The Anxiety Sensitivity Index:: Construct Validity and Factor Analytic Structure
The Anxiety Sensitivity Index:: Construct Validity and Factor Analytic Structure
The Anxiety Sensitivity Index:: Construct Validity and Factor Analytic Structure
W + cn,
Prmed m the USA All right, reserved. Copyngh! ‘c IYX7 Perpmon Journal\. Ltd.
The data available thus far suggests that the ASI is a reliable and valid
measure of the personality variable of anxiety sensitivity (fear of fear).
Further data on reliability, factor structure, and independence from other
anxiety, or presumed fear of anxiety, measures are-needed to help eval-
uate the adequacy of the measure.
METHOD
Subjects
Measures
RESULTS
The mean scores on the ASI were similar to those expected for a self-
reported anxious group. The males obtained a mean of 19.69 with a SD
of 9.63, which is_significantly higher than the Reiss et al. (1986) norm
sample scores ofX = 15.4, SD = 8.1 (t (100) = 2.35. p < .OS) but lower
than the x = 25.8 s_cores for the Reiss et al. anxiety disorder group. The
females obtained a X mean ofJ3.60, SD = 10.90, which was significantly
higher than the norm group (X = 20.5, SD = 10.2; t (162) =_ 1.87, p <
.05, one-tailed), and similar to the female anxiety group (X = 23.9).
Thus, the self-reported anxious college students obtained higher ASI
scores than did the Reiss et al. (1986) normal sample.
Internal reliability was evaluated by obtaining an internal consistency
reliability of alpha (.88), and a Guttman split-half reliability c.85). The
factor structure and relationship of the AS1 to other measures was ob-
tained through a series of factor and correlational analysis. Four factors
were extracted by principal components analysis to the criterion of ei-
genvalues of 1.0 or greater. Those factors were rotated to an oblique
simple structure. Two factors were associated with an a priori “fear of
consequences” factor and two with the a priori “fear of physical sensa-
tion” factor and accounted for 60.9% of the variance. A varimax rotation
analysis, with a specified two factor structure, was used to further eval-
ANXIETY SENSITIVITY INDEX 119
uate the a priori factor. The two uncorrelated factors; resulted in 1I of the
16 items loading on Factor I. A second factor contained five items with
the heaviest loading on that factor, but three of the five items also loaded
relatively high on Factor 1. ’
The AS1 total score, RRAQ total score, and the CSAQ total score
were factor analyzed using an oblique rotation. The ASI formed one
factor, whereas the RRAQ and CSAQ formed the other factor (see Table
l), with the two factors having only 29% shared variance (r = S4). A
second analysis entered the a priori content factor of the ASI and the
unweighted content factor scores proposed fo the CSAQ and RRAQ into
an oblique rotation factor analysis. The AS1 subscales and the CSAQ and
RRAQ subscales formed separate factors as shown in Table 2, with a
correlation of .53 between the two factors.
Another method of determining the amount of common variance be-
tween measures is through the use of a regression analysis. The data
were submitted to a stepwise regression analysis in which the AS1 served
as the dependent measure and the RRAQ and CSAQ as predictor vari-
ables. The predictor variables accounted for thirty-nine percent of the
variance of the AS1 (adjusted R 2 = .392), which is a significant propor-
tion of variance, but, more importantly for the question of amount of
independence of measures, leaves the majority of variance of the AS1
unaccounted for by anxiety measures.
DISCUSSION
The internal consistency of the AS1 is appropriate and the measure
produced group scores consistent with the expectations for the self-de-
fined sample of anxious college students. Females, in general, obtain
higher AS1 scores than males. This may reflect a sex difference in the
valence of anxiety symptoms or a sex difference in willingness to report
sensitivity to anxiety symptoms.
The factor structure results raise an issue about the best way to use the
measure. The four correlated factors resulting from the oblique rotation
are factors with only a few items in each factor. Thus, these factors may
be unreliable or lack sufficient power to strongly relate to other variables.
TABLE I
REGRESSION WEIGHTS FOR OBLIQUE ROTATION ON TOTAL SCALE SCORES
Factor 1 Factor 2
’ Factor loadings are available from the first author upon request.
130 R. A. PETERSONANDR. L. HEILBRONNER
TABLE'
OBLIQUEROTATION FAON THEFACTORSCORESFORTHEASI.CSAQ AND RRAQ
Factor I Factor 2
AS1
-
SENS -0.01460 0.95331
CONS 0.06655 0.X962
CsAQ
RRAQ
The varimax rotation for a two factor structure resulted in the second
factor containing only five items. Also, the majority of the second factor
items loaded highly on Factor 1 as well as Factor 2. The loading on the
first factor is very similar to the first factor loadings reported by Reiss et
al. (1986), suggesting a strong single factor. Whether or not the second
factor will be reliable, and whether different relationships exist for the
two factors, remains to be demonstrated. The ASI total scale score has
demonstrated validity (McNally & Lorenz, 1986; Reiss et al., 1986).
Thus, the total scale score should be used in future clinical and research
work until evidence for the validity of factor scores has been demon-
strated.
The separation in factors of the ASI and the CSAQiRRAQ and the AS1
variance unaccounted for by the CSAQ and RRAQ in the regression anal-
ysis, along with previous data which indicates the AS1 is measuring a
construct (anxiety sensitivity) different from the anxiety construct and
usual anxiety measures (Reiss et al., 1986), suggests the AS1 measures
anxiety sensitivity, while the CSAQ and RRAQ appear to measure anx-
iety. The relationship between the AS1 and anxiety measures appears to
depend on the type of measure and population under investigation.
McNally and Lorenz (1986) obtained a .36 (p < .05) correlation with the
Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale for an agoraphobic population. while Sei-
denberg and Peterson (1986) obtained a correlation of .33 (p < .OS) be-
tween the AS1 and Spielberger state score and .07 (p < .05) between the
AS1 and Spielberger trait score in a sample of first year medical students.
The relationship between the AS1 and other measures of anxiety is ex-
pected to be stronger (more related) in a high anxiety population than a
low anxiety population. Since anxiety sensitivity is conceptualized as a
ANXIETY SENSITIVITY INDEX 121
REFERENCES
Chambless. D. L.. Caputo. C.. Gallagher. R.. & Bright. P. (1984). Assessment of fear of fear
in agoraphobia: The Body sensations questionnaire and agorphobic cognitions question-
naire. Jolrwctl of Cottsrrltinpand Clinical Psychology, 52, 1090- 1097.
Heide. E J.. 8i Borkovec. T. D. (1983). Relaxation-induced anxiety: Paradoxical anxiety
enhancement due to relaxation training. Jolrr~tal ofCons~r/ting und Clinkal Psychology.
51, p. 171-182.
Maller. R.. & Reiss. S. (1985. November). Anxief~ sensititirx and unxiet_v incubation.
Poster presented at the meeting of Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy,
Houston. TX.
McNally. R. J.. & Lorenz. M. (1986). Anxiety srrlsitilify in ugomphobics. Manuscript sub-
mitted for publication.
Reiss. S.. Br McNally,. R. J. (1985). The expectancy model of fear. In S. Reiss and R. R.
Bootzin IEds. ). Theoreticcrl issrre.7 in hehrrt?or therupy. New York: Academic Press.
Reiss. S.. Peterson. R. A.. Gursky. D. M.. & McNally. R. J. (1986). Anxiety sensitivity.
anxiety frequency. and the prediction of fearfulness. Bclm~ior Research and Therupy.
24. l-8.
Schwartz. G. E.. Davidson. R. J.. 8: Goleman. D. J. (1978). Patterning of cognitive and
somatic processes in the self-regulation of anxiety. Effects of meditation versus exer-
cise. P.~~cl~c~.so~~tnr;~~ Medicine. 40, 32 l-328.
Seidenberg. M.. & Peterson. R. A. (1986). [Prediction of success in medical school]. Un-
published rau data.