Cognitive Dissonance: Saul Mcleod
Cognitive Dissonance: Saul Mcleod
Cognitive Dissonance: Saul Mcleod
Attitudes may change because of factors within the person. An important factor here
is the principle of cognitive consistency, the focus of Festinger's (1957) theory of
cognitive dissonance. This theory starts from the idea that we seek consistency in our
beliefs and attitudes in any situation where two cognitions are inconsistent.
Leon Festinger (1957) proposed cognitive dissonance theory, which states that a
powerful motive to maintain cognitive consistency can give rise to irrational and
sometimes maladaptive behavior. According to Festinger, we hold many cognitions
about the world and ourselves; when they clash, a discrepancy is evoked, resulting in
a state of tension known as cognitive dissonance. As the experience of dissonance is
unpleasant, we are motivated to reduce or eliminate it, and achieve consonance (i.e.
agreement).
Aim
Festinger and Carlsmith (1959) investigated if making people perform a dull task
would create cognitive dissonance through forced compliance behavior.
Method
In their laboratory experiment, they used 71 male students as participants to perform a
series of dull tasks (such as turning pegs in a peg board for an hour).
They were then paid either $1 or $20 to tell a waiting participant (a confederate) that
the tasks were really interesting. Almost all of the participants agreed to walk into the
waiting room and persuade the confederate that the boring experiment would be fun.
Results
When the participants were asked to evaluate the experiment, the participants who
were paid only $1 rated the tedious task as more fun and enjoyable than the
participants who were paid $20 to lie.
Conclusion
Being paid only $1 is not sufficient incentive for lying and so those who were paid $1
experienced dissonance. They could only overcome that dissonance by coming to
believe that the tasks really were interesting and enjoyable. Being paid $20 provides a
reason for turning pegs and there is therefore no dissonance.
Critical Evaluation
There has been a great deal of research into cognitive dissonance, providing some
interesting and sometimes unexpected findings. It is a theory with very broad
applications, showing that we aim for a consistency between attitudes and behaviors,
and may not use very rational methods to achieve it. It has the advantage of being
testable by scientific means (i.e. experiments).
There are also individual differences in whether or not people act as this theory
predicts. Highly anxious people are more likely to do so. Many people seem able to
cope with considerable dissonance and not experience the tensions the theory predicts.
Finally, many of the studies supporting the theory of cognitive dissonance have
lowecological validity. For example, turning pegs (as in Festinger's experiment) is an
artificial task that doesn’t happen in everyday life. Also, the majority of experiments
used students as participants which raise issues of a biased sample. Could we
generalize the results from such experiments?
Further Information
Attitudest
Cognitive Dissonance Theory
Elaboration Likelihood Model
Cognitive Dissonance
References
Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Festinger, L. (1959). Some attitudinal consequences of forced decisions. Acta
Psychologica, 15, 389-390.
How to cite this article:
The theory of cognitive dissonance
Cognitive Dissonance Theory, developed by Leon Festinger
(1957), is concerned with the relationships among cognitions. A
cognition, for the purpose of this theory, may be thought of as a
³piece of knowledge.² The knowledge may be about an attitude,
an emotion, a behavior, a value, and so on. For example, the
knowledge that you like the color red is a cognition; the
knowledge that you caught a touchdown pass is a cognition; the
knowledge that the Supreme Court outlawed school segregation
is a cognition. People hold a multitude of cognitions
simultaneously, and these cognitions form irrelevant, consonant
or dissonant relationships with one another.
Cognitive Irrelevance probably describes the bulk of the
relationships among a person¹s cognitions. Irrelevance simply
means that the two cognitions have nothing to do with each
other. Two cognitions are consonant if one cognition follows
from, or fits with, the other. People like consonance among their
cognitions. We do not know whether this stems from the nature
of the human organism or whether it is learned during the
process of socialization, but people appear to prefer cognitions
that fit together to those that do not. It is this simple observation
that gives the theory of cognitive dissonance its interesting form.
Two cognitions are said to be dissonant if one cognition follows
from the opposite of another. What happens to people when they
discover dissonant cognitions? The answer to this question
forms the basic postulate of Festinger¹s theory. A person who
has dissonant or discrepant cognitions is said to be in a state of
psychological dissonance, which is experienced as unpleasant
psychological tension. This tension state has drivelike properties
that are much like those of hunger and thirst. When a person has
been deprived of food for several hours, he/she experiences
unpleasant tension and is driven to reduce the unpleasant tension
state that results. Reducing the psychological sate of dissonance
is not as simple as eating or drinking however.
To understand the alternatives open to an individual in a state of
dissonance, we must first understand the factors that affect the
magnitude of dissonance arousal. First, in its simplest form,
dissonance increases as the degree of discrepancy among
cognitions increases. Second, dissonance increases as the
number of discrepant cognitions increases. Third, dissonance is
inversely proportional to the number of consonant cognitions
held by an individual. Fourth, the relative weights given to the
consonant and dissonant cognitions may be adjusted by their
importance in the mind of the individual.
If dissonance is experienced as an unpleasant drive state,the
individual is motivated to reduce it. Now that the factors that
affect the magnitude of this unpleasantness have been identified,
it should be possible to predict what we can do to reduce it:
Changing Cognitions
If two cognitions ar discrepant, we can simply change one
to make it consistent with the other. Or we can change each
cognition in the direction of the other.
Adding Cognitions
If two cognitions cause a certain magnitude of dissonance,
that magnitude can be reduced by adding one or more
consonant cognitions.
Altering importance
Since the discrepant and consonant cognitions must be
weighed by importance, it may be advantageous to alter the
importance of the various cognitions.
The material above is the background reading for the
Cognitive Dissonance Lab. These are excerpts from
Frederick M. Rudolph¹s page on Social Psychology. For a
more detailed discussion on cognitive dissonance and
related theories, visit
http://www.mindspring.com/~frudolph/lectuires/SOC/soc1.
htm