Morphologization in Turkish: Implications For Phonology in Grammaticalization
Morphologization in Turkish: Implications For Phonology in Grammaticalization
Morphologization in Turkish: Implications For Phonology in Grammaticalization
Phonology in Grammaticalization1
1. Introduction
Grammaticalization theory assumes that the gradual progression from
a content item to a grammatical marker is accompanied by a number
of interdependent phonological, morphosyntactic, and functional
processes. Accordingly, morphologization processes, such as
cliticization and compounding, are said to be concomitant with
phonological erosion and desemantization (Lehmann [1982] 1995,
Heine & Reh 1984, Heine, Claudi & Hünnemeyer 1991, Hopper &
Traugott 1993, Croft 2003). Some proponents of this theory even
claim that the loss of autonomy and substance defines
grammaticalization as opposed to other mechanisms in language
change, for instance reanalysis (Haspelmath 1998).
The role of phonology in grammaticalization has recently been
reconsidered in the context of a cross-linguistic study on cliticization
(Schiering 2006). A number of phonological processes, namely
structure preservation, assimilation, weakening and strengthening, can
accompany ongoing grammaticalization. The distribution of these
phonological rules can be predicted by a rhythm-based typology of
language which distinguishes between mora-, syllable- and stress-
based languages. With respect to erosion, mora- and syllable-based
languages tend to retain the phonological substance of cliticized
elements which ultimately leads to disyllabic clitics and affixes.
Stress-based languages, on the other hand, tend to reduce and delete
1
The research for this paper was carried out at the University of Leipzig as part of
the project ‘Typology and Theories of the Word’, supported by the German Science
Foundation (DFG). For comments and suggestions I am grateful to Balthasar Bickel,
Ann Denwood, Geoff Haig, Barış Kabak.
2
2
All information and data on Old Turkic and Modern Turkish have been taken from
the following reference grammars: Gabain (1950), Erdal (2004), Kornfilt (1997) and
Lewis (2000).
3
labial assimilation are not restricted to smaller domains, but span the
word domain, e.g. anla-yacak ‘s/he will understand’ and göz-lük-çü
‘optician, oculist’.
The distribution of the various phonological properties over the
rhythm prototypes is illustrated in Figure 1. Note that Turkish behaves
like a typical representative of syllable-based rhythm. In fact, the
rhythmic profile of Turkish has not changed in the course of the
development from Old Turkic to Modern Turkish.
Figure 1: Selected phonological properties of rhythmic prototypes
Mora-based Syllable-based Stress-based
Accent none/weak none/weak strong
Stress effect none/weak none/weak strong
Syllable types simple moderate complex
Length contrasts unrestricted restricted restricted
Vowel harmony word word disyllabic
The rhythm-based typology of language makes a number of
predictions with respect to possible phonological effects of
grammaticalization. Within stress prosodies, such as Turkish, the
model predicts gradual stress reduction, i.e. from primary stress to
secondary stress to unstressed, and gradual integration into the word
domain for stress placement, i.e. from unstressable to stressable (see
Selkirk 1995 for a formal analysis). Whereas this stress reduction goes
hand in hand with segmental reduction in stress-based rhythm,
languages of the other rhythm classes have no reductive potential in
their phonologies and retain the grammaticalized element.
Accordingly, we would not expect erosion to accompany
grammaticalization in a syllable-based language like Turkish. Since
vowel harmony processes are sensitive to the word domain in
languages of this rhythmic type, gradual integration into the word
domain should also manifest itself in the inclusion of the
grammaticalized element into the vowel harmony domain. Since
morphophonological rules operating at the morpheme boundary occur
in languages of all rhythmic classes, junctural processes such as
cluster simplification and coalescence are possible and provide the
5
3. Morphologization in Turkish
To test the predictions made by the typology outlined in the previous
section, we will examine two cases of grammaticalization in Turkish,
namely the cliticization of personal pronouns which led to subject
agreement marking (Givón 1976) and the univerbation of verbal
complexes which led to new aspect-tense markers (Lehmann [1982]
1995).
In Old Turkic, sentences with pronominal subjects were formed
with a postponed pronoun at the end of the non-verbal or verbal
predicate. For the sake of emphasis or contrast, another personal
pronoun could be placed in preverbal subject position. In thirteenth
century texts, the postponed pronouns appear cliticized to the
preceding word. In Modern Turkish, the cliticized subject pronouns
form the back bone of the z-paradigm of subject agreement marking
(cf. Adamović 1985, Kornfilt 1996, Good & Yu 2005). The various
stages in this diachronic development are exemplified for the first
person singular in the examples (1)-(3).
(1) (ben) kelür ben ‘I am coming’
(2) (ben) gelür-ven ~ (ben) gelür-em ‘I am coming’
(3) (ben) gelír-im ‘I come’
Although the construction as such did not change significantly, the
phonological status of the bound morphemes underwent a number of
changes. Assuming that both kelür and ben constitute words in (1), the
combination is prosodized as a phonological phrase in which the first
word receives stress. In (2) and (3), this phrasal stress has been
reinterpreted as irregular word stress in which the prominence lies on
the last syllable of the host (cf. Kabak & Vogel 2001 and Inkelas &
Orghun 2003). Thus, the cliticized element does not get prosodically
integrated in the domain for word-final stress. The reduction of stress
from phrasal to word-level stress is not accompanied by segmental
reduction but by the integration of the cliticized element into the
6
References
Adamović, Milan 1985. Konjugationsgeschichte der türkischen Sprache. Leiden:
Brill.
Ağcagül, Sevgi 2004. Grammaticalization of Turkic postverbial construction.
Orientalia Suecana 53, 5-14.
Auer, Peter 2001. Silben- und akzentzählende Sprachen. In: Haspelmath, Martin &
König, Ekkehard & Oesterreicher, Wulf & Raible, Wolfgang (eds.) Language
typology and language universals. Berlin, New York: de Gruyter. 1391-1399.
Bainbridge, Margaret 1988. -(y)E-bil- and the conjunction dE: splitting the
unsplittable. In: Koç, Sabri (ed.) Studies in Turkish linguistics. Ankara:
METU. 95-115.
Beckman, Mary E. 1986. Stress and non-stress accent. (Netherland phonetic
archives 7.) Dordrecht: Foris.
Blevins, Juliette 1995. The syllable in phonological theory. In: Goldsmith, John A.
(ed.) The handbook of phonological theory. (Blackwell handbooks in
linguistics, 1). Oxford: Blackwell. 206-244.
Bybee, Joan & Chakraborti, Paromita & Jung, Dagmar & Scheibman, Joanne 1998.
Prosody and segmental effect. Some paths of evolution for word stress.
Studies in Language 22, 267-314.
Croft, William 2003². Typology and universals. (Cambridge textbooks in
linguistics.) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dufter, Andreas 2003. Typen sprachrhythmischer Konturbildung. (Linguistische
Arbeiten 475.) Tübingen: Niemeyer.
Erdal, Marcel 1979. Die Konverb- und Aoristendungen des Alttürkischen.
Ural-Altaische Jahrbücher 51, 104–126.
Erdal, Marcel 2004. A grammar of Old Turkic. (Handbook of Oriental studies,
Section 8, Central Asia 3.) Leiden: Brill.
Gabain, A. von 1950². Alttürkische Grammatik. (Porta linguarum orientalium 23.)
Leipzig: Harrassowitz.
10
Givón, Talmy 1976. Topic, pronoun, and grammatical agreement. In: Li, Charles N.
(ed.) Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press. 149-188.
Good, Jeff & Yu, Alan C. L. 2005. Morphosyntax of two Turkish subject
pronominal paradigms. In: Heggie, Lorie & Ordóñez, Francisco (eds.) Clitic
and affix combinations. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 315-341.
Haspelmath, Martin 1998. Does grammaticalization need reanalysis? Studies in
Language 22, 315-351.
Heine, Bernd & Claudi, Ulrike & Hünnemeyer, Friederike 1991.
Grammaticalization. A conceptual framework. Chicago, London: University
of Chicago Press.
Heine, Bernd & Kuteva, Tania 2002. World lexicon of grammaticalization.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Heine, Bernd & Reh, Mechthild 1984. Grammaticalization and reanalysis in African
languages. Hamburg: Buske.
Hopper, Paul J. & Traugott, Elizabeth C. 1993. Grammaticalization. (Cambridge
textbooks in linguistics.) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Inkelas, Sharon & Orgun, Cemil O. 2003. Turkish stress: a review. Phonology 20,
139-161.
Johanson, Lars 1998. The structure of Turkic. In: Johanson, Lars & Csató, Éva Á.
(eds.) The Turkic languages. London, New York: Routledge. 30-66.
Kabak, Barış & Vogel, Irene 2001. The phonological word and stress assignment in
Turkish. Phonology 18, 315-360.
Kabak, Barış & Schiering, René 2004. A corpus study on the distribution of Turkish
ise and its clitic form. Turkic Languages 8, 232-244.
Kornfilt, Jaklin 1996. On copular clitic forms in Turkish. ZAS Papers in Linguistics
6, 96-114.
Kornfilt, Jaklin 1997. Turkish. (Descriptive grammars.) London, New York:
Routledge.
Lehmann, Christian [1982] 1995. Thoughts on grammaticalization. (Lincom studies
in theoretical linguistics 1.) München: Lincom Europa.
Lewis, Geoffrey 2000². Turkish Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Schiering, René 2006. Cliticization and the evolution of morphology. A cross-
linguistic study on phonology in grammaticalization. Konstanz: Konstanzer
Online-Publikations-System, Bibliothek der Universität Konstanz.
[http://www.ub.uni-konstanz.de/kops/volltexte/2006/1872/]
Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1995. The prosodic structure of function words. In: Beckman,
Jill N. & Dickey, Laura W. & Urbanczyk, Suzanne (eds.) Papers in
optimality theory. (University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers 18.)
Amherst: GLSA. 439-469.