Couples' Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies: Secure and Insecure Attachment Differences and Similarities
Couples' Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies: Secure and Insecure Attachment Differences and Similarities
Couples' Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies: Secure and Insecure Attachment Differences and Similarities
1
Karin du Plessis and Dave Clarke
Massey University, New Zealand
Abstract
Twenty two heterosexual couples living in New Zealand participated in this study regarding
helpful, unhelpful and idealized conflict resolution strategies. Thematic analysis was used to
extract key themes, and these were categorized by whether individuals were securely or
insecurely attached to their partners. Both secure and insecure individuals identified similar
helpful conflict strategies, including turn-taking, listening and remaining calm. Differences
emerged between secure and insecure individuals with regards to unhelpful strategies, with
insecure individuals’ descriptions taking on an overall negative slant, whilst secure
individuals either did not use unhelpful strategies or have learned more helpful strategies
over time. Similar ideal conflict strategies emerged for both secure and insecure individuals;
however, secure individuals’ descriptions were much more closely matched to the helpful
strategies they use in their own relationships.
Introduction
This paper reports on the qualitative component of a larger study (e.g., Du Plessis, Clarke, &
Woolley, 2007) that was conducted with 22 couples in New Zealand. It focused on obtaining
participants’ perspectives on the conflict resolution strategies they use in their long-term
relationships, and makes a distinction between secure and insecure attachment styles to
identify differences between these groups. Following a brief review of adult attachment
literature and conflict strategies, the study is introduced and its findings discussed.
Author note: This research was based on the first author’s doctoral dissertation at Massey University,
New Zealand in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a doctoral degree - made possible by a
Massey Doctoral Scholarship. Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Dave Clarke, Department
of Psychology, Massey University, Private Bag 102 904, NSMSC, Auckland, New Zealand; e-mail:
[email protected]
66 Interpersona 1 (2) – December 2007
Attachment
The study of adult attachment grew from Bowlby (e.g., 1979; 1988) and Ainsworth’s
research (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) which focused on the attachment
system of infants and their primary caregivers. Over the last two decades the attention of
attachment research has shifted to the application of attachment theory to adult intimate
and viewing the attachment figure as safe haven and secure base (Collins & Feeney, 2004).
Research has indicated that adults with a secure attachment style describe higher levels of
avoidant attachment style who describe lower levels of these features, and who exacerbate
relational threats (e.g., Collins & Read, 1990; Mikulincer, Gillath, & Shaver, 2002; Simpson,
Rholes, & Phillips, 1996). In another study it was found that husbands and wives with secure
attachment styles were less rejecting and more supportive of their partners than their
Conflict
When conflict arises it can threaten the very heart of the relationship. From an
attachment perspective conflict creates a dilemma: the person, who is generally sought out
when we are distressed, now becomes a source of threat. It is precisely because intimate
relationships are characterized by a shared history and future, as well as strong emotional
investments, that solving conflicts in the intimate setting seem to matter so much to couples
(Peterson, 1983). Conflict is believed to be essentially neither positive nor negative (Sillars,
Canary, & Tafoya, 2003), but rather a foreseeable outcome of the natural process of growth
and change. Hample (2003) suggests that face-to-face arguments can have a variety of
functions: as a means to obtain one’s goals; establish dominance over the other person;
display and define personal identity; and as a recreational activity. Marital conflicts are
defined by Bradbury, Rogge, and Lawrence (2001) as “social interactions in which spouses
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 67
hold incompatible goals” (p.59). Beach (2001) however notes that one should view opposing
goals as potential conflicts, rather than actual conflicts, due to the fact that couples often
find means to interact cooperatively when faced with incompatible goals and interests.
couples occurs when each individual collaborates in creating a solution that meets both
partners’ needs, and conflict resolution experts concur that a key building block to effective
Ury, & Patton, 1991). Generally, two fundamental categories for conflict styles are
distinguished and include conflict strategies that are integrative (those that work toward the
other person and advances relational goals) and those that are distributive (those that work
in opposition to the other person because people are focused on reaching their own goals
with no consideration for their partner’s goals) (Sillars, Coletti, Parry, & Rogers, 1982).
The literature indicates that couples who frequently use a positive problem solving
style (e.g., compromise and negotiation), and infrequently use withdrawal and conflict
engagement, are more satisfied with their relationships (Kurdek, 1994). Pistole and Arricale
(2003) found that securely attached people report less fighting and more effective arguing,
whereas preoccupied individuals view conflict as an attachment threat and focus on re-
establishing togetherness, which might in turn hamper their problem solving ability. These
researchers have also found that securely attached individuals reported less conflict
avoidance than those with dismissing attachment styles (Pistole & Arricale, 2003). Shi (2003)
furthermore reported that secure individuals displayed more positive behaviour and higher
relationship satisfaction than individuals who scored higher on preoccupied and avoidant
attachment dimensions. Creasey (2002) suggests that although secure couples would
occasionally use negative behaviours during conflict, their liberal use of positive behaviours
Attachment styles can become apparent in conflict situations. For example, research
has found that men with insecure attachment styles display more negative affect and engage
in conflict more frequently, than their securely attached counterparts (e.g., Cohn, Silver,
Cowan, Cowan, & Pearson, 1992; Cowan, Cohn, Cowan, & Pearson, 1996; George, Kaplan, &
Main, 1996). The literature also identifies the demand-withdraw pattern as unhelpful when
trying to solve conflict (Kurdek, 1995). In this demand-withdraw pattern women are
generally the demanding party, whereas men tend to withdraw in response (Kurdek, 1995).
satisfaction than any other conflict resolution style and research suggests that it reflects the
intensity and the amount of intimacy that people need in a relationship, with women
generally desiring more intimacy, and men desiring greater separateness (e.g., Christensen,
1987; Jacobson, 1989). Conflict behaviour that is confronting (especially a negative start-up)
has also been linked to unsatisfactory relationships (e.g., Gottman, Coan, Carrere, &
criticizing, contempt, defensiveness and stonewalling (withdrawal) have also been identified
Previous research has shown that ideal partner standards are based around three
Simpson, Thomas, & Giles, 1999). Ideal relationships have been found to be based on two
dimensions of intimacy/loyalty and passion (Fletcher at al., 1999). More recent research
(Fletcher, Tither, O’Loughlin, Friesen, & Overall, 2004) indicates that individuals in long-
term relationships would value the “warm and homely person as opposed to the cold and
attractive person” (p.670). Individuals who view their current relationships and partners as
closely matching their ideal relationships and partners, have also been found to be more
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 69
actual and ideal relationships (Mickulincer & Erev, 1991). Levine (1995) theorized that the
discrepancy between ideal and actual love is a natural function of any long-term
relationship, and it requires the individual to constantly manage this “gap” by using a range
of defenses (e.g., idealization, denial, and rationalization) which in turn might enhance
relationship well-being. More recently Caughlin (2003) also noted with regards to ideal
communication in family relationships, that unmet ideals (discrepancies between ideals and
Proposed themes
The purpose of this study was to focus on perceived helpful, unhelpful and idealized
Secure and insecure partnerships. Firstly, secure attachment will include proximity
maintenance, separation distress, and viewing each other as a safe haven and a secure base.
This will be expected to come forth through descriptions of having high levels of trust,
intimacy, satisfaction and commitment, and describing their partners as supportive (as well
their relationships.
Helpful conflict strategies. Securely attached participants are expected to use positive
behaviours more frequently, than their insecure counterparts, and possibly have a wider
Unhelpful conflict strategies. Both the securely and the insecurely attached participants
are expected to identify unhelpful conflict resolution strategies, although the insecure
participants’ strategies might be more negatively slanted, and their conflict might occur
and additionally strategies such as complaining, criticizing, contempt and defensiveness are
Idealized conflict strategies. Conflict resolution styles which results in greater amounts
of warmth and trustworthiness in the relationship, will be held up as the ideal for long-term
relationships. Greater congruence is expected between securely attached couples’ ideal and
real relationships as well as the manner in which they resolve conflict. Securely attached
couples will report ideal conflict resolution styles close to their own. Insecurely attached
participants are expected to hold more unrealistic ideas of their ideal relationships and how
Method
Participants
advertisements in the local media (i.e., Auckland community newspapers). Couples came to
a testing room at Massey University to complete the questionnaires. The study was approved
by the Massey University Human Ethics Committee. Of the 44 participants (22 couples) the
mean age for the sample was 42 years (SD = 10.27). Participants were required to cohabitate
relationships were 161 months (approximately 13 years; SD = 138.50), with the maximum
being 496 months (approximately 41 years). The majority of the participants were in marital
relationships (77%). Approximately half of the sample had children with their current
partner or from a previous relationship (52%). The majority of the sample was of New
Zealand European descent (75%), with some Maori participants (9%), and other participants
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 71
totaling 16% (e.g., Polynesian, Australian, German, and Singaporean Chinese). Full time
employed participants totaled 52%, whereas 43% were not employed full time, or fell in
anther category (including retired, full time students, part time workers, unemployed, and
full time homemakers). In terms of frequencies, the majority of the participants (59.2%) were
satisfied with their relationships (slightly satisfied, satisfied or very satisfied), with 25%
mixed (neither dissatisfied, nor satisfied), and 15.8% dissatisfied with their relationships (as
measured by the 3-item Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale (KMSS) (Schumm et al., 1986).
Measures
relational partners using a relationship attachment scale developed by Le Poire et al. (1997).
strongly agree). The secure partner attachment subscale (• = .83) (12 items) included items
such as (1) “I believe that my partner is capable of unconditional positive regard”, and (2)
“My partner is always there for me in times of crisis”. The results of the secure attachment
subscale were divided between Secure and Insecure Attachment categories (through median
For the purposes of the results reported here participants also completed four
questions were developed by the researcher and her supervisors to fulfill the research aim
and answer the research questions, which included obtaining a deeper understanding of
conflict resolution strategies that couples perceive as helpful, unhelpful and ideal. The
questions included:
1) Please describe your relationship with your current romantic partner. Include as
much detail as you can and be sure to include characteristics of your relationship
(e.g., “We have a caring and nurturing relationship”) as well as qualities of your
2) In your current relationship are there any ways of sorting out problems and
arguments that work really well? Please give examples and comment;
3) In your relationship with your current partner have you noticed any ways of
sorting out problems and arguments that result in failure to reach a solution to a
problem, or that makes a problem worse? Please give examples and comment; and
4) Imagine your ideal relationship. How would the couple in your ideal relationship
handle conflict?
Analysis
For the purposes of analyzing this data set thematic analysis was used. In line with
Braun and Clarke (2006) thematic analysis was used as an “essentialist or realist method,
which reports experiences, meanings and the reality of participants” (p. 81). Boyatzis (1998)
views thematic analysis as a process that is utilized to encode qualitative data. More
describes and organizes the information (Braun & Clarke, 2006). To this extent thematic
analysis was deemed an appropriate methodology to tease out core themes underlying
participants’ relationships with their intimate partners, as well as the strategies that they use
The coding process followed a three-step progression, and involved (a) developing
concepts and categories to organize data into a framework of ideas, (b) comparing data
instances, cases and categories for similarities and differences, and (c) unifying key themes.
Following on from the expected themes taken from the literature, five key themes were
identified for each of the qualitative questions. Excerpts from the written answers are used
to illustrate themes where relevant, and a summary of the key themes can be found in Table
1.
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 73
Key Themes
Clear differences emerged in the manner that secure and insecure individuals view
their romantic partners and relationships. Securely attached individuals described their
partners as caring, loving and committed, and their relationships as being based on open
integrity.
“He is warm and caring towards me and thinks of me before himself a lot. I knew when I first
met him that he was the man I wanted in my life. He is a hard worker and we are both working
to common goals, and I am looking forward to our future together” (New Zealand European
female, 42).
“I feel that I am not respected or ‘cherished’ by her and that I am more of a burden than a
“… arguments are a daily ‘norm’… When arguing we tend to get off track and become
emotional… I think our main problem is not how we sort out our differences and arguments, but
its just that there are so many – all the time about every little thing.” (Dutch female, 32).
A moment should also be taken to comment on the overall slant of the partner
characteristics, as well as the exceptions to this. The secure participants reported overall a
more positive perspective of their partners, and the reverse was true for the insecure
participants who overall reported a more negative perspective of their partners. However, to
74 Interpersona 1 (2) – December 2007
balance out the pictures that were painted by the previous comments, it should be noted
that individuals in the secure category also had less than flattering comments to make about
“We are very different in most other respects and do not have many interests in common. This
causes problems between us… She becomes very unhappy when she feels she is not getting enough
of my time. I believe she is quite needy in many ways.” (New Zealand European male, 56).
The same also applied to the insecure group where some participants also balanced
“We are lovers, best friends and he means more to me than anyone in this world.” (New
This information fleshes out the picture of secure and insecure attachment, thus
showing the range of experiences as well as the variation within each categorization.
Both secure and insecure individuals were able to identify a number of similar
helpful conflict resolution strategies that they use in their relationships, including calm
discussions, taking turns during discussion, listening and trying to understand the other’s
perspective.
“Listen to what the other person has to say and try to understand why they feel that way without
taking personal objection before making any response.” (New Zealand European female,
41).
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 75
“… we have begun meeting for coffee every Tuesday – just to spend some time together with no
children, and often we end up resolving minor stuff through a chat.” (New Zealand European
female, 38).
discussions, and potentially involving a third party when an impasse is reached. The
insecurely attached individuals also noted the importance of taking responsibility for
including withdrawal, attacking and overly emotional behavior, as well as blame and
personal insults.
“Things that make it worse for me is when my partner simply shuts down and stops listening to
me – he just turns off as if I’m unimportant and shortly after leaves the room. That’s what
A number of participants who are securely attached indicated that they do not fail to
reach resolution to their problems, or have not noticed any strategies that make their
problems worse. In addition, it should be noted that some securely attached participants
“In our early days I used to get sulky and would withdraw. It didn’t help but we sorted that out
Although there was overlap between both groups’ unhelpful strategies, insecure
individuals’ unhelpful strategies were overall more negatively slanted. In addition they
“Just asking for a discussion to try and sort out an issue sets it up for failure as [he] instantly
assumes I will be nagging or criticizing and then he is unwilling to even discuss the matter… in
the event that the discussion is put off and off and off I get more and more frustrated and my
behavior deteriorates out of control – verbally.” (New Zealand European female, 38).
Both groups reported similar ideal conflict strategies, including a desire for no
conflict in ideal relationships, listening well to obtain deeper understanding and calm, in-
depth discussions. The insecure group indicated the importance of finding mutually
acceptable solutions and resolving conflict before going to bed. In addition the securely
attached group identified a focus on problem solving as ideal. A number of securely attached
individuals also commented that they would be using strategies similar to their own in their
ideal relationship.
“Like us! Conflict is a fact of life but our understanding is we will work things through – we are
committed to our marriage, even if at times we do argue.” (New Zealand European female,
50).
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 77
Table 1
Summary Table of Key Themes categorized by Secure and Insecure Partner Attachment (N = 44)
Discussion
Research has indicated that adults with a secure attachment style describe higher
with an avoidant attachment style who describe lower levels of these features (e.g., Collins &
Feeney, 2004). In another study it was found that husbands and wives with secure
attachment styles were less rejecting and more supportive of their partners than their
insecurely attached counterparts (Gao et al., 1997). These results are echoed in the findings
of the current study, with securely attached participants indicating caring, loving and
affectionate relationships, whilst insecurely attached participants felt that there were
relationships, as well as support and consideration for each other. For securely attached
individuals there was also a sense that their relationships were built on trust, integrity and
Previous research has indicated that securely attached people report less fighting and
threat and focus on re-establishing togetherness, which might in turn hamper their problem
solving ability (Pistole & Arricale, 2003). These researchers have also found that securely
attached individuals reported less conflict avoidance than those with dismissing attachment
styles (Pistole & Arricale, 2003). Along similar lines, Shi (2003) reported that secure
individuals displayed more positive behaviour and higher relationship satisfaction than
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 79
individuals who scored higher on preoccupied attachment and avoidant attachment. Both
the secure and the insecure groups were aware of helpful strategies. All of the strategies
described by the participants in this study as ‘helpful’ would fall under the broader heading
of a positive problem solving style. Gross and Guerrero (2002) also found that an integrative
conflict style is generally perceived as the most appropriate and effective style, whereas the
In terms of strategies, both the securely attached and the insecurely attached groups
were able to identify a number of helpful strategies. Both groups identified calm discussions
as helpful, with the insecurely attached group qualifying that the calm discussions should
occur away from distractions. The secure group again mentioned that discussions need to
take place at an appropriate time. The insecure group commented that it was important for
each individual to take responsibility for their own behaviour during the conflict resolution
process. Both groups indicated the importance of taking turns during a discussion, as well
as the importance of listening intently to each other whilst attempting to understand the
other party’s perspective. Some participants in the securely attached group also saw the need
for involving a third party if an impasse is reached. The insecure group indicated the
relationships, reiterates some of these findings, and also suggests some additional gender
differences. It was found (Creasey, 2002) that young women, in particular, with secure
attachment styles used more positive behaviour during discussions of conflict, and female
attachment security also predicted the occurrence of joint couple positive behaviours,
whereas male insecurity predicted the frequency of negative behaviours. Creasey furthermore
suggests that although secure couples would occasionally use negative behaviours during
conflict, their liberal use of positive behaviours enhances the positive emotional content of
their discussions, and this is also in line with the current findings.
80 Interpersona 1 (2) – December 2007
Research to date has indicated that couples who manage their conflicts
constructively, experience more relationship satisfaction that their counterparts who utilize
ineffective conflict resolution styles (e.g., Kurdek, 1994). Although some overlap did occur
between unhelpful strategies for the participants in the secure and insecure groups, it can be
noted that a number of participants in the secure category indicated that they had no
unhelpful conflict resolution strategies. In other words, many of these participants felt that
they only had helpful strategies which would no doubt assist them in solving their conflict
constructively.
Research (e.g., Cohn et al., 1992; Cowan et al., 1996; George et al., 1996) has found
that men with insecure attachment styles display more negative emotions and engage in
conflict more frequently, than securely attached men. These researchers also found that in
conflict situations where both partners exhibited insecure attachment styles, interactions
were more strained. Gross and Guerrero (2000) found that the dominating style and
avoiding style were perceived as inappropriate and ineffective when trying to solve conflict.
In the current study the insecurely attached group also indicated avoidance as being an
Another gender-specific spousal interaction that has been identified in the literature
In the current study both men and women in the secure and the insecure groups indicated
Gottman’s more recent model of relational decay (1994) suggests that couples move
toward divorce due to the negative conflict behaviour they display that systematically leads
identified as corrosive to relationship satisfaction. In this regard, it can be noted that both
the secure and the insecure groups identified attacking and overly emotional behaviour, as
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 81
well as blame and personal insults as unhelpful strategies during conflict. In addition, the
securely attached individuals reported that the timing of the argument can quite often
hamper effective problem solving. For the insecurely attached individuals a focus on
‘winning’ the argument was also reported to interfere with their conflict resolution abilities.
Although there was some overlap between the securely attached and insecurely
attached groups in terms of what they perceived as unhelpful strategies, there was also an
being aware of some ineffective conflict resolution strategies, they had also learned
constructive conflict resolution strategies over time. In contrast some of the insecure
participants indicated behavioral patterns that currently exacerbate their problems, or which
Recent years have seen our cultural obsession with ideal love and ideal relationships
(Evans, 2003) develop into booming enterprises for dating agencies and reality television
programs, as they seek to exploit this phenomenon (Djikic & Oatley, 2004). Participants
were asked to imagine their ideal relationship, and then to imagine how the couple in their
ideal relationship would handle conflict. Firstly, a number of participants in the secure and
insecurely attached groups indicated that in an ideal relationship there would be no conflict,
as both individuals would be in perfect harmony with each other. According to the literature
this does not bode well for their relationships satisfaction, as most couples experience some
degree of conflict. In terms of real and ideal love relationships Coyne (2001) found that the
greater the discrepancies between perceptions of actual and ideal love in the relationship, the
lower the relationship satisfaction. Similarly, greater discrepancies between real and ideal
Simpson, Thomas, & Giles, 1999). The secure group could however be closer to their own
ideal couples, as many indicated that an ideal couple would utilize similar strategies to the
strategies that they themselves use to solve conflict. To this extent Mickulincer and Erev
82 Interpersona 1 (2) – December 2007
(1991) found that securely attached couples’ relationships were characterized by greater
congruence between their actual and ideal relationships. Greater congruence between real
and ideal relationships has also been linked to relationship satisfaction (Fletcher et al.,
1999). Levine (1995) theorized that the discrepancy between ideal and actual love is a natural
function of any long-term relationship, and it requires the individual to constantly manage
this “gap” by using a range of defenses which in turn might either enhance relationship well-
being (possibly the secure group) or destabilize the individual and the relationship (possibly
In terms of other conflict strategies, both groups indicated that couples in their ideal
relationship would be great listeners, and in that manner they would obtain a deeper
understanding of each other. Calm and in-depth discussions would also solve the problems
in an ideal relationship for both groups. The securely attached group also commented that
openness and a focus on problem solving would be helpful. The insecurely attached group
indicated that finding mutually acceptable solutions would be paramount for their ideal
couple, and then interestingly enough a number of participants indicated the importance of
resolving their conflict amicably before going to bed, maybe reflecting their own need for
having closure. All the conflict strategies noted by the secure and insecure groups emulate
adaptive from an evolutionary perspective as it increases the likelihood that the relationship
will be maintained long-term (Fletcher et al., 1999). This, according to Fletcher and his
colleagues, is important if one considers the time investment of both partners in the long-
term relationship and potential offspring (parental investment theory) (Fletcher, Tither,
O’Loughlin, Friesen, & Overall, 2004). The discrepancies between perceived and ideal
standards, and also between perceived and ideal conflict resolution styles, allows individuals
2001), and as such has implications for the longevity of a relationship (Fletcher et al., 2004).
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 83
Limitations
Participants were asked to self-report on their conflict behaviours. These are thus subjective
reports of behaviour, and this data is not based on observed behavioral indicators. Partner
attachment representations are not accessible to direct observation, and self-report is one
potential manner of measurement, which has numerous limitations including memory and
recall of attachment behaviour. In addition it should be noted that people possess multiple
attachment schemas which are influenced by the various attachments they form to people in
their lives (e.g., Mikulincer & Arad, 1999; Pierce & Lydon, 2001) and this research project
Attachment styles and other relationship variables, such as conflict resolution styles are
relatively stable over time (e.g., Crowell, Treboux, & Waters, 2002), but new experiences
could also potentially affect changes. The self-report questionnaire was administered once
only, thus providing a snapshot view of perspectives on partner attachment and real and
ideal conflict behaviour. This did not allow for measurement of changes in these variables
qualitative perspective, the number of participants allowed for adequate variability to allow
for a thematic analysis of the data (Boyatzis, 1998). However, Boyatzis also identified the
following obstacles to effective thematic analysis, which could potentially have influenced
the results in this study: projection, sampling, mood and style. Projection is viewed as
(Boyatzis). Future studies could prevent or lessen the potential effect of projection by using
several people to encode the information, thereby establishing consistent judgments and
greater reliability of findings (Boyatzis). Adequate sampling minimizes the number of errors
present in the data, so that the researcher can be sure that he/she is processing information
that is not contaminated by other variables or unknown factors (Boyatzis). For the most part
84 Interpersona 1 (2) – December 2007
the themes that were identified from the qualitative data were consistent with the literature,
thus it can be assumed that adequate sampling of the relevant factors were allowed for.
Lastly, Boyatzis identifies mood and style as an obstacle to effective data analysis, and by this
he implies that one’s mood and cognitive style can influence one’s ability to identify the
themes. It can be noted that the researcher did find the analysis of participants’ attachments
to their partners, as well as some of the stories that went with it more trying than some of
the other questions due to the nature of the answers, including references to physical abuse,
addictions and conflict in the family. However, the researcher did pace herself when
analyzing these themes, and allowed adequate debriefing with supervisors to lessen the
Conclusion
were all touted as helpful strategies during conflict resolution, whereas attacking, overly
emotional behaviour, blame, personal insults and withdrawal were seen as unhelpful. In
ideal relationships many couples believed there would be no conflict. In addition ideal
well as calm and in-depth discussions. This project set out to obtain a more in-depth
perspective of helpful, unhelpful and ideal conflict strategies in couples, and a number of
important differences and similarities were noted between securely attached and insecurely
attached groups. Although relevant to the general population (in particular New Zealand)
these findings are also particularly relevant to couple therapists and clinicians with regards
studies comparing couples in therapy with happy couples would shed more light as to
whether these differences become more pronounced, in comparison to this study. Future
studies could also quantify these differences with quantitative methodologies. As indicated
by some participants they have noticed that their conflict strategies have changed over time.
du Plessis & Clarke: Couples’ Helpful, Unhelpful and Ideal Conflict Resolution Strategies 85
Longitudinal research could examine the processes involved with these changes in conflict
References
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A
Beach, S.R.H. (2001). Expanding the study of dyadic conflict: The potential role of self-
Bradbury, T.N., Rogge, R., & Lawrence, E. (2001). Reconsidering the role of conflict in
marriage. In A. Booth, A.C. Crouter, & M. Clements (Eds.), Couples in conflict, (pp. 59-
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in
Psychology, 3, 77-101.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Routledge.
Boyatzis, R.E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: Thematic analysis and code
Caughlin, J.P. (2003). Family communication standards: What counts as excellent family
communication and how are such standards associated with family satisfaction.
Goldstein, (Eds.), Understanding major mental disorder: The contribution of family interaction
Campbell, L., Simpson, J.A., Kashey, D.A., & Fletcher, G.J.O. (2001). Ideal standards, the self,
and flexibility of ideals in close relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
27, 447-462.
86 Interpersona 1 (2) – December 2007
Cohn, D., Silver, D., Cowan, C., Cowan, P., & Pearson, J. (1992). Working models of
childhood attachment and couple relationships. Journal of Family Issues, 13, 432-449.
Collins, N.L., & Feeney, B.C. (2004). An attachment theory perspective on closeness and
intimacy. In D.J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 163 –
Collins, N.L., & Read, S.J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship
quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663.
Cowan, P., Cohn, D., Cowan, C., & Pearson, J. (1996). Parents’ attachment histories and
Coyne, M. (2001). Differences in real and ideal love in adult children of divorce. Dissertation
375.
Crowell, J.A., Treboux, D., & Waters, E. (2002). Stability of attachment representations: The
Djikic, M., & Oatley, K. (2004). Love and personal relationships: Navigating on the border
between the ideal and the real. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 34, 198-209.
Du Plessis, K., Clarke, D., & Woolley, C.C. (2007). Secure attachment conceptualizations:
The influence of general and specific relational models on conflict beliefs and conflict
http://www.interpersona.org
Fletcher, G.J.O., Simpson, J.A., Thomas, G., & Giles, L. (1999). Ideals in intimate
Fletcher, G.J.O., Tither, J.M., O’Loughlin, C., Friesen, M., & Overall, N. (2004). Warm and
homely or cold and beautiful? Sex differences in trading off traits in mate selection.
Gao, Y., Crowell, J., Treboux, D., & Waters, E. (1997). Is it easier for a secure person to use
and serve as a secure base? Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for
Gottman, J.M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes and marital
Gottman, J.M., Coan, J., Carrere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). Predicting marital happiness and
stability from newlywed interactions. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 5-22.
Gross, M.A., & Guerrero, L. K., (2000). Managing conflict appropriately and effectively: An
Hampel, D. (2003). Arguing skill. In J.O. Greene & B.R. Burleson (Eds). Handbook of
communication and social interaction skill (pp. 439-470). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jacobson, N.S. (1989). The politics of intimacy. Behaviour Therapist, 12, 29-32.
Kurdek, L.A. (1994). Conflict resolution styles in gay, lesbian, heterosexual nonparent, and
heterosexual parent couples. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 705-722.
Kurdek, L.A. (1995). Predicting change in marital satisfaction from husbands’ and wives’
conflict resolution styles. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 153-164.
Le Poire, B.A., Haynes, J., Driscoll, J., Driver, B.N., Wheelis, T.F., Hyde, M.K., et al. (1997).
Levine, S.B. (1995). On love. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 21, 183-191.
Levinger, G. (1966). Sources of marital dissatisfaction among applicants for divorce. Journal
Marchand, J.F. (2004). Husbands’ and wives’ marital quality: The role of adult attachment
Mikulincer, M., & Erev, I. (1991). Attachment style and the structure of romantic love. British
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P.R. (2002). Activation of the attachment system in
adulthood: Threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental representations
of attachment figures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 881-895.
Peterson, D.R. (1983). Conflict. In H.H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christensen, J.H. Harvey, T.L
Huston, G. Levinger, et al., (Eds.), Close relationships (pp. 360-396). San Francisco, CA:
Freeman.
Pistole, M.C. & Arricale, F. (2003). Understanding attachment: Beliefs about conflict. Journal
Schumm, W.R., Paff-Bergen, L.A., Hatch, R.C., Obiorah, F.C., Copeland, J.M., Meens, L.D., et
al. (1986). Concurrent and discriminant validity of the Kansas Marital Satisfaction
Sillars, A.L., Canary, D.J., & Tafoya, M. (2003). Communication, conflict, and the quality of
Sillars, A.L., Coletti, S.F., Parry, D., & Rogers, M.A. (1982). Coding verbal conflicts:
Simpson, J.A., Rholes, S.W., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships: An
attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 899-914.
Shi, L. (2003). The association between adult attachment styles and conflict resolution in