 214, 228, 229
Cooksey, E. C., 115, 137 Cummings, S., 180, 198
Cooley, C. H., 391, 399 Curtis-Boles, H., 205, 228
Cooper, H., 349, 373 Cutrona, C., 156, 172
Cooper, J. E., 117, 137 Cutting, A. L., 148, 172
Cooper, P. J., 37, 67
Copeland, J. M., 209, 232
Coplan, R. J., 20, 29 D
Corns, K. M., 58, 69
Corsaro, W., 330, 338 Daddis, C., 167, 175
Corter, C., 111, 112, 137, 138 Dadds, M. R., 18, 20, 29, 224, 229
Costa, P. T., 39, 64 Daggett, J., 17, 22, 29
416 AUTHOR INDEX
Eiden, R. D., 37, 58, 65, 69 Fauber, R. L., 207, 208, 213, 229
Eisenstadt, J. W., 352, 357, 370, 372 Fazio, R. H., 26, 30
Elder, G., 40, 54, 63, 114, 137 Feeney, J. A., 209, 229
Elder, G. H., 297, 302, 303, 304, 313, 316 Feinberg, M., 148, 172
Elder, G. H., Jr., 38, 54, 63, 65, 66, 69, Feinberg, M. E., 221, 223, 229
225, 228 Feingold, J., 384, 385, 401
Eley, T. C., 52, 65 Feins, J. D., 306, 307, 308, 317
Elins, J. L., 160, 176 Feiring, C., 58, 67
Elkin, I., 47, 66 Fendt, K. H., 358, 359, 361, 372
Elkins, I. J., 157, 172 Ferguson, M., 86, 97
Ellison, C. G., 7, 8, 28, 30 Festinger, L., 90, 98
Elmen, J. D., 281, 296 Fidler, D. J., 186, 187, 188, 189, 194, 195,
Emde, R. M., 367, 370 198, 199
Emde, R. N., 206, 212, 215, 229 Field, I., 121, 139
Emery, R. E., 207, 212, 215, 217, 228, 229, Field, T. M., 92, 98, 323, 339
233 Fincham, F., 43, 62
Emory, G., 186, 197 Fincham, F. D., 43, 67, 207, 209, 210, 212,
Engfer, A., 44, 65 213, 214, 215, 216, 225, 226, 227,
English, D., 379, 399 229, 230, 231, 232
Entwisle, D. R., 12, 22, 28, 30, 321, 322, Fine, D., 75, 95, 98
339 Fineman, N. R., 353, 370
Epstein, J. N., 349, 369 Finucane, B., 178, 198
Erel, O., 208, 210, 211, 216, 229 Fischel, J. E., 393, 397, 401
Erickson, F., 352, 370 Fischer, C., 240, 241, 242, 243, 244, 272
Erickson Warfield, M., 47, 65 Fischer, K. W., 150, 171
Erikson, E., 127, 138 Fish, M., 125, 136
Erikson, K., 312, 316 Fisher, P. A., 59, 64, 150, 151, 154, 160,
Erikson, M., 110, 138 174
Erikson, M. T., 51, 64 Fiske, S. T., 4, 26, 30
Erwin, L. J., 247, 273 Fisman, S. N., 187, 188, 200, 201
Espinosa, M., 37, 61 Fitzgerald, H., 80, 81, 87, 99
Essex, E. L., 182, 198 Fitzgerald, H. E., 37, 70, 168, 173
Fleming, A. S., 110, 111, 112, 137, 138
Essex, M. J., 162, 174, 284, 294, 295
Fletcher, A. C., 305, 316
Evans, D. W., 185, 199
Flor, F. L., 22, 28
Ey, S., 46, 68
Flouri, E., 131, 138
Eyberg, S., 220, 221, 227
Floyd, F. J., 220, 222, 225, 230, 232
Flyr, M. L., 132, 142
Fogg, L., 164, 173
F Follmer, A., 48, 62
Fonagy, P., 345, 372
Fabricius, W., 117, 142 Fontaine, R., 52, 64
Fagan, J., 114, 121, 133, 134, 138 Forehand, R., 213, 224, 227, 229
Fagan, W. T., 357, 369 Foster, C. L., 75, 99
Fagot, B., 163, 172 Foster, E. M., 54, 65
Fagot, B. I., 164, 168, 172, 173 Fowles, D. C., 163, 172
Falco, F. L., 393, 397, 401 Fracasso, M. P., 332, 340
Faleigh, M. J., 6, 30 Fraiberg, S., 47, 53, 59, 65
Farber, B., 180, 198 Franz, C., 131, 139
Farber, E., 58, 65, 303, 316 Franz, C. E., 132, 138
Farrington, D., 82, 100 Franzetta, K., 75, 100
Fauber, R., 213, 229 Freeman, S. F. N., 189, 199
418 AUTHOR INDEX
Haynes, O. M., 5, 8, 9, 28, 322, 338 Hofferth, S. L., 107, 108, 109, 114, 119,
Hazelwood, L., 39, 64 121, 122, 139, 143, 144
Head, M. R., 289, 291, 294 Hoffman, L. W., 328, 335, 339
Heath, A. C., 51, 64 Holden, G. W., 3, 4, 5, 7, 17, 24, 25, 30,
Heath, D. H., 126, 127, 139 31, 155, 173, 213, 227
Heath, H. E., 126, 127, 139 Hollingshead, A. B., 320, 339
Heavey, C. L., 216, 228 Hollon, S. D., 51, 69
Hebbeler, K. M., 352, 357, 358, 366, 370 Holman, J., 117, 137
Heinicke, C. M., 212, 230, 353, 367, 370 Holroyd, R. M., 186, 199
Heiss, J., 118, 139 Hommerding, K. D., 58, 70
Helms-Erikson, H., 289, 290, 291, 293, 294 Hooper, F. H., 106, 143
Heming, G., 205, 228 Hooven, C., 131, 138
Hemphill, S. A., 161, 175 Hopkins, J., 212, 229
Henderson, C., Jr., 239, 240, 241, 243, 252, Hoppes, K., 187, 199
254, 271 Hornby, G., 185, 189, 199
Hossain, T., 121, 139
Henderson, C. R., 349, 355, 362, 363, 366,
Hotz, V. J., 75, 76, 77, 78, 99
371, 372
Howard, J., 37, 61
Henderson, C. R., Jr., 393, 400
Howes, P., 216, 230
Hernandez, D. J., 275, 287, 295
Hsieh, K., 223, 227
Hernandez, F., 352, 357, 373
Hsu, H., 265, 272
Hertzig, M. E., 149, 150, 176
Hubbs-Tait, L., 86, 99
Herz, E. J., 89, 100
Huesmann, R. L., 117, 118, 138
Herzog, A., 14, 29
Hughes, C., 148, 172
Hesketh, L. J., 185, 198 Hughes, J. P., 188, 200
Hess, C. R., 92, 99 Hughes, S., 89, 98
Hess, R. D., 5, 9, 31, 367, 370 Hunsberger, B., 4, 7, 29
Hesse, E., 49, 66, 67 Hunter, J. E., 53, 67
Hetherington, E. M., 57, 68, 148, 159, 163, Hurlbut, N., 86, 99
166, 172, 173, 174, 222, 229, 297, Hurrelmann, K., 323, 339
315, 379, 387, 388, 399 Huston, T., 126, 141
Hewitt, J. K., 51, 64 Huston, T. L., 225, 231
Hewlett, B. S., 106, 116, 139 Hwang, C. P., 106, 140
Hiatt, S., 356, 371 Hyde, B. L., 118, 139
Hickok, G., 185, 190, 197 Hyde, J., 284, 294
Hildebrandt, K. A., 154, 168, 173, 175 Hyde, J. S., 284, 295
Hill, J., 22, 32, 46, 47, 68 Hyman, C., 75, 100
Hill, N. E., 335, 339
Hillenmeier, M., 83, 98
Hiller, K. A., 10, 32 I
Hinde, R., 256, 272
Hipwell, A. E., 37, 66 Iacono, W. G., 157, 172, 382, 400
Hiraga, Y., 304, 317 Ianni, F. A., 314, 316
Ho, D. Y. F., 12, 31 Iannotti, R. J., 206, 207, 229
Hochschild, A., 127, 139 Imamoglu, E. O., 329, 335, 339
Hodapp, R. M., 178, 181, 182, 185, 186, Irvin, N., 181, 198
187, 188, 189, 190, 192, 193, 194, Irwin, N., 358, 373
195, 196, 198, 199, 200 Ishii-Kuntz, M., 124, 139
Hodges, J., 379, 388, 400, 401 Isley, S., 131, 139
Hoeksma, J. B., 162, 176 Israelashvilli, R., 41, 42, 67
Hoff, E., 11, 31, 321, 327, 329, 339 Iverson, T. J., 17, 31
AUTHOR INDEX 421
Lerman, R., 118, 140 Love, J. M., 348, 350, 354, 361, 365, 368,
Lerner, J. V., 92, 99, 158, 173 370, 371
Levant, R. F., 113, 114, 136 Lovejoy, J., 106, 140
Leve, L. D., 164, 173 Lowe, R., 225, 230
Leventhal, T., 297, 303, 306, 316, 317 Lucca, N., 9, 33
Levin, H., 131, 143, 149, 175 Luckey, D., 393, 400
Levine, J., 81, 82, 85, 99 Luckey, D. W., 349, 355, 366, 372
Levine, J. A., 106, 107, 127, 140 Luker, K., 75, 99
Levine, M., 125, 136 Lundberg, S., 76, 99
LeVine, R., 322, 341 Luria, Z., 156, 167, 175, 380, 401
LeVine, R. A., 10, 31, 379, 400 Luster, T., xi, xiii, xv, 75, 80, 81, 82, 87,
Levy, F., 395, 400 89, 90, 91, 92, 93, 94, 96, 97, 97, 98,
Levy-Shiff, R., 41, 42, 67 99, 100, 101, 280, 292, 295, 348,
Lewinsohn, P. M., 46, 50, 63, 65 360, 371
Lewis, C. E., 207, 231 Lustina, M. J., 26, 28
Lewis, I., 359, 372 Ly, T. M., 187, 188, 189, 190, 193, 199, 200
Lewis, M., 58, 67 Lykken, D. T., 382, 398, 400
Lewis, M. A., 207, 231 Lynd, H. M., 275, 295
Lewkowicz, K. S., 106, 143 Lynd, R. S., 275, 295
Leyendecker, B., 11, 30, 32, 324, 325, 326, Lyons-Ruth, K., 51, 56, 57, 67, 363, 371
332, 333, 336, 339, 340, 341 Lytton, H., 154, 167, 171, 173
Li, M., 110, 111, 112, 138
Liaw, F., 359, 361, 371
Lichtenstein, P., 52, 65 M
Lieberman, A. F., 53, 59, 67, 360, 371
Liebman, J. B., 306, 307, 308, 317 Maarala, M., 50, 69
Liker, J. K., 54, 65 MacArthur, D., 186, 199
Lindahl, K. M., 212, 216, 218, 231 Maccoby, E. E., 106, 131, 140, 143, 149,
Linder, C. W., 303, 316 166, 167, 174, 175, 297, 315, 327,
Lindsay, L. L., 216, 229 328, 340, 379, 387, 388, 399
Linfield, K. J., 209, 230 MacDonald, K., 105, 132, 140
Linn, M. I., 352, 357, 373 Machamer, A. M., 6, 31
Lipovsky, J. A., 53, 66 MacKinnon-Lewis, C., 43, 52, 67
Little, C., 37, 62 MacPhee, D., 245, 260, 272
Lobato, D., 180, 200 Madden, T., 336, 341
Lobel, M., 47, 63 Maes, H. H., 51, 64
Lochman, J., 75, 100 Magnuson, K. A., 321, 323, 324, 335, 338,
Locke, H. J., 209, 231 340
Loeber, R., 51, 64 Magnusson, D., 52, 69
Loehlin, J. C., 157, 174, 382, 383, 400 Maguire, M. C., 115, 137, 287, 294
Lollis, S., 165, 175 Magyary, D., 348, 369
Long, N., 207, 208, 213, 229 Main, M., 49, 66, 67
Longfellow, C., 40, 67 Maital, S., 5, 8, 9, 28
Longo, L. C., 26, 30 Makhijani, M. G., 26, 30
Lonigan, C. J., 349, 369, 393, 397, 401 Malach, R. S., 6, 31
Lopez, I. R., 47, 64 Malik, N. M., 216, 218, 231
Lopez, M., 11, 33 Malphurs, J. E., 121, 139
Lorenz, F., 40, 54, 63 Mammone, N., 156, 173
Lorenz, F. O., 38, 66, 237, 272 Mandara, J., 118, 120, 140
Losel, F., 310, 317 Manetti, M., 242, 265, 272
Losoya, S., 41, 42, 43, 67 Mangan, T. W., 177, 197
424 AUTHOR INDEX
Mangelsdorf, S., 39, 41, 58, 64, 65, 67, 162, McCullough, M., 84, 100
174, 220, 230 McDermid, S. M., 126, 141
Mangelsdorf, S. C., 220, 222, 232 McDevitt, S. C., 170, 175
Manke, B., 293, 294 McDevitt, T. M., 5, 9, 31
Manlove, J., 73, 75, 100, 101 McDowell, D. J., 132, 133, 141, 142
Manly, J. T., 59, 70 McElroy, S. W., 75, 76, 77, 78, 99
Mansbach, I. V., 11, 31 McFadyen-Ketchum, S. A., 18, 32, 43, 52,
Mantizicopoulos, P. Y., 15, 32 68
Marakovitz, S., 51, 62 McFarlane, E., 364, 369, 370
Marcoen, A., 56, 70 McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V., 3, 4, 14, 15,
Margolin, G., 207, 210, 213, 216, 219, 220, 22, 32, 33
221, 223, 224, 228, 230, 231 McGroder, S. M., 37, 67
Markel, K. S., 116, 138 McGue, M., 157, 172, 174, 382, 398, 400
Markman, H., 212, 218, 231 McGuigan, W. M., 364, 371
Markman, H. J., 216, 230 McGuire, S., 148, 159, 174
Marks, L. D., 18, 32 McHale, J., 220, 230
Marks, S. R., 282, 295 McHale, J. P., 219, 220, 221, 225, 226, 231,
Markus, H. R., 329, 340 232
Marshall, L. A., 4, 24, 31 McHale, S., 126, 141
Marshall, N., 266, 272 McHale, S. M., 115, 137, 148, 160, 172,
Marsiglio, W., 110, 130, 131, 140 174, 275, 287, 289, 290, 291, 293,
Martin, J., 394, 398 294
Martin, J. A., 327, 328, 340 McKee, T. R., 132, 139
Martinez, P. E., 302, 317 McKeering, H., 128, 141
Marx, F., 266, 272 Mckenry, P. C., 45, 66
Masciadrelli, B. P., 108, 109, 112, 114, 126, McKinney, M., 348, 360, 371
128, 130, 131, 142 McLanahan, S., 107, 138, 253, 272
Mash, E. J., 161, 174 McLanahan, S. S., 47, 68, 129, 130, 138,
Mason, C. A., 304, 317 141
Mason, P. L., 118, 140 McLoyd, V., 92, 100, 263, 271, 319, 320,
Masten, A. S., 163, 174 323, 324, 340
Matheson, P., 113, 137 McLoyd, V. C., 114, 137, 306, 311, 315
Maughan, A., 59, 70 McMahon, R., 95, 101
Maughan, B., 46, 68 McMahon, R. J., 304, 305, 315
Maumary-Gremaud, A., 75, 100 McNeilly-Choque, M. K., 131, 132, 139
Maxwell, J. A., 117, 140 McNichol, K., 163, 175
Maxwell, S., 90, 101 McWilliam, R., 351, 373
Maynard, A., 331, 333, 334, 339 Meany, M. J., 106, 141
McAdoo, H., 81, 100 Measelle, J., 224, 228
McAdoo, H. P., 379, 400 Meens, L. D., 209, 232
McAdoo, J. L., 118, 119, 140 Mehrabian, A., 47, 62
McAllister, R. A., 20, 29 Meisels, S. J., 359, 361, 371
McAnarney, E. R., 94, 101 Melby, J., 54, 63
McBride, B. A., 112, 126, 140, 142, 161, Melhuish, E. C., 37, 66
163, 174 Melson, G. F., 265, 272
McBride Murry, V., 304, 306, 315 Melzer, W., 323, 339
McCarthy, J., 40, 64 Menaghan, E. G., 114, 115, 137, 141, 275,
McCartney, K., 157, 175, 266, 272, 350, 279, 280, 288, 291, 293, 295, 296
371, 372, 382, 400 Mendell, N. R., 45, 68
McClay, J., 394, 398 Merrick, S., 58, 70
McClelland, D., 132, 138 Mervis, C. B., 185, 190, 200
McCrae, R. R., 39, 64 Meyer, D., 107, 138
AUTHOR INDEX 425
Noller, P., 209, 229 Osborne, L. N., 207, 212, 213, 214, 216,
Noonan, A., 266, 272 230, 232
Norton, R., 209, 232 O’Shea, G., 49, 65
Notaro, P. C., 131, 144 Osofsky, J., 80, 86, 98, 99, 300, 302, 317
Nucci, L., 330, 340 Osofsky, J. D., 79, 100
Nunez, N. N., 9, 29 Ossiander, E., 95, 101
Otaki, M., 113, 138
Overstreet, E. J., 267, 272
O Owen, M. T., 58, 64, 206, 232
Owens, E. B., 56, 70
Oberklaid, F., 166, 175
Obiorah, F. C., 209, 232
O’Brien, M., 17, 22, 29, 80, 98, 226, 232 P
O’Brien, R., 349, 355, 356, 366, 371, 372
O’Connell, M., 108, 141 Pachter, L. M., 326, 327, 335, 339
O’Connor, C., 388, 400 Paff-Bergen, L. A., 209, 232
O’Connor, T. G., 148, 151, 152, 153, 154, Painter, L. M., 5, 8, 9, 28
159, 160, 166, 167, 169, 170, 171, Pakenham, K. I., 128, 141
172, 173, 174, 378, 379, 388, 389, Paley, B., 104, 137, 211, 228
400 Palkovitz, R., 107, 128, 137, 141
Ogaki, L., xiii, xv Pancoast, D., 298, 315
Ogbu, J. U., 10, 32, 277, 278, 296, 379, 400 Panzarine, S., 89, 100
Ogino, M., 5, 8, 9, 28 Papas, M. A., 92, 99
Okagaki, L., 3, 5, 6, 9, 12, 13, 22, 32, 97, Papillo, A. R., 75, 100
100, 155, 174, 194, 200, 292, 295, Papoušek, H., 9, 32
357, 372 Papoušek, M., 9, 32
Olds, D., 346, 351, 356, 357, 358, 360, 363, Parcel, J. L., 114, 141
367, 370, 371, 393, 400 Parcel, T. L., 279, 280, 288, 291, 293, 296
Olds, D. L., 349, 355, 362, 363, 366, 371, Pardo, C., 300, 302, 303, 316
372 Park, S., 162, 171, 174
O’Leary, D. A., 45, 68 Parke, R. D, 56, 63
O’Leary, K. D., 45, 61, 213, 224, 232 Parke, R. D., 25, 30, 103, 104, 105, 106,
O’Leary, S., 44, 69, 213, 214, 232, 233 107, 110, 112, 113, 116, 117, 121,
O’Leary, S. G., 20, 22, 33 122, 125, 127, 130, 131, 132, 133,
Olsen, C. L., 188, 200 134, 135, 136, 137, 139, 140, 141,
Olsen, S. F., 131, 132, 139 142
Olshanksy, S., 182, 200 Pasch, L. A., 225, 226, 232
Olson, D. R., 345, 372 Pascual, L., 5, 8, 9, 28
Oltmanns, T. F., 224, 232 Patenaude, R., 44, 66
O’Neal, C., 344, 369 Paternoster, R., 305, 317
O’Neil, R., 11, 30, 107, 115, 127, 131, 132, Patterson, G., 40, 64, 305, 317
133, 138, 139, 141, 142, 282, 283, Patterson, G. R., 51, 68, 150, 151, 154, 160,
295 174, 304, 317, 381, 399
Ong, A., 336, 341 Paulsell, D., 348, 350, 354, 361, 365, 368,
Ontai, L., 47, 59, 69 370, 371
Ooms, T., 118, 140 Pawl, J., 59, 67
Oppenheimer, D., 332, 333, 340 Pawl, J. H., 59, 67, 360, 371
O’Reilly, A. W., 104, 113, 137 Pawletko, T. M., 148, 174
Orobio de Castro, B., 52, 68 Payne, C., 47, 62, 242, 255, 271
Orr, L., 306, 307, 308, 317 Payne, C. K., 282, 283, 295
Orsmond, G. I., 187, 188, 200 Payne, M. R., 346, 365, 370
AUTHOR INDEX 427
Silva, P., 75, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 85, 99 Spieker, S., 348, 369
Silva, P. A., 46, 52, 61 Spieker, S. J., 95, 101, 348, 356, 363, 369
Silverberg, S., 40, 65, 333, 338 Spiker, D., 351, 352, 357, 359, 363, 369,
Silverman, R., 59, 67 370, 373
Silverstein, L. B., 117, 143 Spitz, R. A., 49, 69, 379, 401
Simeonsson, R., 182, 197 Spotts, E. L., 157, 176
Simmens, D., 47, 66 Sprecher, S., 168, 173
Simonof, E., 51, 64 Spritz, B., 56, 61
Simons, R., 40, 54, 63, 69 Srivastava, S., 38, 66
Simons, R. L., 114, 137, 237, 272, 304, 306, Sroufe, L. A., 54, 58, 65, 70, 305, 318
315 Stagg, V., 258, 272
Simpson, K. S., 207, 229 Stallings, J., 111, 138
Sims, K., 348, 360, 371 Stang, P. E., 75, 99
Sinclair, R., 220, 231 Staples, R., 122, 143
Skeels, H., 385, 401 Stark, M., 180, 181, 182, 201
Skeels, H. M., 49, 69 Stattin, H., 51, 52, 69, 289, 295, 296, 305,
Skinner, M. L., 54, 69 316
Skodak, M., 49, 69, 385, 401 Steele, C. M., 26, 28, 33
Slater, E., 89, 100 Steinberg, J., 106, 140
Slaughter, D. T., 349, 354, 372 Steinberg, L., 6, 33, 118, 140, 162, 174,
Slavens, G., 303, 316 281, 296, 297, 298, 304, 305, 315,
Slavin, R. E., 345, 372 316, 318, 379, 387, 388, 399
Slep, A., 44, 69, 213, 232 Steiner, M., 111, 138
Slep, A. M. S., 20, 22, 33 Stemmler, M., 46, 68
Slep, A. S., 214, 233 Stern, M., 154, 156, 167, 175
Small, S., 75, 101 Sternberg, R. J., 6, 9, 32, 382, 400
Small, S. A., 75, 100 Stevenson, H. W., 9, 12, 33
Smart, D., 161, 175 Stevenson, J., 52, 65, 157, 173
Smetana, J. G., 167, 175, 330, 340 Stevenson-Hinde, J., 256, 272
Smith, J., 323, 324, 338 Stevens-Simon, C., 94, 101
Smith, K. E., 247, 273 Stewart, J., 106, 141, 384, 385, 401
Smith, P. K., 105, 143 Stewart, M. A., 50, 62
Smith, R., 85, 101, 163, 176 Stewart, S., 163, 175
Snarey, J., 126, 127, 128, 143 Stewart, S. L., 5, 7, 29, 159, 162, 165, 172,
Snow, M. E., 166, 174 175
Snyder, H. N., 300, 302, 306, 318 Stifter, C., 162, 171
Snyder, J. R., 217, 233 Stigler, J. W., 12, 33
Snyder, P., 351, 373 Stinnett, N., 297, 318
Snyder, S., 351, 373 Stocker, C. M., 162, 175, 206, 233
Solnit, A., 180, 181, 182, 201 Stollak, G., 53, 67
Sommer, K., 90, 101 Stone, G., 206, 207, 213, 228
Sorenson, J. L., 50, 67 Stoolmiller, M., 383, 401
Sorri, A., 50, 69 Storey, A. E., 111, 143
Sotsky, S. M., 47, 66 Stouthamer-Loeber, M., 305, 317
Spagnola, M., 59, 70 Stovall, K. C., 59, 64
Spangler, G., 58, 71 Strauss, C., 325, 341
Spanier, G. B., 209, 233 Stright, A. D., 221, 222, 233
Sparling, J., 359, 372 Suess, G., 58, 70
Sparling, J. J., 358, 359, 361, 372 Sullivan, H. S., 60, 69
Speechley, M., 187, 201 Sullivan, K. T., 214, 231
Spence, J. T., 9, 33 Sumner, G., 348, 369
Spencer, S. J., 26, 33 Sun, L. C., 106, 124, 143
AUTHOR INDEX 431
A parent–child, 213–214
Agreeableness, and personality, 42
Academic difficulties, of children born to Alan Guttmacher Institute, 73
adolescent mothers, 81–82 Alliance of Genetic Support Groups, 184t
Adolescent mothers American Association on Mental Retarda-
children of tion, 178
characteristics of, 91–92 The Arc of the United States, 184t
ecological perspective on, 73–101 Asians, and fathering, 123–125
outcomes for, 80–88 Attachment
resilient, 86–88 interventions for, 58–59
ecological perspective on, 73–101 stability of, 58
outcomes for, 76–80 Attitudes
as recurring pattern, 85 parental, 7–8
resilient, 85–86 and childrearing behaviors, 17–18
Advice, personal social networks and, paternal, maternal and, 112–113
261–264
Attributions
Affect, in parent–child interactions,
and close relationships, 43–44
212–213
and depression, 50
Affect-specific processes, 44
African American families, complexity of marital, and parenting, 214–215
social cognitions in, 23 parental, 9
African Americans and childrearing behaviors, 20–21
and early childbearing, 74 Authoritarian parenting, 327–328
and fathering, 117–120 Authoritative parenting, 327–328
Age, of child, and parenting, 166–167 Availability
Aggression definition of, 106–107
origins of, 51–53 emotional, 133
435
436 SUBJECT INDEX
J Mothers
attitudes of, and father involvement,
Jobs. see Employment 112–113
Justice, U.S. Department of, 300 employment and work hours of, 288–290
marital relationships and, 215–217
personal social networks of, child-related
L outcomes of, 264–266
roles of, versus fathers, 104–105
Latina/os. see Hispanic Americans Moving to Opportunities (MTO) program,
Life events, personal social networks and, 306–308
254–256 Murder, of juveniles, community influences
Logic model, 346 on, 300–301
M
N
MAOA gene. see Monoamine oxidase A
gene National Clearinghouse on Child Abuse
Marital conflict and Neglect Information, 300
definition of, 209–210 National Organization of Rare Disorders
and marital satisfaction, 210 (NORD), 183, 184t
and parenting, 212–214 National Research Council, 53, 345, 349,
research on, 225 361–362, 365
and triadic interaction, 218
Native American parents, beliefs of, 6
Marital power, and triadic interaction,
Native Americans, and fathering, 122–123
218–219
Marital quality, issues in, 208–210 Negative affectivity. see Neuroticism
Marital relationships Neglected children, 389
and child development Neglectful parenting, 327
direct effects, 206–207 Neighborhoods
indirect effects, 208–219 characteristics of, and adolescent moth-
coparenting and, 220–223 ers and their children, 93
and father–child relationships, 113 and parenting, 297–318
fathering and, 126–127 Neuroticism, and personality, 38–41
intervention research on, 223–225 NICHD Early Child Care Research Net-
parenting and, 205–233 work, 39, 108, 114, 121
models of association between, No Child Left Behind Act, 345
210–211
Nonresident fathers, contact with children,
research directions for, 219–226
129–130
Marital satisfaction
NORD. see National Organization of Rare
definition of, 209
Disorders
and marital conflict, 210
and parenting, 211–212
Maternal mourning, 180
Men, development of, fathering and, O
125–128
Mental retardation, genetic forms of, 178,
179t One-parent families, and personal social
Minimum standard of care, maintenance networks, 249–251
of, 312 Openness to experience, and personality,
Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene, 394 42
440 SUBJECT INDEX
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), envi- goals for, socioeconomic status and,
ronmental stress and, 302–303 328–331
Poverty parental, and child outcomes, 375–401
and adolescent mothers and their chil- Social networks
dren, 92–93 personal, and parenting, 235–273
concentration of, parenting and, versus social support, 237–239
298–300 Social support, and adolescent mothers
Prader–Willi syndrome, 178, 179t and their children, 93–94
and parenting, 185, 189–191 Societal generativity, 128
Web site on, 184t Society, changing views of developmental
Prolactin, and paternal behavior, 111–112 disabilities, 177–178, 183–184
Proximal processes, 375–376 Socioeconomic status (SES). see also Class
Psychological differentiation, 37 versus genetic influences on child out-
Psychological functioning, development in comes, 383–384
childhood, 55–57 measurement of, issues in, 320–324
5p- syndrome, 179t and parental expectations, 10–11
Web site on, 184t and parenting, 319–341
PTSD. see Posttraumatic stress disorder versus ethnicity, 335–337
Puerto Rican families independence/interdependence ap-
goals and expectations in, 10 proach and, 334–336
personal social networks of, 247 and parenting styles, 328–331
Special Olympics International, 184t
Spillover effect, 210–211
R processes in, 211
Staffing arrangements, of parenting pro-
Race, and personal social networks, grams, 353
244–249 Stereotype-threat, 26
Relationship factors, and parenting, 35–71 Stress
Research. see Intervention research on adolescent parents, contextual
Resilience, of adolescent mothers and their sources of, 92–96
children, 85–88 Down syndrome parenting and, 186
Responsibility, definition of, 106–107 environmental, parental responses to,
Restrictive parenting style, high-risk com- 302–304
munities and, 303–304 healthy adaptation to, factors in, 310
work-related, and parenting, 285–287
Stress-buffering hypothesis, 260–261
S Stress-coping perspectives, on developmen-
tal disabilities, 180–181
Scientifically-based research, 345–346 Structural features, of parenting programs,
SES. see Socioeconomic status 352–353
Skill development, workplace and, 281–282 Support
Smith–Magenis syndrome, 179t for adolescent parents, contextual
Web site on, 184t sources of, 92–96
Social cognitions communities and, 308–313
complexity of, 23–25 versus social networks, 237–239
contextual influences on, 4–14 social networks, and parenting, 235–273
and parental behavior, 3–33
research on, 23–27
methodological developments in, T
25–27
Socialization Teen mothers. see Adolescent mothers
442 SUBJECT INDEX
Temperament W
of child, influence on parenting,
161–164 Williams syndrome, 178, 179t
moderators of, 164–166 and parenting, 185, 189–191
definition of, 160–161 Web site on, 184t
Testosterone, and paternal behavior, Within-group comparisons
111–112 in developmental disability studies,
Triadic family interactions, 217–219 181–182
in intervention research, 350–351
Workplace
and acquisition of parenting values,
U 278–280
characteristics of, and childrearing,
University Centers of Excellence, 184 277–287
and family, 287–292
opportunities and constraints for
parenting, 280–284
V policies of, and parenting, 283–284
relationships in, and parenting, 282–283
Values, parenting, work and, 278–280 stress of, and parenting, 285–287