Discourse Analysis and Constructionist Approaches Theoretical Background
Discourse Analysis and Constructionist Approaches Theoretical Background
Institutional Repository
by the/an author.
Qualitative Research Methods for Psychology and the Social Sciences. Leicester:
Additional Information:
Jonathan Potter
Discourse and Rhetoric Group
Loughborough University
Loughborough
Email: [email protected]
19 March, 2012
1
This chapter is structured in terms of questions and answers. There are several reasons for
adopting this format. First, people often consult a handbook to find the answers to questions
so the format may simplify this task. Second, most constructionist approaches place a
most prototypical forms. Third, constructionist researchers have been at the forefront of
I shall start with some general questions about constructionism and its place in psychology,
and then I shall move on to focus on issues of method and analysis. I shall concentrate upon
general principles and arguments, however, this is not intended to be a how-to-do-it chapter.
Chapter 11, by Rosalind Gill, provides a more fleshed out example of a particular style of
constructionist research.
What is constructionism?
On the face of it this seems like a sensible question with which to start. What could be wrong
with give a broad characterisation, offering a compact definition, and then going on to
describe constructionism in detail? The problem is that this would be a profoundly anti-
constructionist approach to this question. It would imply that there is a simple thing —
constructionism that can be neutrally and objectively described and defined. This would be a
realist account of constructionism. That is, it would do precisely the thing that
another approach would be to consider the way constructionism is itself constructed: how it is
psychologists and sociologists; how different perspectives are treated as constructionist, and
what is taken to hang on this ascription. However, I do not intent to attempt this ambitious
enterprise here.
2
Having cautioned against treating definitions realistically, they can nevertheless be a useful
Shotter and Ken Gergen’s important series of books with a constructionist theme:
[Social constructionism] has given voice to range of new topics, such as the social
method and theorizing. The common thread underlying all these topics is a concern
with the processes by which human abilities, experiences, commonsense and scientific
knowledge are both produced in, and reproduce, human communities (Shotter and
The quote implies a unity, but listing also shows mix and match of different theoretical
perspectives.
Elsewhere Ken Gergen (1994) identifies five basic assumptions for a social constructionist
science:
1. The terms by which we account for the world and ourselves are not dictated by the
2. The terms and forms by which we achieve understanding of the world and ourselves are
3. The degree to which a given account of the world or self is sustained across time is not
dependent on the objective validity of the account but on the vicissitudes of social process.
(1994:51)
4. Language dervies its significance in human affairs from the way in which it functions
3
Another way to come at the question is not to attempt a definition, but to consider the
different approaches that have been commonly called constructionist (with the warning that
One of the features of the approaches that have been called constructionist is that they often
have often developed at the margins of disciplines, in the spaces where psychology blurs into
sociology, where literary studies borders on political science, where feminism and rhetoric
intersect. A rather cursory survey of constructionist approaches can easily gather together a
dozen perspectives (see Table One). Note, however, that there are all sorts of potentially
contentious features of this list and its absences. Reviewing literature is itself a constructive
and sometimes highly contentious business (Ashmore, et. al., 1995); what should count as an
Approach Example
4
Symbolic Interactionism Hewitt (1994)
been carried on in almost complete isolation from the problematics of psychology. Several of
these perspectives have some psychological adherents, but have their main site of
identify features that they share in common. Yet it is hard to find a single characteristic
central to them all. The idea of a family resemblance gives something of a sense of the
pattern. First, they all tend to be oppositional movements of one kind or another to traditional
social science positions, and in particular their realist assumptions. Second, they all tend to
stress the way mind and action are contingent on specific cultural forms. They see minds as
not having fixed essences but being built from the symbolic resources of a cultures; indeed, in
some constructionisms mind is not a mental entity at all, but a discursive move: a set of
stories that people tell, or different discursive practices for dealing with one another as moral
and accountable (cf. Harré, 1983; Coulter, 1989). Third, they all tend to treat discourse —
Clearly constructionism cuts across traditional disciplinary boundaries. In many cases current
boundaries are a product of particular views of the social and human sciences that were
established during university expansion after the Second World War, particularly in the US
and UK. Lack of expansion in the late 70s and 80s during the proliferation of constructionist
approaches has meant that there is often a disparity between the intellectual and bureaucratic
5
structure of departments. Constructionist researchers in psychology often have more in
common with colleagues in areas of linguistics or sociology of science than they do with their
One of the challenges for constructionists is to be able to thrive in the traditional structures
while operating in a genuinely interdisciplinary manner. This can cut both ways — at times it
can be a struggle to establish to legitimacy of this kind of research with more mainstream
psychological colleagues; yet at other times it can be a context that allows superficial
In psychology work has normally proceeded under the title social constructionism with
concern being expressed that social constructivism could be confused with the artistic
knowledge constructivism is a well established perspective (with no concern about the artistic
movement), but constructivism (without the social) is increasingly favoured over social
constructionism (cf. Latour and Woolgar, 1979, 1986). One reason for this is that social
construction is associated here with rather limited perspectives which relate knowledge to
often see such accounts as reductive (Mulkay, 1979; Woolgar, 1988). Moreover, they may
want to make recourse to processes in texts and rhetoric which are not social in this more
traditional way (Knorr-Cetina, 1995). More radically, they have started to question the
coherence of the very distinction between the social and non-social (Latour, 1987).
The short answer to this is no. If anything there is even more variation of method than of
theory. For many of these approaches it is not clear that there is anything which would
6
correspond to what psychologists traditionally think of as a method. Perhaps the most
appropriate consideration in many cases would be what Billig (1988a) calls scholarship. That
is, lack of method in the sense of a formally specified set of procedures and calculations, does
not mean a lack of argument or rigour; nor does it mean that the theoretical system is not
* * *
For the rest of this chapter, I want to concentrate on discourse analysis which is an approach
where it makes more sense to talk about research method. Although even here method is
understood very differently than is normal in psychology. For example, one point of contrast
provided by carrying out the procedure of analysis correctly and fully. A sample is collected,
some ‘variables’ operationalized, conventional statistical tests are carried out, and so on. In
contrast, in discourse analysis, the analytic procedure is largely separate from how claims are
justified.
Discourse analysis focuses on talk and texts as social practices, and on the resources that are
drawn on to enable those practices. For example, discourse analytic studies of racism have
been concerned with the way descriptions are marshalled in particular contexts to legitimate
the blaming of a minority group (Potter and Wetherell, 1988), and with the resources
(‘interpretative repertoires’) that are available in a particular cultural setting for legitimating
racist practices (Nairn and McCreanor, 1991; Wetherell and Potter, 1992).
Discourse analysts have focused on issues of stake and accountability, looking at the way
people manage pervasive issues of blame and responsibility (Antaki, 1994; Edwards and
Potter, 1993; Gill, 1993) and have studied the way descriptions are put together to perform
7
actions and manage accountability (Potter, forthcoming). For example, Edwards (1994)
studied ‘script formulations’ in a set of telephone conversations, showing the way events
could be described to present them as regular and routine, to treat them as a characteristic
consequence of personal dispositions or, to make them out as an unusual result of outside
pressures. Such descriptions manage questions of fault and provide legitimation for courses
of action.
Discourse analysts have rejected the traditional cognitive explanations of psychology. Rather
than try and explain actions as a consequence of mental processes or entities, their interest has
been in how mentalist notions are constructed and used in interaction. For example, instead
of attempting to explain sexism, say, in terms of the attitudes of individuals, the concern is
with how evaluations are managed in particular interactions, and either linked up with, or
These are some of the characteristic strands of discourse work, but they do not define it. New
studies are being done, pushing back the limits of discourse work and the problematics of
Does that mean there is a discourse analytic answer to any psychological question?
No. One of the mistakes that people sometimes make when they are starting discourse work
question: ‘I am interested in the factors that cause people to smoke, should I use an
that, first, discourse analysis is not just a method but is a whole perspective on social life and
its research, and, second, that all methods involve a range of theoretical assumptions.
Traditional psychology has often been concerned with factors and outcomes, and these ideas
are thoroughly enmeshed in thinking about experimentation and questionnaire design. The
8
logic of discourse analysis is a rhetorical and normative one. Any rhetorical move can have a
1985). Effectiveness is not guaranteed as it should be a with a causal process; for a norm is
not a mechanical template. Norms are oriented to, but they are also regularly deviated from,
One of the skills involved in discourse analytic work is in formulating questions that are
theoretically coherent and analytically manageable. Simply importing a question cold from a
have become devoted to hypothetico deductivism, where quality research is seen to hang on a
assumption free inductivism, discourse researchers have often found it productive to collect
and explore a set of materials — interview transcripts or natural records of some kind —
without being hampered by the need to start from a specific hypothesis. Indeed, the devotion
to a fully formulated prior hypothesis has probably been one of the reasons why psychologists
have been so reluctant to study records of natural interaction such as everyday conversations
I noted above that discourse analysis is concerned both with the organization of texts and talk
in practices, and with the discursive resources that those practices draw on. The notion of
often used with stylistic and grammatical coherence, and often organized around one or more
central metaphors. They are historically developed and make up an important part of the
common sense of a culture; although some may be specific to certain institutional domains.
9
The idea of an interpretative repertoire is intended to accommodate to the twin considerations
that there are resources available with an off-the-shelf character that can be used in a range of
different settings to do particular tasks, and that these resources have a more bespoke
flexibility which allows them to be selectively drawn on and reworked according to the
setting. It is the attempt to accommodate to this flexible, local use that distinguishes
interpretative repertoires from the more Foucaultian notion of discourses (Parker, 1992;
Potter, et al., 1990). Participants will often draw on a number of different repertoires, flitting
between them as they construct the sense of a particular phenomenon, or as they perform
different actions. Billig (1992) refers to this as the kaleidoscope of common sense.
The classic research using this notion is Gilbert and Mulkay’s (1984) study of scientists
discourse, which records the way scientists use one interpretative repertoire in their formal
writing for justifying facts, and another in their informal talk when accounting for why
competing scientists were in error. More recently, the notion has been developed in a number
of studies with a more social psychological focus (e.g. Marshall and Raabe, 1993; Potter and
Reicher, 1987; Wetherell et al, 1987; Wetherell and Potter, 1992). The overall analytic goal
in these studies is the identification of repertories and the explication of the practices they are
part of. For methodological discussions relating to repertoire analysis see in particular Coyle
(1995), Potter and Wetherell (1987, ch. 7, forthcoming) and Wetherell and Potter (1992, ch.
Although the notion of interpretative repertories has proved analytically fruitful, it has some
limitations. For example, it is much more difficult to make clear and consistent judgements
about the boundaries of particular repertories outside of constrained institutional settings such
as science discourse. Another problem is that the generality of the repertoire notion may
obscure local interactional business done by particular discourse forms (see Potter,
10
Is repertoire analysis the main task of discourse analysis?
replaced by studies of the way specific actions are accomplished, or of the devices and
procedures through which factual versions are constructed. These are studies asking the
following sorts of questions. How is a blaming achieved? How is a particular version of the
world made to seem solid and unproblematic? How are social categories constructed and
managed in practice? Such questions require an understanding of what Billig (1987) calls the
witcraft of rhetoric: the detailed, contextually sensitive manner in which versions are
organizations in which such procedures are embedded. Indeed, it is here that the study of
discourse shades both into the study of rhetoric and work on conversation analysis.
There is less written on methodological aspects of this style of discourse work. However,
Potter and Wetherell (1994) describe the methodological decisions and analytic practices in
one study of this kind, trying to show how specific conclusions were arrived at. Wooffitt
Conversation analysis has developed from pioneering work of Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson
stereotyped by psychologists as the study of the rules of turn taking in trivial conversations.
However, the point of conversation analysis is to explicate the fundamental sense that
interaction has for its participants. It is a fast growing and notably cumulative field which has
highlighted major deficiencies in the speech act approaches that psychologists often look to
for an account of language practices (Levinson, 1983; Schegloff, 1988) as well as providing
11
Conversation analysis is relevant to discourse analysis in two ways. First, it provides a
powerful and general understanding of interaction that has the potential to illuminate a wide
range of research questions. After all, much human interaction is performed through
conversation and to understand many of the more psychological and social phenomena that
discourse analysts are interested in it is necessary to understand how they emerge out of the
general pragmatics of conversation. This involves being mindful of basic features such as
considering the findings of many studies showing the delicate way in which actions are
for producing high class DA work. The best and most accessible current introduction is
undoubtedly Nofsinger (1991), although Heritage (1984) has an excellent chapter situating
There are specific areas where discourse and conversation analysis come together. Like
discourse analysts, conversation analysts have paid a lot of attention to the way versions are
constructed and actions performed. One of the points argued by Sacks (1992) is that
interaction is not merely organized in its general forms, but is also organized in its particulars.
Any level of detail in talk — hesitations, repairs, pauses — can be crucial for a piece of
interaction; indeed, much of the business of interaction may be happening in the details.
approaches to interaction, and at the same time attempted to identify grounds for making
inferences about cognitive entities (Coulter, 1989; Pomerantz, 1990/91). CA and DA are
reasons. CA highlights a symmetry between the position of the participant and analyst in a
going on. In most cases, a turn of talk is based upon and displays some sort of analysis of the
12
sense of the previous turn of talk. A turn may be responded to as a question, a criticism, an
invitation and so on — and in responding to it in this way the speaker displays their
understanding. And if the displayed understanding is faulty various repair mechanisms can
come into play in the next turn to sort things out. What this provides, then, is a way for the
analyst to use the participants’ own, situated, analyses to help check the adequacy of their
own analysts’ claims. The point is not that the analyst is forced to take such displayed
understanding at face value, nor that interaction is always well oiled and explicit;
This ability to use participants’ understandings to build up the analysts’ account distinguishes
this work from other types of constructionist research which have focused on texts or
documents, or considered talk abstracted from its conversational context. While discourse
analysts have often worked with interview material, conversation analysts have worked
almost exclusively with natural occurring records of interaction collected with tape recorders
There are a number of discussions focused on CA methods. Atkinson and Heritage (1984b),
Heritage (forthcoming), Wooffitt (1990) and Wootton (1989) cover a range of practical
issues, Psathas (1990, 1995) attempts to characterise the analytic mentality that is involved in
this work, and Drew (in press) introduces CA in a way that is designed specifically for
psychologists.
The study of rhetoric was revived in the 1970s and 80s with a particular concern with the
argumentative organization of texts and the different rhetorical forms used to make them
persuasive (Perelman and Olbrecht-Tyteca, 1971). Billig (1987) has highlighted the way
rhetorical ideas can be used to reformulate thinking in psychology. For example, that the
metaphor of an argument can be used to make sense of thought processes; instead of viewing
13
thought as the operation of some calculating mechanism on internally consistent systems of
belief, thought can be seen as riven with argumentative dilemmas whose structure comes
from the available interpretative repertoires of a culture (Billig, et al., 1988). So while a more
a person’s individual attitude, a rhetorical analyst might be concerned to reveal the way that
sequentially organized, but are not necessarily so. Sometimes they may be expressed as direct
and explicit argumentative claims using the speech act vocabulary of argument (‘I don’t agree
with that’); at other times rhetorical contrasts may be built implicitly, often through
Counsel: And during the evening, didn’t Mr. O [the defendant] come over to sit with
you?
(See Drew, 1992; Edwards and Potter, 1992). Discourse analytic studies sometimes collect a
becomes possible to identify the rhetorical targets and oppositions of particular arguments and
descriptions.
Interviews have been used extensively in discourse analytic work; however, they are
construed in a novel manner. Traditionally, the goal of an interview was to produce a piece
of colourless, neutral interaction. However, in practice interviews are as complex and vivid
14
as any other type of interaction, and responses to answers which may seem neutral and non-
committal in the abstract may have an important impact on the trajectory of the interaction.
In discourse research interviews have been used extensively because they allow a relatively
achieve when collecting naturalistic materials. They also allow a high degree of control over
participants interpretative practices rather than an instrument for accessing a veridical account
of something that happened elsewhere, or a set of attitudes and beliefs (Mischler, 1986; Potter
and Mulkay, 1985). An interview can be a particularly effective way of getting at the range of
interpretative repertoires that a participant has available as well as some of the uses to which
those repertoires are put. Billig (1992) and Wetherell and Potter (1992) are both extended
discourse based studies which work principally from interview material and illustrate some of
the analytic possibilities they provide. Widdicombe and Wooffitt (1995) draw extensively on
CA to show how interviews can be dealt with as an interaction rather than a research
instrument, exploring the way that different social categories are worked up, used and
Despite the virtues of this use of interviews, there are problems in relating the practices that
happen in interviews to what goes on elsewhere and avoiding the interaction being swamped
by the interviewer’s own categories and constructions. Even the most open ended of
Moreover, even when an interview is understood as an interaction in its own right, the
dominant question and answer format is not ideal for getting at the sorts of turn-by-turn
display of action and understanding that conversation analysts have utilised so effectively.
Partly for these reasons, discourse analysts have been increasingly turning to the study of
15
What is the role of records of natural interaction?
Arguably, one of the most astonishing omissions in 20th century psychology has been the
study of what people do; their interactions in the home and workplace. The few attempts in
this direction were marred by a simplistic behaviourism which ignored interaction or reduced
natural interaction, discourse analysts have started to work with transcripts of conversations,
The term ‘natural’ here should be taken contrastively. These settings are made up of natural
interaction in the sense that it is not ‘got up’ by the researcher. The test is whether the
interaction would have taken place, and in the form that it did, if the researcher had not been
born. Of course, the use of recording technology itself can have an impact on participants’
understandings of a situation and their actions. However, in practice there are a range of
techniques for minimizing the intrusive effects of recording, such as using a period of
acclimatisation. Practice suggests that such effects are often surprisingly small.
In most cases, such records cannot be used directly — a tape, particularly a video tape, is a
very clumsy way to deal with materials. What is required is a transcription that turns the
record into a form that can be read through quickly, that allows different sections to be
compared, and that can easily be reproduced in research papers. The transcript does not
replace the tape — often it is most helpful to work in parallel with both.
A transcript is not a neutral, simple rendition of the words on a tape (Ochs, 1979). Different
with a speech therapy interest will need a system that records phonetics; a sociolinguist
concerned with language variety will need some way of showing accent. What system will a
16
discourse analyst need? One common approach claims that discourse analysis concerned
with broad content themes such as interpretative repertoires needs only a relatively basic
scheme which represents the words and relatively gross features such as corrections and
hesitations, while analysis more concerned with interactional specifics will need to represent
pause length, emphasis, various intonational features, overlap and so on. Although there is
some sense to this, it obscures some tensions that need careful consideration.
In the first place, it is not easy, nor analytically satisfactory, to make a strong distinction
between content and interaction in this way. Indeed, one of the consequences of using a basic
transcription scheme is that it will often fail to capture those features which show how the
content is occasioned by the interaction that is taking place. For example, with a research
interview it may obscure just how much the participants’ ‘responses’ are a product of various
activities (some very subtle) on the part of the interviewer. Moreover, one of the virtues of
discourse analytic work is that readers should be able to assess the interpretations that are
made because at least a selection of the original analytic materials have been reproduced. It
might be argued that, even if the analyst is not making use of interactional specifics, a reader
should have them available so they can make their own judgements.
Having made this strong argument for a fuller approach to transcription, it is important to
stress that doing a good transcript is very demanding and time consuming. It is hard to give a
standard figure for how long it takes because much depends on the quality of the recording
(fuzzy, quiet tapes can double the time necessary) and the type of interaction (a couple talking
down the phone present much less of a challenge than a multi-party conversation with a lot
overlapping talk and extraneous noise); nevertheless, a ratio of one hour of tape to twenty of
transcription time is not unreasonable. However, this time should not be thought of as dead
time before the analysis proper. Often some of the most revealing analytic insights come
during transcription because a profound engagement with the material is needed to produce
good transcript — it is always useful to make analytic notes in parallel to the actual
transcription.
17
The most commonly used system in conversation analysis, and increasingly discourse
analysis, was developed by Gail Jefferson. It is a system developed to be easily used with the
symbols on a standard keyboard, and records features of interaction that have been found to
be important for talk-in-interaction. For fuller accounts of Jefferson’s system see Atkinson
and Heritage (1984), Button and Lee (1987), Jefferson (1985) and Psathas and Anderson
(1990).
There are numerous CA studies that show how features of interaction that are often missed
out in more basic transcripts can be analytically useful and interactionally consequential.
Most of the work on ‘preference organization’, for example, (the differential marking of
Here is a fragment of talk from a relationship counselling interview. The Counsellor (C) is
asking the woman (W) about the sequence of events leading to the request for counselling.
Counsellor: Wha- (.) what happened at that point.
Y’know?
(DE-JF/C2/S1 — p.4)
18
The Transcription Symbols Used in the Extract
A dash marks a sharp cut off of the prior word.
intonation.
Two things may be immediately striking about this extract. First, the transcription symbols
may make it hard to read easily. Second, there seems to be a lot of ‘mess’ in it: repairs and
changes of gear. On the first point, reading transcript is itself something of a skill which
develops with familiarity. After a period getting used to materials of this kind it is the
transcript without the symbols that looks odd — idealized, cleaned and shorn of its specificity
— while the fuller transcript becomes evocative of the interaction captured on the tape. It
On the second point, what seems on first reading to be mess is quickly understandable as
something much more organized. The woman breaks off her direct answer to provide a
description of her partner. This description is reformulated and emphasised until it has a
precise sense that is suitable for the business in hand (briefly and over-simply: starting to
display how the relationship problem is his fault — for more on this, see Edwards,
forthcoming; Potter, forthcoming). The careful transcript here allows us to see this final
version being actively shaped, and gives us a feel for the versions that are rejected as
unsatisfactory.
19
What about reliability and validity?
The notions of reliability and validity have increasingly taken on a mix of everyday and
perhaps a triangulation from different research methods. Because of the different theoretical
assumptions in discourse work, along with its largely non-quantitative nature, these
approaches to reliability and validity are largely unworkable here. Nevertheless, these are
important considerations which can be, and have been, addressed in this work.
Reliability and validity are not so clearly separated in discourse work. Various considerations
are relevant; four important ones being deviant case analysis, participants’ understanding,
Deviant case analysis. A discourse analytic study will often work with a collection of
instances of some putative phenomenon with the aim of showing some pattern or regularity.
For example, an analyst might claim that news interviewees generally avoid treating
interviewers as responsible for views expressed in questions (Heritage and Greatbatch, 1996).
One of the most useful analytic phenomena are cases which seem to go against the pattern or
are deviant in some way. In this type of work such deviant cases are not necessarily
disconfirmations of the pattern (although they could be); instead their special features may
help confirm the genuineness of the pattern (Heritage, 1988). For example, when a news
interviewee does treat the interviewer as accountable for a view posed in a question it can
create serious trouble for the interaction (Potter, forthcoming). The deviant case can show up
just the kind of problem that show why the standard pattern should take the form it does.
important elements is its use of participants’ own understandings. Thus instead of the analyst
20
saying that this turn of talk is a compliment, say, the focus is on how the participants’ treat it.
that’s very sweet of you, its just an old top I picked up cheap?’ A common critique of
discourse analytic work is that there is no check on its interpretations. However, a close
Coherence. One of the features of conversation analysis and increasingly of discourse work
is its cumulative nature. A set of studies can be combined together, and can build on the
insights of earlier work. For example, work on fact construction builds on the insights about
accountability from earlier studies, and its success provides a further confirmation of the
validity of those studies (Edwards and Potter, 1993). There is a sense, then, that each new
study provides a check on the adequacy of previous studies that are drawn on. Those studies
which capture something about interaction can be built on, those that do not are likely to
become ignored.
Readers’ Evaluation. Perhaps the most important and distinctive feature in the validation of
discourse work is the presentation of rich and extended materials in a way that allows readers
of discourse studies to evaluate their adequacy. This has two facets. On the one hand, it
allows them to assess the particular interpretation that is made as it is presented in parallel
with the original materials. This is not the case in much ethnographic work where the
interpretations have to be taken largely on trust and what data that is presented is largely pre-
theorized; nor is it the case with much traditional experimental and content analytic work
where it is rare for ‘raw’ data to be included and more than one or two illustrative codings
presented. On the other hand, readers are themselves skilled interactants with a wide range of
recipients of compliment, and so on. Thus the judgements they can make are not merely
abstract ones of the relation between materials and interpretations, but of the adequacy of
21
A final comment on validity. These features are not all present in all discourse studies; nor
Some researchers have treated the issue of quantification as a definitional one; it is part of the
However, I think this gives too much importance to the quantity/quality divide and risks an
uncritical support of qualitative work and criticism of quantitative. I would rather see
easily miss the point that the research goal is often that of explicating what should count as an
The issue of how far quantification is appropriate in conversation and discourse analytic work
has received some detailed attention recently. A useful source is a discussion between
different perspectives on the role of counting (Wieder, 1993). Schegloff (1993) and Heritage
(forthcoming) both provide clear arguments for being cautious when attempting to quantify
because of a range of difficulties that arise when transforming discursive materials into
numerical form. However, the issue of quantification is undoubtedly going to become more
increasing interest in study quantification practices as a topic. Such work has looked at
quantification in both technical and everyday situations. Some of the classic work in this area
22
concerned to show that mundane practices of quantification are not a poor imitation of the
seemingly more precise and accurate technical practices of statisticians and scientists, but
have a subtle and sophisticated logic of their own. For example a request for an appointment
at half past four is not merely a less precise way of requesting an appointment at four twenty
eight. Rather, different sets of expectations about punctuality, the type of meeting it is, and
what delay would count as late are called into play in each case.
Other work in ‘ethnostatistics’, as Gephart (1988) has dubbed the study of statistics as a
cultural practice, has looked at the way health economists have performed cost-benefit
analyses (Ashmore et al, 1989) the construction of accounts of the success of charity funded
cancer treatment (Potter, et al, 1991) and the textual practices for representing the ‘subjects’
in social psychology experiments (Billig, 1994). This work eats away at the idea that there is
a straightforward choice between doing quantitative and qualitative research, and that entirely
Is that it?
There are many other considerations about the nature of discourse research and
constructionist work more generally that could be addressed here. However, coming to see
these abstract considerations is quite different from learning to do analysis which is very
much a craft skill like bike riding or chicken sexing. There is no substitute for learning by
doing, and such learning is almost always better done collaboratively so that interpretations
and ideas can be explored with co-workers. Such learning is time consuming, hard work and
often frustrating. However, the goal is to develop an analytic mentality that is sensitive to the
23
References
Antaki, C. (1994) Arguing and Explaining The social organization of accounts. London;
Sage.
Ashmore, M., Mulkay, M. and Pinch, T. (1989). Health and Efficiency: A sociological study
Ashmore, M., Myers, G. and Potter, J. (1994). Seven days in the library: Discourse, rhetoric,
Atkinson, J.M. (1984). Our Master’s Voices: The Language and Body Language of Politics.
London: Methuen.
Atkinson and Heritage (1984a). Introduction. In J.M. Atkinson and J. Heritage (Eds)
Atkinson, J.M. and Heritage, J. (Eds.) (1984a). Structures of Social Action: Studies in
Billig, M. (1989a). The argumentative nature of holding strong views: A case study.
24
Billig, M. (1988b). Rhetorical and historical aspects of attitudes: the case of the British
Billig, M. (1994). Repopulating the depopulated pages of social psychology, Theory and
Psychology.
Billig, M., Condor, S., Edwards, D., Gane, M., Middleton, D.J. and Radley, A.R. (1988).
University Press.
Clifford, J. and Marcus, G.E. (Eds.) (1986). Writing Culture: the poetics and politics of
Drew, P. (1992) Descriptions in legal settings. In P. Drew and J. Heritage (Eds) Talk at
25
Edwards, D. (1994) Script formulations: An analysis of event descriptions in conversation.
28 (4), 319-50.
Edwards, D. and Potter, J. (1993) Language and causation: A discursive action model of
London: Sage.
Gergen, K.J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American
Burman and I. Parker (Eds). Discourse Analytic Research: Repertoires and Readings
Harré, R. (1992). Personal Being: A theory for individual psychology. Oxford; Blackwell.
Harré, R. (1992). Social Being: A theory for social psychology (2nd Ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.
Sage.
26
Heritage, J.C. (1995). Conversation analysis: Methodological aspects. In U. Quasthoff (Ed.),
Aspects of oral communication (pp. 391-418). Berlin and New York: De Gruyter.
Hewitt, J.P. (1994). Self and Society: A symbolic interactionist social psychology (6th Ed).
Hollway, W. (1989). Subjectivity and Method in Psychology: Gender, meaning and science.
London: Sage.
Jefferson, G. (1985). An exercise in the transcription and analysis of laughter. In T. van Dijk
Latour, B. and Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory Life: the social construction of scientific
Latour, B. and Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory Life: the construction of scientific facts (2nd
Burman and I. Parker (Eds) Discourse Analytic Research: Repertoires and readings
Mischler, E.G. (1986). Research Interviewing: Context and narrative. Cambridge, Mass.;
Mulkay, M. (1979). Science and the Sociology of Knowledge. London; Allen Unwin.
Nairn, R. and McCreanor, T.N. (1991). Race talk and common sense: patterns of discourse
27
Nofsinger, R.E. (1991). Everyday Conversation. London; Sage.
Perelman, C. and L. Olbrechts-Tyteca (1971). The New Rhetoric. University of Notre Dame
Press.
London; Sage. Potter, J. and Mulkay, M. (1985) Scientists’ interview talk: Interviews
Brown and D. Canter (Eds.) The Research Interview: Uses and Approaches. London:
Academic Press.
Potter, J. and Reicher, S. (1987) ‘Discourses of community and conflict: The organization of
social categories in accounts of a ‘riot’’, British Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 25-
40.
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. (1987) Discourse and Social Psychology: Beyond Attitudes and
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. (1988) Accomplishing attitudes: Fact and evaluation in racist
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. (1993) Analyzing discourse. In A. Bryman and B. Burgess (Eds)
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. (1995) Discourse analysis. In J. Smith, R. Harré and L. van
Potter, J., Wetherell, M., and Chitty, A. (1991). Quantification rhetoric — cancer on
Potter, J., Wetherell, M., Gill, R. and Edwards, D. (1990). Discourse — noun, verb or social
28
Psathas, G. (1990) Introduction. In Psathas, G. (Ed.). Interactional Competence.
Radke, H.L. and Stam, H.J. (1994). Power/Gender: Social relations in theory and practice.
London; Sage.
Schegloff, E.A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defence of
Shotter, J. and Gergen, K. (1994). Series blurb. In T.R. Sarbin and J.I. Kitsuse (Eds)
Wertsch, J.V. (1991). Voices of the Mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action.
Wetherell, M. and Potter, J. (1992). Mapping the Language of Racism: Discourse and the
University Press.
Wetherell, M., H. Stiven and J. Potter (1987). Unequal egalitarianism: a preliminary study of
Harvester/Wheatsheaf.
29
Wieder, D.L. (Ed.)(1993). Colloquy: On issues of quantification in conversation analysis,
In P. Luff, D. Frohlich and G.N. Gilbert (Eds.), Computers and Conversation. New
Wooffitt, R. (1992a). Analyzing accounts. In Gilbert, G.N. (Ed.) Researching Social Life.
London; Sage.
Wooffitt, R. (1992b). Telling Tales of the Unexpected: The organization of factual accounts.
London: Harvester/Wheatsheaf.
30