Action Research in Reading Intervention
Action Research in Reading Intervention
Action Research in Reading Intervention
Many reading interventions are used to help students at-risk for reading
disabilities succeed. Several studies and theories have addressed reading since the early
1900s. Dating back to the 1970s, many landmark studies have been done on reading. In
recent years, the focus of many studies has turned to the effects reading interventions
have on students. Several questions remain surrounding the topic of reading, including
the possible benefits of reading interventions.
A student who is a-risk is hard to define. Many factors could place a student at-
risk (Swanson, et al., 2014). Many kinds of struggling readers could be candidates for
intervention (Gustafson, Falth, Svensson, Tjus, & Heimann, 2014). Numerous teachers
and researchers are seeking evidence to help identify or prevent reading disabilities
(Swanson et al., 2014). A need exists for thorough research on how well interventions
work and the most effective intervention techniques. “It is important to know that a
student who achieves at a low level academically is not automatically LD” (Sze, 2012, p.
1015). We need to find the best ways to teach these struggling readers.
THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
Besides being able to incorporate theories to reading, theories are made about
reading. One landmark reading theory is founded by the principles of perceptual
development, as noted by Gibson and Levin (1975). The seven keys of Gibson and
Levin’s (1975) theory are distinctive features, invariant relations in events, higher order
structures, abstraction, ignoring irrelevant information, peripheral mechanisms, and
reduction of uncertainty. With an emphasis on the different types of reading and
various readers, Gibson and Levin have done much research into the psychology of
reading.
RESEARCH QUESTIONS
DEFINITION OF TERMS
At-risk readers. “Conditions that place children at risk for reading difficulties include
poverty, cultural and linguistic difficulties, neurologically based problems, inadequate
instruction, limited development-enhancing opportunities, or familial history of reading
disabilities” (Swanson et al., 2014, p. 2).
Dialogic Reading. “The storytelling role is gradually shifted from the adult reader to the
child through various techniques” (Swanson et al., 2014, p.10).
Encoding. “Encoding instruction is not limited to just teaching spelling patterns and
memorization skills. Encoding instruction also includes explicitly teaching beginning
readers and spellers to write words according to their phoneme-grapheme
correspondences, to build words using manipulatives (e.g., letter tiles, plastic letters,
etc.), and to learn to manipulate phoneme-grapheme relationships to make new words
(e.g., pat and tap, stop and pots) (Weiser & Mathes, 2014, p. 3).
Onsets and Rimes. “An onset in a syllable is the initial consonant(s) (e.g., the c in cat);
the rime comprises the vowel and consonants that follow (e.g., the at in cat)” (Hines,
2012, p. 21).
READ 180. “READ 180 aims to address the diversity in student reading profiles by
providing differentiated instruction in each of the components of reading: phonemic
and phonological awareness, fluency, vocabulary, and reading comprehension” (Kim,
Capotosto, Hartry, & Fitzgerald, 2014, p.4).
Reading Recovery. “In Reading Recovery lessons, children are shown how to self-
monitor, to check their understanding using all the strategies available to them, to
predict and to confirm. In other words, they are shown how to develop and make use of
metacognitive strategies in their reading” (Hurry & Sylva, 2010, p. 3).
SEM-R. “The SEM-R’s three phases follow this learning approach, as Phase 1 focuses on
exposing students to brooks, Phase 2 incorporates differentiated instruction, including
specific reading strategy instruction, applied to self-selected independent reading, and
Phase 3 allows students to pursue self-selected enrichment activities and projects
related to reading” (Reis, McCoach, Little, Muller, & Kaniskan, 2013, p. 5).
How does the use of early intervention affect k-3 student reading progress?
After reading studies about the effectiveness of onset and rime instruction,
Hines (2012), performed a study of her own on seven first grade students from the
eastern United States. Upon completion of a screening, the four participating students
were those found to be of the seven most at-risk students of all of the first grade
students in the district that were given parent consent. The other three students were
disqualified due to lack of knowledge, too many absences, and a holiday vacation
disruption of the intervention schedule. The screening qualifications measured
intervention letter name knowledge, intervention consonant sound knowledge, and
recognizing intervention CVC/CVCC words. To be included in the study, the student must
identify 80% of the letter names, provide 75% of the consonant sounds, and failure to
decode CVC/CVCC words with more than 15% accuracy. Some of the participants were
already receiving reading intervention prior to the study. Each of the students during
this intervention read eight “Rime to Read” books. If the student made more than five
errors in the first four books or eight errors in the last four books, the book was reread
during the next session. After completion of the first four books and then again the last
four books, a color coded flashcard sort was completed by the child containing the four
sets of rimes from the books. After the child completed the sort, it was done again
without the color coding. The amount of time each student needed to get through the
intervention program varied greatly.
Hines (2012) administered the study with the four first graders on her own, but
taped her sessions. Fidelity at an average of 96% was measured by a second party based
on how well the script and correction procedures were followed. “The reliability was
86% with a range from 67% to 100% for the measure of instructional words, 89% with a
range from 75% to 100% for the measure of near-transfer words, and 91% with a range
of 67% to 100% for the measure of far-transfer words (Hines, 2012, p. 26). As a result,
the four students found to increase in instructional words by an average of 73% over
baseline. The students also had positive results on the near-transfer measure on
average of 56% over baseline. Hines (2012) felt that the onset and rime reading
intervention was positive.
Substantial progress was shown in the short-term for students with the Reading
Recovery intervention compared to the controls. In English schools already teaching
Reading Recovery with a trained teacher, nearly four-hundred children age six to six-
and-a-half years old were included in the study. With seven months of interventions
given by a trained teachers provided by the schools, the effect size was medium to large
and the students were believed to have an eight month advantage (Hurry & Sylva,
2010). The phonological training was more specific than Reading Recovery, but not
secure. Phonological training was ahead of the controls on phonemic awareness. The
effect size was small within school comparison. A significantly higher effect size was
found with between-school comparisons on the Diagnostic Survey. No significant effect
on reading was found.
Four years after the initial intervention was administered, the students were
assessed again by the researchers (Hurry & Sylva, 2010). Both interventions were
somewhat above the controls, but many intervention effects were negligible or small.
The effects were statistically insignificant with the exception of spelling in the
phonological training group. Better progress for reading and spelling was significantly
associated with phonological training in between school control groups, but not in the
within-school comparison.
Research has proven that early intervention is the key to helping students catch
up with their peers faster (Sze, 2012, p. 1016). It is clear that early intervention in
reading for at-risk students has shown positive results. All of the studies have shown
success for students. Despite the positive results, it is unclear as to what extent the
interventions work in the long term for the students. Which intervention strategies work
the best for teaching reading is also an unknown. With the best intervention practices,
teachers can provide students with more success and achievement.
Which intervention strategies work the best to teach reading?
Decoding has been a topic of studies and theories for many years, including the
inclusive investigation work of Resnick and Waver (1979). The importance of encoding
instruction has also been studied (Weiser & Mathes, 2014). Through the use of analysis
and syntheses, the authors provide empirical evidence to support their directional
hypotheses that encoding instruction will enhance student abilities to read and spell. A
total of 960 students between kindergarten and third grade that read below a third
grade level were involved in the eleven studies. Each study was administered in a variety
of ways for different amounts of times, but each study was using encoding
interventions.
After studying theories and results, the researchers feel they have the evidence
needed for a reasonable, informed conclusion (Weiser & Mathes, 2014). The qualitative
and quantitative evidence points to both special needs and general education students
benefiting from encoding instruction. When encoding instruction is integrated with
decoding instruction, the researchers believe substantial gains are made in phoneme
awareness, alphabetic decoding, word reading, spelling, fluency, and comprehension. All
of the studies showed positive effects for struggling readers and spellers as well as
decoding instruction as a context for teaching phoneme–grapheme correspondences,
blending, and segmenting. The average Cohen’s d effect size for spelling was 0.60.
Cohen’s average d effect size was 0.84 (SE = 0.07), supporting the importance of helping
struggling readers with encoding instruction to benefit the alphabetic principle.
Encoding instruction shows benefits for phonemic awareness with an average Cohen’s d
effect size of 0.87 (SE = 0.08). Results from the various studies showed encoding and
decoding instruction together increased reading skills of real and non-words (mean
Cohen’s d = 0.70).
Although the results suggest a positive treatment effect in one urban school, no
significant effect was shown in the other four schools (Reis et al., 2014). The SEM-R
outperformed the control in reading fluency in one suburban school and showed some
benefit at an urban school in reading comprehension. The attitude of the teachers did
not appear to have significance. The SEM-R groups are believed to be greater engaged
and have more enjoyable reading experiences. No detriment was found to students
using SEM-R, so the researchers concluded that SEM-R works as well as or better than
traditional approaches to reading instruction. However, the study did have limitations in
fidelity, which are hard to quantify, random assignment, and an uncertainty in which
activities worked. Also, no measurement is found for engagement and the researchers
had a lack of SES data that could affect results.
How can the same strategies be used to teach reading to all students?
The reading intervention strategies that were researched seem to provide little
to significant positive effects on all students. The many types of readers and reading
difficulties may contribute to the lack of significant positive effects on all students. A
landmark theory by Gibson and Levin (1975) focused on the psychology of reading and
the variety of readers and types of reading. Some intervention strategies seem to work
better with certain types of students, but no detriment to students has been found. The
studies by Kim, Caposto, Harty, and Fitzgerald (2014) as well as Gustafson et al. (2014)
are two studies that show positive effects on students that have no reading difficulties.