Reid J. Meloy - The Psychopathic Mind - Origins, Dynamics, and Treatment-Jason Aronson, Inc. (1988) PDF
Reid J. Meloy - The Psychopathic Mind - Origins, Dynamics, and Treatment-Jason Aronson, Inc. (1988) PDF
Reid J. Meloy - The Psychopathic Mind - Origins, Dynamics, and Treatment-Jason Aronson, Inc. (1988) PDF
MIND
ORIGINS,DYNAMICS,AND
TREATMENT
THE PSYCHOPATHIC
MIND
ORIGINS, DYNAMICS, AM)
TREATMENT
PO Box 317
Oxford
OX2 9RU, UK
Meloy, J. Reid.
The psychopathic mind : origins, dynamics, and treatment / J. Reid Meloy.
p. cm.
Bibliography: p.
Includes index.
ISBN 0-87668-3 11-1 (paper) ISBN: 978-0-87668-3 1 1-8
1. Psychology, Pathological. 2. Psychiatry. 3. Psychotherapy. I. Title.
RC454.M37 2002
61 6 . 8 9 4 ~ 2 1 88003454
Printed in the United States of America
@The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of American
National Standard for Information Sciences-Permanence of Paper for Printed Library
Materials, ANSIINISO 239.48-1992.
About the Author
Acknowledgments .................................................................xlu
...
Foreword by James S. Grotetein .........................................xvii
PART I
ORIGINS
Chapter 1. Introduction......................................................3
Historical Antecedents
The Psychoanalysts
Object Relations
PART 111
VIOLENCE, PSYCHOSIS, AND RELATED STATES
Clurpter 6. Modes of Aggression .....................................186
Aggression and Violence
Affective Aggression
Predatory Aggression
Psychopathy and Predatory Violence
PART IV
TREATMENT
Index ......................................................................................461
Acknowledgments
HISTORICAL ANTECEDENTS
Millon (1981) noted the historical vacillation between clinical
understanding and social condemnation of the psychopath. Pine1
(1801)first observed and documented a certain group of patients
who behaved in impulsive and self-destructive ways yet evi-
denced no defects in reasoning ability. He termed these patients'
disorders manie sans delire (insanity without delirium). A de-
cade later, the American physician Benjamin Rush (1812) char-
acterized Pinel's diagnostic group in a morally reprehensible
light and foreshadowed the writings of the British psychiatrist
Prichard (1835), generally credited as the originator of the term
mom1 insanity and the leader of generations that have contam-
inated scientific objectivity with moralizing. Millon (1981) noted
the striking similarity between Lombroso's exposition
(1872-1885) of the psychopath's behavior, Gouster's (1878) list of
symptom clusters, and the contemporary Diagnostic and Statis-
tical Manual of Mental Disorders: third edition, description of
the antisocial personality disorder (American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation 1980).
The German psychiatric community introduced the term
psycbputhic inferiority (Koch 1891) and attempted to define a
physical basis for a group of disorders whose discriminative
validity had become unmanageable because of the wide cluster of
8 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
THE PSYCHOANALYSTS
Freud had little to say about the psychopath, although his com-
ments pointed to the psychopath as an exception to the majority
of criminals: "Among adult criminals we must no doubt except
those who commit crimes without any sense of guilt, who have
either developed no moral inhibitions or who, in their conflict
with society, consider themselves justified in their actions"
(Freud 1916, p. 333).
Freud's thoughtful paper "Some Character Types Met with
in Psychoanalytic Work" (1916) stimulated Alexander to write
several papers spanning a decade that investigated the "robust,
expansive, instinctual life" (Alexander 1936,p. 279) of the psycho-
path. He elaborated both the psychogenic and biogenic origins of
psychopathy, although he preferred to conceptualize his discus-
sion along four rather general levels of psychopathology that he
labeled neurosis, neurotic chamcter, psychosis, and criminulit y
(Alexander 1923,1930,1935). Aichhorn (1925) published his sem-
inal text Wayward Youth during this same period and developed
an understanding of psychopathy that centered upon oedipal
configurations, narcissism, and the failure of early identifica-
tions. Coriat (1927) and Partridge (1927)postulated that infantile,
oral fmations explained the psychopath's behavior.
Wittels (1939, Karpman (1941)' and Levy (1951) developed
simple typologies of psychopathy based upon psychoanalytic
constructs. Wittels (1937) suggested that neurotic psychopaths
feared their bisexuality, whereas the simple psychopath in-
dulged it. Karpman (1941) discriminated between the idiopathic
psychopath (the true guiltless criminal) and the symptomatic
psychopath (the neurotic character described by Alexander
[19353), the latter synonymous with the individual motivated by
unconscious guilt (Freud 1916). Levy (1951) foreshadowed cur-
rent object relations theory and its understanding of narcissistic
pathology in his differentiation of the deprived and indulged
psychopath. Millon (1969) respectively termed these the aggres-
sive and narcissistic psychopathic types.
Cleckley published the first edition of Mask of Sanity in
1941 and psychodynamically attributed psychopathy to a con-
cealed psychosis. He introduced an alternative term, aemantic
dementia, to account for the pathological lying of the psychopath
and to delimit the use of the term p a y c ~ t hbut
~ ,it did not take
hold. Most important, he described sixteen behavioral criteria for
psychopathy, some of which had clear psychodynamic implica-
tions, such as a lack of remorse or shame, poor judgment and
failure to learn from experience, pathologic egocentricity and
incapacity for love, general poverty in mdor affective reactions,
and specific loss of insight. These criteria have been empirically
defined and measured most recently by Hare (1980). Later edi-
10 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
OBJECT RELATIONS
BIOLOGICAL SUBSTRATES
Neuroanatomical Structure
Genetic Predispositions
Neumchemical Set
Table 2-1
Correlations between Four Neurotransmit-
ters and Affective-PredatoryAggression
Affective Predatory
Serotonin -
Norepinephrine +
Dopamine +
Acetylcholine +
are available. Among those that have been done, however, the
results validate the animal neuroregulation research. Greenberg
and Coleman (1976) found that in 83 percent of their patients
studied, hyperactive and aggressive behaviors were associated
with a fall of blood !5-hydroxyindoles (serotonin metabolites).
Brown and colleagues (1979) reported statistically significant
correlations of cerebrospinal fluid biogenic arnine metabolites
with independently scored aggression rating scales in a sample of
personality-disordered military personnel. Results indicated ag-
gressiveness negatively correlated with Shydroxyindoleacetic
acid (serotonin) and positively correlated with 8methoxy-
4-hydroxyphenylglycol (norepinephrine); 80 percent of the vari-
ance in aggression scores was accounted for by the serotonin
metabolites in the cerebral spinal fluid.
Hormonal Influences
Autonomic Reactivity
INTERNALIZATION
Hartmann (1939) called the process of internalization an evolu-
tionary and phylogenetic transfer of functional-regulatory mech-
anisms from the external world to the internal world of the
organism. Schafer (1968) defined internalization as "all those
processes by which the subject transforms real or imagined
regulatory interactions with his environment, and real or imag-
ined characteristics of his environment, into inner regulations
and characteristicsn(p. 9).
I t is my hypothesis that the psychopathic process is funda-
mentally a virtual failure of internalization. There appears to be
a paucity of deep and unconscious identifications with, initially,
the primary parent figure and ultimately the archetypal and
guiding identifications with the society and culture and human-
kind in general.
The failures of internalization begin, as mentioned above,
with the organismic distrust of the sensory-perceptual environ-
ment. Throughout this early sensorimotor period (Piaget and
Inhelder 1969), and corresponding to the symbiotic phase of
Mahler and colleagues (1975), occur early incorporative failures. I
am using Schafer's (1968) definition of incorporation as a specific
and wishful primary process ideation of taking in the actual
object through the mouth or other body orifice. It expresses the
primitive wish to introject the other person within the self, and
therefore carry on a relationship, or the wish to modify the self
through union with the part of the object that is perceived, the
process of identification. Incorporation is the developmental heir
to empathy, and with early incorporative failures, the ground is
fertile for subsequent failures and distortions of the internaliza-
tion process and its twofold expression through identification and
introjection.
Let us pause here briefly and c l w the use of the terms
identifiation and introjection. Identification is "that modifica-
tion of the self which is caused by union with an object" (Grotstein
1982, p. 74). Schafer (1968) similarly defined identification as the
modification of the self or behavior to increase resemblance to
the object. In my words, it is the nascent self-representation
becoming like the perceived object.
Introjection is a process by which perceived objects are
internalized as representations yet continue to carry on a rela-
tionship to the self. I am inclined to agree with both Schafer
(1968) and Volkan (1976) that introjections are likely to occur in
times of distress and crisis for the infant when object supplies are
urgently needed but not available. Volkan (1976) rightfully cau-
tioned that the metaphorical implication of the term should not
be forgotten, despite its obvious use in conceptualizing what the
patient experiences as an internal presence separate from the
subjective sense of "I." I would classify the term introject as an
"object percept" that is often experienced as a visual image "seen"
in the mind, or as an auditory sound "heard" in the mind, yet that
is subjectively experienced as "not-I" (Meloy 1985). An introject
is both primitive and relational and is oftentimes confused with
an auditory hallucination by both patient and doctor unless care
is taken to locate the "sound" and describe its perceptual charac-
46 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
GRANDIOSE SELF-STRUCTURE
ATTACHMENT
CONCLUSIONS
REPTILIAN STATES
As the sun rose over the land and Lot entered Zoar,
Yahweh rained on Sodom and Gomorrah brimstone
and fire. . . . He overthrew these towns and the whole
plain, with all the inhabitants of the towns, and every-
thing that grew there. But the wife of Lot looked back,
and was turned into a pillar of salt. [Genesis 19:23-261
sister, thus acting out his wish that the baby be invisible and
perhaps warding off his own wish that the baby be afflicted by
the 'evil eye." Freud (1900) referred to his mentor Ernst Brucke's
"terrible blue eyes. ... No one who can remember the great
man's eyes . . . and who has ever seen him in anger, will find it
difficult to picture the young sinner's emotions" (p. 422). Later,
Freud (1919) theorized that the functional significance of
watching or being watched in manifest dreams was a product of
superego formation. Peto (1969) asserted that this representa-
tion of archaic superego elements by the threatening eye was a
remnant of the earliest phases of childhood sensory-perceptual
experience. In a thoughtful argument incorporating the work of
others (Hartmann 1924, Hermann 1934, Szekely 1954), he
stressed the significance of the Cyclopean glaring eye, specifi-
cally the primary perception of a red glowing eye, for the subse-
quent structuralization of anxiety in a biologically determined
real danger situation for the infant. This external traumatic
agent becomes internalized as a parental introject, or in my
terms, an object percept (Meloy 1985). Regression to the psy-
chotic level of elementary visual hallucinations of red (Hartmann
1924)or the archaic primary perception, urwahmzehmungen, the
red glow of the retaliatory parental eye (Hermann 1934), is
illustrated in the following case of a nonpsychopathic patient:
Pleasure
Table 4-1
Comparison of Texture (T) Responses among Four
Sample Groups (Exner 1986a)
-- --
Anger
"I had supper, washed up, played with the kids until
about eight o'clock, put them to bed, sat down and
watched TV-it came over about her . . . I knew it
wasn't me. I didn't want to believe it. It's so difficult to
explain to you. I knew it was me who did it, but why I
did it and everything else- I don't know why. I was not
excited. I didn't think about it; I sat down to dinner and
didn't think about it at all." [Frank 1966, p. 3021
Structure and mnarnics 89
Depression
The conscious experience of depression as an affect within the
psychopathic process probably does not exist. It is psychodynarn-
92 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
breasts, waists, and hips: hence, the police label "the measuring
man." DeSalvo was arrested on March 17,1961, and, at that time,
was married with two small children and living in suburban
Malden, Massachusetts. He was psychiatrically examined at
Westborough State Hospital and was diagnosed as a sociopathic
personality. On May 4, 1961, he was sentenced on conviction of
Assault and Battery, brought by some of the women he had
measured, and Attempted Breaking and Entering. Due to De-
Salvo's good behavior in custody and his attorney's plea that his
family needed him, he served eleven months and was released in
April, 1962. Two months later DeSalvo killed and raped his first
victim, 55-year-old Anna Slesers (Frank 1966).
I have already written of the relationship of envy and greed
to devaluation, the defense necessary to ward off unconscious
fantasies of oral rage and destruction. Devaluation both adapts to
and perpetuates the interoceptive feeling of emptiness, but at
great affective cost to the psychopath.
More commonly the psychopath will repetitively navigate
this unconscious affect-impulse-defense triad (envy-greed-
devaluation) through the conscious pursuit of behaviors that
result in feelings of exhilaration and contempt. But what is the
psychodynamic relationship between unconscious envy and
greed and conscious exhilaration and contempt?
Joseph (1960) wrote of the psychopath, 'What he gets he
spoils and wastes; he feels frustrated and deprived and the greed
and demands start again" (p. 527). Envy is commonly assumed to
be a derivative of oral aggression (Kernberg 1980) and contains
within it the infant's hatred of the actual object as actively
withholding supplies. This hatred creates wishes to destroy the
object, which, in turn, would eliminate the envy. Klein (1957)
understood the importance of envy as hatred of the good object,
in contrast with the infant's immediate dislike of unpleasant
sensory-perceptual experiences, which would be internalized and
eventually conceptualized as bad objects. Greed, a desire rather
than an affect, was a wish to have all the contents of the good
104 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Boredom
The conscious experience of boredom in the psychopathic process
is a complex affective state that is a product of object relational,
Structure and Dynamics 107
safe, for they conjure up feelings of envy for "the person who once
was" and therefore must be devalued to ward off oral aggression.
The aging psychopath may yearn for an early death as he
realizes the cumulative failures of his emotional life and the
incessant cycles of greed, spoiling, and devaluation prompted by
boredom. The young psychopath may idealize death and com-
mingle it with his own grandiose strivings, wishes that defend
against the fear of an empty, futureless life. As I began this
chapter with a quote from David Berkowitz, I will let him end it
in his own words:
As a child I had tremendous fascination with death.
When I thought about dying, I thought of being trans-
ported into a world of bliss and happiness. . . . When I
arrived at Stratford Avenue [in the Bronx] I used to
stand looking out our window with a fantastic view.
Throughout the year, rain, snow, or cold, I would look
out the window and pray to God to kill me, that I would
be hit by lightning. I begged to God for death, I used to
sit on the fire escape and thought of throwing myself
down, wanting to jump. . . . After Pearl's death, after
my [adoptive] mother died, I never had these suicidal
thoughts. I still wanted to die, but with heroism, with
honor. I wanted to die while saving lives, battling a
blaze. This is why I wanted to become a fireman,
helping people, rescuing them, and being a hero, or
possibly dying in the blaze. [Abrahamsen 1985, p. 311
I do love death. I've always loved it. I've wished for it,
and tried to understand it. Death is fascinating . . . its
power, its hold; it is wonderful. [Abrahamsen 1985, p.
%I
CONCLUSIONS
The psychopathic process is not devoid of conscious affect, but it
is emotionally colored in intensely narcissistic hues. Psychopaths
114 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
". . . Behavioral science does not use or rely upon the concepts of
'volition' or 'free will.' Accordingly, Dr. Metzner was not testi-
fying as a scientist when he testified that respondent's command
hallucinations impaired his 'volitional capacity.' Furthermore,
even if Dr. Metzner only meant to testify that command halluci-
nations are, in a statistical sense, coercive, his testimony finds no
support in the professional literature, and is contrary to clinical
experiencen(Ennis 1986, pp. 25-26). The Supreme Court ruled
that the Miranda warning was designed only to protect suspects
from coercive tactics of the police, not from any internal psycho-
logical pressures.
The psychopathic process is clinically relevant to this ques-
tion because of the commonly held belief, oftentimes correct, that
psychopaths lie a lot. Their deceit is consciously and intentionally
chosen. Most clinicians with whom I have discussed this question
vigorously defend this "conscious choicen hypothesis, simply be-
cause they have observed psychopaths' ease with which they can
choose, given the correct interpersonal determinants, not to lie,
or more dynamically, to also undo a lie.
One psychopathic patient told me an elaborate reconstruc-
tion of a distressing childhood event that he felt explained much
of his current behavior. When I challenged the credulity of it, he
laughed and said, 'Well, doc, would you believe this?" and pro-
ceeded to unravel an equally compelling tale of woe.
In a forensic setting the complex interplay between con-
scious choice and unconscious defense is most apparent when
questions of malingering are asked. There is not, as yet, any
psychological technique to detect malingering with any reason-
able certainty, but several researchers (Beaber et al. 1985,
Brandt et al. 1985, Schacter 198613) have begun to analyze this
perplexing clinical phenomenon.
Conscious choice and unconscious defense are also implicit
in the historical differentiation between malingering and facti-
tious disorder in descriptive psychiatry. The DSM-111-R (Arner-
ican Psychiatric Association 1987) postulated conscious intent in
Structure and Dynamics 119
.
father to them. .acts tenderly. . .tries to show them
that he has a fatherly instinct and they are boys he can
..
take care of. As I mentioned, these are . young men
and boys who are at the height of the oedipal them-
selves, who have the characteristics of the developed
.
phallus. . they are boys. . . he can identify with, who
he recognizes as having qualities that he feels. ..
"Now, he'd get these boys and he would begin to
project onto them the qualities that he had inside
himself and that he didn't like . . . He could say that
they are selling themselves, as he sold himself to try
and impress his father. He could say that they were
degraded. . .dehumanized as his father made him." He
begins to feel it is not himself he is hating, but the
young men.
At first, Rappaport said, Gacy would have been at
least unconsciously aware that the familiar hatred he
sensed in the boys actually existed within him. But "at
some point in the scenario that he goes through" Gacy
would progress from simple projection to projective
identification. "He then feels that these qualities . . .
exist in the other person. They are no longer a part of
him. They are in this other person and he feels ex-
.
punged or cleansed . . Now he sees these persons as
.. . ..
. bad . . homicidal . threatening. . .
"He is then the father in identifying with the aggres-
sor, and these victims, these boys, are then himself. He
.. ..
can . kill them . and in a way rid himself forever of
these qualities that are inside of himself: the hostile
threats and frightening figures that pervade his uncon-
scious. He is so convinced that these qualities exist in
this other person, he is completely out of touch with
.
reality . . and he has to get rid of them and save
.
himself . . he has to kill them." [Cahill 1986, pp.
339-3401
Structure and Qmurnic8 149
.
. . But the significance of this particular occasion was
that while he stayed off the streets and vowed he'd
never do it again and recognized the horror of what he
had done and certainly was frightened by what he saw
happening [italics added], it took him only three
months to get over it. . . .(p. 110)What happened was
this entity inside him was not capable of being con-
.
trolled any longer . . at least not for any considerable
period of time. I t began to try to justify itself, to create
rationalizations for what it was doing. Perhaps to sat-
isfy the rational, normal part of the individual. One
160 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
been all peaches and cream, your life, all together. . . ."
(O'Brien
1985,p. 234)' Watkins proceeded with his thirty-minute hypnotic
induction of Bianchi, which suggested the emergence of a sepa-
rate personality:
Bianchi's left hand rose from the chair. Watkins had provided
ample opportunity for Bianchi to consciously imitate a multiple
personality disorder and furthermore created a supportive envi-
ronment in which Bianchi's unconscious simulative processes
could begin to work. Watkins proceeded to establish a receptive
atmosphere for Bianchi to exaggerate the opposite-valenced
affects of splitting, or dissociative defenses, with which he was
probably quite familiar:
O'Brien (1985) noted that at this point Bianchi had erred in his
deception, since Bianchi had already said that "Ken" did not hate
anyone and was always kind and polite. Watkins, moreover, gave
no indication in the transcript of noticing this slip. Moments later
Bianchi repeated his mistake, "cause Ken hates women," but
quickly interjected in the feigned role of Steve, "I mean, I hate
women" (p. 239).
Bianchi appeared to draw on his cousin, Angelo Buono, for
his characterization of "Steve." This suggests that quite con-
scious imitation, as well as the unconscious process of malignant
pseudoidentification, existed throughout his apparent depen-
dency on Buono during their sadistic killings in Los Angeles.
Bianchi also fashioned "Steve" after one of the subpersonalities of
Sybil, portrayed by Sally Field in the movie he had viewed for the
second time on March 12,1979.
During the psychiatric and psychological investigations of
Bianchi's feigned multiple personality disorder, he implied ther-
apeutic "progress" through his jail diary:
I'm scared
my stomach hurts
there's no place to run
now,
it was easy to run away
before.
I feel strong, in control
but still unsure
of someone I've come to know,
someone I don't understand
as well as I know myself now. ..
I'm so alone now, somewhat
I feel naked.
I'm knowing me.
I wish I were free of him.
I want help.
I don't care for him
and he doesn't like me.
I feared confinement but
I'm thankful for it now.
[pp. 250-2511
AFFECTIVE AGGRESSION
This first mode of aggression subserves aggressive displays that
have traditionally been characterized as irritable, intermale,
territorial, or maternal. It is the most common mode of aggres-
sive vertebrate behavior and the mode of aggression that under-
lies most human violence. It begins with an intense and pat-
terned sympathetic activation of the autonomic nervous system
due to external or internal threatening stimuli. It is accompanied
by threatening and defensive postures and increased vocaliza-
tion. Often it is an end in itself and seldom correlates with feeding
or predation in animals. It may, however, sequentiallyprecede or
follow predatory aggression in humans.
I t appears that neuronal pathways mediating affective ag-
gression are widely distributed in the central nervous system of
vertebrates. Many of the pathways are closely related to the
spinothalamictract and periaquaductal gray, which may neuroa-
natomically explain the close association between affective ag-
gression and pain responses (Eichelman et al. 1981).
As noted in Table 2-1 (p. 26) circulating levels of serotonin
Violence, Psychosis, and Related States 193
PREDATORY AGGRESSION
..
Almost every waking moment . I find myself fanta-
sizing... .
But I'm greatly troubled by my fantasies.
Violence,Psychosia, and Related States 227
PSYCHOPATHYANDPREDATORYVIOLENCE
Identity Integration
Defensive Organization
2My thanks to Judith Meyers, Psy. D., and Eugene Schiller, L.C.S.W.,for
permission to use portions of their evaluation,
Violence, Psgchosis, and Related States 259
.
appears slightly younger than his stated age. . . He
was oriented in all three spheres. There were, how-
ever, periods when he did evidence mild disorganiza-
.
tion and confusion. . When asked about the instant
offense, he states: "I was suffering from a delusion
about the people who lived in the house. I thought that
they were my aunt and uncle. They were trying to sell
it, and I wanted to buy it. I was not looking for any-
thing. I had a delusion that I lived in the house before."
When asked why he committed the crime he
stated that voices were telling him to do it. He stated
the voices had given him the combination to the
.
safe. . . R. D. was questioned about the delusions that
he was King Tut and famous baseball players. He
smiled, denying he currently thought he was King Tut,
"I do not have a logical explanation." He indicated that
he just wanted to be great, but thought these identities
were basically harmless.
R. D. was asked if he had any regrets or remorse
about the crime. He stated that he did. "If I could turn
back the hands of time, I'd rather be without the
incident. I regret losing three years of my life for
something that is not mine. I could have been on the
street."
R. D. was asked about an incident in 1985during a
psychotherapy group while in custody. At that time he
had expressed a desire to return to the house and kill
the occupants. When confronted with this, he stated he
was not serious and was "just trying to make people
.
laugh. . ."
R. D. denied any drug or alcohol abuse at any time
..
in his life .as he relaxed, however, anxietal interfer-
ences to memory lessened. There were gaps and incon-
sistencies, however, in his reporting, probably due to
his denial system . . . he reports a history of antisocial
260 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
group that he owned a Lear Jet, had twenty children, and used to
play professional football when he was 12 years old.
Formal thought disorder in the psychopathic character is
also pathognomonic of psychotic personality organization. The
most striking and revealing example of formal thought disorder
expressed to me by a psychopathic personality was the patient
who blurted, "I have skeptophrenia." Here a neologism uncon-
sciously betrayed the individual's intent to malinger and exag-
gerate certain symptoms for behavioral gain; and, in a prescient
manner, foreshadowed my own doubt as to his credibility.
The multiple variations of formal thought disorder are the
phenotypic expressions of the primary process mechanisms of
condensation and displacement (Meloy 1986b):
severe levels are affected (Spohn et al. 1986). And, most germane
to the psychopathic process, formal thought disorder appears to
be qualitatively, rather than quantitatively, distinctive when
comparing manic and schizophrenic psychoses (Andreasen and
Grove 1986, Holzman et al. 1986).
I have not seen any clinical expression of formal thought
disorder in the psychopathic process that distinguishes it from
thought disorder in the absence of psychopathy. Its differential
expression in mania and schizophrenia, particularly paranoid
schizophrenia, however, does facilitate the oftentimes difficult
diagnosis of type of psychosis when seen in the context of a
psychopathic character. I t is especially relevant to a hypothesis
that I will elaborate later in this chapter; namely, that the
characteristic avenues of expression of psychosis i n the psycho-
pathic process are mania or paranoia.
Andreasen and Grove (1986) examined the frequency of
thought disorder in 100 psychiatric patients, including two sam-
ples of twenty-five manic disordered patients and twenty-five
paranoid schizophrenic disordered patients, using their Scale for
the Assessment of Thought, Language, and Communication (An-
dreasen 1978). Considering the eighteen types of thought abnor-
malities, the manic sample most frequently displayed the fol-
lowing types (in descending order): pressure of speech,
derailment, loss of goal, perseveration, circumstantiality, illogi-
cality, incoherence, and distractible speech. The paranoid schiz-
ophrenic sample most frequently displayed the following types
(in descending order): derailment, poverty of speech, loss of goal,
incoherence, perseveration, circumstantiality, tangentiality, and
pressure of speech. They concluded that patients with mania
tend to be more fluent and disorganized, with a more prominent
"positive" thought disorder, whereas patients with schizophrenia
tend to be more empty and disorganized, with a more prominent
"negative" or impoverished thought disorder. Among the schizo-
phrenias the degree of disorganization may differentiate
subtypes; on the other hand, across the entire range of functional
264 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Table 7-1
Factors Discriminating Manic and Schizophrenic Patients
TDI Categories
I. Irrelevant intrusion 2. Flippant
13. Looseness
11. Combinatory thinking 8. Incongruous combinations
14. Fabulized combination
15. Playful confabulation
111. Fluid thinking 9. Relationship verbalization
17. Fluidity
21. Contamination
IV. Confusion 5. Word finding difficulty
12. Confusion
18. Absurd responses
22. Incoherence
23. Neologism
V. Idiosyncratic verbalization 4. Peculiar verbalization
RealiQ Testing
Paranoid Annihilation
Manic Tn'umph
Malingering
Dissembling
The concealment of psychosis by the psychopathic character is
most commonly seen in forensic commitment settings where
issues of restoration of competency or insanity are being consid-
ered. The positive reinforcement for such behavior is usually the
prospect of transfer to a less restrictive environment.
As with malingering, the clinician should assume dissem-
bling in a psychopathic character with a history of psychosis,
especially in postconviction, commitment settings. Rigorous clin-
ical attempts should be made to disprove the hypothesis that the
patient is no longer psychotic. The following assumptions should
not be made without strong clinical support: The patient is
symptom-free because he no longer verbalizes hallucinations or
delusional thought content; the patient's reality testing is ade-
quate because he verbalizes an understanding of his psychotic
disorder; the absence of reported hallucinations means the ac-
companying delusions have remitted; the patient would tell the
clinician if he was still experiencing psychotic symptoms; the
patient does not have the ego control to intentionally conceal his
psychotic symptoms; the psychosis either exists or it doesn't -
there is no such thing as psychotic envelopment or partial rernis-
sion.
The case of R. D. described earlier illustrates the capacity of
the psychopathic character to dissemble psychotic symptoms.
His dissembling was revealed by careful perusal of his hospital
records, conversations with several hospital staff members over
a period of time, and administration of the Rorschach and MMPI
as complementary projective and objective personality meas-
ures.
Violence, Ps.ychosis, and Related States 29 1
'Many thanks to Judith Meyers, Psy.D., and Edward Calix, Ph.D., for these
excerpts from their report.
296 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
'1 would like to thank Richard Rappaport, M.D., and Park Dietz, M.D., for
this adept description of some arsonists.
302 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
FUNDAMENTAL PREMISES
For those clinicians that have both the heart and soul to attempt
psychotherapeutic treatment of psychopathically disturbed indi-
310 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Psychopathy as Process
Assessment of Severity
1. Glibnesslsuperficial charm
2. Grandiose sense of self-worth
3. Need for stimulation/proneness to boredom
4. Pathological lying
5. Conninglmanipulative
6. Lack of remorse or guilt
7. Shallow affect
8. Callousllack of empathy
9. Parasitic lifestyle
10. Poor behavioral controls
11. Promiscuous sexual behavior
12. Early behavior problems
13. Lack of realistic, long-term goals
14. Impulsivity
15. Irresponsibility
16. Failure to accept responsibility for own actions
17. Many short-term marital relationships
18. Juvenile delinquency
19. Revocation of conditional release
20. Criminal versatility
elements to it. The fear will be atavistic in nature and may be felt
quite primitively as a sense of predation. One severely psycho-
pathic patient whom I evaluated for treatment while in custody,
and who was ostensibly quite revealing, told me at the end of the
interview that I now 'knew too much" about him, and such
knowledge had gotten other people killed. For a moment I was
frozen because I could not evacuate the knowledge he had given
me about himself, and it had suddenly become a malignant threat
to my well-being rather than a benign source of clinical informa-
tion. This experience of projective identification, where a portion
of his mental content, in this case self-knowledge, was placed in
me, I could not expel it, and he could use it to control me, was
quite disconcerting.
Attempts to evoke representational images of others that
are affectional will usually be unsuccessful. Since the object
concepts of the psychopath are so vacuous, if the clinician per-
sists the descriptions of others become descriptions of the self.
When one psychopathically disturbed patient was asked to de-
scribe his wife, he said, "she's head-over-heels in love with me."'
As the clinician proceeds through the second phase of the
structural interview, transference and countertransference pre-
dictions for treatment should be more apparent. As primitive
defenses are mobilized during this probing of character pathol-
ogy, the clinician may feel a loss of a sense of freedom in
interacting with the patient (Kernberg 1984). With psychopathi-
cally disturbed patients, this may be experienced as a sense of
being "under his thumb." The clinician may also feel an inner
sense of devaluation, affectively experienced as a diminution of
self-esteem, without being able to pinpoint any behavioral
trigger by the patient. A sensitivity to spontaneous and fleeting
sadomasochistic visual images in the mind of the clinician may
also be diagnostic of the transference-countertransference par-
adigm that will be intensified during treatment.
COUNTERTRANSFERENCE
Although there are many understandings of countertransference
in the literature, I will begin with the definition proposed by
Reich (1951) and Greenson (1974), namely, the psychotherapist's
324 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Therapeutic Nihilism
PREDICTABLE RESISTANCES
DURING PSYCHOTHERAPY
Manipulative Cycling
Deceptive Practice
is quite useful. This can be the first step in the patient's analysis
of his deception rather than its exploitation as a resistance to
treatment.
Malignant Pseudoidentification
The conscious imitation and unconscious simulation (see Chapter
5) of the psychopathic individual contribute to the process of
malignant pseudoidentification during psychotherapy. This re-
sistance is recognized by the internalization and reflection of the
clinician's narcissistic vulnerabilities for purposes of control.
The dearth of internalizations within the psychopathic pro-
cess prompts a hunger for such identifications, but this psycho-
logical appetite is tainted by three characteristics: The identifi-
cations will invariably be superficial and short-lived; they
resonate most easily with the nefarious and narcissistic charac-
teristics of others; and they are used for hurting and controlling
the object of identification, in this case, the psychotherapist.
In a sense, however, these malignant pseudoidentifications
are pathways toward the core identification of the psychopath,
the stranger selfobject (Grotstein 1982). This is oftentimes the
most deeply internalized selfobject representation, and it usually
has its roots in identification with a cruel and aggressive primary
parental object. The malignant pseudoidentifications can direct
the clinician to this core element of the grandiose self-structure
through interpretation and confrontation of their meaningless-
ness for the psychopathic individual.
The imitation and simulation of various affective states is
also pathognomonic of malignant pseudoidentification. Patients
will either consciously imitate the expression of certain affects,
usually through words to imply a certain feeling, or they will
unconsciously simulate certain feeling states, usually through
nonverbal channels, such as posturing or facial expressions.
The conscious imitation of affect begins with the patient
intentionally choosing certain feelings that he believes are so-
336 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
cially appropriate and desirable. The patient then will talk about
these feelings 'as if" they are genuinely felt. Usually a clinical
question such as "Tell me more about your feeling" or more
directly "How do you know you're feeling ? Can you
describe to me what it's like for you to feel ?" will
elicit material that will differentiate the genuinely felt emotional
state from the imitated one.
The unconscious simulation of affect is more difficult to
discriminate. I t is the psychopathic propensity to identify, in a
chameleonlike manner, with socially desirable emotional states
without having any understanding of the empathic or meaningful
dimensions of such emotion. The clinician's countertransference
reaction to such simulated affect is most diagnostic, but this
assumes training and education that allows the clinician to differ-
entiate between his own endogenous and reactive countertrans-
ference states.
Simulated affect on the part of the psychopathically dis-
turbed patient may leave the clinician feeling skeptical and
distant, rather than empathically resonant. The patient will not
gradually recompensate from the affective expression but will
end it as if the performance is over. Treatment of the psychopa-
thically disturbed patient, however, must embrace the possi-
bility of genuine affective expression, or therapeutic gain will be
impossible. Again, the sources of genuine affect are inextricably
bound to the stranger selfobject and are clinically characterized
by persecutory fear, hatred, envy, rage, sadistic pleasure, con-
temptuous delight, and boredom. Without accessing these affec-
tive complexes the treatment must remain at a superficial, pseu-
doidentification level.
Sadistic Control
CONCLUSIONS
problem for the patient and not the primary focus of clinical
concern. In such cases, an understanding of psychopathy as a
clinical problem, but not necessarily the overriding clinical con-
cern, is important to its containment and the resolution of other
symptomatic or more treatable conditions.
In this chapter I have focused on the hndarnental premises
when considering treatment of psychopathic individuals, expect-
able countertransference reactions during such treatment, and
predictable resistances to treatment. These premises and predic-
tions, despite their psychoanalytic grounding, are applicable to
all modes of treatment of psychopathy regardless of the partic-
ular technique that is being applied. If one is a mental health
professional, the interpersonal encounter with the patient funda-
mentally defines the humanity, or lack of humanity, of the treat-
ment: a task that is most rigorously tested when the psycho-
pathic patient is commonly perceived, at least in part, as
inhuman.
Appendix I
tinued to talk to his girlfriend. Mr. Guard then states that his
male individual left again, and came back, wherein he knelt in
front of his girlfriend, who at this time was sitting in the seat to
the left of Mr. Guard. The patient then tells me that at one point
during the concert, he stood up and began clapping for the band,
and this unidentified male lunged upward from a crouched posi-
tion in front of his girlfriend, and with his right hand thrust a
five-inch knife into Mr. Guard's abdomen. The patient states he
grabbed the perpetrator's hand and the knife and attempted to
hold onto the knife so that he could not be stabbed again. He
states, "I was trying to hold my stomach, I couldn't feel any-
thing." He then tells me he fell forward onto the floor, and was
there for approximately ten minutes. He also adamantly states
that he received only one stab wound from this assault.
Later in the interview I asked Mr. Guard to once again
detail for me this "assault" at the Forum. The patient essentially
reported the same details, but added that he had attempted to
force this unidentified individual out of his seat, and the indi-
vidual had said, "don't threaten me." The patient denied any loss
of consciousness during this entire event. He stated that he told
the authorities it was not a knife fight because he is currently on
prison parole, and this would be used against him. He denies any
prior knife fights, and reports this is the first time he has ever
been stabbed.
In my search to confirm whether the patient was assaulted
or whether these were self-inflicted wounds, I pursued several
avenues. I was unable to establish telephone contact with the
Inglewood Police Department or the UCLA physician that
treated Mr. Guard following the stabbing. I was, however, able
to speak with Ms. W. by telephone. She reported to me that she
also had not seen the police arrest report or the medical exarnin-
er's report. She reported that much of their information was
gathered from newspaper reports concerning this particular
event, and she read to me statements from the Daily News, a San
Fernando Valley newspaper dated July 29,1986: "Guard stabbed
Appendix I 347
examine Mr. Guard's chest to see what scars were present. That
examination indicated that Mr. Guard had five scars on his
abdomen and thorax, all between 1 and 4 cm in length. The
patient acknowledged during this exam the presence of two
scars, and stated that one was for the abdominal surgery and one
was the original wound. When confronted by Dr. B. concerning
the other three scars in his thorax area, the patient attempted to
squeeze one of the scars, telling us, "it's a pimple." When faced
with our skepticism of his statement, he sat back in his chair,
pulled down his t-shirt, and said, "Oh, forget it."
In the absence of a police arrest report and the direct
testimony of the examining emergency room physician from
UCLA, it is not possible to determine beyond a reasonable doubt
whether Mr. Guard's wounds were self-inflicted or not. But the
preponderance of the evidence is that they were.
PERSONALITY PROFILE
ASSESSMENT OF DANGEROUSNESS
RECOMMENDATION
I would recommend that Mr. Guard be recommitted to Patton
State Hospital and outpatient conditional release be revoked at
this time.
If you have any further questions, please do not hesitate to
telephone me at 544-2435.
J. Reid Meloy, Ph.D.
Community Program Director
Chief, Forensic Mental Health Services
San Diego County
CROSS-EXAMINATION OF JONATHAN GUARD
District Attomzep: Mr. J. G., you are a habitual liar, are you
not?
J. Guard: In your opinion, that's for sure.
D.A.: You haven't done a lot of lying recently?
J.G.: I have done a lot of eluding the truth recently.
D.A.: Is that a lie? Is eluding the truth a lie?
J. G.: I t depends on the context.
D.A.: Well, let's talk about a couple of contexts. You told the
Secret Service agent in January of 1986 that you had previously
used a satanic cult fixation to manipulate the system, correct?
J.G.: Yes, I think1 did tell him that.
D.A.: Which meant that sometime long ago you lied to
psychiatrists, correct?
J.G.: I've talked to so many psychiatrists, I'm sure that I
haven't told them all the absolute truth a hundred percent of the
time, but that doesn't mean that I was purposely out to lie to
them or to try to deceive them.
D.A.: You told Agent P. of the Secret Service that you were
in love with your niece and you wanted to show the world your
love for your niece so you stabbed yourself?
J.G.: I do not have any recollection of that. When the Agent
came to see me, I was in intensive care, heavily medicated. I don't
hardly remember him coming, much less what I talked to him
about.
D.A.: So those words came rolling off, your lies?
J.G.: I don't remember what I said to him.
D.A.: You told Mr. B. of L.A. County Mental Health that
you stabbed yourself, didn't you?
J. G.: That's true.
D.A.: And then you told Dr. Meloy that that was a lie, that
you really hadn't stabbed yourself, that somebody else had
stabbed you?
J.G.: Yes, I told Dr. B, that.
D.A.: Which one of those statements was a lie?
358 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Louis Cypher-Paranoid
Schizophrenia and Psychopathy
Honorable Richard D. Jenkins
San Diego County Superior Court
Department 54
the last one on August 5. As the patient talks about his relation-
ship to the doctor, it is apparent he is quite angry and mistrustful
of him. He states he requested both Ritalin and Triavil from the
doctor. The former is methylphenidate hydrochloride, a mild
central nervous system stimulant. The latter drug, Triavil, is a
combination of antidepressant and antipsychotic. The patient
states, "The fusion (of Triavil) makes the beauty of the pill." He
admits to me that he doesn't trust Dr. E. and would prefer to be
treated by Ernest G., M.D., a psychiatrist that he first met when
admitted to the Psychiatric Security Unit in 1984. The patient is
especially angry at Dr. E. for not injecting him with a "golden
yellow" Prolixin that Mr. Cypher had brought to his appoint-
ment; and instead, the doctor injected him with some of his own
"crystal clear" Prolixin.
I have had two telephone conversations with Dr. E. and he
generally concurs with my assessment of this patient. He has
been treating Mr. Cypher for several years, and feels knuch less
comfortable with him than he used to. He feels Mr. Cypher is
much more guarded and hostile in recent months. I advised Dr.
E. of the patient's anger toward him, and he agreed to telephone
me following the subsequent appointment by the patient. I t is
clear that Dr. E. no longer wants to treat this patient.
PSYCHODIAGNOSIS
Axis I: Schizophrenia, paranoid type, chronic.
Opioid dependence, in remission.
Appendix II 371
ASSESSMENT OF DANGEROUSNESS
RECOMMENDATIONS
Addendum
said I was the President, the date was 1896 and I was going to
hire a hit man she said who do you want hurt and just off the top
of my head I said your name. I mean if she hadn't asked me it
would not have come out. I don't even know anyone in the Mafia
and even if I did I would wish you no harm.
You have a big responsibility and you are trying to show
that your program works. I am just a victim of yours ,and your
program.
I love you.
your ex-patient, L.C.
Appendix I11
RORSCHACH PROTOCOL I
The following Rorschach response protocol was produced by the
patient in Appendix I. To refresh the reader's memory, this
patient was diagnosed using the DSM-I11 criteria (American
Psychiatric Association 1980) with an Axis I mixed substance
abuse disorder and an Axis I1 antisocial personality disorder and
borderline personality disorder. J.G. was also assessed using the
Hare Psychopathy Checklist (1980) and received a quantitative
score of 34 out of a maximum of 40 points. This places him above
the 30-point cutoff for psychopathy (Hare 1986),and in my clinical
opinion would qualify him as severely psychopathically dis-
turbed. His two-point MMPI profile was 94, also supporting his
psychopathic diagnosis (Graham 198'7).
I have selected this Rorschach protocol since it has substan-
tial external validity as a raw psychological product of a psycho-
pathic personality disorder. I will present his associative and
inquired responses without commentary or scoring, and then
analyze and interpret it within the context of the four approaches
noted above.
I. 1. Absolutely symmetrical, looks like it could be a cater-
pillar on a mirror. (Inquiry response: that's the reflec-
tion in the mirror.)
2. Or a butterfly coming out of a cocoon. (Inquiry re-
sponse: looks like the cocoon around our house in the
high desert. (1)It just looks like that.)
11. 3. They're all symmetrical, two elephants kissing each
other. (Inquiry response: standing on three legs, touch-
ing, not necessarily kissing, because the trunks are
touching, their ears here.)
111. 4. It looks like a cricket or an insect because of the eyes
380 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Test-Taking Apprwrch
The psychopathic character's approach to the Rorschach is as-
sessed by interpreting number of responses (R), Lambda (L),
whole responses (W),synthesis responses, blends, space re-
sponses (S),and personalized responses (PER).
1. Number of responses (R) is predicted to be low due to a
restricted and defensive attitude toward the test. If Lambda
exceeds .85 in a record of 10-12 responses, guardedness may be
indicated. A record of less than 10 responses should always be
considered invalid. Intellectual deficits and neurological impair-
ments will also limit R.
2. Lambda (L) represents the proportion of pure F re-
sponses in the protocol, which are the most simple and affect-free
associations. I t is predicted to be higher than normal (mean = -59
in normative sample, 1.51 in character disordered sample [Exner
1986al) in the psychopathic protocol due to his uninvolved, con-
stricted, and defensive attitude. Pure F responses ignore the
complexities of the stimulus field and may represent situational
382 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
Internal Opemtiona
As the reader will note, I have listed only the more defini-
tive hypotheses from the text. What is most striking is the
absence of certain variables that define the internal operations of
394 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
External Opemtions
Table 111-1
Developmental Analysis of Object Representations
(Adapted from Blatt and Lerner 1983)
---
1. Accuracy:
F-, F +
2. Differentiation:
Quasi-human detail: (Hd)
Human detail: Hd
Quasi-human: (H)
Human: H
3. Articulation:
Inappropriate (-) or appropriate (+)
Perceptual: Size (Sz), Posture (Po), Hair Style (Hsy),
Clothing (Cl), Physical Structure (PSt).
Functional: Sex (Sex), Age (Age), Role (Ro), Specific
Identity (SpId).
4. Motivation for Action:
No action (NoAct)
Unrr~otivatedaction (Unmot)
Reactive action (React)
Intentional action (Int)
5. Integration of Object and Action:
Fused (Fused)
Incongruent (Incon)
Non-specific (NonSp)
Congruent (Con)
6. Content of Action:
Malevolent (Mal)
Benevolent (Ben)
7. Nature of Interaction:
Active-Passive (A-P)
Active-Reactive (A-R)
Active-Active (A-A)
Active (A-A). Rorschach administration to score this procedure
must follow the guidelines of Rapaport, Gill, and Schafer (1946).
Inquiry follows each card, and the card is removed from the
408 THE PSYCHOPATHIC MIND
RORSCHACH PROTOCOL I1
(HUMANRESPONSES ONLY)
111. 1. It's two people pulling a crab apart. (Response to in-
quiry: Here's one person and here's another person and
here's their hands, and it looks like they're pulling some-
thing apart. I said it's a crab because that's what I said
last time.)
2. Someone tumbling [D2]. (Response to inquiry: Yea,
here's his head and here's his body.)
VII. 3. Two Cupids facing each other. (Response to inquiry:
Yea, they have wings [DdZl]. This is their hair and their
faces. They look like they're on a stand.)
4. It's a girl looking at herself in the mirror. (Response to
inquiry: Here's her head, here's her arm, and then this
part's her body.)
IX. 5. This is a space man, like the cover on a science fiction
[WJ. (Response to inquiry: He's got a big head [D81, the
orange is the aura going out from him [D3], the green
could be hands, and the red the rest of his body. He has
an ominous look. He's an unfriendly bastard. I just
noticed this, he's giving you the finger [Dd31]. I didn't
see that at first.)
umbilical cord and the guts are shooting out. Can that
really happen? Can it bleed to death? (Response to inqui-
ry: the nose, the color, more of a flesh tone. He's red when
he comes out, this is green and orange guts coming out.)
X. 7. Some guy's handing another guy something. [D6] (Re-
sponse to inquiry: the blue here, on a cliff.)
8. Looks like a bug here [DA. Someone used a drill press on
him [D15], blood here [D13], drilling through one leg, the
handle and power unit here [D15]. (Response to inquiry:
cockroaches, two antenna and feet, and a wishbone here,
the yellow handle.)
9. A man here, pulled along by two seahorses [DlO]. Like
with Sharnu, guy gets on his back, you ever been there?
(Response to inquiry: the hair, feet, and seahorses; a shot-
gun here.)
case in the instant offense, since the victim was bound and
gagged during his torture and murder.
Yet the irony of this patient, perhaps intrapsychically pre-
dictable if one looks carefully at his shift from an unmotivated,
fused, and malevolent response (8) to an intentional, nonspecific,
and benevolent last response (9), was his behavior immediately
following the killing:
Bach-y-Rita, G., Lion, J., Climent, C., and Ervin, I?. (1971). Episodic
dyscontrol: a study of 130 violent patients. American Joumzal of
Psychiatq 127:1473-1478.
Bailey, K. (1987). Human paleopsychology: roots of pathological aggres-
sion. In Origins of Human Aggression, ed. G. Neuman, pp. 60-63.
New York: Human Sciences.
Bailey, K., Tipton, R. M., and Taylor, P. F. (1977). The threatening
stare: differential response latencies in mild and profoundly re-
tarded adults. American Joumull of Mental D e m y 31599-602.
Bandler, R. J., Jr., and Moyer, K. E. (1970). Animals spontaneously
attacked by rats. Communications i n Behavioml Biology
5:177-182.
Bandura, A., and Walters, R. H. (1958). Dependency conflicts in aggres-
sive delinquents. J o u m l of Social Issues 14:52-65.
References 427
Lacey, B., and Lacey, J. (1974). Studies of heart rate and other bodily
processes in sensorimotor behavior. In Cadiovascular Psy-
chophysiologg: Current Issues in Response Mechanisms, Biofeed-
back, and Methodology, ed. P. Obrist, A. Blac, J. Breuer, and L.
Dicara, pp. 538-664. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.
Laing, R. (1976). The Facts of Life. Middlesex, England: Penguin
Books.
Lange, C. (1886). Om Sindsbevaegelser et psyko. Fysiolog. Studie.
Copenhagen: Kroner.
Laschet, U. (1973). Antiandrogen in the treatment of sex offenders:
mode of action and therapeutic outcome. In Contempmrg Sexual
Behavior, ed. J. Zulbin and J. Money. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins
University Press.
Lashley, K. (1938). The thalamus and emotion. Psychological Review
45:42-61.
Lehman, L. (1974). Depersonalization. American Journal of Psychi-
atqj 131:1221-1224.
Lerner, H. (1986). An object representation approach to Rorschach
assessment. In Assessing Object Relations Phenomena, ed. M.
Kissen, pp. 127-142. Madison, CT: International Universities
Press.
Lerner, H., Sugarman, A., and Gaughran, J. (1981). Borderline and
schizophrenicpatients: a comparative study of defensive structure.
Journul of Nervous and Mental Disease 169:705-711.
Lerner, P., and Lerner, H. (1980). Rorschach assessment of primitive
defenses in borderline personality structure. In Borderline Phe-
nomena and the Rorschach Test, ed. J. Kwawer, H. Lerner, P.
Lerner, and A. Sugarman, pp. 257-274. New York: International
Universities Press.
Lester, D. (1986). Suicide and homicide on death row (letter). American
Jounzal of Psychiatry 143:569-660.
References 445
Reid, W. H., Dorr, D., Walker, J., and Bonner, J. (1986). Unmaking the
Psphopath. New York: Norton.
Reiser, M. (1984). Mind, Brain, Body. New York: Basic Books.
-(1985). Converging sectors of psychoanalysis and neurobiol-
ogy: mutual challenges and opportunity. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association 33:ll-34.
Reiss, S., Levitan, G. W., and Szyszko, J. (1982). Emotional disturbance
and mental retardation: diagnostic overshadowing. American
Journal of Mental Defkimcy 86:567-574.
Reiterman, T,,and Jacobs, J. (1982). Raven: The Untold S t o q of the
Reverend Jim Jones and His People. New York: Dutton.
Revitch, E., and Schlesinger, L. (1981). PsphopaUlolog21 of Homicide.
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Richardson, L. (1960). Statistics of Deadly Quurreh. Pittsburgh: Box-
wood Press.
Riedel, M., Zahn, M., and Mock, L. (1985). The Nature and Patterns of
American Homicide. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
Office.
Rinsley, D. (1982). Borderline and Other Self Disorders. New York:
Jason Aronson.
Ritzler, B., Zambianco, D., Harder, D., and Kaskey, M. (1980). Psy-
chotic patterns of the concept of the object on the Rorschach test.
J o u r ~ ofl Ab-1 Psych0log21 89:46-55.
Robins, E., Purtell, J. J., and Cohen, M. E. (1952). "Hysteria" in men.
New England Journal of Medicine 146:667-685.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant Children Grown Up: A Sociological and
Psychiatric Study of Sociopathic Personality. Baltimore: Williams
& Wilkins.
Rogers, R. (1986). Conducting Insanity Evaluations. New York: Van
Nostrand Reinhold.
Rorschach, H. (1921). Psychodiagnostics. Bern: Bircher, 1942.
Rosanoff, A. J. (1938). Manual of Psychiutrg and Mental Hypkne. New
York: Wiley.
Rosenfeld, H. (1964). On the psychopathology of narcissism: a clinical
approach. I n t e r n a t k l Journal of Psycho-Analysis 45:332-337.
Rothstein, A. (1980). The Narcissistic Pursuit of Pe@ectiun. New York:
International Universities Press.
464 References
Yeudall, L., 22