44MoriniTasteFoodChoice JPediatrGastroenterolNutr2012
44MoriniTasteFoodChoice JPediatrGastroenterolNutr2012
44MoriniTasteFoodChoice JPediatrGastroenterolNutr2012
net/publication/51923716
CITATIONS READS
42 1,108
10 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Gabriella Morini on 04 April 2018.
ABSTRACT
(pleasant) tastes, as sweet and umami (the savory taste of some
Objectives: The extent to which variation in taste perception influences
L-amino acids), elicited by energetic compounds as carbohydrates
food preferences is, to date, controversial. Bitterness in food triggers
an innate aversion that is responsible for dietary restriction in children.
and proteins, trigger attraction towards nutritive food. Bad (unplea-
We investigated the association among genetic variations in bitter receptor
sant) tastes, as bitter and sour, evolved to detect potentially lethal
TAS2R38 and food choices in healthy children in the Mediterranean area, compounds as plant secondary metabolites or microbial toxins.
to develop appropriate tools to evaluate the relation among genetic pre- Salty taste, developed to ensure ions and water homeostasis when
disposition, dietary habits, and feeding disorders. The aims of the study life moved from sea to land, can be good or bad depending on the
were to get a first baseline picture of taste sensitivity in healthy adults and concentration of sodium and on the physiological context.
their children and to explore taste sensitivity in a preliminary sample of Ancestral feeding behavior was retained by contemporary
obese children and in samples affected by functional gastrointestinal humans, although food choices do not pertain to life or death.
diseases. Nevertheless, taste is still a critical determinant of food selection,
Methods: Individuals (98 children, 87 parents, 120 adults) were recruited especially in children. As a legacy of the prehistoric age, neophobia,
from the general population in southern Italy. Bitterness sensitivity was the refuse of new unknown food, restrains the dietary habits of
assessed by means of a suprathreshold method with 6-propyl-2-thiouracil. infant babies and is related to feeding behavioral disorders.
Genomic DNA from saliva was used to genotype individuals for 3 The taste sensors are specific receptors located in the taste
polymorphisms of TAS2R38 receptor, A49P, A262 V, and V296I. Food buds of the gustatory papillae on the tongue and soft palate. Sweet,
intake was assessed by a food frequency questionnaire. umami, and bitter tastants are recognized by G-coupled protein
Results: Children’s taste sensation differed from that of adults: we receptors encoded by the TAS1R and TAS2R taste receptors gene
observed a higher frequency of supertasters among children even in the families, whereas salty and sour compounds are detected by ion
mother–child dyads with the same diplotypes. Among adults, supertaster channels. The T1R receptors are closely related and consist of the
status was related with proline-alanine-valine (taster allele) homozygous heterodimers TAS1R2/TAS1R3, which are able to recognize a wide
haplotype, whereas supertaster children were mainly heterozygous. range of sweet-tasting compounds, and TAS1R1/TAS1R3, respon-
Regarding the food choices, we found that a higher percentage of taster sible for the perception of the umami taste of L-glutamate.
children avoided bitter vegetables or greens altogether compared with The pivotal role of bitter perception to avoid accidental
taster adults. Taster status was also associated with body mass index in ingestion of potentially harmful substances is outlined by the
boys. presence in the human genome of 25 members of the T2R gene
Conclusions: Greater sensitivity to 6-propyl-2-thiouracil predicts lower family, mapping as clusters on chromosomes 7q and 5p. Human
preferences for vegetables in children, showing an appreciable effect of T2R genes display a high degree of polymorphism potentially
the genetic predisposition on food choices. None of the obese boys was a involved in the variance of individual bitterness sensitivity; how-
supertaster. ever, the functional consequences of the most of the receptor
variants on taste sensitivity is not yet known.
Key Words: bitter taste, food choices, 6-propyl-2-thiouracil, TAS2R38 Allelic variations affecting the perception of some bitter
compounds, such as salicine, quinine, and aloin, have been reported
(JPGN 2012;54: 624–629) in the TAS2R16, TAS2R19, and TAS2R43/44 genes, respectively, but
the linkage between the genotype and the trait variance is poor (1).
TAS2R38, a receptor for the thiourea compounds phenyltiocarba-
as the less common alanine-alanine-valine (AAV) and the rare Their age (45F/53M) ranged from 3 to 19 years. One hundred
haplotypes proline-valine-isoleucine (PVI) and alanine-alanine- twenty healthy young adults (medical or nursing students) were
isoleucine (AAI) can be observed (2). recruited on a voluntary base, after dissemination seminars at the
In vivo and in vitro studies reported PAV as the major university. Overall, the study sample mirrored the genetic and
determinant of taster status and AVI as the major nontaster hap- cultural environment of the local urban setting. Subjects were
lotype, so individuals with 2 copies of the AVI allele are basically considered eligible for the study if they were not on dietary
nontasters, whereas individuals with 1 or 2 copies of the PAV allele restrictions, with the exception of people with celiac disease,
are medium tasters or supertasters. PROP sensitivity of heterozy- who were on a gluten-free diet. The study was scrutinized and
gous common haplotypes PAV or AVI in combination with less of a approved by the ethics committee of the University ‘‘Federico II’’
common or a rare variant appears to lie in between (3,4). of Naples. Informed consent was obtained from individuals or
The genotype-phenotype relation is not stringent; this is guardians.
partly the result of the subjective methods of identification of
the phenotype but also of the likely complexity of the genetic
factors controlling taste. More genes were suggested to cooperate in Genotyping
the control of the phenotype (1,5,6). Several reports showed that the
Genomic DNA was obtained from saliva with the phenol-
perception of bitter taste is related not only to the specific taste of
chloroform extraction method following a protocol developed in our
bitter compounds but also to the wide behavior spectrum of the
laboratory. The allelic variations of the TAS2R38 gene C145G
individual in relation to food choices (7). Hypersensitive indivi-
(rs713598), C785T (rs1726866), and G886A (rs10246939) were
duals have a more restricted diet, compared with sensitive or
genotyped by means of real-time polymerase chain reaction with
insensitive individuals (8).
7900HT fast (Applied Biosystems, Carlsbad, CA) using allele-
These issues are of paramount importance in children after
specific probes and primers (Applied Biosystems).
weaning: this is the time when many children gradually develop
To estimate the population prevalence of the different hap-
erratic food acceptance, often generating anxiety in mothers.
lotypes, we excluded from the analysis the children of the mother–
Maternal expectations may not coincide with the transmission of
child dyads because their data are not independent of those of the
only 50% of the genetic predisposition to food choices, because the
mothers and would overrepresent maternal haplotypes. The
other partner is responsible for the other 50%. The genetic predis-
expected error of genotyping was estimated to be <1% by previous
position of the child is naı̈ve, because transformations brought by
work in our laboratory.
experience, social behavior, and traditions have no time to modify
genetic predisposition. There is indeed scope to explore mother–
child dyads to verify the concordance between them and the PROP Tasting
conditioning of the genetic predisposition.
There are few studies (9) analyzing the relation among food A 2-step approach was adopted to estimate the tasting
choices, sensitivity to bitter taste, and genetics in the Mediterranean phenotype of the subjects. All of the subjects underwent a supra-
population. Such studies may help us to understand the preference threshold test to assess the sensitivity of perception at suprathres-
for bitter vegetables and other greens in this population. hold concentrations. For a random sample of the subjects, after the
The specific research question at the base of the present study first suprathreshold test, we applied the threshold method, which
is to explore the phenotype-genotype correlation in bitter taste assesses the individual’s ability to discriminate low concentrations
sensitivity in either normal adults or mother–child dyads. A further of the stimulus. Suprathreshold intensity ratings are often used
task is to explore bitter taste sensitivity in obese children and those to correctly separate medium tasters from supertasters because
affected by a variety of food-related symptoms or diseases. their distribution overlaps substantially at threshold. The detection
threshold, based on the lowest concentration at which a person
METHODS detects the presence of a sensation, was instead described as the best
method to classify individuals as tasters or nontasters.
Population The test was offered to children and adults, who fasted for at
least 1 hour. The children were instructed not to swallow the
1. A total of 120 healthy adult volunteers (medical students) þ 44 solutions and, if required, underwent brief training in the ‘‘sip
healthy children and spit’’ method. Subjects tasted in ascending order 2 solutions of
2. A total of 54 children affected by a variety of functional 280 and 560 mmol/L PROP (6-propyl-2-thiouracil; Aldrich
disturbances Chemical, Milwaukee, WI) in distilled water, rinsing with water
3. A total of 92 mother–child dyads, including 41 of the 44 before and after each solution. The reported feeling or, in extremely
healthy and 51 of the 54 affected children young children, the facial expression was recorded. The sensitivity
evaluation was performed by constructing a 4-point scale in which
Inside the second group, there were children affected by: the labels ‘‘no taste,’’ ‘‘weakly unpleasant’’ (bitter, barely percep-
Feeding disturbances (6) tible), ‘‘unpleasant’’ (bitter), and ‘‘very unpleasant or terrible’’
1.
(extremely bitter) corresponded to values between 0 and 3. Facial
2. Functional gastrointestinal disturbances (10)
reactions were referred to a 4-point hedonic scale corresponding to
3. Celiac disease (18) ‘‘no taste/neutral,’’ ‘‘weakly unpleasant/depression of mouth cor-
4. Organic diseases (familial hypercholesterolemia, diabetes, ners,’’ ‘‘unpleasant/frown and depression of mouth corners,’’ and
obesity, metabolic dysfunctions) (10) ‘‘very unpleasant/frown and grimace.’’ According to the score
5. Food allergy (10) achieved, subjects were classified as nontasters (NT, score 0–2),
medium tasters (MT, score 3–4), and supertasters (ST, score 5–6).
Forty-four unselected healthy children, with their mothers, For quality control, to estimate the intraindividual variability
were recruited consecutively at a well baby clinic in the field; of the suprathreshold test, we repeated the same test in 48 of the 144
54 children affected by a variety of disturbances were recruited healthy individuals. The intraindividual concordance between the
from outpatients in the Department of Pediatrics. first and the second suprathreshold tests was 78.7%. In addition, we
www.jpgn.org 625
Copyright 2012 by ESPGHAN and NASPGHAN. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Negri et al JPGN Volume 54, Number 5, May 2012
applied the detection threshold method to a random sample of 10% TABLE 1. Phenotypic classification of the sample by PROP
(n ¼ 30) of the study population. PROP threshold was determined status
using a ‘‘forced choice’’ procedure, in which individuals tasted
2 samples, 1 of water and 1 of PROP solution, and were asked to Frequency %
identify the sample with the stronger taste. Six PROP solutions,
from 0.032 to 3.2 mmol/L in distilled water, were used. Threshold Adults Nontasters 47 22.5
values were identified as the first concentration correctly selected in Medium tasters 128 61.2
2 subsequent presentations. Subjects were classified as nontasters if Supertasters 34 16.3
the threshold was 0.15 mmol/L and tasters if 0.1 mmol/L. Total 209 100.0
Through the classification of individuals by the 2 methods a Children Nontasters 22 22.9
good agreement was obtained in separating insensitive from sensitive
Medium tasters 45 46.9
subjects, because the threshold of the subjects classified as nontasters
by the suprathreshold test was 0.56 mmol/L and that of the medium Supertasters 29 30.2
tasters or supertasters ranged from 0.032 to 0.1 mmol/L. Total 96 100.0
Total 305
Food Preferences PROP ¼ 6-propyl-2-thiouracil.
626 www.jpgn.org
Copyright 2012 by ESPGHAN and NASPGHAN. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
JPGN Volume 54, Number 5, May 2012 Taste Perception and Food Choices
adults (17.1%) with the same genotype (x2 ¼ 8.83, P < 0.002) addition, among the nontaster adults a sex difference was observed:
(Table 4). Individuals who were AVI homozygous were, as 22 of 31 (71%) female nontasters ate no legumes compared with
expected, more often nontasters in adults (26/31 ¼ 84%) and in only 3 of 15 (20%) male nontasters (x2 ¼ 10.584; P < 0.001).
children (6/7 ¼ 86%). The evaluation of bitter vegetable intakes by genotype
Concerning the subjects with an AAV haplotype, the hetero- revealed that there were no differences between AVI/AVI or
zygous AAV/AVI were similar to nontaster AVI homozygous, AVI/AAV individuals and PAV homozygous or heterozygous
rating the PROP solutions as ‘‘weak’’ or ‘‘barely detectable’’ (8 among adults, whereas 48 of the 63 children with the PAV allele
of 9 individuals), whereas 6 of 10 individuals who carried the (76.2%) consumed no bitter vegetables compared with 4 of
AAV/PAV diplotype were medium tasters and the remaining 3 7 children with nontaster genotype. Finally, the PROP status did
were supertasters, as observed for the PAV/AVI heterozygous. not influence the overall intake of fruits or salty snacks.
The genotype-phenotype analysis in the mother–child dyads With regard to the relation between the bitter sensitivity
showed that there is a high degree of concordance between mother phenotype and body mass index, in our sample of children we had
and child genotype (55/62 ¼ 88.7%) (Table 5). Similarly, the 42 normal weight, 35 overweight, and 11 obese children; never-
correlation of phenotypes between mother and child was strong theless, we observed that none of the obese boys or girls was a
(65/81 ¼ 80.3%) (Table 5). supertaster, as compared with 32% of children of normal weight
(x2 ¼ 4.88; P ¼ 0.027).
www.jpgn.org 627
Copyright 2012 by ESPGHAN and NASPGHAN. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Negri et al JPGN Volume 54, Number 5, May 2012
TABLE 5. Analysis of concordance between genotypes and phenotypes in the mother–child dyads
Child
Mother AVI PAVy Total
Genotype
AVI 7 5 12
PAVy 2 48 50
Totalz 9 53 62
Nontaster Taster Total
Phenotype
Nontaster 5 9 14
Taster 7 60 67
Total§ 12 69 81
AVI: AVI/AVI, AVI/AAV.
y
PAV: PAV/PAV, PAV/AVI, PAV/AAV.
z
k ¼ 0.6, 95% confidence interval 0.35 < 0.6 > 0.84.
§
k ¼ 0.268, 95% confidence interval 0.05 < 0.268 > 0.45.
aim was to observe the relation between taste sensitivity and food Nevertheless, the distribution of the taster groups in our
choices and to evaluate the weight of genetic predisposition by sample was in agreement with the frequencies estimated in the
comparing phenotypes and genotypes in the population, as well as European population (2). In addition, we observed more super-
in mother–child dyads. tasters among children than in adults, suggesting that innate bitter
Increased sensitivity to bitterness is associated with child sensitivity may be modified during life, in agreement with other
neophobia, responsible for children’s rejection of new, unfamiliar reports about the decline of PROP/PTC sensitivity in adult life, with
foods, which results in a diet particularly poor in food variety (10). increasing age (12).
The data obtained in this observational study provide a solid Sex differences in taste sensitivity were reported among
basis to explore feeding disorders and food-related diseases adults by several authors seemingly resulting from anatomical
in children. differences, in that women have more fungiform papillae and
Taste receptors and effectors that mediate gustatory signals more taste buds than men (13). Sex and age also can influence
in the oral cavity have been found in gastrointestinal mucosa, the expression of phenotype in children: it has been shown that the
suggesting a role in chemosensing that triggers the physiological percentage of nontasters does not differ by sex in young children
responses to luminal content, such as absorption of beneficial or (14,15), but at puberty more nontaster individuals are boys and
rejection of toxic foods. In this regard it is likely that genetic more taster individuals are girls (16). We did not find sex diversity
variations in taste receptors could modify food intake, leading to between taster groups, neither among preschool children nor
aberrant conditions such as feeding disorders up to obesity and among adolescents.
related metabolic dysfunctions. Because polymorphisms at the TAS2R38 locus explain the
The distribution of PROP phenotypic groups in our majority of the phenotypic variation in PROP sensitivity (17), we
population was suggestive of a model of Mendelian inheritance assessed how heritable variability in bitter taste perception predicts
of bitterness sensitivity, the nontaster individuals being almost one- the phenotypic groups in our population. Haplotype analysis of the 3
quarter of all (22.6%), and the supertasters (20.7%) and the medium TAS2R38 polymorphisms allowed us to identify 2 major forms, the
tasters roughly 50% (56.7%). taster allele PAV and nontaster allele AVI, accounting for the 95%
The mode of inheritance of PROP/PTC sensitivity remains of the sample analyzed, and the rare variants AAV, AAI, and AVV.
uncertain, showing features between a simple and a complex trait The occurrence of the TAS2R38 genotypes was similar to that of the
(11). Phenotypic variation in taste perception depends on various sample (representative of the population) of Italian ancestry, geno-
factors controlling a more general taste ability, such as sex and typed by Sacerdote et al (18).
ethnicity. The agreement between haplotype and phenotype was extre-
mely high; nevertheless, the genotype-phenotype association
TABLE 6. Bitter vegetables intake by taster groups estimated among supertasters was different in adults compared with children.
by a 3-day dietary recall Whereas the hypersensitive children were homozygous as well as
heterozygous, the hypersensitive adults were mainly homozygous.
Consumption It may be argued that the prevalent association between strongest
sensitivity to PROP and the PAV homozygous diplotype in the
Status Yes No Total adults may be related to a decrease in PROP sensitivity across adult
life (12).
Nontaster (%) 18 (31.6) 39 (68.4) 57 (100) A genetic factor able to influence the sensitivity to PROP has
Medium taster (%) 60 (44.4) 75 (55.6) 135 (100) been identified in the gustin gene, the salivary isoform of carbonic
Supertaster (%) 16 (27.6) 42 (72.4) 58 (100) anhydrase, with a suggested role in taste bud development (19).
Total (%) 94 (37.6) 156 (62.4) 250 (100) Individuals homozygous for the gustin polymorphism associated
with a fully functional protein were more frequent supertasters,
x2 ¼ 8.779; P < 0.067. whether they were PAV homozygous or heterozygous, whereas
628 www.jpgn.org
Copyright 2012 by ESPGHAN and NASPGHAN. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
JPGN Volume 54, Number 5, May 2012 Taste Perception and Food Choices
www.jpgn.org 629
Copyright 2012 by ESPGHAN and NASPGHAN. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.