Sbrady Edrd 830 Final Paper
Sbrady Edrd 830 Final Paper
Sbrady Edrd 830 Final Paper
Stacie P. Brady
Abstract
This article synthesized the selected explicit vocabulary instruction studies conducted with
students in preschool through third grade between 2006 and 2018. Ten studies were synthesized
that focused primarily on explicit vocabulary instruction or intervention, including defining and
discussing, target words, as well as, using multi-media to expand word knowledge. There was a
total of 1,649 participants. Of the studies meeting the criteria, three studies focused on the use of
vocabulary and concept development with the use World of Words (WOW) supplemental multi-
media explicit vocabulary intervention. Four of the selected studies focused on increasing
dosage or supplemental instruction along with the use of storybook read-alouds. Two of the
studies addressed educator support. Three of the studies addressed long term vocabulary effects.
Additionally, the lasting effects of explicit vocabulary instruction was investigated by three of
the studies. Although there was a range in effect sizes computed for the various outcome
measures (0.42 to 1.73), findings indicate stronger effects were associated with explicit
vocabulary instruction. However, the studies used a variety of types and factors to address
explicit vocabulary techniques as the independent variable, including Text Talk, extended
instruction, Bringing Words to Life, and WOW. Overall, the studies reviewed showed positive
Learning to read is a complex task that involves decoding, fluency, vocabulary, relating
(Edmonds et al., 2009). Competently using these intertwined tasks allows a reader to develop
efficient reading skills. One critical skill that children begin to develop in their early years is
vocabulary. Students depend on their vocabulary knowledge for oral language and reading
comprehension (Biemiller, 2001). As children learn to read, they begin to match their oral
language word knowledge with the printed words they encounter while reading, building their
vocabulary (Kamil, 2004). Vocabulary is critical for comprehension of oral language and
development of reading comprehension, which both contribute to their overall academic success
Students begin school in preschool or kindergarten with varying oral language skills,
including receptive and expressive vocabulary (Cuticelli, Coyne, Ware, Oldham, & Rattan,
2015). Oral language skills are a good predictor of future literacy skills. For example, a
student’s vocabulary level at the beginning of first grade can help predict the student’s reading
level in 11th grade (Biemiller, 2001). Children who begin school with a high vocabulary level
can easily extract meaning from listening to stories using a low cognitive load (Perfetti & Hart,
2002; Verhoeven, van Leeuwe, & Vermeer, 2011; Damhuis, Segers, & Verhoeven, 2014). Some
students struggle to acquire new vocabulary. Early language skills affect the development of
literacy skills; therefore, it is critical that vocabulary is addressed to assist those acquiring
One factor that affects students’ literacy success is socioeconomic status (SES). Children
typically enter school with a vocabulary of approximately 20,000 words (Neuman & Dwyer,
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 4
2011). Students from a low SES enter school knowing approximately 5,000 words. A limited
vocabulary impedes success in reading (Marulis & Neuman, 2010). Research has shown that
word learning instruction is needed to close that vocabulary gap (Marulis & Neuman, 2013).
vocabulary instruction. They analyzed 52 studies and categorized vocabulary instruction into
types of repetition. Findings revealed that combining definitional and contextual information
while addressing vocabulary appears to be more effective than using either strategy
independently. Vocabulary instruction stimulated new interest in learning words and allowed
students to generalize and continue learning new words (Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986). Their
research suggested that vocabulary instruction is a useful addition to naturally learning from
context.
between vocabulary interventions and children’s oral language development. They analyzed 67
studies that targeted vocabulary instruction in an educational setting with preschool and
kindergarten children. The most prevalent interventions used in the studies were storybook
reading and dialogic reading, but also included computer-based interventions. Correlational,
experimental, and intervention studies provide evidence that storybook reading promotes
different aspects of language development including vocabulary (Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000).
students by asking them inferential questions and discussing new vocabulary during a shared
reading. Dialogic reading is a form of explicit instruction which provides a pathway to grow
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 5
vocabulary skills (Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000). Marulis and Neuman’s (2010) meta-analysis
revealed that students benefited from the vocabulary instruction. Another explicit intervention
that was included in this meta-analysis was the use of computer-based interventions. Through
examination of the variety of these instructional techniques, the researchers were unable to
identify a specific intervention as being the single most effective (Marulis & Neuman, 2010).
Examining the instructional features of these vocabulary interventions, the analysis revealed that
the type of pedagogical approach made a significant difference in effect size. Programs that used
explicit instruction were associated with larger effect sizes (Marulis & Neuman, 2010).
A more recent meta-analysis by Marulis and Neuman (2013) analyzed research studies
that targeted students at-risk for reading difficulties. Snow, Burns, and Griffin (1998) defined
students at-risk for reading difficulties as having characteristics that can be measured by the
community, home, or school that have been associated with poor progress in learning to read.
published and unpublished research studies. The analysis revealed strong support for the
efficacy of vocabulary intervention for at-risk learners prior to learning to read. Students at-risk
for experiencing reading problems may struggle to become efficient readers. Minority students,
students eligible for free or reduced-price lunch, and English language learners are often
considered at-risk students (Logan & Petscher, 2010). One of the most significant findings from
Marulis and Neuman’s (2013) meta-analysis is that the younger the age of the child, the stronger
Vocabulary Instruction
There are various types of vocabulary instruction and few studies have compared their
different instructional methods (Alamri & Rogers, 2018). Two approaches of vocabulary
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 6
instruction are implicit and explicit word learning. Implicit, or incidental, acquired vocabulary
knowledge refers to teaching words through context. Implicit learning occurs during activities
such as conversation or book reading. During implicit learning, a student does not memorize
words but should be given multiple exposures to absorb new words into their vocabulary
(Mirzaii, 2012). Explicit vocabulary learning occurs through direct instruction focused on word
learning by engaging students in visualization activities that provide multiple encounters with
specific words, as well as, promoting a deep level of cognitive processing (Mirzaii, 2012). Due
to the differences in students’ oral language skills, including receptive and expressive
vocabulary, there is a growing need for the inclusion of direct, explicit vocabulary instruction in
the early grades (Cuticelli et al., 2015). As Ellman, Lindo, Morphy, and Compton (2009) stated,
“If we are to impact students’ ability to independently gain knowledge from text, we must better
understand what types of interventions are most effective at increasing students’ ability to
comprehend what they are reading. One promising area of intervention research is vocabulary
instruction” (p. 2). The purpose of this paper is to explore types of explicit vocabulary
instruction and determine if explicit vocabulary instruction achieves long term effects. The
following research question will be addressed: What are efficacious explicit vocabulary
instruction models?
Methods
Data Collection
enhanced word learning. A search was conducted on the following three databases: Academic
Search Complete, Education Research Complete, and ERIC from the year, 2007 to the present.
To perform this search, the following key terms were used in various combinations: vocabulary,
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 7
intervention, explicit, implicit, instruction, word learning, preschool, early, and elementary
years. In addition, ancestry and descendent searches were also completed to find additional
relevant research. Ancestry searches were conducted using Biemiller (2001), Ellman et al.
(2009), and Marulis and Nueman (2013). To complete a descendant search, the database
Education Research Complete was used for the article Spencer, Goldstein, and Kaminski (2012),
as well as, Kelley (2017). During the process of identifying research focused on morphological
intervention, several articles that conducted meta-analyses were identified: Stahl and Fairbanks
(1986), Marulis and Neuman (2010), and Marulis and Neuman (2013).
Studies were included in the literature review if they met these specific criteria: (a) the
use of explicit vocabulary intervention or instruction, (b) students were attending public school
or a preschool, such as Head Start, (c) participants were in preschool through third grade, and (d)
Studies were excluded from the literature review if: (a) they were not empirical
intervention research, (b) they focused on aspects of reading other than vocabulary (spelling,
sight words, fluency), (c) the participants were adults, or (d) late elementary to post-secondary
students.
Coding
Ten experimental studies were coded on a code sheet borrowed from a previous synthesis
(Reed, 2008). Data were collected on participants (e.g. grade, exceptionality) and research
design characters (e.g. intervention and outcomes). The features from the intervention studies,
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 8
Analysis
supplemental instruction. Four of the ten articles selected for the literature review Commented [AWP1]: APA format requires text between
each level of heading/subheading, and Supplemental should
be capped in this level of subheading. I printed the
studied the use of explicit vocabulary through extending the instruction time or adding PurdueOWL headings page to keep handy when I write- it’s
not exactly intuitive!!
supplemental instruction within a broad literacy program. One study (Coyne, McCoach, &
Kapp, 2007) researched the use of extended, embedded instruction. Their research consisted of
two studies within one journal article. Coyne et al. (2007) used an experimental design with two Commented [AWP2]: Try rephrasing and condensing this
section- maybe- “Coyne and colleagues (Coyne, McCoach,
& Kapp, 2007) investigated the use of extended, embedded
independent variables: extended instruction and incidental exposure. Graduate students provided vocabulary instruction through two related studies. They
found…”
the intervention to the 31 participants who were randomly selected to be in group A or B. The
extended-time group received the intervention with target vocabulary words in context of story
reading. The group was also provided interaction opportunities to expand their understanding of
the target words, as well as, provide multiple exposures. The control group was read the same
story but not explicitly taught the target words. Results indicated that the intervention group
Coyne’s et al. (2017) second study explored the differences between extended vocabulary
instruction and embedded instruction added to small group read-aloud time. The study included
32 at-risk students between the ages of five and seven years. The intervention group followed
the same within-subjects design procedures as study one. The comparison group was provided
simple definitions during read aloud. Results indicated that students who received extended
instruction scored significantly higher on word knowledge. Overall, Coyne et al. (2007)
determined that the use of extended instruction resulted in improved word learning. The
multiple exposure to storybooks that use a variety of complex words, also further instruction on
Arthur and Davis (2016) applied a “double-dose” (p. 173) of intervention by extending
vocabulary instruction. The study included 278 participants in preschool through grade 3. The
participants were randomly assigned to three groups (basic and double-dose intervention groups
and a control group). The vocabulary interventions were embedded in a broad-based language
curriculum, Let’s Know! The double-dose condition differed from the basic condition by
including an extra support lesson following each basic vocabulary lesson. The results indicated
that both the Basic and Double-Dose group had significant effects greater than the control group.
Researchers found that vocabulary instruction improved the vocabulary scores with either a
first graders at-risk for future reading difficulties. The 66 participants in this study were selected
from a larger study that used the Read Aloud curriculum. The participants in Puhalla’s (2011)
study were divided into three groups: intervention, no intervention, and peer comparison. The
Read Aloud curriculum used thematic units which included the use of read-alouds with dialogic
talk. The intervention group was given an additional twenty-minute scripted lessons per
thematic unit based on the Bringing Words to Life: Robust Vocabulary Instruction. Results
indicated that all three groups increased their vocabulary knowledge, but the intervention group
with the supplemental instruction, scored higher than the other two groups, indicating that
students who are at-risk for reading difficulties can develop a deeper understanding of complex
vocabulary words.
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 10
Roskos et al. in 2009. Their study examined three groups of preschool students: typical
developing, at-risk for experiencing reading difficulties, and students diagnosed with a disability.
Teachers measured gains using curriculum-based measures. All three groups gained vocabulary
knowledge while using the Doors to Discovery literacy program that is a broad-based literacy
curriculum which includes early literacy skills: phonological awareness, alphabet knowledge,
print knowledge, and vocabulary. Roskos et al. (2009) noted that students increased retention of
the new vocabulary when the words were grouped semantically. Explicit instruction using
semantic grouping and conceptualization was examined in three articles included in the review.
Newman, and Dwyer (2011), Neuman, and Dwyer (2011) and Neuman and Kaefer (2013))
focused on building and retaining vocabulary through the use of categorization and multimedia.
All of the studies focused on the same population: at risk preschoolers that attend Head Start.
Additionally, all of the three studies used the multimedia supplemental program, World of
Words (WOW) to expand vocabulary and concept development, although each approached the
research with a different purpose. Neuman and Dwyer (2011) hypothesized that categorical
development would influence the acquisition and retention of word knowledge. Results
indicated that students were able to semantically categorize new, explicitly taught vocabulary
and provide a rationale for their actions. Neuman et al. (2011) studied the use of the WOW as an
instructional tool for teaching word learning and categorization. Evidence is growing that
indicates there are benefits to implementing word learning through semantic categorization
(Nueman et al, 2011; Booth, 2009; Chi &. Koeske,1983; Glaser, 1984).
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 11
The third study that used the concept of categorization and multimedia, but also focused
on group configuration is Neuman and Kaefer (2013). Their results indicated that for word
learning and categorical and concept development, the treatment group significantly out-
performed the control group and group size did not influence learning.
Pinkham, and Kaefer (2015) and Beck and McKeown (2007). Both groups selected at-risk
preschoolers for the participants in their studies. Neuman et al. (2015) measured teacher’s
teacher’s child-directed language. Results of the study showed strong support for improvements
in the quality of teacher’s language. Beck and McKeown’s (2007) research used Rich
Instruction to support student’s vocabulary growth and Text Talk for read-alouds. Overall, the
results from both studies revealed increases in the student’s vocabulary when rich, well-planned
long term effects. Three of the 10 articles examined lasting effects by using a delayed
post-test. Coyne et al. (2007), Neuman et al. (2011), and Damhuis et al. (2014) all addressed the
long-term effects of explicit vocabulary instruction. Coyne et al. (2007) studied the difference
between extended versus embedded instruction. They concluded that with direct, explicit
instruction, students are able to maintain their learned vocabulary. Neuman et al. (2011)
followed their participants with a six-month delayed post-test to identify differences. Students in
the control group maintained their word knowledge and the ability to identify categories, but
these students were not able to retain their conceptual development advantage. Damhuis et al.
(2014) investigated the difference between long term effects on implicit versus explicit word
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 12
learning. No significant differences in lasting effect were noted between the two intervention
required to generalize and work toward closing the achievement gap. These three vocabulary
studies determined that lasting effects are possible but may be more difficult for more complex
words. Both implicit and explicit word-learning provided access to new vocabulary words, as
Ten articles met the criteria for inclusion in this literature review. The articles were
published between 2007 and 2016 in the following journals: Learning Disability Quarterly,
Special Education. Across the studies, there were 1,406 students ranging from preschool to third
grade that had been identified by their schools as at-risk for experiencing reading difficulties or
having a language impairment. All of the studies involved the participants receiving explicit
vocabulary instruction to increase word knowledge. However, the studies used a variety of
explicit instructional techniques as the independent variable, including the use of categorization,
extended intervention, educator support, and defining words. Overall, the studies suggest that a
variety of approaches using explicit vocabulary instruction should benefit students, especially,
Validity
Internal and external validity were calculated by using a system interpreted by Troia
(1999). The appendix displays the validity criteria divided into four sections: Internal validity
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 13
including general design characteristics and measurement; external validity, which included a
the study, such as operationalized measures and treatment fidelity, and effect sizes.
Of the ten studies, 5 used random selection experimental design (Coyne et al., 2007;
Damhuis et al., 2016; Neuman & Kaefer, 2013; Neuman et al., 2015; Puhalla, 2011.) Arthur and
Davis (2016) and Beck and McKeown (2007) both used a quasi-experimental design. Finally,
Neuman et al. (2011) used a cluster randomization design. Three of the 10 designs used multiple
experimental groups without a control group (Beck & McKeown ,2007; Roskos et al., 2009;
Neuman & Kaefer, 2013.) Coyne et al., (2007) and Arthur and Davis (2016) both used an
experimental and control group. All studies explicitly described their treatment conditions.
Each of the ten selected studies were examined to identify statistical differences and
effect sizes. Six of the 10 studies reported effect sizes using Cohen’s d (Beck & McKeown,
2007; Damhuis, et al., 2014; Neuman et al., 2011; Neuman and Kaefer, 2013; Neuman et al.,
2015)). Beck and McKeown (2007) reported for the pre and post-test, verbal task (d=2.09), and
the picture task (d = 2.71). Damhuis et al. (2014) examined the differences between explicit and
implicit instruction (d = 0.42) Additionally, Neuman et al. (2015) analyzed differences between
a treatment and control group (d =.69). Coyne et al. (2007) reported effect sizes for expressive
definitions (d = .32), receptive definitions (d = .11), and context (d = .12). Effect sizes were
reported for three other studies (Roskos et al., 2009; Puhalla, 2011; Neuman and Dwyer, 2011).
Overall, the effect sizes reported were between 0.11 through 2.71.
Results
The following research question guided the literature review: What are efficacious
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 14
explicit vocabulary instruction models? To address the research question, the results of each
study must be considered. Neuman et al. (2011), Neuman and Dwyer (2011), and Neuman and
Kaefer (2013) addressed word learning through the use of conceptual development. Neuman et
al. (2011) identified that preschool students at-risk for reading difficulties were able to learn
curriculum-related vocabulary, but also were able to identify concepts and categories. Using
categorical concepts may be an effective way to learn and retain new words. Coyne et al. (2007)
focused on extended instruction with a tri-level approach found that using this vocabulary
approach is a “promising” (p. 87) way to increase vocabulary knowledge. Roskos et al (2009)
and Arthur and Davis (2016) studied supplemental and additional vocabulary instruction and
found that vocabulary scores increased with post-testing. Beck and McKeown (2007) and
Neuman et al (2015) focused on complex word learning through teacher support addressing
teacher-student oral communication and focused instruction. They found that learning the
meaning of words is time consuming and not efficient, therefore, not all words need extensive
and elaborate instruction. Puhalla (2011) and Neuman and Kaefer (2013) conducted their
research using whole class vocabulary instruction embedded into a literacy curriculum and
determined that the students in the whole class intervention setting are able to increase
vocabulary knowledge. Neuman and Kaefer (2013) stated, “the configuration of the group did
not affect word learning” (p.600). Overall, this research question identified that the use of
categories, teacher support, extended time, and tri-level approach are effective explicit word
learning strategies and can be completed with whole class instruction with lasting effects
Limitations
This synthesis was designed to expand the research of explicit vocabulary instruction
conducted by Marulis and Neuman (2010). They completed a meta-analysis studying the effects
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 15
focused on a variety of explicit vocabulary interventions including the use of various programs,
group configuration, and teacher-child language with students in preschool through third grade.
Although many types of explicit vocabulary were examined, one limitation of this research study
is the few articles examined. Following the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 10 articles were
identified that specifically addressed explicit vocabulary instruction this could suggest a
Additionally, even though the focus of this article is explicit instruction, it would be
advantageous to have several comparison articles between implicit and explicit vocabulary or the
use of a combination of both implicit and explicit. Implicit instruction is well researched, and it
is well known that reading books to students improves vocabulary knowledge (Damhuis et al.,
2014). Several of the articles reviewed in this study suggested the advantage of both implicit and
and implicit to be more effective than implicit instruction alone. It is of interest to me to explore
in the future the effects of the combination of the two types of intervention.
Future Research
Of the 10 articles examined, several noted the need for further research to explore
reviewed also had time constraints and therefore a longitudinal study would be beneficial to
determine the long-term, broad effects of vocabulary instruction. Future research could also
include qualitative or mixed methods approach. Qualitative research provides the information
from the teachers regarding how the intervention worked. Through this literature review an
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 16
interest of mine was piqued to complete a synthesis that focuses on a combination of explicit and
Table 1
2. Beck & McKeown treatment/ pre- S1-98 at-risk K,1 9 weeks teachers word learning
(2007) posttest comparison (5 x week)
S1 between-subject design (multiple treatments) S2-76 at-risk
quasi-experimental
S2 within-subject
comparison
quasi-experimental
3. Coyne, McCoach, & treatment/ pre- S1-32 at-risk Preschool (3 x week, graduate students word learning
Kapp (2007) posttest comparison S2-32 at-risk 30 min) generalization
experimental within (multiple treatments)
subject
4. Damhuis, Segers & treatment/ pre- 48 K 2 weeks researcher word learning
Verhoeven (2014) posttest comparison
(multiple treatments)
experimental multiple groups
5. Neuman & Dwyer treatment/ pre- 178 at-risk preschool 16 weeks teachers, expressive
(2011) posttest comparison (5x week12 researcher, language,
(multiple treatments) min) graduate word labels,
experimental multiple groups assistants word properties
sorting
6. Neuman, Newman, treatment/ pre- 604 at-risk preschool one school teachers word knowledge
& Dwyer (2011) posttest comparison year conceptual/
(multiple treatments) categorical
cluster randomization inferences/
generalizations
7. Neuman & Kaefer treatment/ pre- 108 at-risk preschool 2 weeks teachers word learning
(2013 posttest comparison (4x week,
10-12 min) concept
experimental, within development
subject design
8. Neuman, Pinkham treatment/ pre- 143 at-risk preschool 31 sessions teachers word learning
& Kaefer (2015) posttest comparison (2x 45 min.)
multiple groups concept
experimental development
experimental
Table 2
Coyne, McCoach, and Kapp (2007) Researcher designed measures: story word No significant difference between post-test
S1 Experimental group definitions (receptive, expressive, context), and delayed posttest.
Extended instruction PPVT ES=NR
S2
Extended instruction
Embedded instruction
Damhuis, Segers, and Verhoeven (2014) PPVT Explicit vs. Implicit (d = 0.42)
definitions
Neuman and Dwyer (2011) WOW expressive vocabulary test, PPVT, Expressive language WOW ES= 0.64
World of Words (WOW) Early Language and Literacy Observation Word Labels ES= 0.16
(ELLCO) Word Properties ES= 0.84
Sorting taught ES=1.19, not taught ES=.99
Neuman and Kaefer (2013) PPVT-III, 2 researcher created measures for Significant gains in word knowledge (d=
WOW following directions and identify categories 1.23), concepts (d= 1.08), categories
and properties of target words (d=1.05)
Neuman , Newman, and Dwyer (2011) Woodcock-Johnson Picture Vocabulary Word Knowledge (d=0.44)
WOW Subtest (Form A and B) Conceptual and Categorical (d=0.63)
Inferences and Generalizations (d=0.46)
Neuman, Pinkham, and Kaefer (2015) PPVT Treatment and Control Group (d=.69)
WOW Researcher designed Teacher Talk measure
teacher talk support
Puhalla (2011) DIBELS subtests: PSF (Phonemic Post-test ES= .28
Bringing Words to Life Segmentation Fluency), NWF (Nonsense
word fluency
Roskos, Ergul, Bryan, Burstein, Christie, and PPVT-III Receptive vocabulary growth ES= 0.46
Han Significant improvements in receptive and
Doors to Discovery expressive growth
References
Alamri, K. & Rogers, V. (2018). The effectiveness of different explicit vocabulary teaching
*Arthur, A. M. & Davis, D. L. (2016). A pilot study of the impact of double-dose robust
vocabulary repertoires through rich and focused instruction. The Elementary School
Biemiller, A. (2001) Teaching vocabulary, early, direct, and sequential. American Educator, 1-
2001/teaching-vocabulary
Booth, A. E. (2009). Causal supports for early word learning. Child Development, 80(4), 1243-
*Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., & Kapp, S. (2007). Vocabulary intervention for kindergarten
Cuticelli, M., Coyne, M. D., Ware, S. M., Oldham, A., & Rattan, S. L. (2015) Improving
*Damhuis, C. M., Segers, E. and Verhoeven, L. (2014). Sustainability of breadth and depth of
10.10880/1034912X.2014.932562
Edmonds, M. S., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J. Reutebuch, C., Cable, A., Tackett, K. K., &
Elleman, A. M., Lindo, E. J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D. L. (2009). The impact of vocabulary
10.1080/19345740802539200
Hargrave, A. C., Sénéchal, M. (2000) A book reading intervention with preschool children who
have limited vocabularies: The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early
Glaser, R. (1984). Education and thinking: The role of knowledge. American Psychologist,
Izura, C., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Age of acquisition effects in word recognition and production
https://www.uv.es/psicologica/paraARCHIVES/2002.html
Kamil, M. (2004). Vocabulary and comprehension instructions: Summary and implications from
the National Reading Panel findings. In p. McCardle & Chhabra, V. (Eds.), The Voice of
10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0074
Logan, J. A. & Petscher, Y. (2010). School profiles of at-risk student concentration: Differential
growth in oral reading fluency. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 163-186. doi:
10.1016/j.jsp.2009.12.002
Marulis, L., M., & Neuman, S. B. (2010). The effects of vocabulary intervention on young
335. doi:10.3102/0034654310377087
Marulis, L. M. & Neuman, S. B. (2013) How vocabulary interventions affect young children at
Mirzaii, M. (2012). Implicit vs. explicit vocabulary learning: Which approach serves long-term
recall better? Southeast Asian Journal of English Language Studies, 18(2), 1-12.
Retrieved from
http://mutex.gmu.edu/login?url+http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=
ehh&AN=94988611&site=ehost-live
Neuman, S. B., (2011). Teaching Vocabulary in Early Education. In Neuman, S. B. & Dickinson,
D.K(Eds.), Handbook of Early Literacy Research (pp. 358-372) New York: The Guilford
Press.
*Neuman, S. B., & Dwyer, J. (2011). Developing vocabulary and conceptual knowledge for
Neuman, S. B., Dwyer, J., Koh, S., &Wright, T. (2007). The World of Words: A vocabulary
intervention for preschoolers. Ann Arbor, MI: Read to Learn Research Program.
*Neuman, S. B., & Kaefer, T. (2013) Enhancing the intensity of vocabulary instruction for
10.1086/669937
*Neuman, S. B., Pinkham, A., & Kaefer, T., (2015). Supporting vocabulary teaching and
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P.
supplemental booster instruction. Remedial and Special Education, 32(6), 471-481. doi:
10.1177/0741932510362495
Reed, D. K. (2008). A synthesis of morphology intervention and effects on reading outcomes for
students in grades K-12. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23(1), 36-49. doi:
10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00261
*Roskos, K., Ergul, C., Bryan, T., Burstein, K., Christie, J., Han, M. (2009). Who’s learning
what words and how fast? Preschoolers’ vocabulary growth in an early literacy program.
Snow, C., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children.
Spencer, E. J., Goldstein, H., & Kaminski, R. (2012). Teaching vocabulary in storybooks:
56001072
Troia, G.A. (1999). Phonological awareness intervention research: A critical review of the
10.1598/RRQ.34.1.3
Verhoeven, L. & van Leeuwe, J. & Vermeer A. (2011). Vocabulary growth and reading
development across the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 8-25. doi:
10.1080/10888438.2011.536125
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 24
Appendix
Internal and External Validity/Evaluating the Evidence/synthesis project
ARTICLE:
YEAR:
SOURCE/FUNDING:
Summary:
Questions:
EXPLICIT VOCABULARY INSTRUCTION 25
Stacie, you’ve researched and developed a strong synthesis on vocabulary instruction with young
children. The synthesis discussion clearly denotes the effect sizes, designs, and features of the
compiled group of studies. I’d like to see more thematic discussion in the early part of your paper
rather than individual study summaries. As you gain more practice, I think that will become
easier. Overall, your writing is very clear and easy to follow. You have a good sense of academic
style and explain your points succinctly. There are minor APA issues to attend to before your
next paper submission- particularly headings- but they don’t interfere with readability. Your
paper and class presentation reflect your deep understanding of your topic and your ability to
read and understand research. It’s not easy, and you’ve mastered it!
I do think that you can develop this into a publishable article, particularly by taking a more
teacher-friendly approach to “here’s what we know about vocab instruction and how you can
effectively use it in your classroom”. I’d be happy to help if you want to tackle it.
Grade: A