EBP

You are on page 1of 33

What is Evidence-Based Practice (EBP)?

The most common definition of Evidence-Based Practice (EBP) is from Dr. David Sackett. EBP is
“the conscientious, explicit and judicious use of current best evidence in making decisions about the
care of the individual patient. It means integrating individual clinical expertise with the best available
external clinical evidence from systematic research.” (Sackett D, 1996)EBP is the integration of
clinical expertise, patient values, and the best research evidence into the decision making process for
patient care. Clinical expertise refers to the clinician’s cumulated experience, education and clinical
skills. The patient brings to the encounter his or her own personal preferences and unique concerns,
expectations, and values. The best research evidence is usually found in clinically relevant research
that has been conducted using sound methodology. (Sackett D, 2002)

The evidence, by itself, does not make the decision, but it can help support the patient care process.
The full integration of these three components into clinical decisions enhances the opportunity for
optimal clinical outcomes and quality of life. The practice of EBP is usually triggered by patient
encounters which generate questions about the effects of therapy, the utility of diagnostic tests, the
prognosis of diseases, and/or the etiology of disorders.

Evidence-Based Practice requires new skills of the clinician, including efficient literature searching,
and the application of formal rules of evidence in evaluating the clinical literature.

The Steps in the EBP Process:

ASSESS 1. Start with the patient -- a clinical problem or question


the patient arises from the care of the patient

ASK 2. Construct a well built clinical question derived from the


the question case
ACQUIRE 3. Select the appropriate resource(s) and conduct a search
the evidence
APPRAISE 4. Appraise that evidence for its validity (closeness to the
the evidence truth) and applicability (usefulness in clinical practice)
APPLY: 5. Return to the patient -- integrate that evidence with
talk with the patient clinical expertise, patient preferences and apply it to
practice
Self-evaluation 6. Evaluate your performance with this patient
ASK

ASSESS 1. Start with the patient -- a clinical


the patient problem or question arises from the care
of the patient
ASK 2. Construct a well built clinical question
the question derived from the case

OUR CASE EXAMPLE:

The patient is a 65 year old male with a long history of type 2 diabetes
and obesity. Otherwise his medical history is unremarkable. He does
not smoke. He had knee surgery 10 years ago but otherwise has had no
other major medical problems. Over the years he has tried numerous
diets and exercise programs to reduce his weight but has not been very
successful. His granddaughter just started high school and he wants to
see her graduate and go on to college. He understands that his diabetes
puts him at a high risk for heart disease and is frustrated that he cannot
lose the necessary weight. His neighbor told him about a colleague at
work who had his stomach stapled and as a result not only lost over 100
lbs. but also "cured" his diabetes. He wants to know if this procedure
really works.

The next step in this process is to take the identified concern or problem and construct a question that
is relevant to the case and is phrased in such a way as to facilitate finding an answer.

Anatomy of a good clinical question: PICO

PICO is a mnemonic that helps one remember the key components of a well focused question. The
question needs to identify the key problem of the patient, what treatment or tests you are considering
for the patient, what alternative treatment or tests are being considered (if any) and what is the
desired outcome to promote or avoid.

P= Patient Problem: How would you describe a group of patients similar to yours? What are the
most important characteristics of the patient? This may include the primary problem, disease, or co-
existing conditions. Sometimes the gender, age or race of a patient might be relevant to the diagnosis
or treatment of a disease.
I= Intervention, prognostic factor or exposure: Which main intervention, prognostic factor, or
exposure are you considering? What do you want to do for the patient? Prescribe a drug? Order a
test? Order surgery? Or what factor may influence the prognosis of the patient - age, co-existing
problems, or previous exposure?

C= Comparison: What is the main alternative to compare with the intervention? Are you trying to
decide between two drugs, a drug and no medication or placebo, or two diagnostic tests? Your
clinical question may not always have a specific comparison.

O= Outcome: What can you hope to accomplish, measure, improve or affect? What are you trying
to do for the patient? Relieve or eliminate the symptoms? Reduce the number of adverse events?
Improve function or test scores?

OUR CASE:
Patient Problem obese, diabetes type 2, male
Intervention stomach stapling (gastric bypass surgery;
bariatric surgery)
Comparison standard medical care
Outcome remission of diabetes; weight loss;
mortality
For our patient, the clinical question might
be: In patients with type 2 diabetes and
obesity, is bariatric surgery more effective
than standard medical therapy at
increasing the probability of remission of
diabetes?

Two additional elements of the well-built


clinical question are the type of question and
the type of study. This information can be
helpful in focusing the question and
determining the most appropriate type of
evidence or study.

Type of Question

Two additional elements of the well-built clinical question are the type of
question and the type of study. This information can be helpful in focusing the
question and determining the most appropriate type of evidence or study.
The type of question is important and can help lead you to the best
study design:

Most common type of Type of study:


questions:

Diagnosis
prospective, blind comparison to a gold
how to select and interpret
standard or cross-sectional
diagnostic tests

Therapy
how to select treatments that do
more good than harm and that randomized controlled trial > cohort study
are worth the efforts and costs of
using them

Prognosis
how to estimate the patient’s
likely clinical course over time
cohort study > case control > case series
(based on factors other than the
intervention) and anticipate likely
complications of disease

Harm/Etiology
how to identify causes for disease cohort > case control > case series
(including iatrogenic forms)

Type of Study
As you move up the pyramid the study designs are more rigorous and allow for
less bias or systematic error that may distract you from the truth.

Case series and Case reports consist of collections of reports on the


treatment of individual patients or a report on a single patient. Because they
are reports of cases and use no control groups to compare outcomes, they have
little statistical validity.

Case control studies are studies in which patients who already have a specific
condition are compared with people who do not have the condition. The
researcher looks back to identify factors or exposures that might be associated
with the illness. They often rely on medical records and patient recall for data
collection. These types of studies are often less reliable than randomized
controlled trials and cohort studies because showing a statistical relationship
does not mean than one factor necessarily caused the other.

Cohort studies identify a group of patients who are already taking a particular
treatment or have an exposure, follow them forward over time, and then
compare their outcomes with a similar group that has not been affected by the
treatment or exposure being studied. Cohort studies are observational and not
as reliable as randomized controlled studies, since the two groups may differ in
ways other than in the variable under study.

Randomized controlled clinical trials are carefully planned experiments that


introduce a treatment or exposure to study its effect on real patients. They
include methodologies that reduce the potential for bias (randomization and
blinding) and that allow for comparison between intervention groups and control
(no intervention) groups. A randomized controlled trial is a planned experiment
and can provide sound evidence of cause and effect.

Systematic Reviews focus on a clinical topic and answer a specific question.


An extensive literature search is conducted to identify studies with sound
methodology. The studies are reviewed, assessed for quality, and the results
summarized according to the predetermined criteria of the review question.

A Meta-analysis will thoroughly examine a number of valid studies on a topic


and mathematically combine the results using accepted statistical methodology
to report the results as if it were one large study.

Cross-sectional studies describe the relationship between diseases and other


factors at one point in time in a defined population. Cross sectional studies lack
any information on timing of exposure and outcome relationships and include
only prevalent cases. They are often used for comparing diagnostic
tests. Studies that show the efficacy of a diagnostic test are also
called prospective, blind comparison to a gold standard study. This is a
controlled trial that looks at patients with varying degrees of an illness and
administers both diagnostic tests — the test under investigation and the “gold
standard” test — to all of the patients in the study group. The sensitivity and
specificity of the new test are compared to that of the gold standard to
determine potential usefulness.

Qualitative Research answers a wide variety of questions related to human


responses to actual or potential health problems.The purpose of qualitative
research is to describe, explore and explain the health-related phenomena
being studied.

Retrospective cohort (or historical cohort) follows the same direction of


inquiry as a cohort study. Subjects begin with the presence or absence of an
exposure or risk factor and are followed until the outcome of interest is
observed. However, this study design uses information that has been collected
in the past and kept in files or databases. Patients are identified for exposure
or non-exposures and the data is followed forward to an effect or outcome of
interest.
For our patient, the clinical question is:

In patients with type 2 diabetes and obesity, is bariatric surgery more effective t
probability of remission of diabetes?

AQUIRING THE EVIDENCE

ACQUIRE 3. Select the appropriate resource(s) and conduct a search


the evidence

In the previous section, we learned how to construct a well-built clinical question. Using that
question, we will move on to the literature search.

For our patient, the clinical question is:

In patients with type 2 diabetes and obesity, is bariatric surgery more effective than
standard medical therapy at increasing the probability of remission of diabetes? It is a
therapy question and the best evidence would be a randomized controlled trial (RCT).
If we found numerous RCTs, then we might want to look for a systematic review.

Constructing a well-built clinical question can lead directly to a well-built search strategy. Note that
you may not use all the information in PICO or well-built clinical question in your MEDLINE
strategy. In the following example we did not use the term “male.” We also did not include the
word therapy. Instead we used the Clinical Query for Therapy or the publication type, randomized
controlled trial, to get at the concept of treatment. However, you may consider the issue of gender
later when you review the articles for applicability to your patient.

PICO Clinical Question Search Strategy

Patient / Problem obese, diabetes type 2, male


diabetes type 2, obesity

stomach stapling (gastric bypass


Intervention bariatric surgery
surgery; bariatric surgery)
Comparison (if any) standard medical care

remission of diabetes; weight


Outcome
loss; mortality

Type of Question therapy (see below)

Clinical Query – Therapy/narrow


or
Type of Study RCT
Limit to randomized controlled
trial as publication type

Evaluating the Validity of a Therapy Study

We have now identified current information which can answer our clinical question. The next step is
to read the article and evaluate the study. There are three basic questions that need to be answered for
every type of study:

 Are the results of the study valid?


 What are the results?
 Will the results help in caring for my patient?

This tutorial will focus on the first question: are the results of the study valid? The issue of validity
speaks to the "truthfulness" of the information. The validity criteria should be applied before an
extensive analysis of the study data. If the study is not valid, the data may not be useful.

The evidence that supports the validity or truthfulness of the information is found primarily in the
study methodology. Here is where the investigators address the issue of bias, both conscious and
unconscious. Study methodologies such as randomization, blinding and follow-up of patients help
insure that the study results are not overly influenced by the investigators or the patients.

Evaluating the medical literature is a complex undertaking. This session will provide you with some
basic criteria and information to consider when trying to decide if the study methodology is sound.
You will find that the answers to the questions of validity may not always be clearly stated in the
article and that readers will have to make their own judgments about the importance of each
question.

Once you have determined that the study methodology is valid, you must examine the results and
their applicability to the patient. Clinicians may have additional concerns such as whether the study
represented patients similar to his/her patients, whether the study covered the aspect of the problem
that is most important to the patient, or whether the study suggested a clear and useful plan of action.

Note: The questions that we used to test the validity of the evidence are adapted from work done at
McMaster University. See the References/Glossary unit: 'Users' Guides to the Medical Literature.
Read the following article and determine if it meets the validity criteria. (Click on title to access
free full text).

Mingrone G. Bariatric surgery versus conventional medical therapy for type 2 diabetes. N Engl J
Med. 2012 Apr 26;366(17):1577-85. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1200111. Epub 2012 Mar 26. PubMed
PMID:22449317.

1. Were patients randomized? The assignment of patients to either group (treatment or control)
must be done by a random allocation. This might include a coin toss (heads to treatment/tails to
control) or use of randomization tables, often computer generated. Research has shown that random
allocation comes closest to insuring the creation of groups of patients who will be similar in their risk
of the events you hope to prevent. Randomization balances the groups for known prognostic factors
(such as age, weight, gender, etc.) and unknown prognostic factors (such as compliance, genetics,
socioeconomics, etc.). This reduces the chance of over-representation of any one characteristic
within the study groups.

2. Was group allocation concealed? The randomization sequence should be concealed from the
clinicians and researchers of the study to further eliminate conscious or unconscious selection bias.
Concealment (part of the enrollment process) ensures that the researchers cannot predict or change
the assignments of patients to treatment groups. If allocation is not concealed it may be possible to
influence the outcome (consciously or unconsciously) by changing the enrollment order or the order
of treatment which has been randomly assigned. Concealed allocation can be done by using a remote
call center for enrolling patients or the use of opaque envelopes with assignments. This is different
from blinding which happens AFTER randomization.

3. Were patients in the study groups similar with respect to known prognostic variables? The
treatment and the control group should be similar for all prognostic characteristics except whether or
not they received the experimental treatment. This information is usually displayed in Table 1, which
outlines the baseline characteristics of both groups. This is a good way to verify that randomization
resulted in similar groups.

4. To what extent was the study blinded? Blinding means that the people involved in the study do
not know which treatments were given to which patients. Patients, researchers, data collectors and
others involved in the study should not know which treatment is being administered. This helps
eliminate assessment bias and preconceived notions as to how the treatments should be working.
When it is difficult or even unethical to blind patients to a treatment, such as a surgical procedure,
then a "blinded" clinician or researcher is needed to interpret the results.

5. Was follow-up complete? The study should begin and end with the same number of patients in
each group. Patients lost to the study must be accounted for or risk making the conclusions invalid.
Patients may drop out because of the adverse effects of the therapy being tested. If not accounted for,
this can lead to conclusions that may be overly confident in the efficacy of the therapy. Good studies
will have better than 80% follow-up for their patients. When there is a large loss to follow-up, the
lost patients should be assigned to the "worst-case" outcomes and the results recalculated. If these
results still support the original conclusion of the study then the loss may be acceptable.
6. Were patients analyzed in the groups to which they were first allocated? Anything that
happens after randomization can affect the chances that a patient in a study has an event. Patients
who forget or refuse their treatment should not be eliminated from the study results or allowed to
“change groups”. Excluding noncompliant patients from a study group may leave only those that
may be more likely to have a positive outcome, thus compromising the unbiased comparison that we
got from the process of randomization. Therefore all patients must be analyzed within their assigned
group. Randomization must be preserved. This is called "intention to treat" analysis.

7. Aside from the experimental intervention, were the groups treated equally? Both groups
must be treated the same except for administration of the experimental treatment. If
"cointerventions" (interventions other than the study treatment which are applied differently to both
groups) exist they must be described in the methods section of the study.

Evaluating the Validity of a Diagnostic Test Study

Are the results valid?

1. Did participating patients present a diagnostic dilemma?

The group of patients in which the test was conducted should include patients with a high, medium
and low probability of having the target disease. The clinical usefulness of a test is demonstrated in
its ability to distinguish between obvious illness and those cases where it is not so obvious or where
the diagnosis might otherwise be confused. The patients in the study should resemble what might be
expected in a clinical practice.

2. Did investigators compare the test to an appropriate, independent reference standard?

The reference (or gold) standard refers to the commonly accepted proof that the target disorder is
present or not present. The reference standard might be an autopsy or biopsy. The reference standard
provides objective criteria (e.g., laboratory test not requiring subjective interpretation) or a current
clinical standard (e.g., a venogram for deep venous thrombosis) for diagnosis. Sometimes there may
not be a widely accepted reference standard. The author will then need to clearly justify their
selection of the reference test.

3. Were those interpreting the test and reference standard blind to the other results?

To avoid potential bias, those conducting the test should not know or be aware of the results of the
other test.

4. Did the investigators perform the same reference standard to all patients regardless of the
results of the test under investigation?
Researchers should conduct both tests (the study test and the reference standard) on all patients in the
study regardless of the results of the test in question. Researchers should not be tempted to forego
either test based on the results of only one of the tests. Nor should the researchers apply a different
reference standard to patients with a negative results in the study test.

Key issues for Diagnostic Studies:

 diagnostic uncertainty
 blind comparison to gold standard
 each patient gets both tests

What are the results?


Reference Standard
Reference Standard
Disease Negative
Disease Positive

Study Test
True Positive False Positive
Positive

Study Test
False negative True Negative
Negative

Sensitivity: = true positive / all disease positives

measures the proportion of patients with the disease who also test positive for the disease in this
study. It is the probability that a person with the disease will have a positive test result.

Specificity: Specificity = true negative / all disease negatives

measures the proportion of patients without the disease who also test negative for the disease in this
study. It is the probability that a person without the disease will have a negative test result.

Sensitivity and specificity are characteristics of the test but do not provide enough information for
the clinician to act on the test results. Likelihood ratios can be used to help adapt the results of a
study to specific patients. They help determine the probability of disease in a patient.

Likelihood ratios (LR):

LR + = positive test in patients with disease / positive test in patients without disease

LR - = negative test in patients with disease / negative test in patients without disease
Likelihood ratios indicate the likelihood that a given test result would be expected in a patient with
the target disorder compared to the likelihood that the same result would be expected in a patient
without that disorder.

Likelihood ratio of a positive test result (LR+) increases the odds of having the disease after a
positive test result.

Likelihood ratio of a negative test result (LR-) decreases the odds of having the disease after a
negative test result.

How much do LRs change disease likelihood?

LRs greater than 10 or less than 0.1 cause large changes

LRs 5 – 10 or 0.1 – 0.2 cause moderate changes

LRs 2 – 5 or 0.2 – 0.5 cause small changes

LRs less than 2 or greater than 0.5 cause tiny changes

LRs = 1.0 cause no change at all

Want to know more about nomograms? See How to use a nomogram (pretest-probability) with a
likelihood ratio

More about likelihood ratios: Diagnostic tests 4: likelihood ratios. JJ Deeks & Douglas G Altman
BMJ 2004 329:168-169

How can I apply the results to patient care?

Will the reproducibility of the test result and its interpretation be satisfactory in your clinical
setting? Does the test yield the same result when reapplied to stable participants? Do different
observers agree about the test results?

Are the study results applicable to the patients in your practice? Does the test perform
differently (different LRs) for different severities of disease? Does the test perform differently for
populations with different mixes of competing conditions?

Will the test results change your management strategy? What are the test and treatment
thresholds for the health condition to be detected? Are the test LRs high or low enough to shift
posttest probability across a test or treatment threshold?
Will patients be better off as a result of the test?Will patient care differ for different test results?
Will the anticipated changes in care do more good than harm?

Based on: Guyatt, G. Rennie, D. Meade, MO, Cook, DJ. Users’ Guide to Medical Literature: A
Manual for Evidence-Based Clinical Practice, 2nd Edition 2008.

Evaluating the Validity of a Prognosis Study

Are the results Valid?

1. Was the sample of patients representative?

The patients groups should be clearly defined and representative of the spectrum of disease found in
most practices. Failure to clearly define the patients who entered the study increases the risk that the
sample is unrepresentative. To help you decide about the appropriateness of the sample, look for a
clear description of which patients were included and excluded from a study. The way the sample
was selected should be clearly specified, along with the objective criteria used to diagnose the
patients with the disorder.

2. Were the patients sufficiently homogeneous with respect to prognostic factors?

Prognostic factors are characteristics of a particular patient that can be used to more accurately
predict the course of a disease. These factors, which can be demographic (age, gender, race, etc.) or
disease specific (e.g., stage of a tumor or disease) or comorbid (other conditions existing in the
patient at the same time), can also help predict good or bad outcomes.

In comparing the prognosis of the 2 study groups, researchers should consider whether or not the
patient’s clinical characteristics are similar. It may be that adjustments have to made based on
prognostic factors to get a true picture of the clinical outcome. This may require clinical experience
or knowledge of the underlying biology to determine if all relevant factors were considered.

3. Was the follow-up sufficiently complete?

Follow-up should be complete and all patients accounted for at the end of the study. Patients who are
lost to follow-up may often suffer the adverse outcome of interest and therefore, if not accounted for,
may bias the results of the study. Determining if the number of patients lost to follow up affects the
validity depends on the proportion of patients lost and the proportion of patients suffering the
adverse outcome.
Patients should be followed until they fully recover or one of the disease outcomes occur. The
follow-up should be long enough to develop a valid picture of the extent of the outcome of interest.
Follow-up should include at least 80% of participants until the occurrence of a major study end point
or to the end of the study.

4. Were objective and unbiased outcome criteria used?

Some outcomes are clearly defined, such as death or full recovery. In between, can exist a wide
range of outcomes that may be less clearly defined. Investigators should establish specific criteria
that define each possible outcome of the disease and use these same criteria during patient follow-up.
Investigators making judgments about the clinical outcomes may have to be “blinded” to the patient
characteristics and prognostic factors in order to eliminate possible bias in their observations.

Key issues for Prognosis Studies:

 well-defined sample
 similar prognosis
 follow-up complete
 objective and unbias outcome criteria

What are the results?

How likely are the outcomes over time?

 What are the event rates at different points in time?


 If event rates vary with time, are the results shown using a survival curve?

How precise are the estimates of likelihood?

 What is the confident interval for the principle event rate?


 How do confidence intervals change over time?

Prognostic Results are the numbers of events that occur over time, expressed in:

 absolute terms: e.g. 5 year survival rate


 relative terms: e.g. risk from prognostic factor
 survival curves: cumulative events over time
Evaluating the Validity of a Harm Study

Are the results of this article valid?

FOR COHORT STUDIES: Aside from the exposure of interest, did the exposed and control
groups start and finish with the same risk for the outcome?

1. Were patients similar for prognostic factors that are known to be associated with the
outcome (or did statistical adjustment level the playing field)?

The two groups, those exposed to the harm and those not exposed, must begin with the same
prognosis. The characteristics of the exposed and non-exposed patients need to be carefully
documented and their similarity (except for the exposure) needs to be demonstrated. The choice of
comparison groups has a significant influence on the credibility of the study results. The researchers
should identify an appropriate control population before making a strong inference about a harmful
agent. The two groups should have the same baseline characteristics. If there are differences
investigators should use statistical techniques to adjust or correct for differences.

2. Were the circumstances and methods for detecting the outcome similar?

In cohort studies determination of the outcome is critical. It is important to define the outcome and
use objective measures to avoid possible bias. Detection bias may be an issue for these studies, as
unblinded researchers may look deeper to detect disease or an outcome.

3. Was follow-up sufficiently complete?

Patients unavailable for complete follow-up may compromise the validity of the research because
often these patients have very different outcomes than those that stayed with the study. This
information must be factored into the study results.

FOR CASE CONTROL STUDIES: Did the cases and control group have the same risk
(chance) of being exposed in the past?
1. Were cases and controls similar with respect to the indication or circumstances that would
lead to exposure?

The characteristics of the cases and controls need to be carefully documented and their similarity
needs to be demonstrated. The choice of comparison groups has a significant influence on the
credibility of the study results. The researchers should identify an appropriate control population that
would be eligible or likely to have the same exposure as the cases.

2. Were the circumstances and methods for determining exposure similar for cases and
controls?
In a case control study determination of the exposure is critical. The exposure in the two groups
should be identified by the same method. The identification should avoid any kind of bias, such as
recall bias. Sometimes using objective data, such as medical records, or blinding the interviewer can
help eliminate bias.

Key issues for Harm Studies:

 similarity of comparison groups


 outcomes and exposures measured same for both
groups
 follow-up of sufficient length (80% or better)

What are the results?

How strong is the association between exposure and outcome?


* What is the risk ratio or odds ratio?
* Is there a dose-response relationship between exposure and outcome?

How precise was the estimate of the risk?


* What is the confidence interval for the relative risk or odds ratio?

Strength of inference:

For RCT or Prospective cohort studies: Relative Risk

Cases Controls

Outcome present Outcome not present


Exposure Yes a b

Exposure No c d
Relative Risk (RR) = a /(a + b) / c/(c + d)
is the risk of the outcome in the exposed group divided by the risk of the outcome in the unexposed
group:

RR = (exposed outcome yes / all exposed) / (not exposed outcome yes / all not exposed)

Example: “RR of 3.0 means that the outcome occurs 3 times more often in those exposed versus
unexposed.”

For case-control or retrospective studies: Odds Ratio

Cases Controls

Outcome present Outcome not present


Exposure Yes a b

Exposure No c d

Odds Ratio (OR) = (a / c) / (b / d)


is the odds of previous exposure in a case divided by the odds of exposure in a control patient:

OR = (exposed - outcome yes / not exposed - outcome yes) / (exposed - outcome no / not exposed -
outcome no)

Example: “OR of 3.0 means that cases were 3 times more likely to have been exposed than were
control patients.”

Confidence Intervals are a measure of the precision of the results of a study. For example, “36
[95% CI 27-51]“, a 95%CI range means that if you were to repeat the same clinical trial a hundred
times you can be sure that 95% of the time the results would fall within the calculated range of 27-
51. Wider intervals indicate lower precision; narrow intervals show greater precision.

Confounding Variable is one whose influence distorts the true relationship between a potential risk
factor and the clinical outcome of interest.

Read more on odds ratios: The odds ratio Douglas G Altman & J Martin Bland BMJ 2000;320:1468
(27 May)

Watch more on odds ratios: Understanding odds ratio with Gordon Guyatt. (21 minutes.)

How can I apply the results to patient care?

Were the study subjects similar to your patients or population? Is your patient so different from
those included in the study that the results may not apply?
Was the follow-up sufficiently long? Were study participants followed-up long enough for
important harmful effects to be detected?

Is the exposure similar to what might occur in your patient? Are there important differences in
exposures (dose, duration, etc) for your patients?

What is the magnitude of the risk? What level of baseline risk for the harm is amplified by the
exposure studied?

Are there any benefits known to be associated with the exposure? What is the balance between
benefits and harms for patients like yours?

Source: Guyatt, G. Rennie, D. Meade, MO, Cook, DJ. Users’ Guide to Medical Literature: A
Manual for Evidence-Based Clinical Practice, 2nd Edition 2008.

Evaluating the validity of a Systematic Review

Are the results of this article valid?

1. Did the review explicitly address a sensible question?

The systematic review should address a specific question that indicates the patient problem, the
exposure and one or more outcomes. General reviews, which usually do not address specific
questions, may be too broad to provide an answer to the clinical question for which you are seeking
information.

2. Was the search for relevant studies detailed and exhaustive?

Researchers should conduct a thorough search of appropriate bibliographic databases. The databases
and search strategies should be outlined in the methodology section. Researchers should also show
evidence of searching for non-published evidence by contacting experts in the field. Cited references
at the end of articles should also be checked.

3. Were the primary studies of high methodological quality?

Researchers should evaluate the validity of each study included in the systematic review. The same
EBP criteria used to critically appraise studies should be used to evaluate studies to be included in
the systematic review. Differences in study results may be explained by differences in methodology
and study design.

4. Were selection and assessments of the included studies reproducible?


More than one researcher should evaluate each study and make decisions about its validity and
inclusion. Bias (systematic errors) and mistakes (random errors) can be avoided when judgment is
shared. A third reviewer should be available to break a tie vote.

Key issues for Systematic Reviews:

 focused question
 thorough literature search
 include validated studies
 selection of studies reproducible

What are the results?

Were the results similar from study to study?


How similar were the point estimates?
Do confidence intervals overlap between studies?

What are the overall results of the review?


Were results weighted both quantitatively and qualitatively in summary estimates?

How precise were the results?


What is the confidence interval for the summary or cumulative effect size?

More information on reading forest plots:

Deciphering a forest plot for a systematic review/meta-analysis (UNC)

Ried K. Interpreting and understanding meta-analysis graphs: a practical


guide. Aust Fam Physician. 2006 Aug;35(8):635-8. PubMed PMID: 16894442.

Greenhalgh T. Papers that summarise other papers (systematic


reviews and meta-analyses). BMJ. 1997 Sep 13;315(7109):672-5.
PubMed PMID: 9310574.

How can I apply the results to patient care?

Were all patient-important outcomes considered?


Did the review omit outcomes that could change decisions?

Are any postulated subgroup effects credible?


Were subgroup differences postulated before data analysis?
Were subgroup differences consistent across studies?

What is the overall quality of the evidence?


Were prevailing study design, size, and conduct reflected in a summary of the quality of evidence?
Are the benefits worth the costs and potential risks?
Does the cumulative effect size cross a test or therapeutic threshold?

Based on: Guyatt, G. Rennie, D. Meade, MO, Cook, DJ. Users’ Guide to Medical Literature: A
Manual for Evidence-Based Clinical Practice, 2nd Edition 2008.

PRACTICE CASE 1 (PROBIOTICS)

This is a self-correcting exercise. Click on the letter (A, B, or C) or "Quiz


Yourself" for a pop-up box with the response.

Note: If you do not see a pop-up box, check your pop-up blocker settings in
your browser. If that is not the problem, try closing the previous pop-up box or
check to see if the pop-up box is behind your browser window. If you still do
not see the pop-up box, you may need to update your browser.

ASSESS the patient

Start with the patient -- a clinical problem or question arises from the care of
the patient
Ms. MP is an 86 year old highly-functional
resident of Croasdaile who presents to the
emergency room after a fall in her
home. Workup identifies a right femur
fracture. The orthopaedic surgeon
recommends urgent repair and perioperative
antibiotics. Ms. MP, who with her husband
operated a health food store for 40 years,
has spent her life avoiding medications and
using natural remedies. Her husband
mentions concern over the possibility of
diarrhea as a side effect of the antibiotics
and asks if using fermented milk with
lactobacillus or some other probiotic product
would prevent this side effect. Should she
start taking a form of probiotic to prevent
this problem?
ASK the question

Choose the BEST clinical question:

What is the best treatment for diarrhea?

In patients taking antibiotics, are probiotics helpful in preventing diarrhea?

Do probiotics prevent diarrhea in the elderly hospitalized patient?

ACQUIRE the evidence

Choose the best PubMed search strategy to address the clinical


question.

Search: diarrhea AND best treatment Limited to Free Full Text

Search: diarrhea AND probiotics AND elderly AND female AND adult Limited to
English

Search: antibiotics AND diarrhea AND probiotics Limited to randomized


controlled trial
APPRAISE the evidence

Read the following article and determine if it meets the validity


criteria. (Click on title to access free full text)

Beausoleil M. Effect of a fermented milk combining Lactobacillus


acidophilus Cl1285 and Lactobacillus casei in the prevention of
antibiotic-associated diarrhea: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-
controlled trial. Can J Gastroenterol. 2007 Nov;21(11):732-6. PMCID:
PMC2658588 PMID: 18026577

ABSTRACT:

BACKGROUND: Antibiotic-associated diarrhea is an important problem


in hospitalized patients. The use of probiotics is gaining interest in the scientific
community as a potential measure to prevent this complication. The main objective
of the present study was to assess the efficacy and safety of a fermented milk
combining Lactobacillus acidophilus and Lactobacillus casei that is widely available
in Canada, in the prevention of antibiotic-associated diarrhea. METHODS: In this
double-blind, randomized study, hospitalized patients were randomly assigned to
receive either a lactobacilli-fermented milk or a placebo on a daily basis.
RESULTS: Among 89 randomized patients, antibiotic-associated diarrhea occurred
in seven of 44 patients (15.9%) in the lactobacilli group and in 16 of 45
patients (35.6%) in the placebo group (OR 0.34, 95% CI 0.125 to 0.944; P=0.05).
The median hospitalization duration was eight days in the lactobacilli group,
compared with 10 days in the placebo group (P=0.09). Overall, the lactobacilli-
fermented milk was well tolerated. CONCLUSION: The daily administration of a
lactobacilli-fermented milk was safe and effective in the prevention of antibiotic-
associated diarrhea in hospitalized patients.

Are the results valid?

You will need to read the methodology section of the article to address the
validity questions. Evaluating the medical literature is a complex undertaking.
You will find that the answers to the questions of validity may not always be
clearly stated in the article and that you may have to use your own judgment
about the importance and significance of each question.

Randomization: Were patients randomized?

Concealed allocation: Was group allocation concealed?

Baseline characteristics: Were patients in the study groups similar with respect
to known prognostic variables?
Blinding: To what extent was the study blinded?

Follow-up: Was follow-up complete?

Intention to Treat: Were patients analyzed in the groups to which they were
first allocated?

Equal treatment: Aside from the experimental intervention, were the groups
treated equally?

What are the results & how can I apply them to patient care?

Results: Among 89 randomized patients, antibiotic-associated diarrhea


occurred in seven of 44 patients (15.9%) in the lactobacilli group and in 16 of
45 patients (35.6%) in the placebo group (OR 0.34, 95% CI 0.125 to 0.944;
P=0.05). The median hospitalization duration was eight days in the lactobacilli
group, compared with 10 days in the placebo group (P=0.09). Overall, the
lactobacilli-fermented milk was well tolerated.

Outcome Present Outcome NOT present

ADD occurrence ADD no occurrence


lactobacilli group 7 37
placebo group 16 29

Experimental (lactobacilli) Event Rate = 7 / 44 = 15.9%

Control (placebo) Event Rate = 16 / 45 = 35.5%

Absolute Risk Reduction = 35.5% - 15.9% = 19.6%

Relative Risk reduction = 19.6% / 35.5% = 53.5%

Number Needed-to-Treat = 1/ 19.6% = 5

The results of Beausoleil and colleagues are consistent with those of a recent
systematic review showing that probiotics reduce the frequency of antibiotic-
associated diarrhea and accelerate resolution of C. difficile–associated colitis.
The study was underpowered to demonstrate an effect on more clinically
important outcomes. The intervention is simple, rational, and presumably
relatively inexpensive. Thus, it could be reasoned that it should be implemented
more widely without further study. On the other hand, there have been isolated
reports of systemic infection with organisms used as probiotics. Moreover, in
light of the potential importance of minimizing the adverse effects of systemic
antibiotics in hospitalized patients, further studies should be powered to probe
the effects of probiotics on rates of acquisition of nosocomial infections (with
both typical nosocomial pathogens and the probiotic itself) and on clinically
important outcomes, such as length of stay and nutritional status. In addition,
further work to determine which microbial species exert optimal probiotic
effects will aid in defining a role for probiotics in routine clinical practice. [ACP
Journal Club 15 July 2008 - Volume 149, Number 1)

Questions to consider before applying the results of a study to your


patient:

 Were the study patients similar to my population of interest?


 Does your patient match the study inclusion criteria? If not, are there
compelling reasons why the results should not apply to your patient?
 Were all clinically important outcomes considered?
 Are the likely treatment benefits worth the potential harm and costs?

APPLY: talk with the patient

Return to the patient -- integrate that evidence with clinical expertise, patient
preferences and apply it to practice

Think about your patient, her goals, your treatment


recommendation for her, the cost and adverse effects of
probiotics, and help her decide whether to start taking the
probiotics.
Practice Case 2: Physical therapy and the Magnetic Bracelet

This is a self-correcting exercise. Click on the letter (A, B, or C) or "Quiz


Yourself" for a pop-up box with the response.

Note: If you do not see a pop-up box, check your pop-up blocker settings in
your browser. If that is not the problem, try closing the previous pop-up box or
check to see if the pop-up box is behind your browser window. If you still do
not see the pop-up box, you may need to update your browser.

ASSESS the patient


Start with the patient -- a clinical problem or question arises from the care of
the patient

You are working in an out-patient Physical Therapy Department in a rural community in North
Carolina. Your regular patient, Ms. Baxter is here for her monthly visit for physical therapy
related to osteoarthritis of the hip. She is 65 years old, recently retired and has no
other serious medical problems. She shows up wearing a new magnetic
bracelet which her daughter sent her. Her daughter swears that this will help
the hip pain. Ms. Baxter is very skeptical and asks you if you would
recommend the bracelet for pain control. You don't know if there is any good
evidence to demonstrate the effectiveness of the magnetic bracelets but
promise to look into it before her next visit. Will this bracelet help her hip
pain?

ASK the question

Choose the BEST clinical question:

Do magnets help control pain in elderly patients?

Do magnetic bracelets help control pain in patients with osteoarthritis?

Do magnetics help control pain in patients with osteoarthritis of the hip?

ACQUIRE the evidence

Choose the best PubMed search strategy to address the clinical


question.

Search: magnets AND pain AND elderly Limited to English

Search: magnetics AND osteoarthritis AND hip Limited to randomized


controlled trial

Search: magnetics AND pain Limited to randomized controlled trial

APPRAISE the evidence

This are a couple of randomized controlled trials found in PubMed for


this topic. This one is Free in PubMed Central: Harlow T. Randomised
controlled trial of magnetic bracelets for relieving pain in osteoarthritis
of the hip and knee. BMJ. 2004 Dec 18;329(7480):1450-4. PubMed
PMID: 15604181; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC535975.
Abstract
OBJECTIVE: To determine the effectiveness of commercially available magnetic
bracelets for pain control in osteoarthritis of the hip and
knee. DESIGN: Randomised, placebo controlled trial with three parallel
groups. SETTING: Five rural general practices. PARTICIPANTS: 194 men and
women aged 45-80 years with osteoarthritis of the hip or
knee. INTERVENTION: Wearing a standard strength static bipolar magnetic
bracelet, a weak magnetic bracelet, or a non-magnetic (dummy) bracelet for 12
weeks. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Change in the Western Ontario and
McMaster Universities osteoarthritis lower limb pain scale (WOMAC A) after 12
weeks, with the primary comparison between the standard and dummy groups.
Secondary outcomes included changes in WOMAC B and C scales and a visual
analogue scale for pain. RESULTS: Mean pain scores were reduced more in the
standard magnet group than in the dummy group (mean difference 1.3 points,
95% confidence interval 0.05 to 2.55). Self reported blinding status did not
affect the results. The scores for secondary outcome measures were consistent
with the WOMAC A scores. CONCLUSION: Pain from osteoarthritis of the hip
and knee decreases when wearing magnetic bracelets. It is uncertain whether
this response is due to specific or non-specific (placebo) effects.

Are the results valid?

You will need to read the full article to address the validity questions. Click on
the link above to get a free copy of the article. Evaluating the medical literature
is a complex undertaking. You will find that the answers to the questions of
validity may not always be clearly stated in the article and that you may have
to use your own judgment about the importance and significance of each
question.

Randomization: Were patients randomized?

Concealed allocation: Was group allocation concealed?

Baseline characteristics: Were patients in the study groups similar with respect
to known prognostic variables?

Blinding: To what extent was the study blinded?

Follow-up: Was follow-up complete?

Intention to Treat: Were patients analyzed in the groups to which they were
first allocated?

Equal treatment: Aside from the experimental intervention, were the groups
treated equally?
What are the results & how can I apply them to patient care?

Results: Mean pain scores were reduced more in the standard magnet group than in the dummy group
(mean difference 1.3 points, 95% confidence interval 0.05 to 2.55). Self reported blinding status did not affect
the results. The scores for secondary outcome measures were consistent with the WOMAC A
scores. Conclusion: Pain from osteoarthritis of the hip and knee decreases when wearing
magnetic bracelets. It is uncertain whether this response is due to specific or non-specific
(placebo) effects.

The results are reported as a continuous variable (change in scores). This does not allow for
calculations of Absolute and Relative differences or Number-Needed-to-Treat.

Questions to consider before applying the results of a study to your patient:

 Were the study patients similar to my population of interest?


 Does your patient match the study inclusion criteria? If not, are there compelling
reasons why the results should not apply to your patient?
 Were all clinically important outcomes considered?
 Are the likely treatment benefits worth the potential harm and costs?

APPLY: talk with the patient

Return to the patient -- integrate that evidence from the study with clinical
expertise, patient preferences and apply it to practice

Think about your patient, her goals, the evidence you found, the adverse effects
of the magentic bracelet and what you would discuss with her at your next visit.

Practice Case 3: Nursing and the case for dry heat

This is a self-correcting exercise. Click on the letter (A, B, or C) or "Quiz


Yourself" for a pop-up box with the response.

Note: If you do not see a pop-up box, check your pop-up blocker settings in
your browser. If that is not the problem, try closing the previous pop-up box or
check to see if the pop-up box is behind your browser window. If you still do
not see the pop-up box, you may need to update your browser.
ASSESS the Problem
Start with the problem -- a clinical problem or question arises from the care of
the patients

Peripheral IV catheter insertion is a common nursing procedure often required for the administration of
chemotherapy, antibiotics, blood products, fluids, and other medical therapies in hematologic patients with cancer.
Although necessary and usually brief, IV insertion often is a source of patient anxiety and discomfort and can be
extremely difficult to achieve, particularly in individuals receiving repeated courses of chemotherapy. Unfortunately,
not all IV insertions are successful on the first attempt; multiple attempts may occur, which may cause patient
distress and anxiety and increase costs as a result of additional supplies and nursing time. Nurses currently use
various techniques, including heat, to improve the success rates of IV insertion; however, few are based on evidence.
(Fink RM, 2009) In an effort to improve the patient experience you are asked by the Nursing Council to look at the
evidence for using dry versus moist heat for IV catheterization.

ASK the question

Choose the BEST clinical question:

Is dry or moist heat more effective at reducing pain and anxiety?

Does heat help improve the time to insertion of an IV catheter?

Is dry or moist heat helpful in reducing pain and time of IV catheter insertion in
patients undergoing chemotherapy?

ACQUIRE the evidence

Choose the best PubMed search strategy to address the clinical


question.

Search: heat AND chemotherapy Limited to randomized controlled trial

Search: hyperthermia AND catheterization Limited to randomized controlled


trial

Search: heat AND IV catheter Limited to Human and English and last 5 years

APPRAISE the evidence


Fink RM. The impact of dry versus moist heat on peripheral IV catheter insertion
in a hematology-oncology outpatient
population. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2009 Jul;36(4):E198-204. doi: 10.1188/09.ONF
.E198-E204. PubMed PMID: 19581223.

Abstract

PURPOSE/OBJECTIVES: To determine whether dry versus moist heat applicat


ion to the upper extremity improves IV
insertion rates. DESIGN: Two-group, randomized, controlled clinical design. SE
TTING: An academic cancer infusion center in the western United States. Samp
le: 136 hematologic outpatients with cancer or other malignancies. METHODS:
Participants were randomly assigned to dry or moist heat with warmed towels w
rapped around each patient's arm for
seven minutes prior to IV insertion. Skin and room temperatures were monitore
d pre- and postwarming. Two experienced
chemotherapy infusion nurses performed the venipunctures according to protoc
ol. Outcomes were examined using varianceanalysis, with 34 patients for each c
ombination of nurse and heat type. MAIN RESEARCH VARIABLES: Number of
IV
insertion attempts, time to achieve IV insertion postheating, patient anxiety lev
els pre- and postheating, and patient
comfort.Findings: Dry heat was 2.7 times more likely than moist heat to result i
n successful IV insertion on the first
attempt, had significantly lower insertion times, and was more comfortable. Hea
t type had no effect on patient anxiety.
CONCLUSIONS: Dry heat application decreases the likelihood of multiple IV ins
ertion attempts and procedure time and is
comfortable, safe, and economical to use in an outpatient oncology setting.

You will need to read the full article to address the validity questions. Click on
the link above to get a free copy of the article. Evaluating the medical literature
is a complex undertaking. You will find that the answers to the questions of
validity may not always be clearly stated in the article and that you may have
to use your own judgment about the importance and significance of each
question.

Randomization: Were patients randomized?

Concealed allocation: Was group allocation concealed?

Baseline characteristics: Were patients in the study groups similar with respect
to known prognostic variables?

Blinding: To what extent was the study blinded?


Follow-up: Was follow-up complete?

Intention to Treat: Were patients analyzed in the groups to which they were
first allocated?

Equal treatment: Aside from the experimental intervention, were the groups
treated equally?

What are the results & how can I apply them to patient care?

Results: Controlling for prewarming vein status, dry heat was 2.7 times more
likely to result in successful IV insertion. After controlling for preinsertion
anxiety, vein status, and the participants’ number of venipunctures in the prior
year, dry heat resulted in significantly lower insertion times than moist heat. No
significant difference was found between the heat modalities or between nurses
on postinsertion patient reported anxiety scores. Dry heat was associated with
significantly higher participant self-reported comfort after controlling for
preinsertion anxiety and vein status and the participants’ numbers of
venipunctures in the prior year.

The results are reported as a continuous variable (change in scores). This does not
allow for calculations of Absolute and relative differences or number -needed-to-
treat.

Questions to consider before applying the results of a study to your


patient:

 Were the study patients similar to my population of interest?


 Does your patient match the study inclusion criteria? If not, are there compelling
reasons why the results should not apply to your patient?
 Were all clinically important outcomes considered?
 Are the likely treatment benefits worth the potential harm and costs?

APPLY: talk with the patient

Return to the Nursing Council -- integrate that evidence with clinical expertise,
patient preferences and apply it to practice
You bring this article to the Nursing Council and any
other studies that you found to address this issue. The
Nursing Council needs to consider the results of thess
studies, the benefits, the adverse effects, the cost and
the training involved in changing clinical procedures. All
of these factors need to be factored into a decision.

You might also like