Evolutionary in Uence On Human Landscape Preference: Environment and Behavior July 2010
Evolutionary in Uence On Human Landscape Preference: Environment and Behavior July 2010
Evolutionary in Uence On Human Landscape Preference: Environment and Behavior July 2010
net/publication/249624620
CITATIONS READS
112 1,614
2 authors, including:
John H. Falk
Oregon State University
112 PUBLICATIONS 6,880 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by John H. Falk on 06 July 2014.
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Environmental Design Research Association
Additional services and information for Environment and Behavior can be found at:
Subscriptions: http://eab.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Citations http://eab.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/42/4/479
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Article
Environment and Behavior
42(4) 479–493
Evolutionary Influence © 2010 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permission: http://www.
on Human Landscape sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0013916509341244
Preference http://eab.sagepub.com
Abstract
Individuals residing in the rainforest belt of Nigeria were shown photographs
of five biomes: rain forest, deciduous forest, coniferous forest, savanna, and
desert. Subjects overwhelmingly selected savanna scenes as representing the
most desirable place to live. These results, coupled with extensive American
data, support the hypothesis that humans possess an innate preference
for savanna-like settings, which then is modified through experience and
enculturation. Findings are discussed in relation to anthropological, biological,
and psychological research.
Keywords
landscape preference, evolutionary psychology, savanna, cross-cultural, human
evolution, habitat preference
1
Oregon State University
2
Willamette University
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
480 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Falk and Balling 481
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
482 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Falk and Balling 483
Crooks, 2000; Kaplan & Herbert, 1987). Herzog et al. replicated the earlier study
by Kaplan and Herbert, but included a broader sample of settings and examined
several subcultural groupings. Familiarity once again emerged as a significant
predictor of preference and is consistent with our earlier thesis that preferences
can be modified by experience and enculturation. Six landscape categories were
selected: including rivers, dry lake beds, flood plains, terraces, mallee (a type of
Eucalyptus) plains and cultural images. American college students were com-
pared to several Australian subcultural groups. Australian students were divided
further by age: primary, secondary, college students, and adults. College students
were further subdivided into three groups and adults into four professional groups.
On a 5-point scale, participants rated the 60 slides for preference, defined as how
pleasing they found the setting, or how much they liked it. The two cultures
showed high correlations, with the greatest divergence coming from the Austra-
lian adults. In the cross-cultural perception comparison test, participants inde-
pendently divided the 60 slides into six of their own categories: vegetation, open
smooth, open coarse, rivers, agrarian, and structures. The two open catego-
ries, featuring little or no vegetation with vast open views, and the structures cat-
egory, represented by man-made materials present in the scene, were similarly
least preferred. Two categories, vegetation, consisting of near trees and bushes,
and agrarian, consisting of agricultural settings with trees present, were signifi-
cantly more preferred, but similar in preference to each other. The most preferred
category, significantly above all others, was rivers. Each of these six categories
was represented by one photograph each in the published study. Although none
of the photographs clearly represented a savanna-like setting, it is interesting to
note that the top three preferred categories included trees relatively close to the
observer with large open areas in view, with the highest preference ratings com-
ing from the youngest participants.
Our initial study (Balling & Falk, 1982), conducted in the eastern United
States, represented one culture from a region dominated by one biome, decid-
uous forest. The study reported here extends our research to a very different
cultural and environmental milieu and provides support for our previous
conclusions.
Method
In February 1981, J.F. sampled three populations of West Africans from Riv-
ers State, Nigeria. Two of the samples were drawn from “First Form” second-
ary school children, ranging in age from 12 to 18 years. Sample A (N = 27)
was taken from a coeducational public secondary school, in an upland rain-
forest area, roughly 100 km north-west of Port Harcourt. The dominant local
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
484 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
vegetation was slash and burn agricultural land interspersed with scattered
pockets of remnant rainforest. Sample B (N = 36) was from a girls’ parochial
school located in a small fishing village on one of the hundreds of small
islands in the Niger Delta. The dominant local vegetation was mature tropical
rainforest and mangrove forest; very little of the land was under cultivation.
A total of 87% of subjects from both samples A and B had been born in and
had never traveled beyond areas vegetationally comparable to their home
town area. Each participant was individually interviewed and asked to exam-
ine 45 pairs of 3″ × 5″ (8.6 cm × 12.8 cm) photographs of landscapes repre-
senting all possible unique binary combinations of 10 scenes, two each of
tropical rainforest, temperate deciduous forest, temperate coniferous forest,
tropical savanna, and mid-latitude desert.1 Except for vegetational density, all
pictures were as comparable as possible (see Balling & Falk, 1982). The
participant was asked, for each pair, to indicate which photo looked most like
a place where he or she would like to live. Although subjects varied greatly
in age, they were all at similar levels of schooling. Performance on this rather
hypothetical task, which demands making judgments without recourse to real
world knowledge, should tend to be more a function of years in school than
chronological age (Sharp, Cole, & Lave, 1979).
Sample C (N = 37), aged 20-39 years, was drawn from a class of students
at a technical college in Port Harcourt, Nigeria. These students’ homes were
located throughout Rivers State, both delta and upland areas. A total of 73%
had never traveled outside of the rainforest belt of West Africa. This sample
was shown 20 randomly ordered slides, representing four replicates of each
of the five biomes previously mentioned. This was the same procedure used
with most of our American subjects. Subjects were asked to indicate on a
5-point Likert-type scale the degree to which the scene depicted appeared to
be a favorable place in which to live.
Results
For samples A and B, the total number of times each slide was preferred to
every other slide was calculated. As indicated in Table 1, for subjects in
samples A and B, savanna was highly preferred relative to all other biomes
(Χ2(4) = 119.6, p < .001). Pair-wise tests revealed that rainforest, desert,
deciduous forests, and coniferous forests were not significantly different
from one another. As indicated by Thurstone scale values also shown in
Table 1, a positive preference was demonstrated only for savanna; subjects
were either neutral or slightly negative toward all other biomes. There were
no statistical differences between the two samples.
Falk and Balling 485
Biome
Biome
Discussion
We have previously argued that the tendency to purposefully create savanna-
like landscape elements and arrangements is not accidental, but rather at
least partially the result of an innate preference for savanna-like settings
486 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
(Balling & Falk, 1982; Falk, 1977). Such a preference for the savanna-like
landscape would have theoretically developed over the course of human evo-
lution, initially as an adaptation to surviving in the savannas of East Africa.
Although life experience modifies preference, the affinity younger subjects
have for savanna-like settings seems to suggest that a preference for savanna-
like environments likely represents a vestigial trait of early human biology; a
trait which expresses itself in modern humans as an aesthetic preference.
The belief that human biology and behavior were significantly molded
by adaptations our early ancestors made in order to survive in savanna-like
environments grew out of a series of major hominid fossil discoveries in
southern and eastern Africa during the early part of the 20th century. Associ-
ated fauna and flora led anthropologists to the reasonable inference that the
evolution of modern hominids was guided by the change in habitats through-
out Africa. The increase in aridity was thought to have propelled early homi-
nids away from the comfort of tropical habitats into the more hostile savanna.
Recent studies indicate that the landscape of human evolution was not
exclusively savanna, but rather a mosaic of environments including grass-
lands, savanna, open and closed forests (Butzer, 1977; Foley, 1996; Kingston,
Marino, & Hill, 1994; Leakey, Feibel, McDougall, & Walker, 1995; Scott,
1990; White, Suwa, & Asfaw, 1994).
The growing body of information about the habitats in which early humans
evolved remains insufficient to make a strong case for or against the hypoth-
esis that humans evolved a habitat preference for savanna-like environments.
We do know that human ancestors lived in or near savannas. We also know
they lived in or near a wide range of other habitats. However, the presence of
hominid fossils in these other settings does not in and of itself inform where
these hominids lived, since remains may have been transported to other envi-
ronments by predators or through physical events (e.g., flash floods). Hence,
fossil remains alone provide insufficient evidence to say which, if any, of
these many environments were the preferred habitat of our immediate ances-
tors. Given the diversity of environments inhabited by early hominids,
Chamberlain (2000) argued that a human preference for savanna habitats
may have arisen relatively recently with the emergence of Homo sapiens.
Evidence in other living primate groups suggests that environmental prefer-
ences are a species-specific adaptation rather than primitive or generalized
traits that have been inherited by all members of a genus. Furthermore, as
advances in our understanding of the human genome reveal, modern humans
are exceedingly closely related (Davies, 2001; Ridley, 2000; Wells, 2002).
For example, any two modern humans, from anywhere on the globe, possess
greater genetic similarity than do any two chimpanzees; even those selected
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Falk and Balling 487
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
488 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
seamlessly interconnected. Several studies have been conducted which show that
even such “basic,” presumed biologically influenced human perceptions as color
recognition (Berlin & Kay, 1969; Garro, 1986; Heider, 1972; Kay & Kempton,
1984; Lucy & Schweder, 1979), facial expressions (Gerber, 1975, 1985; Levy,
1973, 1990; Lutz, 1982, 1985; Poole, 1985) and emotion (Wierzbicka, 1986;
Gerber, 1985; Lutz, 1986) are culturally bound. So why not landscape preference
as well? According to this view, all objects in the world, including landscapes are
cultural constructs and represent a socially mediated form of culturally specific
conversation between the producers of that medium and the user (Moll & Green-
berg, 1990); landscape preferences are not innate but acquired.
Against this mix of opinion, we try to make sense of the current data. The
three modern human populations sampled in this study represented a diversity
of sociocultural backgrounds and experiences (Agrarian, fishing and urban
professional; rural and urban). When compared to previously sampled Ameri-
cans, with dramatically different sociocultural and economic histories, we
find a striking convergence in the data. The results reported here are consistent
with our earlier findings, leading to greater confidence in the accuracy of our
earlier thesis: humans appear to possess an innate preference for savanna-like
settings, but that preference can and typically is modified through experience
and enculturation. Beyond a strong modal tendency to prefer savanna, how-
ever, responses to landscapes become much more difficult to predict in differ-
ent populations. We have earlier postulated that experience and familiarity
with nonsavanna environments can lead to an elevation of preference for these
landscapes (Balling & Falk, 1982); or as suggested by Sample C in this study,
an aversion. The data strongly support the idea that life experience modifies
preference (a variable rarely taken into consideration in much of the current
research on landscape preference where samples are drawn primarily from popu-
lations of self-selected college students). Such an interpretation is generally
reinforced in Samples A and B where rainforest was the second most preferred
biome. By contrast, Americans of comparable age gave the rainforest a low
preference rating and deciduous forest high ratings. However, in Population
C, rainforest dropped to the least preferred position despite the fact that sub-
jects were highly familiar with this biome. One explanation is that this aver-
sion was brought about by a purposeful rejection of their “bush” roots by these
newly urbanized youth. Whatever the reason for the aversion by the urban
Population C to rainforest, the main effect remains—all three of these groups
demonstrated an overwhelming preference for a savanna environment; an
environment with which most were totally unfamiliar. This evidence strongly
supports the hypothesis that some kind of an innate, biological preference
exists for the savanna landscape, despite familiarity with another biome and
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Falk and Balling 489
regardless of prior experience with the savanna landscape. The data do sug-
gest that landscape preference is a complex amalgam of factors, with innate
preferences forming a foundation which is then overlain by both sociocultural
and personal experience factors.
Other theories can be used to supplement our understanding of these find-
ings, including Gibson’s (1979) affordances theory, the coherence, legibil-
ity, mystery, complexity theory of Kaplan and Kaplan (1989), and Ulrich’s
(1977) five factors theory. All of these hypotheses share an underlying con-
ceptual framework rooted in a Darwinian view of landscape preference. It is
highly likely that something as behaviorally complex as a habitat preference
involves multiple genetic loci, and as indicated above is strongly influenced
by the interaction of the phenotype with the environment. Accordingly, factors
such as affordance, coherence, legibility, mystery, complexity, and sensitivity
to the fractal properties of the visual environment cannot be ruled out as
important psychological dimensions associated with a genetically influenced
ability to discriminate between preferred and nonpreferred landscapes.
In conclusion, the results of this study plus the equally compelling data
from our earlier American research support our hypothesis that humans,
regardless of origin and experience begin life with a preference for savanna-like
environments. The data strongly suggest that human evolutionary history has
had a major influence not only on such physical features as human posture,
brain size and diet but also on some of our most fundamental behaviors and
preferences as well.
Authors’ Note
Special thanks to G. Wokeh, D. DuBey, and B. Junge, without whose assistance these
data could never have been collected. Thanks also to the Smithsonian Institution who
provided sabbatical leave for J.F., allowing conduct of this research. Finally thanks to
Steve Kaplan for encouraging J.F. to publish this research after all these years.
Note
1. Images were identical to those used and previously published by Balling and Falk
(1982).
References
Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of landscape. New York: John Wiley.
Appleton, J. (1994). How I made the world: Shaping a view of landscape. Elloughton:
University of Hull Press.
Balling, J. D., & Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preferences for natural
landscapes. Environment and Behavior, 14, 5-28.
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
490 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
Barker, J. S., Starmer, W. T., & Fogleman, J. C. (1994). Genotype-specific habitat selec-
tion for oviposition sites in the cactophilic species Drosophila buzzatii. Heredity,
72, 384-395.
Butzer, K. W. (1977). Environment, culture, and human evolution. American Scientist,
65, 572-584.
Chamberlain, A. T. (2000). On the evolution of human aesthetic preferences.
Retrieved July 3, 2008, from http://www.assemblage.group.shef.ac.uk/5/chamberl
.html#paper
Davies, K. (2001). Cracking the genome: Inside the race to unlock human DNA.
New York: Free Press.
Day, H. (1967). Evaluations of subjective complexity, pleasingness and interestingness
for a series of random polygons varying in complexity. Perception and Psycho-
physics, 2, 281-286.
Dutton, D. (2003). Aesthetics and evolutionary psychology. In J. Levison (Ed.), The
Oxford handbook for aesthetics. New York: Oxford University Press.
Falk, J. H. (1977). The frenetic life forms that flourish in suburban lawns. Smithson-
ian, 8, 90-96.
Foley, R. A. (1996). An evolutionary and chronological framework for human social
behavior. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 95-117.
Fox, C. W. (1993). A quantitative genetic analysis of oviposition and larval perfor-
mance in the bruchid beetle Calloscobruchus maculatus. Evolution, 47, 166-175.
Garro, L. C. (1986). Intracultural variation in folk medical knowledge: A comparison
between curers and non-curers. American Anthropologist, 88, 351-370.
Gerber, E. (1975). The cultural patterning of emotions in Samoa. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation. University of California, San Diego.
Gerber, E. (1985). Rage and obligation: Samoan emotion in conflict. In G. Whilte,
and J. Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Person, self and experience (pp. 121-167). Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-
Mifflin.
Hagerhall, C. M., Purcell, T., & Taylor, R. (2004). Fractal dimension of landscape
silhouette outlines as a predictor of landscape preference. Journal of Environmen-
tal Psychology, 24, 247-255.
Han, K.-T. (2007). Responses to six major terrestrial Biomes in terms of scenic
beauty, preference, and restorativeness. Environment and Behavior, 39, 529-556.
Heath, T., Smith, S. G., & Lim, B. (2000). Tall buildings and the urban skyline: The
effect of visual complexity on preferences. Environment and Behavior, 32, 541-556.
Herzog, T. R., Herbert, E. J., Kaplan, R., & Crooks, C. L. (2000). Cultural and devel-
opmental comparisons of landscape perceptions and preferences. Environment
and Behavior, 32, 323-346.
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Falk and Balling 491
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
492 Environment and Behavior 42(4)
Orians, G. (1980). Habitat selection: General theory and applications to human behav-
ior. In J. S. .Lockard (Ed.), The evolution of human social behavior (pp. 49-63).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In
J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolution-
ary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 555-579). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Partridge, L. (1978). Habitat selection. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.)
Behavioral ecology: An evolutionary approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Petit, L. J., & Petit, D. R.(1996). Factors governing habitat selection by prothonotary
warblers: Field tests of the Fretwell-Lucas Models. Ecological Monographs, 66,
367-387.
Rausher, M. D. (1984). The evolution of habitat preference in subdivided populations.
Evolution, 38, 596-608.
Ridley, M. (2000). Genome: The autobiography of a species in 23 chapters. New York:
Perennial.
Rogers, A., & Jorde, R. B. (1995). Genetic evidence and modern human origins.
Human Biology, 67, 1-36.
Rourke, T. (2007). The application of affordance theory to explain the landscape pref-
erences of travelers. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Clemson University, SC.
Scott, L. (1990). Palynological evidence for Late Quaternary environmental change
in South Africa. Proceedings of the IXth biennial conference of the Southern African
Society for Quaternary Research, 21, 259-268.
Singer, M. C., & Thomas, C. D. (1988). Heritability of oviposition preference and its
relationship to offspring performance within a single insect population. Evolution,
42, 977-985.
Sharp, D., Cole, M., & Lave, J. (1979). Education and cognitive development: The
evidence from experimental research. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 44 (1-2, Serial No. 178).
Sommer, R., & Summit, J. (1995). An exploratory study of preferred tree form. Envi-
ronment and Behavior, 27, 540-557.
Stroud, D. (1975). Capability Brown. Lonon: Faber & Faber.
Summit, J., & Sommer, J. (1999). Further studies of preferred tree shapes. Environ-
ment and Behavior, 31, 550-576.
Ulrich, R. S. (1977). Visual landscape preference: A model and application. Man-
Environment Systems, 7, 279-293.
Wecker, S. C. (1963). The role of early experience in habitat selection by the prai-
rie deer mouse, Peromyscus maniculatis bairdi. Ecological Monographs, 33,
307-325.
Wells, S. (2002). The Journey of Man. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010
Falk and Balling 493
White, T. D., Suwa, G., & Asfaw, B. (1994). Australopithecus ramidus, a new species
of early hominid from Aramis, Ethiopia. Nature, 371, 306-312.
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and uni-
versals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wohlwill, J. F. (1968). Amount of stimulus exploration and preference as differential
functions of stimulus complexity. Perception and Psychophysics, 4, 307-312.
Bios
John H. Falk is Sea Grant professor of free-choice learning at Oregon State University,
Corvallis, Oregon. Before joining the faculty at Oregon State University, he founded
and directed the Institute for Learning Innovation; was an early child science educator
at the University of Maryland; and spent 14 years at the Smithsonian Institution where
he held a number of senior positions, including special assistant for education to the
assistant secretary for science and director, Smithsonian Office of Educational
Research. He is known internationally for his research on free-choice learning, the
learning that occurs in settings like museums, parks, and on the Internet. He earned
his graduate and postgraduate degrees in zoology and a joint doctorate in biology and
science education, all from the University of California, Berkeley.
Downloaded from http://eab.sagepub.com at OREGON STATE UNIV LIBRARY on June 21, 2010