Ventromedial and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Interactions Underlie Will To Fight and Die For A Cause
Ventromedial and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Interactions Underlie Will To Fight and Die For A Cause
Ventromedial and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Interactions Underlie Will To Fight and Die For A Cause
Clara Pretus1,2, Nafees Hamid1,3, Hammad Sheikh1,4, Ángel Gómez1,5, Jeremy Ginges1,4, Adolf
1
Artis International, Scottsdale, AZ, USA
2
Departament de Psiquiatria i Medicina Legal, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Cerdanyola
del Vallès, Spain
3
Department of Security and Crime Science, University College London, London, England
4
Department of Psychology, New School for Social Research, New York, NY, USA
5
Departamento de Psicología Social y de las Organizaciones, Universidad Nacional de
Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain
6
School of Politics and Global Studies, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
7
The Changing Character of War Centre, Pembroke College, University of Oxford, Oxford,
England
8
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Institut Jean Nicod – Ecole Normale Supérieure,
Paris, France
9
Gerald Ford School of Public Policy and Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan,
Ann Arbor, MI USA
Corresponding authors:
Abstract
Willingness to fight and die (WFD) has been developed as a measure to capture willingness to
incur costly sacrifices for the sake of a greater cause in the context of entrenched conflict. WFD
Our aim was to identify the neural underpinnings of WFD, focusing on neural activity and
A sample of Pakistani participants supporting the Kashmiri cause was selected and invited to
participate in an fMRI paradigm where they were asked to convey their willingness to fight and
As predicted, higher compared to lower WFD was associated with increased ventromedial
prefrontal activity and decreased dorsolateral activity, as well as lower connectivity between the
Our findings suggest that WFD more prominently relies on brain areas typically associated with
subjective value (vmPFC) rather than integration of material costs (dlPFC) during decision-
making, supporting the notion that decisions on costly sacrifices may not be mediated by cost-
benefit computation.
Keywords
Costly sacrifices; Will to fight and die; sacred values; fMRI; functional connectivity
2
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Introduction
In previous studies, our research group has studied beliefs and values embedded within
protracted political conflict across a number of contexts, such as the Israeli-Palestinian conflict
(Sheikh et al., 2013), the US-Iranian nuclear conflict (Dehghani et al., 2009), the Indian-
Pakistani conflict over Kashmir (Sachdeva & Medin, 2009) and the frontline battle against ISIS
commitment to their in-group and the values, often showing willingness to personally fight and
die to defend them. Thus, the need emerged to develop a suitable measure to assess motivation
for incurring extreme personal costly sacrifices including risking one’s life and the lives of
others to defend highly cherished beliefs. To this end, willingness to fight and die (WFD) was
whether involving active supporters (Swann et al., 2009; Gómez et al., 2011; Atran et al.,
Behavioral field studies show that decisions over willingness to engage in costly
sacrifices for the sake of a greater cause escape instrumental reasoning and cost-benefit
computation (Ginges et al., 2011). For example, willingness to partake in political violence
solutions to a conflict was insensitive to quantity in terms of number of saved lives and number
dorsolateral (dlPFC) and the ventromedial (vmPFC) prefrontal cortex usually favoring adaptive
behavior (Basten, Biele, Heekeren, & Fiebach, 2010; Hare, Camerer, & Rangel, 2009; Rudorf &
Hare, 2014). Whereas the vmPFC has been proposed as a global value comparator that
computes subjective value (Basten et al., 2010; Camille et al., 2011), the dlPFC is thought to
modulate value encoding during decisions that integrate costs (Hare et al., 2009) or reweight
stimulus attributes (Rudorf & Hare, 2014). However, the interplay between the dlPFC and the
3
An fMRI functional connectivity study
vmPFC during value-based decisions has not yet been investigated in the context of willingness
to fight and die. Although WFD should broadly recruit brain networks associated with value-
based choice, such as the ventromedial (vmPFC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC)
(Plassmann et al., 2007; Hutcherson et al., 2012), decisions involving high compared to low
WFD might be expected to show substantial differences in the extent to which brain regions
Accordingly, the goal of the present study was twofold. First, we sought to identify the
neural correlates of willingness to fight and die by comparing values receiving high versus low
willingness to fight and die ratings, especially focusing on activity in usual decision-making
neural hubs such as the dlPFC and vmPFC. Second, we endeavored to establish a functional
connectivity profile between these brain regions, respectively associated with integration of
costs during decision-making (dlPFC) and subjective value computation (vmPFC), during
proceeding of willingness to fight and die. For this purpose, we recruited a sample of male
participants of Pakistani origin living in Barcelona (Spain) who expressly supported the cause
of Kashmiri resistance to Indian rule, and then scanned them while they expressed willingness
to fight and die for a series of values related to Islam and current politics. We then analyzed
neural activity associated with high and low willingness to fight and die, and compared
functional connectivity between the dlPFC and the vmPFC in these two conditions using the
CONN functional connectivity toolbox (The Gabrieli Lab, McGovern Institute for Brain
Research). This toolbox allows quantifying the co-variance among previously selected neural
regions, or between previously selected neural regions and the rest of the brain, in a given
experimental condition.
Given that willingness to fight and die (WFD) for a value is a measure that reflects
disposition to incur extreme costly sacrifices, we predicted high compared to low WFD to be
positively correlated with brain activity that is typically associated with subjective value during
decision-making (vmPFC). Because the personal drive to fight and die for a cause often runs
independently of risks and outcomes for the self (Sheikh et al., 2016), we also anticipated lower
4
An fMRI functional connectivity study
integration of costs, hence, reduced activity in the dlPFC, during high versus low WFD
decisions. For the same reason, we expected functional connectivity between the dlPFC and the
vmPFC to be weak or even negative in the high versus low WFD condition.
Participants
The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) Ethics Committee
according to the Declaration of Helsinki guidelines and informed consent was obtained from all
participants before taking part in the study. Data protection was a priority in this study and
Participants were selected by means of field interviews and surveys with a sample of
Barcelona (Spain), the average age was 30.82 years old (range 18 to 62). All respondents were
Muslim, and most self-identified as Sunni. In line with the recruitment criteria, all participants
in the selected sample expressed: (1) support for a jihadist group which fights for the Pakistani
reclamation of Kashmir (2) agreement with armed jihad (3) agreement that Muslims must unite
against the West by all means necessary and (4) willingness to personally engage or facilitate
violence in the name of armed jihad (to engage in violent protest, to financial support a non-
state militant group, to join a non-state militant group or to fight and die on their own in support
of armed jihad). From those n = 45 respondents who met all three criteria in the field survey, n
The final fMRI study sample consisted on male Muslim participants from Pakistani
origin living in and around Barcelona. The average age was 30.10 years old (sd = 5.37, range
from 18 to 39 years old), 51.7% of them were married, 82.7% finished secondary school and
10.3% held a Bachelor’s Degree, 72.4% reported a well below average income, and 72.4%
stated that religion was the most important thing in their lives. All but one agreed that “India
5
An fMRI functional connectivity study
should have no right to ownership of Kashmir” and all of them reported a favorable or
somewhat favorable opinion of Lashkar-e-Taiba (LeT or “army of the pure”), one of the largest
militant Islamist groups in Pakistan functioning since 1993 and declared a terrorist group by the
US (2002) followed by the UN (2005). The organization fights for the liberation of Kashmir
from Indian control and the unification of all Muslim majority regions beyond Pakistan,
(South Asia Terrorism Portal, www.satp.org). LeT has been attributed several violent attacks
including the 2008 Mumbai bombings, which caused 200 deaths and 300 injured civilians, and
the Indian parliament attacks in 2001 (see Box 1 for more details on the Kashmir conflict).
Box 1. The Kashmir conflict started in 1947 with India’s independence and its partition
from Pakistan. Although it began as a territorial dispute it has evolved into a clash between
Hindu and Muslim religious identities especially salient for the Muslim community (Sachdeva
& Medin, 2009). The first war between India and Pakistan over Kashmir in 1947 caused mass
killings of all ethnic groups and hundreds of thousands of refugees. In 1948 the UN Security
Council imposed a ceasefire, forcing Pakistan to withdraw its troops, and promised a plebiscite
for Kashmiris to democratically vote for their independence. Neither Pakistan obeyed nor did
the plebiscite take place, creating a climate of escalating violence leading to the Sino-Indian war
in 1962 and the Indo-Pakistani war in 1971. The Indian military occupation lead to the
1989, which has caused 70000 deaths and 8000 missings to date. The most recent ceasefire
resolution between Pakistan and India was signed in 2003, although there have been several
violent attacks from both sides ever since. Currently, 43% of the territory is controlled by India,
37% by Pakistan and 20% by China. The general Pakistani view defends that Kashmir should
be independent or annexed to Pakistan, according to its Muslim majority and responding to the
Kashmiri uprising.
Procedure
6
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Participants were invited to the fMRI facilities where they completed a pre-scan survey
and an fMRI task described below. The pre-scan survey was used to determine which values
were perceived as sacred (non-negotiable) and non-sacred (mundane values) by each participant
in order to ensure sufficient variability in the willingness to fight and die measure. A list of 25
candidate values was presented to participants, which was designed based on previous
forces should be expelled from Muslim lands”, “Strict sharia should be applied in all Muslim
countries”, ”Armed jihad should be waged against the enemies of Muslims”, “India should have
Caliphate”, among others (see full list in Table S1). In line with previous work on sacred values
(Baron & Spranca, 1997; Tetlock, 2003; Berns et al., 2012), a value was considered sacred if: a)
participants refused any material incentive to give it up, such as better economic conditions for
their own community, that is, they responded “No” instead of “Yes” or “Maybe” to the question
“Would you be willing to give up this value if adopting the opposite position entailed better
economic conditions for your community?”, and b) their stance was immune to social influence,
that is, they responded “No” instead of “Yes” or “Maybe” to the question “If there was a
democratic consensus over the opposite position, would you find it acceptable?” (Fiske and
Tetlock, 1997; Baron and Leshner, 2000; Ginges et al., 2007; Berns et al., 2012; Sheikh et al.,
2013). Participants had to respond “No” to both questions in order for the value to be
considered sacred. Next, participants ranked their sacred and non-sacred values by importance
and the top 6 sacred values, as well as the bottom 6 non-sacred values were selected for the
fMRI paradigm. There was a considerable amount of overlapping between each participants’
sacred value and non-sacred value sets, with the most frequently employed sacred values being
those including religious content (see Table S1). Measures of identity fusion with seven
different groups (Swann et al., 2009), including “Islam”, “Muslim Ummah”, “Pakistan”,
7
An fMRI functional connectivity study
“Spain”, “Borderless Muslim Caliphate”, “Federated Muslim Caliphate” and “Close Friends”,
were also administered in the pre-scan survey. The identity fusion measure includes 5 pairs of
circles, one representing the self (a small circle) and another representing the group (a big
circle), with varying degrees of overlap between the circles. From the 5 options, only the option
representing the small circle fully inside the big circle denotes fusion. We added an additional
the reluctance of some respondents to accept any contact between the circles in previous field
studies (Gómez et al., 2017). These were intended to measure the extent to which respondents
felt “viscerally connected” to a reference group such as nation or religious community (Gómez
et al., 2017).
fMRI paradigm
The rapid event-related fMRI paradigm was designed using MATLAB 2012a (The
MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States) with Psychtoolbox extensions (Kleiner
et al., 2007). During brain scanning, participants rated their willingness to fight and die (WFD)
for each value (one value per trial) by means of a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not
willing) to 7 (extremely willing). Values were presented in Urdu (see Figure S1). Linguistic
variations of each value produced 80 different trials (40 sacred value trials and 40 non-sacred
value trials) with controlled length and sentence structure in order to increase the number of
trials within the fMRI paradigm without wording repetitions. A post-hoc analysis comparing
stimulus length in Urdu showed no differences in stimulus length neither between sacred and
non-sacred value trials (sacred values: M = 12.05 words, SD = ±.28; non-sacred values: M =
11.97 words; SD = ±.6, t(28) = .598, p = .555) nor between the conditions of interest; that is,
high and low WFD (high WFD: M = 12.08 words, SD = ±.36; low WFD: M = 12.04 words, SD
= ±.76, t(28) = .204, p = .840). Stimuli were presented randomly with jittered inter-trial
intervals in two runs. Inter-trial interval durations varied randomly from 2.5 seconds up to 12.5
seconds in 2.5-second steps. Participants were instructed to read the value and, once they knew
their response, convey their WFD making use of the 7-point Likert scale located below the
8
An fMRI functional connectivity study
value using left/right buttons. The participant’s response was recorded based on their button
candidates from the same population allowed establishing a response time of 10 seconds
(reading + responding), which allowed all participants to respond to most trials. The values and
the 7-point Likert scale were always presented in the same location of the screen and the
actually a 4, they were instructed to go either left or right and then back to 4, otherwise the trial
counted as a null). The value remained on screen until the 10 seconds were over. Keeping
stimuli in place facilitated reading and reduced confusion. Before starting the fMRI paradigms,
participants practiced the task in a computer until fully comprehending the exercise and
MRI acquisition
using a FSPGR sequence (TR: 11.6 ms, TE: 4.8ms, FA: 12, matrix size: 280 x 280, 150 slices,
slice thickness: 1.00mm). An EPI-T2* sequence allowed obtaining the functional volumes, each
comprising forty 3.4 mm thick slices (TR 2500 ms, TE: 27 ms, FA: 90, matrix size: 64 x 64, 40
slices, slice thickness: 3.4 mm). Head motion was tracked by means of center of mass
measurements during image acquisition. None of the participants showed head movement above
voxel size acquisition after a post-hoc evaluation of the six head movement parameters.
and the Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging software were employed for the functional MRI
data analysis. Functional images were despiked (AFNI), corrected for motion-related artifacts
(SPM), normalized to MNI standard space (SPM), smoothed with a 8mm full-width-at-half-
Behavioral analysis
The behavioral analysis included a paired sample t-test comparing WFD ratings in the
high WFD condition and the low WFD condition. The effect of value sacredness on willingness
9
An fMRI functional connectivity study
to fight and die ratings was computed by means of a repeated measures ANOVA assessing
were calculated for each identity fusion scale and correlations between identity fusion and
Neuroimaging analysis
University College London, UK) on MATLAB. Trials were split in high versus low WFD as a
function of each participant’s average. The first-level GLM included 2 regressors for trials
where participants conveyed either high (above each participant’s mean) or low (below each
participant’s mean) willingness to fight and die ratings. The average number of trials was 47.2
(sd = ±8.82) in the high WFD condition, and 32.8 (SD = ±8.82) in the low WFD condition. The
duration of the events spanned from stimulus onset up to the participant’s first button press, and
thus, it varied from trial to trial. Each condition was parametrically modulated by a binary
regressor with ones and zeros including whether the value at stake was sacred (1) or not (0). Of
note, the high and low WFD conditions did not always correspond to responses to sacred and
non-sacred values, respectively. That is, the high WFD condition included, at average, 77.11%
trials with sacred values (sd = 13.96%) and the low WFD condition included, at average,
12.81% trials with sacred values (sd = 13.94%). Because sacred values entail higher levels of
attitude strength, familiarity, salience and emotional intensity than non-sacred values (Pretus et
al., 2018; Hamid et al., in press), the inclusion of a value sacredness parametric regressor was
crucial to account for these confounding effects on the measures of interest (WFD). Missing
trials (no button response throughout the trial), which were, at average, 3.62 trials per
participant (SD = ±5.44) were not modelled in the GLM, and thus, they were processed as part
of the implicit baseline. In turn, in order to account for the confounding effects of motor
preparation and response, we introduced an hrf-convolved button press regressor. Six non-
10
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Group-level effects of the high versus low WFD contrast were evaluated by means of a
t-test on individual level contrasts. In addition, we ran a second-level regression with identity
fusion with Islam and the Muslim Ummah as a predictor of neural activity in the high versus
low WFD contrast as well as in high and low versus baseline contrast, respectively. In order to
be able to capture more subtle differences in neural activity associated with identity fusion,
parameter estimate extraction of vmPFC and left and right dlPFC activity in the high WFD
versus baseline and the low WFD versus baseline contrast using Marsbar in order to illustrate
the amount of ROI activity in each condition . Reported results were corrected for multiple
toolbox CONN on SPM12 (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). To test the interplay
between brain regions associated with executive control (dlPFC) and subjective value
computation (vmPFC) during high and low willingness to fight and die, a series of anatomical
ROIs were defined using aal labels (Neurofunctional Imaging group, Université de Bordeaux).
These ROIs included the left dlPFC (left middle frontal gyrus), the right dlPFC (right middle
frontal gyrus) and the vmPFC (bilateral medial orbitofrontal gyrus). For each participant, a
generalized Psycho-Physiological Interaction (gPPI) analysis was conducted. The gPPI analysis
tests the task-modulated effective connectivity between pairs of a previously defined set of
ROIs (here: right dlPFC, left dlPFC and vmPFC) by means of a multiple regression model
incorporating task effects, the ROIs timeseries and an interaction term (PPI term) between both
parts. The effects of the high and low willingness to fight and die conditions on effective
connectivity between the bilateral dlPFC and the vmPFC were then computed and compared.
Group-level gPPI results were corrected for multiple comparisons at seed level (p < 0.05 FWE).
Results
11
An fMRI functional connectivity study
The average WFD rating in the high WFD condition was 6.67 (SD ±0.48), whereas the
average WFD rating in the low WFD condition was 3.94 (SD ± 1.02). The average response
time was 5.93 seconds (SD = ±1.03), and no differences in response time were detected between
conditions (High WFD condition: M = 5.02, SD = ±1.22; Low WFD condition: M = 5.04, SD
= ±1.38; t(28) = -.089, p = .930). In addition, response time did not correlate with WFD ratings,
condition separately (high WFD condition: r = .070, p = .718; low WFD condition: r = .042, p =
Seventy-five percent of the sample was fused with Islam, 72.4% with the Muslim
Ummah, 93.1% with Pakistan, 31% with Spain, 21% with a Borderless Muslim Caliphate, 24%
with a Federated Muslim Caliphate and 79.3% with Close Friends. Identity fusion with Pakistan
was the only identity fusion measure which correlated with willingness to fight and die (r =
.388, p <.038), although this result did not survive Bonferroni correction.
fMRI signal contrasts between high versus low willingness to fight and die appear in
Table 1. The high compared to the low willingness to fight and die condition was associated
with greater activity in a cluster encompassing the anterior cingulate cortex and the left middle
orbital gyrus (T = 4.49, k = 446, p = 0.013 FWEc) and lower activity in the right inferior
parietal lobule (T = 5.49, k = 1690, p < 0.001 FWEc), right middle frontal gyrus (T = 5.47, k =
424, p = 0.016 FWEc), left superior medial frontal gyrus (T = 5.37, k = 735, p = 0.001 FWEc),
left inferior parietal lobule (T = 5.33, k = 1800, p < 0.001 FWEc) and a cluster including the left
inferior frontal gyrus (pars triangularis) and left middle frontal gyrus (T = 5.11, k = 1230, p <
0.001 FWEc) (see Figure 1A and B). Post-hoc parameter estimates analyses corroborated the
increased vmPFC activity (t(28) = 3.25, p = .003) and decreased left PFC (t(28) = -2.19, p =
.037) and right dlPFC (t(28) = -3.00, p = .006) activity in the high WFD versus baseline contrast
compared to the low WFD versus baseline contrast, although the left dPFC comparison did not
survive Bonferroni correction (see Figure 1B). Differences in willingness to fight and die scores
12
An fMRI functional connectivity study
in the high versus low WFD conditions correlated with neural activity differences in the left
dlPFC ( r = -.578, p = .001) and right dlPFC (r = -.456, p = 0.013), but not the vmPFC, between
Study participants were predominantly fused with Islam and the Muslim Ummah;
however, the available variability was negatively associated with left middle frontal gryus
high versus low WFD contrast (T = 4.38, k = 71, p = .045 FWEc after small volume correction
with the left dlPFC ROI, see Figure 2). However, identity fusion with Islam and the Muslim
Ummah did not significantly correlate with average willingness to fight and die scores (r = .141,
p = .467) nor with WFD scores in the high (r = .058, p = .766) or low (r = .066, p = .735)
willingness to fight and die condition, probably due to a limited variability of identity fusion in
this sample.
The gPPI analysis revealed a negative effective connectivity between the vmPFC as a
seed region and the bilateral dlPFC (F(2)(27) = 2.98, Intensity = 4.25, p = 0.01 FWE seed-level
corrected) when comparing the high versus low WFD conditions (see Figure 3). Results of the
reverse contrast (low > high WFD) did not survive multiple comparisons correction.
Discussion
The present study aimed to identify the neural underpinnings of willingness to fight and
die (WFD) for the sake of jihadi-related values in a sample of Pakistani participants supporting
the cause of Kashmiri resistance to Indian rule. As anticipated, decisions involving high
compared to low WFD (with respect to each participants’ average) correlated with higher
ventromedial prefrontal activity (vmPFC), a brain region typically associated with subjective
value computation during decision-making (Plassmann et al., 2007; Bartra et al., 2013).
Moreover, consistent with behavioral field studies indicating that decisions over costly
sacrifices run somewhat independently from cost-benefit computation, high compared to low
WFD negatively correlated with brain activity associated with integration of costs and stimulus
reweighting, that is, the dlPFC (Todd, Hare, Camerer, & Rangel, 2009; Rudorf & Hare, 2014).
13
An fMRI functional connectivity study
In addition, the effective connectivity analysis revealed a stronger negative correlation between
the vmPFC as a seed region and the bilateral dlPFC in the high versus low WFD condition.
and the dlPFC during value-based choices. For example, Baumgartner et al. (2011) observed
increased dlPFC activity and higher functional connectivity between the dlPFC and the vmPFC
frequently made fair decisions versus participants who made more selfish decisions, but no
higher dlPFC activity and increased dlPFC-vmPFC connectivity during decisions about food
consumption in dieting “self-controllers” (participants evaluating both taste and health for each
food item) versus dieting “non-self-controllers” (evaluating taste only); and there was a
Nevertheless, none of these prior studies investigated the relationship between these
brain areas in regard to decisions involving costs in human lives. Here, we found that decisions
over willingness to fight and die for a cause were modulated by interactions between the
vmPFC and the dlPFC. Particularly, the vmPFC was more active and the dlPFC, together with
other fronto-parietal areas such as the left inferior frontal gyrus and the bilateral parietal lobe,
was less active during decisions involving high willingness to fight and die for a cause. Several
studies point at the vmPFC as an integration hub that reflects the subjective value of a given
choice “all things considered” (Chib et al., 2009; Peters & Büchel, 2010; Bartra et al., 2013;
Shenhav & Greene, 2014). Moreover, the dlPFC has been associated with a myriad of functions
related to executive control, such as stimulus reweighting (Rudorf and Hare, 2014) and
integration of costs (Hare et al., 2009). More generally, the fronto-parietal network, including
areas found to be less active in the high versus low WFD conditions (inferior frontal gyrus and
the parietal lobules), has been associated with cognitive control enabling rapid and flexible
goal-oriented behavior (Marek and Dosenbach, 2018). From this perspective, our results suggest
that high (vs. low) WFD choices could involve higher subjective value, with little contribution
14
An fMRI functional connectivity study
of brain regions associated with integration of costs and cognitive control (including the dlPFC).
In addition, high compared to low WFD was associated with negative effective connectivity
between the vmPFC as a seed region and the bilateral dlPFC. This finding suggests that a
involving high willingness to fight and die for a cause. In this vein, future research should
ratings for causes that would otherwise be associated with high willingness to fight and die
responses.
Finally, most participants were fused with Pakistan, Islam and the Muslim Ummah, in
line with studies showing identity fusion in frontline fighters, who establish family-like bonds
with fellow combatants (Whitehouse et al., 2014; Gómez et al., 2017). Available variability
revealed a positive correlation between identity fusion with Islam and the Mulsim Ummah and
decreased dlPFC recruitment during the high WFD condition (both compared to baseline and
compared to the low WFD condition) , although this did not entail higher WFD ratings. Identity
fusion is associated with commitment and costly sacrifices in defense of in-group values
(Swann et al., 2014). Identity fusion is one of the components of the “devoted actors model”,
which characterizes individuals whose personal identity is merged or “fused” with that of their
social group and to the group’s most cherished (sacred) values (Atran et al., 2014a; Atran, 2016;
Sheikh et al., 2016; Gómez et al., 2017). For similar levels of in-group commitment (similar
WFD ratings), fused participants showed a lower reliance on brain areas typically associated
with integration of costs during decisions involving costly sacrifices (left dlPFC). This finding
suggests that the effect of identity fusion on dlPFC activity alone cannot account for the
observed high willingness to fight and die making use of the present experimental design.
Our results are consistent with behavioral studies showing that decisions over
willingness to engage in costly sacrifices for the sake of a greater cause escape instrumental
reasoning and cost-benefit computation (Ginges & Atran, 2011). In those prior studies,
decisions over willingness to resort to political violence (military action) compared to non-
15
An fMRI functional connectivity study
violence (diplomacy) was mainly predicted by perceived righteousness, with little contribution
conflict was insensitive to quantity in terms of number of saved lives and number of deterred
future attacks by the enemy (Ginges & Atran, 2011). Participants were ready to support a
military intervention to resolve a hypothetical international conflict if it meant saving the life of
only if they involved saving more than 100 hostages or 60 future attacks (Ginges & Atran,
2011).
The present study has several limitations. On one hand, values in the high and low
willingness to fight and die conditions differed in a number of ways, including emotional
intensity, familiarity and salience. This owes to the fact that sacred values (higher emotional
intensity, familiarity and salience) were more often than not included in the high WFD
condition, whereas the opposite was true for non-sacred values. This confound was addressed
by including a binary parametric regressor in the high and low conditions indicating whether the
value at stake was sacred or not. However, we understand that if a value is worth fighting and
dying for, it will almost certainly entail higher levels of emotional intensity and salience. Hence,
we tend to think that such qualities are intrinsic (rather than a confound) to high levels of
willingness to fight and die. On the other hand, it would have been interesting to compare
willingness to fight and die (WFD) and willingness to pay for goods (WTP) in the same
individuals in order to evaluate the degree of overlap between the functional clusters underlying
value-based choice in the social cause versus material goods condition. Further research is
needed to specifically test whether the pattern of dlPFC - vmPFC interactions is consistent
across social and material values, or whether discrepancies modulated by individual traits, such
as identity fusion, exist. Finally, our study is limited by the fact it focuses on a population
involved in a particular conflict. Future research should compare behavioral and neuronal
processes associated with willingness to fight and die across communities involved in different
world conflicts.
16
An fMRI functional connectivity study
In sum, our findings support the notion that WFD relies on brain activity that is
generally associated with subjective value computation (vmPFC). Moreover, in line with
behavioral data showing that decisions on costly sacrifices run independently from cost-benefit
ponderation (Ginges et al., 2011), high compared to low WFD was characterized by decreased
recruitment of brain regions previously associated with integration of costs during decision-
between the vmPFC and the bilateral dlPFC associated with high compared to low WFD. By
providing a neurobiological perspective via fMRI measures and brain functional connectivity,
these findings are potentially relevant to identifying later stages of radicalization that involve
Funding
This work was supported by the Minerva Program and the Air Force Office of Scientific
Research of the U.S. Department of Defense (AFOSR FA9550-14-1-0030 DEF) and the BIAL
Acknowledgements
References
Atran, S. (2016). The devoted actor: Unconditional commitment and intractable conflict across
cultures. Current Anthropology, 57, S192–203
Atran, S., Sheikh, H., Gomez, A. (2014a). Devoted actors sacrifice for close comrades and
sacred cause. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 111, 17702–3
Atran, S., Sheikh, H., Gomez, A. (2014b). For Cause and Comrade: Devoted Actors and
Willingness to Fight. Cliodynamics: The Journal of Quantitative History and Cultural
Evolution, 5
Baron, J., Leshner, S. (2000). How serious are expressions of protected values? Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 183–94
Baron, J., Spranca, M. (1997). Protected Values. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 70, 1–16
Bartra, O., McGuire, J.T., Kable, J.W. (2013). The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-
analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value.
17
An fMRI functional connectivity study
18
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Peters, J., Büchel, C. (2010). Neural representations of subjective reward value. Behavioural
Brain Research, 213, 135–41
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., Rangel, A. (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to
pay in everyday economic transactions. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal
of the Society for Neuroscience, 27, 9984–88
Pretus, C., Hamid, N., Ginges, J., et al. (2018). Neural and Behavioral Correlates of Sacred
Values and Vulnerability to Violent Extremism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2462
Rudorf, S., Hare, T.A. (2014). Interactions between Dorsolateral and Ventromedial Prefrontal
19
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Table 1. Results of the high versus low willingness to fight and die contrast thresholded
at T = 3.4, p < 0.05 FWE cluster-level corrected, singel level p < 0.001.
MNI Coordinates
p-FWEc
Region Label k T(28) x y z
20
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Figure 1. Results of the high versus low willingness to fight and die contrast. (A)
The high versus low willingness to fight and die conidition yielded higher activity in a
cluster including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the vmPFC (in red-yellow)
and lower activity in the superior medial frontal gyrus. the right middle frontal gyrus.
left inferior frontal gryus (pars triangularis). and bilateral inferior parietal lobule (in
blue-green). Results thresholded at T = 3.4. p < 0.05 FWEc. singel level p < 0.001. (B)
Parameter estimates of vmPFC activity were higher in the high WFD vs. baseline
condition compared to the low WFD vs. baseline condition (t(28) = 3.25. p = 0.003),
whilst those of the bilateral dlPFC were lower (left dlPFC: t(28) = 2.19. p = 0.037; right
21
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Figure 2. Correlations between neural activity and identity fusion measures: (A)
surface projection of the left middle frontal gryus activity predicted by fusion with
Islam and the Muslim Ummah in the high WFD versus baseline contrast (T = 5.01, k =
489, p = .024 FWEc) and in the high versus low WFD contrast (T = 4.38, k = 71, p =
.045 FWEc after small volume correction with the left dlPFC ROI), (B) Scatterplot of
the correlation between the fusion score and left middle frontal gryus parameter
estimates found in the high WFD versus baseline contrast (r = -.659, p < .001) and the
high versus low WFD contrast (r = -.627, p <.001), (C) results table of second-level
GLM using identity fusion with Islam and the Muslim Ummah as predictor of brain
22
An fMRI functional connectivity study
Figure 3. Effective connectivity analysis results: (A) Parameter estimates of the high
WFD effects (dark red) and the low WFD effects (light red) on the effective
connectivity between each pair of ROIs including the left and right dlPFC and the
vmPFC (“[seed region] – [target region]”), (B) 3D display of the effective connectivity
differences between the high and low WFD conditions after FWE seed-level correction
(left lateral and superior view), and (C) effective connectivity results table comparing
high > low WFD including a negative correlation between the vmPFC as a seed region
and the bilateral dlPFC, no significant results were found in the reverse contrast (low >
high WFD).
23