JCSP Mspe For Flow 1
JCSP Mspe For Flow 1
JCSP Mspe For Flow 1
Successful athletes often speak about periods of time in which they were able to
achieve peak performance, a condition commonly referred to as the zone. Despite the
regular use of this term in interviews and articles, an agreed-upon scientific defini-
tion for the zone remains elusive (Cooper, 1998). Among the various characteristics
attributed to being in this state are a fusion of body and mind, a heightened sense of
skill mastery, deep concentration, emotional buoyancy, increased self-confidence,
a focus on the present, low self-consciousness, perceptions of effortlessness, feel-
ings of relaxation, self-transcendence, and automaticity of performance (Cooper,
1998; Sugarman, 1998). Although some research indicates that reaching the zone
may not be voluntary (Alessi, 1995; Ravizza, 1984), there is widespread use of
visualization techniques, martial arts, and psychotherapy, as well as an abundance
of books and articles, intended to help athletes achieve this state (Cooper, 1998).
Sport psychologists interested in further elucidating the nature of the zone have
begun comparing this condition to the state of flow, which has been empirically
defined over the last 30 years (Cooper, 1998; Young & Pain, 1999). Csikszentmi-
halyi (1990) described flow as a mindset that typically occurs when an individual
perceives a balance between the challenges associated with a situation and his or
The authors are with The Catholic University of America in Washington, DC.
334
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 335
her capabilities to meet those demands. This balance can promote flow qualities,
including centering of attention, perceptions of control over actions and environ-
ment, lower self-consciousness, losing track of time, merging of action and aware-
ness, and greater intrinsic satisfaction (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Stein, Kimiecik,
Daniels, & Jackson, 1995). As with explanations of the zone, flow theory suggests
that people in this state tend to interpret their situation as optimal (Csikszentmihalyi
& LeFevre, 1989; Stein et al., 1995). Unlike the situation with the zone, however,
sport psychologists now have an understanding of how flow relates to athletic
performance, as well as the factors that can affect its occurrence.
Studies with athletes have delineated several characteristics of peak-perfor-
mance moments. Relaxation, confidence, high energy, present-centered focus,
extraordinary awareness, feeling in control, and detachment from distractions are
among the major factors that have been identified (e.g., Cohn, 1991; Garfield &
Bennett, 1984). Jackson (2000) indicated that such findings suggest the existence
of a clear relationship between the mental characteristics associated with peak
performance and Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) conceptualization of flow. Since flow
entails optimal mental functioning, it is often when athletes are in a state of flow that
they produce their best results (Jackson, 2000). Jackson and Roberts (1992) sug-
gested support for the notion that flow is the process underlying peak performance.
Research has also suggested that certain factors can influence the experience of
flow in sport and that a large percentage of elite and college athletes perceive flow
as controllable (Jackson, 1995; Russell, 2001). Russell identified characteristics that
could promote a state of flow. Among them were elevated confidence, maintaining
constructive thoughts, appropriate focus, optimal precompetitive arousal, and high
intrinsic motivation. Conversely, Jackson, Kimiecik, Ford, and Marsh (1998) dem-
onstrated that anxiety and perceived athletic skill deficits can interfere with flow.
Their analyses showed that it was the cognitive aspects of anxiety (concentration
disruption and worry) that most prevented flow. Another factor that may disturb
flow is perfectionism, which can at times undermine sport performance and foster
dissatisfaction during competition (Flett & Hewitt, 2005).
Catley and Duda (1997) reported that the improved performance and enjoy-
ment that are characteristic of flow states may be fostered through established sport
intervention techniques like goal setting, thought stopping, imagery, and self-talk.
Researchers (e.g., Johnson, Hrycaiko, Johnson, & Halas, 2004; Kornspan, Overby,
& Lerner, 2004) have found at best inconsistent results for these approaches, yet
many athletes, coaches, and sport psychologists use these techniques in an effort
to minimize the impact of negative cognitions and improve athletic performance
(Conroy & Metzler, 2004). A focus on controlling or eliminating maladaptive
thoughts and emotions, however, may not be as beneficial as previously assumed,
since it could paradoxically trigger a monitoring process that searches for negative
or unwanted cognitions, bringing them to awareness (Purdon, 1999; Wegner, 1994).
Such awareness may lead to self- and task-irrelevant focus, which can negatively
impact performance (Gardner & Moore, 2004, 2006, 2007).
Furthermore, Moore (2003) noted that procedures like goal setting, imagery,
and self-talk fail to meet the standards required for designation as efficacious
interventions for the enhancement of sport performance and encouraged the devel-
opment of alternative approaches. Gardner and Moore (2004) stated that one fresh
approach could be a mindfulness-based program, which emphasizes nonjudgmen-
336 Kaufman, Glass, and Arnkoff
strated. For instance, Klinger, Barta, and Glas (1981) found that struggles in game
performance shifted the attentional focus of intercollegiate basketball players from
appropriate external game-related cues to more self-judging future-oriented ones,
potentially worsening performance. In perhaps the first application of mindfulness to
sport, Kabat-Zinn, Beall, and Rippe (1985) reported the results of training the 1984
U.S. Olympic Men’s Rowing Team in mindfulness. Many rowers who medaled felt
the training had helped them to prepare and perform optimally. Further, extending
earlier research that showed a significant connection between mindfulness and flow
experiences in nonathlete performers (e.g., Wright, Sadlo, & Stew, 2006), Kee and
Wang (2008) found that greater mindfulness in university athletes related to higher
levels of the challenge-skill balance, merging of action and awareness, clear goals,
concentration, and loss of self-consciousness aspects of dispositional flow.
The Mindfulness-Acceptance-Commitment (MAC) approach to sport perfor-
mance enhancement was developed by Gardner and Moore (2004, 2006, 2007)
and is an integration and adaptation of MBCT (Segal et al., 2002) and Acceptance
and Commitment Therapy (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) for use with athletes
and other high-level performers. According to Gardner and Moore (2006), seven
modules make up the MAC protocol: (a) psychoeducation, (b) mindfulness, (c)
values identification, (d) acceptance, (e) commitment, (f) skill consolidation and
poise, and (g) maintaining and enhancing MAC. MAC is conducted in these seven
modules (each of which could last for one or more sessions to achieve the goals
of the module). The manualized MAC protocol has garnered initial empirical evi-
dence for its efficacy in enhancing performance. Wolanin (2005) investigated the
efficacy of this approach with college athletes and found (see Gardner & Moore,
2006; Wolanin, 2005) a small overall treatment effect when comparing performance
ratings for a MAC treatment (37% had increased ratings) and a control group (14%
had increased ratings) that included participants both with and without subclinical
psychological difficulties, and a large treatment effect for those who had no subclini-
cal psychological difficulties. As such, the data offered support for this approach
as a performance enhancement intervention for psychologically healthy collegiate
athletes. As indicated by Gardner and Moore (2007), Lutkenhouse, Gardner, and
Moore (2007) conducted a larger trial with collegiate athletes, which was both
randomized and controlled, and found that a significantly greater number of athletes
completing MAC treatment (32% versus 10% of athletes receiving a traditional
psychological skills training protocol) demonstrated a clinically significant increase
in coach ratings of performance at posttest.
The present study was an attempt to explore how a new mental training
approach, Mindful Sport Performance Enhancement (MSPE), could impact flow
states, as well as athletic performance and the psychological factors thought to
influence it. MSPE is conceptualized as a way of extending MBSR and MBCT
to athletes, drawing on exercises in those approaches that target the cultivation of
mindfulness (e.g., sitting meditation, body scan, mindful yoga, walking meditation)
and adding a walking meditation adapted to be specific to fundamental movements
involved in the sport of focus. Like MAC, MSPE emphasizes the development of
mindfulness skills and through them, a degree of acceptance. Unlike MAC, MSPE
does not include a focus on values, value-driven behavior, or commitment.
Athletes involved in self-paced, closed-skill, objectively scored sports requiring
significant mental focus and fine motor movements, such as archery and golf, were
338 Kaufman, Glass, and Arnkoff
thought to benefit most from MSPE training and were thus chosen for this study. It
was hypothesized that archers and golfers who participated in a MSPE workshop
would experience reductions in sport anxiety, perfectionism, and thought disrup-
tion, as well as growth in sport confidence. Furthermore, it was predicted that these
athletes would become more mindful, display greater elevations in performance,
and be more likely to enter flow while participating in their sport. Finally, anxiety,
perfectionism, and thought disruption were expected to be inversely related to
flow, while confidence and mindfulness were expected to relate positively to flow.
Method
Participants
A total of 32 recreational athletes who practice and compete at community facilities
in the Washington, DC metropolitan area registered for the MSPE workshops. Of
these athletes, 11 (34.4%) were archers and 21 (65.6%) were golfers. In addition,
23 (71.9%) of the participants were men and 9 (28.1%) were women. The sample
was predominantly Caucasian (90.6%), but also included one African American
(3.1%), one Asian/Pacific Islander (3.1%), and one Hispanic/Latino (3.1%). Par-
ticipant ages ranged from 18 to 76 years old (M = 52.19). Three of the workshop
registrants (1 archer and 2 golfers) dropped out before the first session. The mean
number of weekly sessions attended by the remaining 29 registrants was 2.97,
with 4 (13.8%) athletes attending one session, 4 (13.8%) attending two sessions,
10 (34.5%) attending three sessions, and 11 (37.9%) attending all four sessions.
Before beginning MSPE, 18 (56.3%) of the athletes reported that they had
previous exposure to sport psychology, and 15 (46.9%) indicated that they had
practiced meditation, yoga, or similar contemplative activities. The length of time
that the athletes had participated in their respective sport ranged from 1 to 50 years
(M = 17.44), and the number of times they reported participating in their sport
during an average month ranged from 1 to 28 times (M = 9.19). Before starting the
MSPE training, the number of archery tournaments these athletes had participated in
during the last 2 years ranged from 0 to 50 (M = 13.82). The golfers’ preworkshop
handicap scores ranged from 0 to 36 (M = 15.81).
Procedure
Archer Recruitment. Target archers (recurve, traditional, and compound) in the
mid-Atlantic region were recruited from clubs and teams, lists of participants in
recent competitions, and by posting flyers at archery shops and tournaments. Each
announcement made invited athletes to a free, 4-week mental training workshop
in mindfulness meditation designed to improve archery performance. Random
assignment to an experimental or wait-list control group, as originally planned, was
not possible because too few archers registered. Thus, one workshop for archers
was run in the spring, a time thought to be convenient for their shooting schedules.
Golfer Recruitment. Golfers of all skill levels were recruited from the mid-
Atlantic region by posting flyers in golf course pro shops and on message
boards in area businesses, advertising on golf association websites and in major
newspapers, as well as by emailing local college golf coaches. Despite initial
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 339
Mindfulness Training
A treatment manual for MSPE was developed for the current study, which inte-
grates and adapts elements of Kabat-Zinn’s (1982, 1990) Mindfulness-based Stress
Reduction and Segal et al.’s (2002) Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy. Among
the many other sources of information that helped shape the MSPE manual were
(a) the authors of the first reported effort to train athletes in mindfulness (B. Beall,
personal communication, March 7, 2006; Kabat-Zinn et al., 1985); (b) additional
experts on mindfulness (K. Brandt, personal communication, April 18, 2006; Z.
Segal, personal communication, April 10, 2006); (c) a renowned archery coach in the
mid-Atlantic region (J. Body, personal communication, February 13, 2006); and (d)
various books on the psychology of archery (Herrigel, 1953; Lee & de Bondt, 2005),
golf (Rotella & Cullen, 2004), and other highly mental sports like tennis (Gallwey,
340 Kaufman, Glass, and Arnkoff
1974). The introductory session of MSPE provides a sport-specific rationale for the
use of mindfulness in athletics, and each session of the training contains exercises
that are key elements of Kabat-Zinn and Segal et al.’s mindfulness programs, such
as the raisin exercise, the body scan, mindful breathing, the sitting meditation,
mindful yoga, and the walking meditation. MSPE additionally includes a unique
component, which is a walking meditation modified to be specific to the sport of
focus. See the Appendix for a summary outline of the MSPE treatment protocol.
Although Kabat-Zinn and Segal et al.’s programs are 8 weeks long, concerns
about asking for that level of commitment from busy people who were neither elite
athletes highly invested in performance enhancement nor seeking treatment for a
debilitating physical or psychological problem led to a decision to make MSPE a
4-week program with slightly longer (2.5–3 hr) sessions. There is precedent for
using a brief mindfulness intervention successfully; for example, nurses and nurse
aides who received four 30-min group mindfulness sessions experienced significant
improvements in burnout symptoms, relaxation, and life satisfaction (Mackenzie,
Poulin, & Seidman-Carlson, 2006). In addition, Jain et al. (2007) reported that a
one-month mindfulness meditation training program reduced distress and improved
positive mood states in students when compared with a control group.
Measures
Background Questionnaire. Two versions of this 12-item measure were designed
for the current study, one specific to archers and one to golfers. All athletes report
their gender, age, ethnic background, the nature of any sport psychological or
meditation experiences they have had, their best athletic performance scores (both
lifetime and within the last 12 months), how long they have participated in their
sport, and frequency of current participation in their sport.
Sport Anxiety Scale (SAS). The SAS (Smith, Smoll, & Schutz, 1990) is a
21-item self-report measure of cognitive and somatic trait sport anxiety, with
each item rated on a 4-point, Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4
(very much so). This instrument contains three subscales assessing somatic anxiety,
worry, and concentration disruption. Support for the internal consistency, test-retest
reliability, convergent validity, and construct validity of the SAS have been found
(Smith et al., 1990). As recommended by Smith, Cumming, and Smoll (2006), three
items were not included in the scoring to maximize the SAS’s factorial integrity.
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS). The MPS (Frost, Marten, Lahart,
& Rosenblate, 1990) has 35 items, each of which is rated on a scale from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), and contains six subscales: concern over mistakes,
personal standards, parental expectations, parental criticism, doubts about actions,
and organization. It has been found to have excellent internal consistency, and to
correlate highly with other perfectionism measures (Antony, Orsillo, & Roemer,
2001; Frost et al., 1990). To date, perfectionism in sport has been measured almost
exclusively by this measure (Gotwals, Dunn, & Wayment, 2003).
Carolina Sport Confidence Inventory (CSCI). The CSCI (Manzo, Silva, & Mink,
2001) is a 13-item self-report instrument designed to assess sport confidence.
After deciding which of two listed statements best describes them, respondents
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 341
select whether the statement is somewhat true for me or very true for me. The CSCI
contains two subscales: dispositional optimism and sport competence. Support for
the internal consistency, convergent validity, and test-retest reliability of the CSCI
have been found (Manzo et al., 2001). For the current study, items on this instrument
were modified slightly to create versions specific to the sports of golf and archery.
Thought Occurrence Questionnaire for Sport (TOQS). The TOQS
(Hatzigeorgiadis & Biddle, 2000) assesses the cognitive interference or thought
disruption that athletes may experience during competition. It contains 17 items,
each of which is rated on a scale from 1 (almost never) to 7 (very often). This
measure includes three subscales: task-related worries, task-irrelevant thoughts, and
thoughts of escape. Analyses have suggested that the TOQS has adequate convergent
validity, concurrent validity, discriminate validity, and internal consistency
(Hatzigeorgiadis & Biddle, 2000). In the current study, the TOQS instructions were
modified slightly to refer specifically to thoughts during a typical competition in
either archery or golf.
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS). The KIMS (Baer, Smith, &
Allen, 2004) is designed to assess the tendency to be mindful in daily life, with the
39 items rated from 1 (never or very rarely true) to 5 (very often or always true).
It has four subscales: observing, describing, acting with awareness, and accepting
without judgment and has been shown to have internal consistency, test-retest
reliability, and content validity (Baer et al., 2004).
Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2). The DFS-2 (Jackson & Eklund, 2002)
is a measure of the tendency to experience flow during a physical activity, with
each of the 36 items rated on a scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). This scale is
theoretically grounded in Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) concept of flow and contains
nine subscales: challenge-skill balance, action-awareness merging, clear goals,
unambiguous feedback, concentration on the task at hand, sense of control, loss of
self-consciousness, time transformation, and autotelic experience. Analyses of the
DFS-2 have indicated that it is a reliable and valid measure of the flow construct
(Jackson & Eklund, 2004). In this study, the DFS-2 instructions were changed
slightly to make it clear that it was asking archers and golfers about experiences
in their respective sport.
Credibility and Expectations Measure (CEM). The CEM was developed for
this study to assess the perceived credibility of the workshop and expectations
of performance improvement following the training. It was adapted from the
Reaction to Treatment Questionnaire (RTQ; Holt & Heimberg, 1990), which itself
was adapted from work by Borkovec and Nau (1972). The CEM has eight items,
each of which is rated on a 10-point, Likert-type scale. Two CEM versions were
created, one worded for archers and one for golfers.
Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS). The TMS (Lau et al., 2006) assesses state
levels of mindfulness immediately following the practice of a mindfulness exercise
(Bishop et al., 2004). Each of the 13 items is rated on a scale ranging from 0 (not
at all) to 4 (very much). Analyses by Lau et al. showed that this instrument is a
reliable and valid measure of state mindfulness, and that it contains two subscales:
curiosity and decentering.
342 Kaufman, Glass, and Arnkoff
Daily Mindfulness Log. Adapted from Segal et al.’s (2002) Homework Record
Form, this measure asks participants to keep a daily account of whether they
practiced mindfulness skills and the length of their practice, as well as any
observations they note.
Daily Sport Performance Log. Created for this study, this log asks participants
for data on their sport performances that occur between workshop sessions,
including the nature of their athletic activities (practice/friendly or competition),
the scores they obtain, and their satisfaction with their scores. Two versions were
developed, one worded for archers and one for golfers.
Flow State Scale-2 (FSS-2). The FSS-2 (Jackson & Eklund, 2002) is designed
to assess flow experiences during a recently completed physical activity. The
items are identical to those on the DFS-2, but are worded in the past tense,
and investigations have suggested that it is a reliable and valid measure of
Csikszentmihalyi’s flow construct (Jackson & Eklund, 2004).
Exit Questionnaire (EXT). Created for this study, the 14-item EXT asks participants
to report their reactions to and experiences during the workshop. The first 8 items
are similar to those on the CEM but reworded to assess reactions. The final 6 items
ask for open-ended responses regarding what participants liked most, what was
most challenging, how performance in their sport was affected, how much they
expect the training to affect their future performance, how nonsport aspects of their
lives have been affected, and recommendations for improvements to the workshop.
A coding manual was developed for the scoring of these open-ended items, and
raters trained until they achieved a kappa of .80. Kappa values for the coded items
ranged from .37–1.00. Two EXT versions were created, one worded for archers
and one for golfers.
Results
Baseline Differences between Athlete Groups
Independent samples t tests were used to determine whether the archers differed
from the golfers in age, number of years playing their sport of concentration, and
frequency of meditation per week, as well as on trait measures of anxiety, perfec-
tionism, confidence, thought disruption, mindfulness, and flow before attending
the MSPE workshop. No significant preworkshop differences were found between
these groups at the time they registered for the training. In addition, chi-square
analyses revealed no differences between the athlete groups in gender or ethnicity.
action and awareness (r = .61, p < .01), clear goals, (r = .49, p < .05), loss of self-
consciousness (r = .49, p < .05), and autotelic experience (r = .48, p < .05) flow
dimensions.
Discussion
The present investigation assessed how a new brief mental training approach, Mind-
ful Sport Performance Enhancement (MSPE), affected the athletic performance and
certain performance-related psychological characteristics of archers and golfers.
These types of athletes were chosen due to the hypothesis that self-paced, closed-
skill, objectively scored sports requiring a high degree of mental focus and fine
motor movement would allow for an optimal analysis of the efficacy of MSPE.
Overall, the results suggest that MSPE has promise as an intervention for use with
athletesto enhance flow, mindfulness, and aspects of sport confidence.
The finding that may have the most substantial implications for the role of
MSPE is that the level of state flow achieved by the athletes during their weekly
performances changed significantly over the course of the training. Specifically,
there were significant changes in the level of overall state flow and the unambigu-
ous feedback (i.e., clarity regarding ongoing evaluation of performance) aspect of
flow. In each of these cases, there was an increase from week to week. Evidence
that MSPE can affect state flow levels is important in performance-enhancement
research, since flow has been linked to the zone and peak performance (e.g., Jack-
son, 2000; Jackson & Roberts, 1992).
Missing data excluded a number of the participants from these analyses, so
it is possible that those athletes who consistently filled out the state flow measure
differed from the rest in terms of their motivation, commitment, or other factors
that could have influenced the findings. It is also interesting, however, that some
significant results were found with such a small number of participants, suggesting
that, with a larger sample, some of these findings could possibly have been stronger.
It seems logical to speculate that the brevity of MSPE could help account for
why there were significant changes in dimensions of state, but not trait flow. Jack-
son and Eklund (2002) differentiated their state from their trait measure of flow
by indicating that the state measure assesses flow experiences within a particular
event, while the trait measure assesses the autotelic personality style or how psy-
chologically equipped one generally is to experience flow. Certainly it would take
longer for a personality change to occur than a change during an athletic event.
Although the current study revealed no significant findings regarding changes
in trait flow, it extends the line of research exploring whether certain psychologi-
cal factors are related to the occurrence of flow in sport (e.g., Jackson et al., 1998;
Kee & Wang, 2008; Russell, 2001). Support was found for the hypothesized pre-
workshop relationships between dispositional flow and the other trait variables,
with flow relating negatively to anxiety, perfectionism, and thought disruption, and
positively to confidence and mindfulness. The strong positive relationship between
flow and mindfulness observed in this study and by Kee and Wang (2008) has
exciting implications for the use of mindfulness-based interventions. Theoretical
parallels between these constructs are clear. Kabat-Zinn (1994) suggested that
mindfulness meditation is synonymous with the practice of nondoing, which is a
kind of effortless effort. It is this concept of effortless effort that seems to lie at the
348 Kaufman, Glass, and Arnkoff
heart of conceptualizations of flow, peak performance, and the zone. For instance,
Csikszentmihalyi (1997) described flow as reflecting the sense of effortless action
felt during life’s best moments.
In addition to flow, changes in mindfulness were also analyzed. There was
significant change in the golfers’ levels of the decentering aspect of state mind-
fulness, which reflects awareness of experience with some distance, as opposed
to being carried away by thoughts and feelings (Lau et al., 2006). Decentering
increased from session to session, and a comparison of the first and fourth sessions
revealed significantly higher decentering levels at the training’s end. Rotella and
Cullen (2004) suggested that if emotions such as anger overwhelm golfers, they
can lose their judgment regarding strategy and can compound an initial error with
subsequent errors until their round is ruined, highlighting the potential importance
of decentering to performance.
The archers experienced a trend toward significant change in their levels of
overall state mindfulness and the curiosity component of mindfulness. Missing
data and the brevity of training may partially account for the failure to find more
significant change in levels of state mindfulness. Segal et al. (2002) described the
importance of patience when learning mindfulness, indicating that the effects may
only become visible over time. They compared the process to gardening, in that the
ground must be prepared, the seeds must be planted, and nourishing must occur
while waiting for tangible results.
There were also some significant changes in dimensions of dispositional mind-
fulness. The archers experienced a significant increase in overall trait mindfulness.
The golfers, on the other hand, showed a significant increase in the describing aspect
of trait mindfulness. In other words, the golfers improved their ability to covertly
label and note present-moment observations (Baer et al., 2004).
It is interesting that significant change occurred for these athletes during this
brief training in dimensions of dispositional mindfulness but not in dispositional
flow, especially given the strong positive relationship between these constructs. The
strength of this relationship was slightly less at postworkshop, possibly because there
was significant change in one construct and not the other. One potential explanation
for why change occurred in dispositional mindfulness but not dispositional flow
is that MSPE specifically taught participants exercises to build mindfulness skills,
rather than flow skills, and, although the correlation between these constructs is
high, their relationship is not perfect. One should also consider that the demand
characteristic of wanting to answer questions correctly after completing a mind-
fulness training program might have led to a change in mindfulness scores, while
since flow was not directly trained, flow scores did not significantly change. This
possibility calls into question whether real change in mindfulness occurred, since
it would be expected that mindfulness and flow should change simultaneously. It is
also possible that a change in mindfulness would precede a change in flow, which
would not be observed until a later point in time.
Future studies should investigate the differences between mindfulness and
flow in more detail. For example, the current study suggests that the transforma-
tion of time flow dimension may not relate significantly to trait mindfulness, with
other aspects of flow relating significantly but to varying degrees. In addition,
future work should explore in more depth whether changes in mindfulness mediate
changes in flow during interventions for athletes. If so, it could be hypothesized
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 349
could standardize practice in terms of bow type, target type, target distance, and
number of arrows scored. Perhaps having coaches/pros rate performance, as done
with Gardner and Moore’s Mindfulness-Acceptance-Commitment (MAC) approach
to performance enhancement (Gardner & Moore, 2004, 2006, 2007; Lutkenhouse
et al., 2007; Wolanin, 2005), would have dividends as well.
Although the current study did not identify a quantitative change in perfor-
mance over the 4 weeks of MSPE training, postworkshop feedback from many of
the athletes indicated that they thought the workshop did benefit their performance
and that they expected additional benefits in the future. This data came from the
approximately two-thirds of the participants who completed the Exit Question-
naire (EXT). Of course, it is impossible to know whether those athletes who did
not complete the EXT had similar opinions. While two-thirds of the participants
completed the EXT, far fewer of the participants consistently provided scoring data
in their performance logs. Responses to the rating scales on the EXT indicated that,
in general, the athletes felt the training had been at least somewhat successful at
improving their sport performance and that they expected to be increasingly more
satisfied with their performance over the next 5 years. On the open-ended items,
three-quarters of the athletes who completed this measure predicted that the train-
ing would improve their performance quality and/or enjoyment if they continued
to practice mindfulness, one-third said that the training helped them become more
focused on the task at hand while participating in their sport, and over half indicated
that the training improved their ability to cope with stress in their lives outside of
their sport. The most common recommendation for how to improve the training
was to lengthen it. Again, it is impossible to know whether those participants who
did not complete the EXT had similar opinions.
Future improvements in athletic performance may be seen as resulting from
an effect of mindfulness on the production of an “economy of effort” (Hatfield &
Hillman, 2001). Reductions in nonessential cortical resources have been associated
with high levels of skill in an activity (Hatfield & Hillman, 2001), since refinement
in cortical activation is likely to result in smooth, fluid, and efficient movement
(Hatfield, Haufler, Hung, & Spalding, 2004). Neuroimaging research on mindful-
ness is only in its infancy (e.g., Davidson et al., 2003), but future studies should
evaluate this hypothesis.
There were some limitations to this study that future research on MSPE
should address. Recruitment and scheduling complications precluded the use of
viable control groups, limiting interpretations of the results to what changed over
the course of MSPE, as opposed to what changes were caused by it. The relatively
small number of participants and their inconsistent completion of the homework
may have also contributed to some of the initial predictions not being supported.
The sample used was not diverse in terms of ethnicity or age, so it is important not
to generalize these findings to the entire population of archers or golfers. Finally,
while the decision to make the training 4 weeks long may have had its recruitment
benefits, it might take longer than a month for many of the predicted changes to
occur, and, in fact, one of the common recommendations made by participants was
for the workshop to be longer.
Despite these limitations, data from the current study suggest that MSPE
has promise as an intervention to enhance flow, mindfulness, and aspects of sport
confidence, and perhaps, upon further study, a performance enhancement method.
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 351
During a month in which these recreational athletes received this training, there were
changes in both trait and state psychological factors that are considered crucial to
successful performance. Future research with control groups and a range of athlete
types should continue to investigate how MSPE can facilitate the attainment of
flow or the zone through the enhancement of mindfulness, thus potentially helping
athletes to achieve peak performance.
References
Alessi, L.E. (1995). “Breakaway into the zone”: A phenomenological investigation from the
athlete’s perspective. Dissertation Abstracts International, 56 (2-B), 1087.
Antony, M.M., Orsillo, S.M., & Roemer, L. (2001). Practitioner’s guide to empirically based
measures of anxiety. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Argus, G., & Thompson, M. (2008). Perceived social problem solving, perfectionism, and
mindful awareness in clinical depression: An exploratory study. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 32, 745–757.
Baer, R.A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empiri-
cal review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125–143.
Baer, R.A., Smith, G.T., & Allen, K.B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report:
The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11, 191–206.
Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N.D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004).
Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 11, 230–241.
Blythe, W. (2006). To hate like this is to be happy forever: A thoroughly obsessive, inter-
mittently uplifting, and occasionally unbiased account of the Duke-North Carolina
basketball rivalry. New York: HarperCollins.
Borkovec, T.D., & Nau, S.D. (1972). Credibility of analogue therapy rationales. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 3, 257–260.
Brehm, B.A. (1998). Turn exercise into a mental vacation. Available at: http://www.just-
workout.co.nz/articles/mentalvacation.htm. Retrieved March 24, 2005.
Catley, D., & Duda, J.L. (1997). Psychological antecedents of the frequency and intensity
of flow in golfers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 309–322.
Cohn, P.J. (1991). An exploratory study on peak performance in golf. The Sport Psycholo-
gist, 54, 1–14.
Conroy, D.E., & Metzler, J.N. (2004). Patterns of self-talk associated with different forms
of competitive anxiety. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 69–89.
Cooper, A. (1998). Playing in the zone: Exploring the spiritual dimensions of sports. Boston:
Shambhala Publications, Inc.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York:
Harper & Row.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement with everyday
life. New York: Harper Collins.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.
Davidson, R.J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli,
S.F., et al. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness
meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564–570.
Flett, G.L., & Hewitt, P.L. (2005). The perils of perfectionism in sports and exercise. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 14–18.
Frost, R.O., & Henderson, K.J. (1991). Perfectionism and reactions to athletic competition.
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 13, 323–335.
352 Kaufman, Glass, and Arnkoff
Frost, R.O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfection-
ism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468.
Gallwey, W.T. (1974). The inner game of tennis. New York: Random House.
Gardner, F.L., & Moore, Z.E. (2004). A mindfulness-acceptance-commitment-based
approach to athletic performance enhancement: Theoretical considerations. Behavior
Therapy, 35, 707–723.
Gardner, F.L., & Moore, Z.E. (2006). Clinical sport psychology. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Gardner, F.L., & Moore, Z.E. (2007). The psychology of enhancing human performance:
The mindfulness-acceptance-commitment (MAC) approach. New York: Springer.
Garfield, C.A., & Bennett, H.Z. (1984). Peak performance: Mental training techniques of
the world’s greatest athletes. Los Angeles: Tarcher.
Gotwals, J.K., Dunn, J.G.H., & Wayment, H.A. (2003). An examination of perfectionism
and self-esteem in intercollegiate athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 26, 17–38.
Hatfield, B.D., Haufler, A.J., Hung, T.M., & Spalding, T.W. (2004). Electroencephalographic
studies of skilled psychomotor performance. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 21,
1–13.
Hatfield, B.D., & Hillman, C.H. (2001). The psychophysiology of sport: A mechanistic
understanding of the psychology of superior performance. In R.N. Singer, C.H. Haus-
enblas, & C.M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 362–386).
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Hatzigeorgiadis, A., & Biddle, S.J.H. (2000). Assessing cognitive interference in sport:
Development of the Thought Occurrence Questionnaire for Sport. Anxiety, Stress, and
Coping, 13, 65–86.
Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K.G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An
experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.
Herrigel, E. (1953). Zen in the art of archery. New York: Pantheon Books, Inc.
Hindman, R. K., Crowley, K. J., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., Bohn, E. G., & Brown, C.
E. (2009, November). The relationship between mindfulness and cognitive aspects of
social anxiety. New York: Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies.
Holt, C.S., & Heimberg, R.G. (1990). The Reaction to Treatment Questionnaire: Measuring
treatment credibility and outcome expectancies. Behavior Therapist, 13, 213–214, 222.
Jackson, S.A. (1995). Factors influencing the occurrence of flow in elite athletes. Journal
of Applied Sport Psychology, 7, 138–166.
Jackson, S.A. (2000). Joy, fun, and flow state in sport. In Y.L. Hanin (Ed.), Emotions in sport
(pp. 135–155). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Jackson, S.A., & Eklund, R.C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The Flow State
Scale-2 and Dispositional Flow Scale-2. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24,
133–150.
Jackson, S.A., & Eklund, R.C. (2004). The flow scales manual. Morgantown, WV: Fitness
Information Technology, Inc.
Jackson, S.A., Kimiecik, J.C., Ford, S.K., & Marsh, H.W. (1998). Psychological correlates
of flow in sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 358–378.
Jackson, S.A., & Roberts, G.C. (1992). Positive performance states of athletes: Toward a
conceptual understanding of peak performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 156–171.
Jain, S., Shapiro, S.L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S.C., Mills, P.J., Bell, I., et al. (2007). A ran-
domized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: Effects
on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral
Medicine, 33, 11–21.
Johnson, J.J.M., Hrycaiko, D.W., Johnson, G.V., & Halas, J.M. (2004). Self-talk and female
youth soccer performance. The Sport Psychologist, 18, 44–59.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain
patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations
and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33–47.
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 353
Siegel, D.J. (2007). The mindful brain: Reflection and attunement in the cultivation of well-
being. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Smith, R.E., Cumming, S.P., & Smoll, F.L. (2006). Factorial integrity of the Sport Anxiety
Scale: A methodological note and revised scoring recommendations. Journal of Sport
& Exercise Psychology, 28, 109–112.
Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Schutz, R.W. (1990). Measurement and correlates of sport-
specific cognitive and somatic trait anxiety: The Sport Anxiety Scale. Anxiety Research,
2, 263–280.
Stein, G.L., Kimiecik, J.C., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S.A. (1995). Psychological antecedents
of flow in recreational sport. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 125–135.
Sugarman, K. (1998). Peak performance. Available at: http://www.psywww.com/sports/
peak.htm. Retrieved March 6, 2005.
Teasdale, J.D., Segal, Z.V., Williams, J.M.G., Ridgeway, V.A., Soulsby, J.M., & Lau, M.A.
(2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based
cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615–623.
Wegner, D.M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.
Wolanin, A.T. (2005). Mindfulness-acceptance-commitment (MAC) based performance
enhancement for Division I collegiate athletes: A preliminary investigation. Disserta-
tion Abstracts International, 65 (7-B), 3735.
Wright, J.J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). Challenge-skills and mindfulness: An explora-
tion of the conundrum of flow process. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research,
26, 25–32.
Young, J.A., & Pain, M.D. (1999). The zone: Evidence of a universal phenomenon for
athletes across sports. Athletic Insight: The Online Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, NP.
Available at: http://www.athleticinsight.com/Vol1Iss3/Empirical_Zone.htm. Retrieved
March 6, 2005.
Evaluation of MSPE on Flow for Athletes 355
Appendix
Summary Outline of the MSPE Treatment Protocol
I. Session 1 (approximately 2.5 hr)
A. Orientation and rationale
1. Concept of the workshop
2. Rationale for the workshop
3. Important definitions associated with mindfulness training
4. Review of key mental factors in the sport of focus
B. Group introductions
C. Raisin Exercise and discussion
D. Introductory mindful breathing exercise and discussion
E. Body Scan Meditation (45 min) and discussion
F. Wrap-up diaphragmatic breathing exercise (3 min)
G. Discussion of home practice for the week, which includes:
1. Body Scan practice 3 times for 45 min each before Session 2
2. Mindful breathing practice 3 times for 10 min each before Session 2
H. Session 1 summary and discussion