Imagining Cog Life Things
Imagining Cog Life Things
Talk Talk
World
Figure 2: A square-cut gem of interaction Figure 3: Pilots jointly imagine the future
Now, instead of adding new elements, we are only adding The system of body/world coordination is rich. In addition
new relations. The new relations hold between body and to the pilot on the left locating and opening to the relevant
body, between talk and talk and between the body of one page in the route manual, the two pilots are engaged in
and the talk of another. The many cognitive properties of mutual recipient design in talk and in gesture. Each is
systems of socially distributed cognition also enter at this shaping his own verbal and non-verbal behavior to suit the
level. needs of the other. Relations of talk to the world
demonstrate constraints in both directions. The spatial
There are six kinds of edges in the square-cut gem of organization of a thing (airport description page) is being
interaction. The edges are loci of coordination and each is used to create sequential temporal structure in the activity
characterized by a distinct set of coordination types. In by adopting a spatial “flow” of attention across the page.
addition to edges, there are facets on the gem, which Reciprocally, talk constrains the engagement of the thing by
correspond to more complex systems of multimodal making deictic reference to the items on the page. Talk and
body are coordinated as co-speech gesture is used to the thumb to the segments. It is clear that the motor
highlight items referred to and to guide own and other’s memory for the touch pattern is learned before the visual
attention. Reciprocally, co-gesture speech provides clues on representation of the array of month names that can be
how to identify the meaningful segments of a complex fluid produced by running the touch pattern in coordination with
gestural movement. Both pilots use their body orientations the standard list of month names. In this case, the body
and postures (construed within the constraints of the work plays a key role in the construction of a complex material
stations) to demonstrate shared attention and interest. The anchor. The hand is opportunistically appropriated as a
relations among the two speaking roles are complexly “thing” possessing a set of regions and a learned motor
organized by social and professional roles and an routine controls the assignment of conceptual content to the
intersubjectively shared understanding of the nature of the regions. In interaction with things, embodied processes can
task. Using these means and others, the pilots jointly become “somatic” anchors for conceptual blends; they are
imagine the future of their airplane from their current internal proxies for abstract relations among the things with
location, 150 miles from their destination, all the way to the which one interacts. I will return to this point in an
arrival gate. The human interaction system is composed of extended example below.
a very complex network of coordination among myriad
dynamic behavioral patterns. I propose to use this vision of Let us return now to the rich systems of interaction that are
interaction to further explore the cognitive life of things. visible in video recordings. An emerging field of inquiry
can be seen in the work of a number of researchers who
The power of embodied interaction with culturally focus on the details of the creation and manipulation of
constructed settings. material representations. In domains as varied as scientific
research (Goodwin, 1994; Alač, 2003), architectural practice
The power of cognitive artifacts to transform human thought (Murphy, 2004), student learning (Singer and Goldin-
is widely recognized. But of what importance are the Meadow, 2005, Alač and Hutchins, 2005), airline piloting
details of the bodily interaction with a cognitive artifact in (Hutchins and Palen, 1997), telecommunications
producing its cognitive power? The study of cognitive troubleshooting (Hindmarsh and Heath, 2000), and surgery
artifacts has not attended closely to the nature of the on- (Goodwin, n.d.), researchers are showing how interactions
going activities in which people think in interactions with between the body and cultural artifacts constitute an
things. This important topic has dropped into the interstices important form of thinking. These interactions are not taken
between academic disciplines. Of course, ergonomics as “indications” of invisible mental processes, rather they
considers the fit between the properties of bodies and the are taken as the thinking processes themselves. In this brief
manipulation of tools. But, we do not yet have a tradition of paper I will try to add to this project. I will illustrate how
thought that appreciates the profoundly situated character of processes of imagination, inference, error detection, and
embodied thinking in complex culturally organized settings. even “Aha!” insight can arise in cultural practices that bring
the body into interaction with the cultural environment in
In my work on material anchors for conceptual blends particular ways.
(Hutchins, 2005), I tried to show how in some kinds of
activities, physical relations become proxies for conceptual Using the body to imagine the dynamics of things
relations. In the linguistic phenomenon known as fictive
The notion of somatic anchors for conceptual blends adds a
motion, a conceptual relationship is mapped onto a real or
new dimension to the now familiar phenomena of
imagined space to produce a trajectory to guide the
environmentally-coupled gestures (Goodwin, Hutchins and
allocation of attention. In the well-known memory
Palen, Becvar, Alac many more). Let me give just two brief
technique called the “method of loci,” and in the techniques examples here in which scientists engage static graphical
that Kirsh (1995) groups under the heading “the intelligent
representations with their moving bodies in order to imagine
use of space,” an imagined trajectory is mapped onto real
the complex dynamics of entities that cannot be observed
things in space, thus producing sequential relations among
directly. The first example comes from the work of my
the things. My analysis of material anchors for conceptual student, Amaya Becvar (Becvar, Hollan, Hutchins (in press
blends was, however, like most other work on cognitive
a, and in press b)). Figure 4 shows the principal investigator
artifacts, disembodied.
of a molecular biology laboratory. The problem at hand is
to imagine how the shape of the thrombin molecule changes
The body did come into play at one point in a way that I can
when another molecule, thrombomodulin, attaches to it. She
now see as suggestive. Some Japanese school kids learn a
has just aligned her left hand with a ribbon diagram of a
technique for mapping the names of months and days of the
thrombin molecule that is depicted on an overhead
week onto their left hand in a way that allows the hand to be
transparency lying on the platen of an overhead projector.
used as a date calculator1. The mapping of names of
She lifts her hand from the transparency as if, by virtue of
months onto the segments of the first three fingers of the left
touching her fingers to the loops in the ribbon diagram, her
hand is learned as a sequential pattern of touching the tip of
hand has become the molecule. She then says, “Our theory
1 is that the molecule moves either like this or like this.”
See Nakahara (1996) for details.
Coincident with each of the two deictic utterances of “this” as-yet-unbuilt building. He gives these gestures the label,
she produces a motion with her hand. In one the finger tips “action in the subjunctive mood.” One important insight
come together like the mouth of a purse. In the other, the here is that these representations are tentative in a way that
fingers rotate around an imaginary axis emerging from the the lines that appear on the building drawings are not. They
palm of the hand. In this way, she imagines two dynamic suggest that representations can have different degrees of
modes for the loops of the thrombin molecule. commitment to the entities that are represented. Some
representations make strong commitments, while others are
more tentative. Some are tangible and permanent (lines in
ink on a building drawing) while others are less permanent
(pencil markings), while still others are ephemeral
(gestures). I will argue below that understanding the
cognitive life of things requires that we understand the full
range of commitment of our embodied engagements with
things.
2
A more detailed discussion of this system appears in Hutchins (in
press).
bearing. Rather, they were then required to compute the true picked up the hoey and moved the arm in the
bearing by adding the corrected magnetic ship’s head to the direction of the 120 degree scale position. It is not
relative bearing of the landmark. The correct equation is: possible to establish exactly where the hoey arm
true bearing equals compass heading plus deviation plus was on the scale when he stopped moving it, nor
magnetic variation, plus the relative bearing of the can one determine whether or which scale values
landmark. (TB = C + D + V + RB). This perturbation he had read at that point. It is clear that he was
disrupted the ability of the crew to plot accurate positions moving the hoey arm across the scale and attending
for the ship. The crew explored various computational to the scale values.
variations of TB = C +V +RB for 38 lines of position. Then 3. While moving the hoey arm toward alignment with
they discovered that a key term, deviation, was missing the scale at the120 degree mark, the plotter was
from their computations. After reconfiguring their work to also imagining the location with respect to the fix
include the deviation term, the team gradually regained the triangle of the LOP that was plotted using 120
functional ability to plot accurate positions. degrees. With his body he imagines a slight
clockwise rotation of the LOP that would make the
The discovery of a missing term triangle smaller. Several representations are
integrated into a single meaning complex at this
How can the discovery that this term was missing be moment. They include the hoey arm and protractor
explained? The discovery appeared as an “Aha!” insight. scale and the imagined clockwise rotation of the
Taken in the context of the computations that the crew was hoey arm on the chart. This combination of
doing, this discovery is, like most creative insights, representations creates the image of a small
mysterious. There is nothing in the pattern of computational clockwise motion of the arm with respect to the
efforts leading up to the discovery that indicates they are hoey base that would decrease the size of the
nearing this development. The processes that underlie the triangle (see figure 6). In this composite image, it
“Aha!” insight remain invisible to a computational can also be seen that a clockwise motion
perspective in part because that perspective represents corresponds to motion across the scale toward
everything in a single mono-modal (or even a-modal) larger numbers (see figure 7). This implies a
system3. A careful examination of the way the body slightly larger numeric value for the bearing to that
engages the tools in the setting, however, helps to solve the landmark.
mystery of how the discovery was made, and why it
happened when it did. The insight was achieved in and
emerged out of the navigator’s bodily engagement with the
setting.
3
In Hutchins (1995), I provide a disembodied analysis of this
event that fails to explain how the discovery of the missing term
was made.
may be a resource for adaptive processes when routine
activity is disrupted.