ARLOW, J. A. (1961) - Ego Psychology and The Study of Mythology PDF
ARLOW, J. A. (1961) - Ego Psychology and The Study of Mythology PDF
ARLOW, J. A. (1961) - Ego Psychology and The Study of Mythology PDF
MYTHOLOGY'
371
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
372 JACOB A. ARLOIV
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND hlYTHOLOGY 373
The interpretations differ widely from each other; which is
right? T h e psychoanalysts, like the philologists, come to the
materials of folklore from the outside, anxious to exploit
them from their own a priori assumptions.
This critique written less than two years ago contains arguments
which might have carried considerable weight in the past, before
the development of ego psychology. Psychoanalytic interpretations
of myths do not necessarily approach the materials of folklore
from the outside, nor is it inevitably true that no validation of
symbolic interpretations is possible. What we encounter in this
critique is another manifestation of “cultural lag.” Dorson’s esti-
mate of psychoanalytic methodology is based primarily on materi-
als studied from the topographic point of view and from a type
of interpretation practiced before the impact of ego psychology
made itself felt on the technique of psychoanalysis. Of course,
Ilot all anthropologists or students of mythology share this view of
psychoanalytic methodoIogy; nor are they a11 victims of this cul-
tural lag. We can hardly expect workers in allied fields, however,
to keep abreast of the subtler implications of newer psychoanalytic
concepts when a similar lag often exists in our own ranks. Even
among the better informed mythologists a number of misconcep-
tions have developed and persist. They believe that psychoana-
lysts assume that myths and dreams are indistinguishable. While
they recognize the closeness of mythology to literature, they do
not appreciate the difference which psychoanalytic ego psychology
introduces in the evaluation of the function of dreams, myths, and
literary creations.
One purpose of this communication is to demonstrate how o u r
knowledge of ego psychology may enable us to establish a frame
of reference within which psychoanalytic study of mythology may
be based on methods which can be validated. A brief historical
retrospect may serve to put the problem into focus. Rank’s first
contribution to the study of mythology, T h e Myth of the Birth
of the Hero (52), and Abraham’s “Dreams and Myths” (1) ap-
peared only nine years after T h e Interpretation of Dreams and
only four years after the Three Contributions to the Theory of
Sex. This was at a time when psychoanalysis was still a very new
science. T h e scientific world rejected its basic concepts and ridi-
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
374 JACOB A. ARLOIV
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND AIYTHOLOGY 375
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
376 JACOB A. ARLOW
set of sensory impressions of a highly gratifying nature. T o the
developing ego falls the task of delaying the immediate discharge
of these impulses or of facilitating their expression in an adaptive,
integrated manner, avoiding intrapsychic conflict, anxiety, or clash
with the world of reality.
Unconscious fantasy thinking is one level of the ego’s integration
of the instinctual demands of the id. Its cathectic potential is active
at a11 moments of the individual’s life, with the possible exception
of deep narcosis or dreamless sleep. T h e intrusion of unconscious
fantasy thinking into conscious mental experience is well known
to us from the study of hallucinations, dreams, and daydreams.
More recent investigation has shown how this tendency plays a
roIe in the structuring of perception and in the interpretation oE
external reality. Potzl (51), Fisher (17, IS), and others have con-
firmed Freud’s clinical conclusion that almost all sensory stimuli
receive some sort of mental registration, often outside the scope of
consciousness. T h e awareness of perceiving requires an additional
mental operation, a certain kind of cathectic investment, which is
related to a very large extent to the instinctual life as expressed in
iantasy wishes. T h e registration of sensory data is a continuous func-
tion (28); awareness is a discontinuous process depending in part on
the intermittent burst of cathectic investment as the unconscious
fantasy function of the ego scans the data of sensory registration
for items useful to its purposes. hlany illusions and misrepresenta-
tions of reality are based upon the intrusion of this activity into
the neutral function of checking the raw data of perception.
Depending on the nature of the data of perception, the level of
cathectic potential, and the state of ego function, different forms
of mental products will emerge. It is out of this common matrix
of ego activity that dreams, symptoms, fantasies, and myths are
created. T h e pressure of the unconscious fantasy wishes orients
one of the aspects of ego activity to be ever alert to incorporate,
integrate, correlate, or misinterpret the data of perception and the
knowledge of the real world in keeping with its pleasure-seeking
purpose of discharge3 Thus the vision of a truly remarkable hero,
as in the case of our first cosmonaut, becomes integrated into
3 Martin H. Stein (66) discussed this matter recently from a somewhat different
point of view. Following Freud. he pIaced the intermittent burst of athexis, the
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND MYTHOLOGY 377
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
378 JACOB A. ARLOW
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND RIYTHOLOGY 379
&rogated nightly in dreams. Personal dreams and daydreams are
made to be forgotten. Shared daydreams and myths are instru-
ments of socialization. T h e myth, like the poem, can be, must be,
remembered and repeated. Externalization or projection of the
impulses which give rise to fantasy not only makes this process
of sharing possible, but, as Bruner (10) states, “Externalization
1nake.s possible the containment of terror and impulses by the
decorum of art and symbolism.” Mythology, art, and even religion,
are subsidiary, institutionalized instrumentalities which bolster
the social adaptation ordinarily made possible by the nightly
abrogation of instinctual renunciation in dreams.
In the genesis of myth, for the group, as for the individual, only
a kernel of realistic experience is necessary. T h e revision and
falsification, or both, of the past and its heroes by the group serve
the purpose of defense, adaptation, and instinctual gratification
for the group and its individual constituents; they also serve in
character building and superego formation. Only the shadow of a
real event is necessary on which to build the structure of the myth.
The essential substance is contributed by the inner needs of the
individual members of the group. Attempts, therefore, to recon-
struct in precise detail the putative events of a particular period
in the history of the group (30, 58, 59) seem to be based on un-
sound methods, if we take into account the defensive distortions
of the ego. I doubt whether such considerations are actually
relative to what psychoanalysis has to contribute to the study of
myth ol ogy.
Having considered the parallel function and development of
both myth and fantasy from the viewpoint of the ego, I believe
that we are in a position to make a number of suggestions which
are pertinent to the study of mythology.
1. It is not sufficient for us to be able to demonstrate, with
monotonous regularity, evidence of the same id wishes in the text
of the myth. By applying our knowing of ego psychology we obtain
insight into the differences between myths, even when these myths
deal with the same theme. Different mythological expressions on
the same basic theme correspond to the different defensive editions
of the unconscious fantasy in the life of the individual, external-
ized and artistically altered in correspondence with needs from
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
380 JACOB A. ARLOW
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND hfYTHOLOGY 381
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
382 JACOB A. ARLOW
the basis of this study it was possible to observe how each had
been, in turn, a mischievous or perhaps psychopathic Jack, a
Prometheus, and a Moses. Identification with these o r similar
figures met the needs of these patients a t different points in their
lives.
T h e fairy-tale version of this problem belonged to the wish-
fulfilling tendency of childhood in which contribution of the
superego is minimal and unformed and the fear of retaliation is
disposed of omnipotently (24). I t was striking to observe in the
analysis of a twenty-five-year-oldman, for example, the persistence
of such primitive expressions of his oedipal wishes. This patient
suffered from severe learning and work inhibitions. H e was over-
awed by his self-centered, seemingly omnipotent father who was
an arrogant, unscrupulous, and successful businessman. This
patient’s favorite motion picture was The Thief of Bagdad. He
remembered it from childhood and arranged repetitively and
compulsively to see every revival of the movie on the screen or on
television. T h e scene in which the young thief enters the temple
high in the Himalayas to steal the magic jewel from the forehead
of the gigantic Buddha filled him with intense and pleasurable
excitement. Almost any plot in second-rate literature or in the
movies was re-edited in his fantasy to correspond almost literally
to the Jack and the Bean Stalk story. Fixated a t this primitive
level of wish fulfillment and overcome by uncontrollable fear
of retaliation, he remained in actuality an eternally frustrated
Prometheus, not daring even for a moment to lay hands on the
divine power of learning. For a brief period during adolescence he
escaped from the paralyzing effect of his father by way of an
identification with a counselor at a religious camp. I n this at-
mosphere, for a period of two months, he became creative and
energetic, demonstrating qualities of leadership and imagination
he never knew he possessed. H e had temporarily found a new
identity, subjectively expressed in keeping with his grandiose
narcissistic needs in terms of being a Moseslike figure, represent-
ing the entire human race in a second confrontation with God.
Unfortunately, when he returned from camp to the realistic
confrontation with his father, this new identity crumbled
ignominiously.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND AlYTHOLOGY 383
T h e myth of Prometheus has been studied extensively, and the
theme of the theft of the phallus was noted early by Abraham (1)
and Freud (29). TVhat is epitomized in this variation is the stage
beyond the untroubled wish fulfillment of the simple fairy tale,
the overwhelming impact of the fear of retaliation. This myth
memorializes the stage of psychic development before the renun-
ciation of the oedipal wishes and the institution of the superego.
It is a primitive parable of moraIity pointing to the desirability
of establishing internal prohibition and condemnation in order to
lvard off punishment and castration, but still without the helpful
reinforcement of the identification with the more neutralized,
more realistic, genuinely moral image of the father.
T h e myth of Moses receiving the Law represents a later and
different elaboration of the same motif. T h e image of Moses as a
rebellious son is given several representations in the Bible, not
only in relation to his foster father, Pharaoh, but also on at least
one occasion in the form of disobedience to God's commands. It
is striking that in all these instances the omnipotent wand figures
importantly.
In the story of Moses at hlt. Sinai one can see a number of
progressive developmental steps in the transformation of the
elements contained in the Prometheus myth. 'CVhat was originally
a crime of defiance and aggression against the gods is, in this later
version, represented as carrying out the wishes of God Himself.
IVhat has intervened is the process of identification, the identifica-
tion between a mortal and a God (between father and son). T h e
identification is on a moral, rather than on an instinctual, level.
The process of identification has eventuated in a sublimation, and
the area of gratification has concomitantly been shifted to the
feelings of narcissistic omnipotence. T h e return of the repressed
does not escape the mythmaker either, and hfoses descending the
mountain with the divine knowledge which is to be the gift for all
mankind is now hardly distinguishable from God. T h e divine
fire suffuses Moses in the form of the enveloping light, and shafts
of light or horns extend from his head. T h e sins of indulgence
and rebelliousness are now displaced onto the unruly mob of
Israelites, and Moses and God, the self (ego) and the moral pre-
cepts of the father generation (superego) become as one in con-
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
384 JACOB A. ARLOW
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND hlYTHOLOGY 385
world from its original state into its present condition. H e is
ictured as killing the monsters which infested the land and as
Piving man the arts that make life worth living. Boas (8) writes:
g
4 ~ ~ the o wTransformer often appears in two guises: he may be a
Prometliean culture hero, a benevolent being who wishes to
protect and benefit mankind, but he is much more likely to appear
as a trickster (such as the Raven, Mink, Bluejay, Coyote, Old Man,
Manabosho, or Glooscap of the American Indians), a n irre-
sporlsible or utterly selfish creature who benefits man incidentally
in pursuing his own libidinous or rapacious desires.” Boas con-
jectures that in the original American mythologies the selfish and
fibidirlous Transformer appeared much more undisguisedly than
he does in later versions, and that he was pictured as a moral
being only gradually “with the progress of society.” Indeed, the
discrepancy in his character sometimes becomes so striking and
“the friction between the two groups of tales” so pronounced,
that the “personage of the Transformer [is] split in two or more
p r t s , the one representing the true culture hero, the other retain-
ing the features of the trickster.”
Chase (13) comments that Boas’s theory is only a conjecture,
and he notes that this theory falls back on the idea of evolutionary
progress to explain the dual character of the Transformer as he
appears in different myths. H e in turn proposes to interpret this
phenomenon on a broad “social role” plane. H e sees, in the
mriant mythological expressions of a fundamental theme, evidence
of the struggle between mankind in general and the Priest-
&thinker, between the magician and the religious man. From the
psychoanalytic point of view the persistence of such contradictory
representations would seem to be more readily accountable in
Perms of the appeal to different levels of fantasy formation. In the
EWO aspects of the Transformers we may recognize once again the
biry-tale-Jack and the Prometheus-levels of ego integration
of the basic id wish.
A similar set of conditions prevails in another group of myths
which superficially seem quite different but which genetically are
idcntical. T h e Bible story of Abraham and Sarah is well known.
This childless couple had been exemplary hosts to three angels,
the messengers of God in disguise. Before the angels depart they
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
386 JACOB A. ARLOJV
predict to Sarah that a son will be born to her before the year is
past. There is a Greek myth about Baucis and Philemon (14, 32).
They too entertained the gods in disguise. TVhen the gods revealed
themselves and wished to reward the couple for their hospitality,
the aged Philemon makes his request in the following words:
“IVe have lived together for many years and in all that time there
has never been a word of anger between us. Now at last we are
growing old and our own companionship is coming to an end.
Grant us this one request that when we come to die we may
perish in the same hour and neither of us be left without the
other.” This wish the gods granted, and when the time finally
came, this devoted couple was transformed into a pair of trees
growing side by side, their leaves and branches interlaced. Baucis
and Philemon had been a childless couple.
T h e erotic significance of death and dying has been described
in the literature, particularly in Abraham’s poetic interpretation
of “The Bride of Death Ritual” (2) and in Jones’s study of the
nightmare (34). Death, furthermore, is a tomorrowland where all
wishes come true. In addition, in two separate contributions (35,
36), Jones demonstrated that the conscious fantasy of dying to-
gether has the unconscious significance of a wish to have a child,
an example of a defensive representation by the opposite. Further
proof that these two myths have a common genetic origin may be
seen in the fact that in each case a definite period of time must
elapse before the wish for a child is fulfilled. Clearly this fantasy
may be traced back no earlier than the period during which the
child becomes aware of the necessary physiological interval pre-
ceding childbirth. T h e defensive significance of premonition,
promise, and prediction has been elucidated by Stein (65) and
more recently by Schlesinger (63). I n essence, therefore, these two
myths must take their place alongside the classic myth of the
Annunciation to the hladonna and the Madonna’s conception
through the ear (37). These are all annunciation stories, that is,
myths of impregnation. I n the latter case, however, the sublimated
religious setting permits an undisguised regressive emergence of
the incestuous nature of the wish for the child.
T h e role of mythology in psychic differentiation and in char-
acter structure is most important in the development of the
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND hIYTHOLOGY 387
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
388 JACOB A. ARLOIV
Anna Freud (19) has pointed out how each society tries to
fashion the younger generation in consonance with the ideals and
goals of the particular society. Through its mythology, the society
tends to induce a climate favorable to the realization of appro.
priate identifications (46, 47). Every society interprets and re-
interprets its history and its heroes in keeping with this need. TVhat
makes this technique so effective is the powerful, motive force
of the childhood instinctual wish through the medium of the
vicarious (unconscious) gratification which comes from identifica-
tion with the hero of the myth. T h e path is prepared for iden-
tification and subsequent character transformations in keeping
with the idealized qualities of the hero. So, for example, while the
little girl gets the unconscious incestuous wish fulfillment from her
identification with the Madonna, she is consciously directed to the
imitation of those ideal qualities of purity, virtue, and love which
are represented by the hladonna.
Several authors have concerned themselves with the problem
of the changing or disappearing myth. This is a well-known
cultural phenomenon, and from the psychoanalytic point of view
it reflects how a change in the group mores revitalizes or devital-
izes the mythical or legendary heroic image. Such a process of
transformation may be observed in certain aspects of Jewish ideal
formation. Before the rise of a militant nationalistic movement
the educational experiences of the young Jewish child were geared
toward creating a character structure adaptable to the needs of a
ghetto Diaspora. T h e appropriate ideal image in this process was
Rabbi Jochanan Ben Zakkai. H e was the teacher who, anticipating
the victory of the Romans and the fall of Jerusalem, arranged to
be smuggled out of the besieged city in a coffin. Brought before
the emperor, he prophesied the victory of Rome, and for these
good tidings was rewarded with permission to found an academy
for the further study of the Law. H e epitomized the qualities of
devotion to tradition by submission to temporal authority, In the
modern era, with its emphasis on nationalism, a reversal of values
has taken place, and history has been reinterpreted and re-evalu-
ated to emphasize, in a mythological way, different heroes-heroes
whose qualities would be in consonance with the adaptive needs
of modern character molding. T h e Maccabean heroes, long held
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND hlYTHOLOGY 389
in disrepute by Talmudic tradition, have been resurrected in
recent years with renewed vitality in keeping with these needs.
Tllese considerations bring us to a set of fascinating problems
for psychoanalysis, namely, the mythology of our age and the quest
for identity. A detailed presentation of my thoughts on these mat-
ters cannot be given in the remaining time. I would like, however,
sketch an outline of some of my ideas.
The solution of interpsychic conflicts by the process of renuncia-
tion and identification, that is, by the formation of the superego,
is never completely successful (16). T h e models for ideal formation
and identification afforded by the established religions have in
recent times clearly lost their appeal. One sees in our culture a
conflict between two competing mythologies, each tending to
mold character and psychic development in different ways. One
mythology, working through established institutions, tries to point
in the direction of internalized inhibitions and intrapsychic trans-
formation. Alongside this tendency of the official institutions,
however, is the opposing trend toward reinstitutionalization of
the ego ideal, in keeping with intensified narcissistic needs, and
tending toward the idealization of grandiosely conceived objects
from childhood. With increasing frequency, we see narcissistic
character difficulties and narcissistic neuroses, together with pa-
tients who cannot contain their conflicts within themselves by
the process of symptom formation, but who are forced to external-
ize them in various forms of acting out. I n her writings on the
narcissistic object choice and on pathological methods for regu-
lating self-esteem, Annie Reich (54, 55) has elucidated the quest of
such patients to realize in actuality an identification with these
exalted instinctualized objects. Disturbance of the sense of self
and’an “as if” confusion of identity are very prominent in these
patients. “It is not simply society which patterns itself on the
idealizing myths, but unconsciously it is the individual man as
well who is able to structure his internal clamor of identities in
terms of the prevailing myth. Life produces the myth and finally
imitates it.”
T h e outstanding cultural aspect of this problem is to be seen in
the worship of the new demigods of the mass media of communica-
tion. These heroes of our modern mythology are only thinly dis-
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
390 JACOB A. ARLOIV
guised versions of the central figures of the Greek pantheon. They
are invested with omnipotent powers by a host of votive priests
and publicists, and after the fashion of the demigods are immune
from criticism for their heroic exploits of instinctual indulgence.
They act out the antisocial daydreams of the community. In
materialized form, made possible by the mass media of communi-
cation, they descend, if not from the heights of Olympus, at least
from hills of Hollywood, to dwell among us as living proof of the
possibility that identification with mythological, narcissistically
grandiose ego ideals of childhood can be realized i n the flesh.
Many sociologists are of the opinion that there is a crisis in
American culture generated by the confusion that has befallen
the myth of the happy man. These authorities feel that we are no
longer a “mythologically instructed community.” As a result one
finds a new generation in quest of a satisfactory or challenging
mythical image which would serve as a model for identification.
JVhen the prevailing myths fail to provide such external models
and fail to fit the varieties of man’s plight, “frustration expresses
itself in mythopoesis and then in the lonely search for internal
identity” (10). HOWdeep this process has progressed is dificult
to tell from the study of individual patients alone, but certain
clinical impressions of this change are reflected in the types of
patients we see, as has been mentioned above. Perhaps because the
official religious myths no longer fit the internal plights of those
who require them, there is a reactivation of the old mythologies.
hlythologically speaking, we are hardly an uninstructed commu-
nity, and the various media of mass communication, comic books,
and literature have been issuing forth a stream of reanimated
mythological figures indistinguishable from their classical proto-
types. Patients in quest of the realization of their narcissistic ego
ideals almost invariably introduce evidence of some such iden-
tification during the course of their treatment, from various repre-
sentations of the Greek gods to the heroes of the comic books. One
patient, for example, had only an imperfect understanding of a
latency fantasy of identification with the comic-book hero Captain
Marvel until she remembered the magic word “Shazam.” This
word served to transform the hero from an ordinary mortal into a
supernatural figure. T h e magic word was an acrostic made up of
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND MYTHOLOGY 391
the initials of the names of the Greek heroes, Socrates, Hercules,
Atlas, Zeus, Apollo, and Mercury. Each hero was introduced as
the representation of a specific ideal quality,
psychoanalysis itself becomes drawn into this myth and may
become part of it. T h e magic aura with which psychoanalysis is
endolved, a projection onto the analyst of the patients’ oivn feelings
of omnipotence, usually culminates during the treatment in some
version of the myth of P y p a l i o n and Galatea. Unfortunately, it
takes time and effort to convince the patient that in this fantasy, as
in all fantasies, the characters are all the patient’s oivn wishes,
projected representations of his own self. An interminable analysis
may result if one fails to resolve this aspect of the transference
mythology. It is even more ominous, Schmideberg (64)says, if the
analyst himseIf subscribes to this myth.
T h e regressive representation of mythology in concrete, audible,
and visual form in the mass media have important implications
for ego structuring (26) and fantasy formation during childhood
and adolescence. They also have bearing on problems of acting
out.
T h e practical, social applications of the study of mythology are
of enormous significance. T h e frightening success in recent history
of the conscious and ofttime malicious creation and exploitation
of myths gives us cause for concern about this aspect of mental life.
This, and many other related subjects, cannot be discussed in so
small a contribution to so grand a subject. What I have tried to do
is to indicate, with this limited introductory study, the great
promise which the proper use of psychoanalytic knowledge holds
for the understanding and perhaps for the mastering of the myth,
one of the most significant manifestations of the human spirit.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
392 JACOB A. ARLOW
7. Beres, D. Perception, imagination and reality. Int. J. Psychoanal., 41:327-334,
1960.
8. Boas, E. Race, Language and Culture. New York: Macmillan. 1940. [Cited by
Chase (13).]
9. Donaparte. hI. Myths of War. London: Imago, 1947.
10. Bruner, J. S. hfyths and identity. In: Myth and Afythrnaking, ed. H. A. hfurray.
New York: George Braziller, 1960.
11. Campbell, J, Marks of God. New York: Viking Press, 1959.
12. Cassirer, E. Language and Myth, tr. S . K. Langer. New York: Dover, 1946.
13. Chase, R. Quest for M y t h . Baton Rouge: Louisiana State University Press, 1949.
14. Coolidge. 0. Greek Myths. Boston: Houghton M a i n , 1949.
15. Dorson, R. hf. Theories of myths and the folklorist. In: Myths and Afythmnk.
ing, ed. H. A. Murray. New York: George Bnziller, 1960.
16. Erikson, E. H. T h e problem of ego identity. T h i s Journal, 4:56-121, 1956.
17. Fisher, C. Dreams and perception: the role of preconscious and primary modes
of perception in dream formation. T h i s Journal, 2:389-445, 1954.
18. Fisher, C. Dreams, images. and perception: a study of unconscious-precon-
scious relations. This Journal, 4:5-47, 1956.
19. Freud, A. Psychoanalysis and the training of the young child. Psychoanal.
Quart., 4:15-24, 1935.
20. Freud. S. Three essays on the theory of sexuality (1905). Standard Edition,
7:125-245. London: Hogarth Press, 1953.
21. Freud, S. T h e relation of the poet to day-dreamipg (1908). Collected Papers,
4:173-183. London: H o g r t h Press, 1934.
22. Freud, S. Contribution to the psychology of love: B special type of choice of
objects made by men (1910). Collected Papers, 4:192-202. London: Hogarth
Press, 1934.
23. Freud, S. Formulations regarding the two principles in mental functioning
(1911). Collected Papers, 4:13-21. London: Hogarth Press, 1934.
24. Freud, S. T h e occurrence in dreams of material from fairy-tales (1913). Col-
lected Papers, 4:236-243. London: Hogarth Press, 1934.
25. Freud, S. T h e unconscious (1915). Collected Papers, 4:98-136. London: Hogarth
Press, 1934.
26. Freud, S. hlourning and melancholia (1917). Collected Papers, 4:152-170. Lon-
don: Hogarth Press, 1934.
27. Freud, S. Group Psychology and the Analysis of the Ego (1921). London:
Hogarth Press, 19-10.
25. Freud, S. A note upon the “mystic writing-pad” (1925). Collected Papers, 5:175-
180. London: Hogarth Press, 1950.
29. Freud, S. T h e acquisition of poiver over fire (1932). Collected Papers, 5:288-294.
London: Hogarth Press. 1950.
30. Freud, S. Moses and Ahnotheism (1937-1939). London: Hogarth Press, 1939.
31. Graves, R. T h e Greek Afyths. New York: George Braziller, 1997.
32. Hamilton, E. Afythology. New York: Little, Brown, 1940.
33. Hartmann. H. Technical implications of ego psychology. Psychontlal. Quart.,
20:31-43, 1951.
34. Jones, E. On the Nightmare (1910-1912). London: Hogarth Press, 1949.
35. Jones, E. On “dying together.” With special reference to Heinrich von Kleist’s
suicide (1911). Essays o n Applied Psychoanalysis, 1:9-15. London: Hogarth Press,
1951.
36. Jones, E. An unusual use of “dying together” (1912). Essays on Applied Psy-
choanalysis, 1:16-21. London: Hogarth Press, 1951.
37. Jones, E. T h e madonna’s conception through the ear (1914). E~sayson Applied
Psychoannlysis, 2:266-357. London: Hogarth Press, 1951.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015
EGO PSYCHOLOGY AND MYTHOLOGY 393
38. Kluckhohn, C. Recurrent themes. In: Myths and Mythmaking, ed. H. A.
Murray. New York: George Braziller, 1960.
39. Kris, E. Ego psychology and interpretation in psychoanalytic therapy. Psycho-
anal. Quart., 20:15-30. 1951.
H). Rris, E. Psychoanalytic Explorations in Art. New York: International Univer-
sities Press, 1952.
41. Kris, E. T h e personal myth: a problem i n psychoanalytic technique. This
journal, 4:653-681. 1956.
42. Leach, E. Levi-Strauss in the Garden of Eden: an examination of some recent
developments i n the analysis of myth. Trans. N.Y. Acad. Sci., Series 2, 23:386-
396, 1961.
43. Levi-Strauss, C. T h e structural study of myth. In: Myth: A Symposium, ed.
T. A. Sebock [Biographical and Special Series of the American Folklore Society,
Vol. 51. Bloomington, Ind.: Indiana University Press, 1955.
#. Loewenstein, R. A f . T h e problem of interpretation. Psychoanal. Q w r t . , 20:l-
14, 1951.
45. Loewenstein, R. M. Ego development and psychoanalytic technique. A m . J.
Psychiat., 107:617-622, 1951.
6.Lubin, A. J. A feminine Moses: a bridge between childhood identification and
adult identity. Int. I. Ps)choanal., 39:535-546, 1958.
47. Lubin, A. J. A boy's view of Jesus. The Psychoanalytic Study of f h e Child,
14: 155-168. New York: International Universities Press, 1959.
48. Malinowski, B. Myth in Primitive Psychology. London: Regan Paul, 1926.
49. Murray, H. A., ed. Myth and Mythmaking. New York: George Braziller, 1960.
50. Niedcrland, IV. G. Jacob's dream. With some remarks on ladder and river
symbolism. J . Hillside Hosp., 3:73-97. 1954.
51. Potzl. 0.T h e relationship between experimentally induced dream images and
indirect vision (1917). In: Preconscious Stimulation in Dreams, Associations,
and Images [Psychological Issues, 2 (3). No. 71. Neiv York: International Univer-
sities Press, 1960.
52. Rank. 0. The Myth of the Birth of the Hero (1909). New York: Robert Brun-
ner, 1952.
53. Rank, 0. Art and f h e Artist. New York: Tudor, 1932.
54. Reich, A. Narcissistic object choice in women. This Journal, 122-44, 1953.
55. Reich, A. P a t h o l o g i d forms of self-esteem regulation. T h e Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child, 15:215-232. New York: International Universities Press, 1960.
56. Reider. N. Reconstruction and screen function. This Journal, 1:389-405, 1953.
57. Reik. T. Ritual (1919). Neiv York: International Universities Press, 1958.
58. Reik, T. Afyth and G u i l f . Neiv York: George Braziller, 1957.
59. Reik, T.Afystery on the Mountain. New York: Harper, 1959.
60. Rbheim, G. Psychoanalysis and Anthropology. New York: International Uni-
yersities Press, 1950.
61. R6heim, G. T h e oedipus complcu, magic and culture. In: Psychoanalysis and
the Social Sciences, 2173-228. New York: International universities Press, 1950.
62. Sachs, H. T h e community of daydreams (1920). In: T h e Creative Unconscious.
Cambridge, Mass.: Sci-Art Publishers, 1942.
63. Schlesinger, H. J. A contribution to a theory of promising. Paper presented a t
the American Psychoanalytic Association. Chicago, May, 1961.
64. Schmideberg, hi. After the analysis. Psychoanal. Quart., 7: 122-142, 1938.
65. Stein, hi. H. Premonition as a defense. Psychoanal. Quart., 2269-74, 1953.
66. Stein, M. H. Trauma and dream. Paper presented before the h'ew York Psycho-
analytic Society, February 28. 1961.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at The University of Auckland Library on March 18, 2015