RAUT - Narcissism and Justice Book Chapter
RAUT - Narcissism and Justice Book Chapter
RAUT - Narcissism and Justice Book Chapter
NARCISSISM OR JUSTICE: IS IT
IN THE EYE OF THE BEHOLDER?
ABSTRACT
Social psychological conceptions of narcissism have conceptualized
it as a dispositional trait, leading to what might be termed the “Passover
question”: How is this trait different from all other traits? This chapter
responds to that question by classifying narcissism instead as a label for
the activation of a universal justice motive (M. J. Lerner 2003, D. T.
Miller 2001). That argument is developed and elucidated through
descriptions of multiple parallels between narcissistic behaviors and
documented responses to justice threat. In addition, multiple experimental
tests that could confirm or disconfirm our perspective are proposed.
INTRODUCTION
Narcissists are preoccupied with dreams of success, power, beauty,
and brilliance. They live on an interpersonal stage with exhibitionistic
behavior and demands for attention and admiration but respond to threats
Corresponding author: Yogesh Raut. Department of Psychology, MSC 3452, New Mexico State
University, P. O. Box 30001, Las Cruces, NM 88003-8001. E-mail: [email protected].
2 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
DEFINITIONS
Researchers conceptualize narcissism as a mélange of intrapersonal
cognitions and affects and interpersonal behaviors broadly organized around
the themes of constructing, presenting, maintaining, and defending the self-
concept (Morf and Rhodewalt 2001). For the purposes of simplicity, in
cataloging the components of the narcissism syndrome below we will accept
any operationalization of narcissism that has been published in a major peer-
reviewed journal; later in the chapter, we will present our own conception.
Nearly all of these operationalizations use some version of the dyadic, forced-
choice, self-report measure known as the Narcissistic Personality Inventory
(NPI, Raskin and Terry 1988).
Justice, for the purposes of this paper, refers to a tightly linked
constellation of cognitions comprising distributive justice (the belief that
treatment and outcomes should be proportional to one’s worth), procedural
justice (the belief that one is entitled to voice objections to, and hear
justifications for, one’s treatment by others), and retributive justice (the belief
that it is permissible to take negative actions against the source of an
injustice1). Justice threat occurs when a violation of distributive or procedural
justice (pertaining either to the self or to others 2) is witnessed; it is
accompanied by a compulsion to act in the service of retributive justice (see D.
T. Miller 2001, for an overview). Justice is a concern of animals (Brosnan and
de Waal, 2003) and very small childrenhuman babies (c.f. Bloom 2013) and is
considered a basic foundation of universal morality (Fehr, Bernhard and
Rockenbach 2008) and is considered a basic foundation of human morality
(Haidt 2012) linked to an adaptive suite of cognitive, affective, and perceptual
1
Some have demarcated retributive justice from restorative justice, which is a dispassionate
motive to exact only a limited amount of punishment, but Gollwitzer (2009) convincingly
argues that this distinction does not exist in practice.
2
See Meindl and M. J. LernerLerner (1983) for evidence that the justice motive extends beyond
the self.
4 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
showing that after justice threat is invoked, justice-related words “pop out”
such that they are more detectable near the threshold of awareness (Gantman
and Van Bavel 2014) and create more interference on a modified Stroop task
(Hafer 2000).
Dualism. Just as narcissists see others as either angels or demons, a
tendency called splitting in the psychodynamic literature (Kernberg 1975,
Kohut 1971, see Siegel 2006 for empirical validation), perceivers tend to
assimilate injustice scenarios to a schema that separates the parties involved
into a moral patient, who is harmed, and moral agent, who does the harming.
This process is known as moral typecasting (Gray, Young and Waytz 2012).
Self-defeating behavior. Vazire and Funder (2006) and J. D. Miller et al.
(2009) both connect narcissism with self-defeating behavior, or behavior that Formatted: Font: Italic
undermines one’s own goals, life success, and well-being. Correspondingly,
while M. J. LernerLerner (2003) notes that combating injustice drains
cognitive resources and impairs the ability to act in one’s best interests (see pp.
394-396). At a broader level, narcissistic behaviors, such as self-enhancement,
are often linked to positive consequences in the short term and negative
consequences in the long term (Colvin, Block, and Funder 1995, Paulhus
1998, Robins and Beer 2001). This pattern is mirrored in the lives of people
who perceive themselves as victims of discrimination; that perception helps
buffer their self-esteem in the short term (Crocker and Major 2003) but has a
deleterious effect on their long-term well-being (Branscombe, Schmitt and
Harvey 1999).
Emotions, motivation, and self-regulation. Viewing or experiencing
injustice is typically accompanied by a combination of anger and a powerful
drive to engage in active punishment (Goldberg, J. S. Lerner and Tetlock
1999, Mikula, Scherer and Athenstaedt 1998), and while linkages remain
speculative at the moment, it is suggestive that narcissists are particularly
prone to both reactive anger (Rhodewalt and Morf 1998) and proactive (or
approach) motivation-related behaviors (Foster and Trimm 2008). In addition,
both narcissists (Tracy, Cheng, Robins and Trzesniewski 2009) and victims of
prejudice (Crocker and Major 1989) engage in self-protective attributions in
order to ward off feelings of shame.
Lack of empathy. Narcissists have the capacity for empathy, but rarely feel
it (Hepper, Hart and Sedikides 2014). It seems plausible that sufferers of
injustice would be equally unlikely to extend a warm heart to non-sufferers
(c.f. D. T. Miller 1977), and the results of DeWall and Baumeister (2006,
Study 4) are indirect but suggestive in this regard. They found that participants
who were giving false feedback stating they would end up alone in life due to
6 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
their personality were unempathetic toward a target who had recently gone
through a breakup.
While the authors did not interpret these results through the lens of justice,
it does not seem unreasonable to assume that being unilaterally informed that
one would will be denied the experience of lifelong companionship, especially
in a culture wherewhen one believes that most people in one’s culture do get to
have that experience, activated activates a sense of both distributive and
procedural injustice. The solicitation of sympathy on behalf of a person whose
suffering was comparatively minor no doubt exacerbated the experience
feelings of relative deprivation, to the extent that one participant
spontaneously responded, “Tough shit!” (p. 10).
Hostility and antagonism. Hostility and antagonism, extremely common
components of the narcissism syndrome (Rhodewalt and Morf 1995, Ruiz,
Smith and Rhodewalt 2001), also rise in response to prejudiced treatment (e.g.,
Major, Kaiser and McCoy 2003).
Politicized collectives, which are often formed around fairness concerns,
make adversarial attributions for the behavior of others (Simon and
Klandermans 2001). Recipients of manipulated social rejection, which as
noted above is possibly construed as justice threat, are more likely to project
hostility into neutral information (DeWall, Twenge, Gitter and Baumeister
2009). Participants who were given a test manipulated to be difficult and told
that it was diagnostic of their achievement were subsequently rated as hostile
and antagonistic by interaction partners, but only when they had high self-
esteem (Heatherton and Vohs 2000); those with low self-esteem likely did not
consider the test to be unfair.
Aggression. Narcissism is generally associated with aggression in
response to threat (Baumeister, Bushman and Campbell 2000), but some of the
most famous studies of narcissistic aggression suggest a pattern in line with
retributive justice concerns. In Bushman and Baumeister (1998), narcissists
who were given gratuitously cruel feedback on an essay they had written, and
were given no chance to answer the criticisms, aggressed against the evaluator,
but not against a third party. This is in line with research on general
populations, who respond to unfairness with aggression (Baron 1988, Baron,
Neuman and Geddes 1999) but mitigate that aggression when they feel that the
target doesn’t deserve it (Johnson and Rule 1986, Kremer and Stephens 1983,
Twenge, Baumeister, Tice and Stucke 2001).
In apparent contrast, Twenge and Campbell (2003) found that threatened
narcissists did aggress against an innocent third party. However, in this case
they had been rejected by an entire group, and as Twenge (2005) pointed out,
Narcissism or Justice: Is It in the Eye of the Beholder? 7
the third party was a student at the same university as the rejecting group and
thus could be classified as part of them. Taken together, these results are
consistent with a rule of personal retribution for a personal crime and
collective retribution for a collective crime. Twenge and Campbell (2003)
even explicitly invoke justice concerns in the title of their article, quoting a
statement by one of the Columbine shooters: “Isn’t it fun to get the respect that
we’re going to deserve?”
As the attendant was about to close the hatch, three White men in
suits entered the plane, were informed they could sit anywhere, and
promptly seated themselves in front of us. Just before take-off, the
attendant proceeded to close all overhead compartments and seemed to
scan the plane with her eyes. At that point she approached us, leaned
over, interrupted our conversation, and asked if we would mind moving
to the back of the plane. She indicated that she needed to distribute
weight on the plane evenly.
For a few seconds she said nothing but looked at me with a horrified
expression. Then she said in a righteously indignant tone, “Well, I have
never been accused of that! How dare you? I don’t see color! I only asked
you to move to balance the plane. Anyway, I was only trying to give you
more space and greater privacy.”
absolute terms, but the extent to which the gap between self-evaluation and
other-evaluation initiates feelings of disrespect and injustice, which in turn
give rise to behavior that is labeled narcissistic by those who do not see it as a
proportionate response.
As for the source of individual differences in narcissistic behavior, they
are accounted for by the objective behavior of the other party and the
individual’s context-specific sensitivity to that behavior. Research by Skitka
(2002; Skitka, Bauman and Sargis 2005) has shown that when the behavior of
another triggers a moral mandate by violating a strong moral conviction (such
as that invoked by justice concerns), individuals respond with a desire to
punish, mediated by anger (Mullen and Skitka 2006) and linked to
endorsement of extreme punitive aggression (Skitka and Houston 2001).
Our view is also consistent with the observation by Vazire and Funder
(2006) and J. D. Miller et al. (2009) that narcissism is marked by a failure to
gain support for one’s beliefs and behaviors in the eyes of others. In their
formulations, narcissism refers to a trait (or cluster of traits) correlated with
the failure to evoke support; in ours, narcissism is a description of what
happens when support is not achieved 3.
However, this raises the question of why narcissism is positively
associated with certain characteristics that should make it easier to gain
support, such as being physically attractive (Holtzman and Strube 2010),
superficially likable (Paulhus 1998), or famous (Young and Pinsky 2006). We
believe this occurs because aggrieved people who gain support in the eyes of
others can end up like gamblers on a winning streak: they bet bigger and
bigger until they lose. This is especially true when a context shift means that
they are no longer evaluated by those who originally offered support (see
Campbell and Campbell 2009).
A case study can be seen in research on the culture of honor in parts of the
United States. Men from states that are part of this culture tend to construe
social situations as insults and feel compelled to respond to insult with
aggression (D. Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle and Schwarz 1996), a propensity that
is condoned in the eyes of others (D. Cohen and Nisbett 1994) and therefore
perpetuated (Nisbett and D. Cohen 1996). Accounts of narcissistic aggression
(c.f. Baumeister 1997) are virtually indistinguishable from this syndrome.
Evidence consistent with a less macro-level version of this process was
found by Sell, Tooby, and Cosmides (2009), who report that what they term
formidable individuals – i.e., those with the greatest power to inflict costs and
3
See Kaiser and Miller (2001) for a discussion of seeking social support for injustice claims.
12 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
benefits on others, which they equate with physical strength in men and
attractiveness in women – feel greater entitlement and anger. Anger is an
inherently interpersonal emotion (Averill 1983) linked to the perception of
insult (Lazarus 1991), and since formidable people almost by definition are
less likely to be the recipients of deliberate insult, formidable peoplethey
appear to be perceiving more insult in ambiguous or neutral situations, just
like narcissists. Moreover, due to their formidability they probably also receive
inflated social support for their hypersensitive appraisals. As with men in the
culture of honor, multivalent labeling allows narcissism-like behaviors to
flourish in formidable people up until the moment when those behaviors come
to the attention of an audience that does not support them, at which point they
are labeled full-blown narcissism.
group (Lind and Tyler 1988), and thus there is logic to the notion that one
would react to injustice by asserting one’s worth through self-enhancement.
Still, while self-enhancing illusions have been documented in narcissists
(Gabriel, Critelli and Ee 1994, Gosling, John, Craik and Robins 1998, John
and Robins 1994), no parallel findings appear in the literature on injustice. We
hope to close this gap with future research.
Finally, we believe it would be instructive to apply our perspective to the
domain of aggression reduction. In line with theories tying narcissism to the
self, researchers have reduced aggression in narcissists by buttressing self-
esteem through a self-affirmation process (Thomaes, Bushman, Orobio de
Castro, G. L. Cohen and Denissen 2009, see Sherman and G. L. Cohen 2006,
for more on self-affirmation generally). We find this consistent with our
perspective, in that self-affirmation might affirm justice by restoring a sense of
voice (c.f. Lind, Kanfer and Early 1990), or it may simply counteract the
aversive affective component of justice threat. However, our perspective
makes the unique prediction that narcissistic aggression can also be reduced by
affirming a sense of justice independent of the self.
While a basic experiment to test this (following the template of Bushman
and Baumeister 1998) would be straightforward – a 1 x 3 design with the
levels being narcissists under no threat, narcissists under threat with no buffer,
and narcissists under threat who are allowed to affirm justice independent of
the self, with interpersonal aggression serving as a dependent variable –
several conceptual concerns arise. One concern has to do with the nature of the
justice-affirmation manipulation: simply reflecting on fairness, or engaging in
an exercise that allows the participant to behave fairly, may actually backfire –
similar to the affirmation backfire described by Sherman and G. L. Cohen
(2006, pp. 218-221) – through triggering feelings of relative deprivation
(Olson, Herman, and Zanna, 1986) by reminding participants of how their own
situation falls short of norms of fairness.
Another issue is that it is not completely clear that if affirming something
– whether self-esteem or justice – reduces narcissistic aggression, then
aggression must have been caused by a threat to that thing in the first place.
After all, taking aspirin removes a headache, but headaches are not caused by
a lack of aspirin; in a similar fashion, affirmations may simply alleviate the
affective symptoms of the original threat.
Also, the nature of the psychological system that connects threat and
defensiveness remains vague (see Hart 2014, for an overview of issues). For
example, some theorists argue that both self-esteem and worldview are part of
a larger integrated system of meaning (Heine, Proulx and Vohs 2006), which
14 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
REFERENCES
Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: implications
Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 28, 1145-
1160.
Baron, R. A. (1988). Negative effects of destructive criticism: Impact on
conflict, self-efficacy, and task performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 73, 199-207.
Baron, R. A., Neuman, J. H. and Geddes, D. (1999). Social and personal
determinants of workplace aggression: Evidence for the impact of
perceived injustice and the Type A Behavior Pattern. Aggressive
Behavior, 25, 281-296.
Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J. and Killian, A. L. (2003). The relation of narcissism
and self-esteem to conduct problems in children: A preliminary
investigation. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32,
139-152.
Narcissism or Justice: Is It in the Eye of the Beholder? 15
Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Evil: Inside human violence and cruelty. New York:
W. H. Freeman.
Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J. and Campbell, W. K. (2000). Self-esteem,
narcissism, and aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or
from threatened egotism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9,
26-29.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L. and Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened
egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem.
Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.
Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D. (2001). Narcissism as addiction to esteem.
Psychological Inquiry, 12, 206-210.
Bies, R. J. and Moag, J. S. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication
criteria of fairness. In: R. J. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard and M. H. Bazerman
(Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (pp. 43-55). Greenwich,
CT: JAI Press.
Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. New York:
Crown.
Bosson, J. K., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Zeigler‐Hill, V., Jordan, C. H.
and Kernis, M. H. (2008). Untangling the links between narcissism and
self‐esteem: A theoretical and empirical review. Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 2(3), 1415-1439.
Bosson, J. and Swann, W. B., Jr. (2009). Self-esteem: Nature, origins, and
consequences. In: R. Hoyle and M. Leary (Eds.), Handbook of individual
differences in social behavior (pp. 527-546). New York: Guilford.
Bradlee, P. M. and Emmons, R. A. (1992). Location of narcissism within the
interpersonal circumplex of the five-factor model. Personality and
Individual Differences, 13, 821-830.
Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T. and Harvey, R. D. (1999). Perceiving
pervasive discrimination among African Americans: Implications for
group identification and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Formatted: Font: Italic
Psychology, 77, 135-149.
Brosnan, S. F. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay.
Nature, 425, 297-299.
Bushman, B. J. and Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism,
self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-
hate lead to violence? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
219-229.
Campbell, W. K. (2001a). Is narcissism really so bad? Psychological Inquiry,
12, 214-216.
16 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
Sell, A., Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of
human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106(35),
15073-15078.
Sherman, D. K. and Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense:
Self-affirmation theory. In: M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183-242). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Siegel, J. P. (2006). Dyadic splitting in partner relational disorders. Journal of
Family Psychology, 20(3), 418-422.
Simon, B. and Klandermans, B. (2001). Politicized collective identity: A
social psychological analysis. American Psychologist, 56, 319-331.
Skitka, L. (2002). Do the means always justify the ends, or do the ends
sometimes justify the means? A value protection model of justice
reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 588-597.
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W. and Sargis, E. G. (2005). Moral conviction:
Another contributor to attitude strength or something more? Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 895-917.
Skitka, L. J. and Houston, D. (2001). When due process is of no consequence:
Moral mandates and presumed defendant guilt or innocence. Social
Justice Research, 14, 305-326.
Sue, D. W. (1998). A personal look at psychology in my life. In: L. T.
Hoshman (Ed.), Creativity and moral vision in psychology (pp. 106-125).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sue, D. W. (2001). Surviving monoculturalism and racism: A personal and
professional journey. In: J. G. Ponterotto, J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki and C.
M. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 45-54).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., Torino, G. C., Bucceri, J. M., Holder, A. M.
B., Nadal, K. L. and Esquilin, M. (2007). Racial microaggressions in
everyday life: Implications for clinical practice. American Psychologist,
62, 271-286.
Swann, W. B., Jr. and Buhrmester, M. (2015). Identity fusion. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 52-57.
Tetlock, P. E. and Fincher, K. (2015). Social functionalism. In: B. Gawronski
and G. V. Bodenhausen (Eds.), Theory and explanation in social
psychology (pp. 266-282). New York: Guilford Press.
Thomaes, S., Bushman, B. J., Orobio de Castro, B., Cohen, G. L. and
Denissen, J. J. A. (2009). Reducing narcissistic aggression by buttressing
22 Yogesh J. Raut and David Trafimow
N.G.