A Brief History of Moral Education: The Colonial Period. As Common School Spread Throughout The
A Brief History of Moral Education: The Colonial Period. As Common School Spread Throughout The
html
In the nineteenth century, teachers were hired and trained with the clear
expectation that they would advance the moral mission of the school
and attend to character formation. Literature, biography, and history
were taught with the explicit intention of infusing children with high
moral standards and good examples to guide their lives. Students'
copybook headings offered morally uplifting thoughts: "Quarrelsome
persons are always dangerous companions" and "Praise follows
exertion." The most successful textbooks during the nineteenth and
early twentieth centuries were the famed McGuffey readers, which were
filled with moral stories, urgings, and lessons. During this period of our
evolution as a nation, moral education was deep in the very fabric of our
schools.
There was, however, something else in the fabric of moral education that
caused it to become problematic: religion. In the United States, as a
group of colonies and later as a new nation, the overwhelming dominant
religion was Protestantism. While not as prominent as during the
Puritan era, the King James Bible was, nevertheless, a staple of U.S.
public schools. The root of the moral code was seen as residing there.
However, as waves of immigrants from Ireland, Germany, and Italy
came to the country from the mid-nineteenth century forward, the pan-
Protestant tone and orthodoxy of the schools came under scrutiny and a
reaction set in. Concerned that their children would be weaned from
their faith, Catholics developed their own school system. Later in the
twentieth century, other religious groups, such as Jews, Muslims, and
even various Protestant denominations, formed their own schools. Each
group desired, and continues to desire, that its moral education be
rooted in its respective faith or code.
Twentieth century. During this same late-nineteenth-century and
twentieth-century period, there was also a growing reaction against
organized religion and the belief in a spiritual dimension of human
existence. Intellectual leaders and writers were deeply influenced by the
ideas of the English naturalist Charles Darwin, the German political
philosopher Karl Marx, the Austrian neurologist and founder of
psychoanalysis Sigmund Freud, and the German philosopher and poet
Friedrich Nietzsche, and by a growing strict interpretation of the
separation of church and state doctrine. This trend increased after
World War II and was further intensified by what appeared to be the
large cracks in the nation's moral consensus in the late 1960s. Since for
so many Americans the strongest roots of moral truths reside in their
religious beliefs, educators and others became wary of using the schools
for moral education. More and more this was seen to be the province of
the family and the church. Some educators became proponents of
"value-free" schooling, ignoring the fact that it is impossible to create a
school devoid of ethical issues, lessons, and controversies.
Ads by Google
Tadika Intisari
Established since 1981
Best & proven pre-school education
www.intisari.edu.my
Pre-Schools in Malaysia
List of Pre-Schools in Malaysia,
Find Addresses, Phones, Maps
www.701panduan.com
A second issue relates to the level of schools and the age of students. The
revival of character education in our schools has been evident to a much
greater degree in elementary schools. Here schools can concentrate on
the moral basics for which there is wide public consensus. The same is
true, but to a somewhat lesser degree, for middle and junior high
schools. And although there are many positive examples of secondary
schools that have implemented broad and effective character education
programs, secondary school faculties are hesitant to embrace character
education. Part of it is the departmental structures and the time
demands of the curriculum; part of it is the age and sophistication of
their students; and part of it is that few secondary school teachers
believe they have a clear mandate to deal with issues of morality and
character.
Piaget's Theory
Jean Piaget is among the first psychologists whose work
remains directly relevant to contemporary theories of moral
development. In his early writing, he focused specifically on
the moral lives of children, studying the way children play
games in order to learn more about children's beliefs about
right and wrong(1932/65). According to Piaget, all
development emerges from action; that is to say, individuals
construct and reconstruct their knowledge of the world as a
result of interactions with the environment. Based on his
observations of children's application of rules when playing,
Piaget determined that morality, too, can be considered a developmental
process. For example, Ben, a ten year old studied by Piaget, provided the
following critique of a rule made-up by a child playing marbles: "it isn't a rule!
It's a wrong rule because it's outside of the rules. A fair rule is one that is in the
game". Ben believed in the absolute and intrinsic truth of the rules,
characteristic of early moral reasoning. In contrast, Vua, aged thirteen,
illustrates an understanding of the reasoning behind the application of rules,
characteristic of later moral thinking. When asked to consider the fairness of a
made-up rule compared to a traditional rule, Vua replied "It is just as fair
because the marbles are far apart"(making the game equally difficult).
This heteronomy results from two factors. The first factor is the young child's
cognitive structure. According to Piaget, the thinking of young children is
characterized by egocentrism. That is to say that young children are unable to
simultaneously take into account their own view of things with the perspective
of someone else. This egocentrism leads children to project their own thoughts
and wishes onto others. It is also associated with the uni-directional view of
rules and power associated with heteronomous moral thought, and various
forms of "moral realism." Moral realism is associated with "objective
responsibility", which is valuing the letter of the law above the purpose of the
law. This is why young children are more concerned about the outcomes of
actions rather than the intentions of the person doing the act. Moral realism is
also associated with the young child's belief in "immanent justice." This is the
expectation that punishments automatically follow acts of wrong-doing. One of
the most famous cases of such childhood thinking was that of the young boy
who believed that his hitting a power pole with his baseball bat caused a major
power blackout in the New York city area.
However, through interactions with other children in which the group seeks a to
play together in a way all find fair, children find this strict heteronomous
adherence to rules sometimes problematic. As children consider these
situations, they develop towards an "autonomous" stage of moral reasoning,
characterized by the ability to consider rules critically, and selectively apply
these rules based on a goal of mutual respect and cooperation. The ability to act
from a sense of reciprocity and mutual respect is associated with a shift in the
child's cognitive structure from egocentrism to perspective taking. Coordinating
one's own perspective with that of others means that what is right needs to be
based on solutions that meet the requirements of fair reciprocity. Thus, Piaget
viewed moral development as the result of interpersonal interactions through
which individuals work out resolutions which all deem fair. Paradoxically, this
autonomous view of morality as fairness is more compelling and leads to more
consistent behavior than the heteronomous orientation held by younger
children.
Piaget concluded from this work that schools should emphasize cooperative
decision-making and problem solving, nurturing moral development by
requiring students to work out common rules based on fairness. This is a direct
rejection of sociologists Emile Durkheim's view of proper moral education
(1925/1961). Durkheim, similar to Piaget, believed that morality resulted from
social interaction or immersion in a group. However, Durkheim believed moral
development was a natural result of attachment to the group, an attachment
which manifests itself in a respect for the symbols, rules, and authority of that
group. Piaget rejected this belief that children simply learn and internalize the
norms for a group; he believed individuals define morality individually through
their struggles to arrive at fair solutions. Given this view, Piaget suggested that
a classroom teacher perform a difficult task: the educator must provide students
with opportunities for personal discovery through problem solving, rather than
indoctrinating students with norms.
On the basis of his research, Kohlberg identified six stages of moral reasoning
grouped into three major levels. Each level represented a fundamental shift in
the social-moral perspective of the individual. At the first level, the
preconventional level, a person's moral judgments are characterized by a
concrete, individual perspective. Within this level, a Stage 1 heteronomous
orientation focuses on avoiding breaking rules that are backed by punishment,
obedience for its own sake and avoiding the physical consequences of an action
to persons and property. As in Piaget's framework, the reasoning of Stage 1 is
characterized by ego-centrism and the inability to consider the perspectives of
others. At Stage 2 there is the early emergence of moral reciprocity. The Stage
2 orientation focuses on the instrumental, pragmatic value of an action.
Reciprocity is of the form, "you scratch my back and I'll scratch yours." The
Golden Rule becomes, "If someone hits you, you hit them back." At Stage 2
one follows the rules only when it is to someone's immediate interests. What is
right is what's fair in the sense of an equal exchange, a deal, an agreement. At
Stage 2 there is an understanding that everybody has his(her) own interest to
pursue and these conflict, so that right is relative (in the concrete individualist
sense).
Moral Education
Kohlberg rejected the focus on values and virtues, not only due to the lack of
consensus on what virtues are to be taught, but also because of the complex
nature of practicing such virtues. For example, people often make different
decisions yet hold the same basic moral values. Kohlberg believed a better
approach to affecting moral behavior should focus on stages of moral
development. These stages are critical, as they consider the way a person
organizes their understanding of virtues, rules, and norms, and integrates these
into a moral choice (Power, Higgins, & Kohlberg, 1989). In addition, he
rejected the relativist view point in favor of the view that certain principles of
justice and fairness represent the pinnacle of moral maturity, as he found that
these basic moral principles are found in different cultures and subcultures
around the world (Kohlberg & Turiel, 1971).
The most common tool for doing this was to present a "moral dilemma"
(see Classroom Practices) and require students to determine and justify what
course the actor in the dilemma should take. Through discussion, students
should then be forced to face the contradictions present in any course of action
not based on principles of justice or fairness.
While Kohlberg appreciated the importance and value of such moral dilemma
discussions, he held from very early on that moral education required more
than individual reflection, but also needed to include experiences for students to
operate as moral agents within a community. In this regard, Kohlberg
reconciled some of the differences in orientation that existed between the
theories of moral growth held by Piaget and Durkheim. In order to provide
students with an optimal context within which to grow morally, Kohlberg and
his colleagues developed the "just community" schools approach towards
promoting moral development (Power, Higgins, & Kohlberg, 1989). The basic
premise of these schools is to enhance students' moral development by offering
them the chance to participate in a democratic community. Here, democracy
refers to more than simply casting a vote. It entails full participation of
community members in arriving at consensual rather than "majority rules"
decision-making. One primary feature of these schools is their relatively small
size (often they are actually schools within schools), aimed at providing the
students with a sense of belonging to a group which is responsive to individual
needs. The central institution of these schools is a community meeting in which
issues related to life and discipline in the schools are discussed and
democratically decided, with an equal value placed on the voices of students
and teachers. An underlying goal of these meetings is to establish collective
norms which express fairness for all members of the community. It is believed
that by placing the responsibility of determining and enforcing rules on
students, they will take prosocial behavior more seriously. At the same time,
this approach stems from the cognitive-developmentalist view that discussion
of moral dilemmas can stimulate moral development.
However, this is not to say that just community school simply leaves students
to their own devices; teachers play a crucial leadership role in these
discussions, promoting rules and norms which have a concern for justice and
community, and ultimately enforcing the rules. This role is not an easy one, as
teachers must listen closely and understand a student's reasoning, in order to
help the student to the next level of reasoning. This requires a delicate balance
between letting the students make decisions, and advocating in a way which
shows them the limits in their reasoning. A primary advantage to the Just
Community approach is its effectiveness in affecting students actions, not just
their reasoning. Students are, in effect, expected to "practice what they preach",
by following the rules determined in community meetings.
These hypothesized distinctions have been sustained through studies over the
past 20 years. These studies have included interviews with children,
adolescents and adults; observations of child-child and adult-child social
interactions; cross-cultural studies; and longitudinal studies examining the
changes in children's thinking as they grow older. An example of the distinction
between morality and convention is given in the following excerpt from an
interview with a four-year-old girl regarding her perceptions of spontaneously
occurring transgressions at her preschool.
MORAL ISSUE: Did you see what happened? Yes. They were playing and
John hit him too hard. Is that something you are supposed to do or not supposed
to do? Not so hard to hurt. Is there a rule about that? Yes. What is the rule?
You're not to hit hard. What if there were no rule about hitting hard, would it be
all right to do then? No. Why not? Because he could get hurt and start to cry.
CONVENTIONAL ISSUE: Did you see what just happened? Yes. They were
noisy. Is that something you are supposed to or not supposed to do? Not do. Is
there a rule about that? Yes. We have to be quiet. What if there were no rule,
would it be all right to do then? Yes. Why? Because there is no rule.
Current work from within domain theory has sought to explore how the child's
concepts of moral and conventional regulation relate to their developing
understandings of personal prerogative and privacy. This work is exploring
how children develop their concepts of autonomy and its relation to social
authority. This has led to a fruitful series of studies of adolescent-parent
conflict with important implications for ways in which parents may contribute
to the healthy development of youth (Smetana, 1996). This work is also being
extended into studies of how adolescents perceive the authority of teachers and
school rules. Moral and Social Values Education The implications of domain
theory for values education are several. First, the identification of a domain of
moral cognition that is tied to the inherent features of human social interaction
means that moral education may be grounded in universal concerns for fairness
and human welfare, and is not limited to the particular conventions or norms of
a given community or school district. By focusing on those universal features
of human moral understanding, public schools may engage in fostering
children's morality without being accused of promoting a particular religion,
and without undercutting the basic moral core of all major religious systems.
Second, educational research from within domain theory has resulted in a set of
recommendations for what is termed "domain appropriate" values education.
This approach entails the teacher's analysis and identification of the moral or
conventional nature of social values issues to be employed in values lessons.
Such an analysis contributes to the likelihood that the issues discussed are
concordant with the domain of the values dimension they are intended to affect.
A discussion of dress codes, for example, would constitute a poor basis for
moral discussion, since mode of dress is primarily a matter of convention.
Likewise, consideration of whether it is right to steal to help a person in need,
would be a poor issue with which to generate a lesson intended to foster
students' understandings of social conventions. A related function of the teacher
would be to focus student activity (verbal or written) on the underlying features
concordant with the domain of the issue. Thus, students dealing with a moral
issue would be directed to focus on the underlying justice or human welfare
considerations of the episode. With respect to conventions, the focus of student
activity would be on the role of social expectations and the social
organizational functions of such social norms.
On the basis of this kind of analysis teachers are also better enabled to lead
students through consideration of more complex issues which contain elements
from more than one domain. By being aware of the developmental changes that
occur in students' comprehension of the role of social convention, and related
changes in students understanding of what it means to be fair or considerate of
the welfare of others, teachers are able to frame consideration of complex
social issues in ways that will maximize the ability of students to comprehend
and act upon the moral and social meaning of particular courses of action.
The best sources for discussion of domain appropriate education, along with
guidelines and examples for how teachers may select materials from existing
school curricula from which to generate lessons and practices which will foster
students' development within both the moral and conventional domains may be
found in: Nucci, L. & Weber, E. (1991) "The domain approach to values
education: From theory to practice" In W. Kurtines & J. Gewirtz (Eds.)
"Handbook of Moral Behavior and Development (Volume 3: Applications)pp.
251 - 266). and also in: Nucci, L. (1989) "Challenging Conventional Wisdom
About Morality: The Domain Approach to Values Education." In L. Nucci
(Ed.) "Moral Development and Character Education: A Dialogue" Berkeley:
McCutchan.
Conclusion
This segment was designed to provide an overview of the major developmental
theories currently influencing research on moral education. To remain current
with new information and approaches in this very active field you should check
this web site on a regular basis for Featured Articles. To learn about available
books in the field check Books of Interest. For persons wanting to learn more
about traditional character education please checkRelated Web Sites.